Abstract
Studies have identified insomnia as having significant influence on chronic pain. A rising body of research has also underscored the association between eveningness and chronic pain. However, co-assessment of insomnia and eveningness in the context of chronic pain adjustment has been limited. The present study sought to investigate the effects of insomnia and eveningness on pain severity, pain interference, and emotional distress (i.e., depressive and anxiety symptoms) over nearly two years among adults with chronic pain in the U.S. Adults with chronic pain (N=884) were surveyed three times via Amazon’s MTurk online crowdsourcing platform: baseline, 9-month follow-up, and 21-month follow-up. Path analysis was conducted to examine the effects of baseline insomnia severity (Insomnia Severity Index) and eveningness (Morningness and Eveningness Questionnaire), as well as their moderating effects on outcomes. Controlling for select socio-demographic variables and baseline outcome levels, greater insomnia severity at baseline was associated with worsening of all of the pain-related outcomes at 9-month follow-up, and pain interreference and emotional distress at 21-month follow-up. We did not find evidence that evening types are at a higher risk of experiencing worsening pain-related outcomes over time compared to morning and intermediate types. There were also no significant insomnia severity and eveningness moderation effects on any outcome. Our findings suggest that insomnia is a more robust predictor of changes in pain-related outcomes as compared to eveningness. Treatment of insomnia can be important in chronic pain management. Future studies should evaluate the role of circadian misalignment on pain using more accurate biobehavioral makers.
Keywords: insomnia, circadian preference, pain, depression, anxiety
Perspective
This study examined the effects of insomnia and eveningness on pain and emotional distress in a large sample of individuals with chronic pain. Insomnia severity is a stronger predictor of changes in pain and emotional distress than eveningness, highlighting insomnia as an important clinical target for chronic pain management.
Introduction
Insomnia is an important modifiable risk factor for the development and progression of chronic pain.1–3 A recent meta-analysis revealed that the overall prevalence of clinically meaningful sleep disturbances ranged from 73–75% among individuals with chronic pain.4 Although sleep and pain are bi-directionally related, evidence from longitudinal studies suggests that the effect of sleep disturbances on pain is stronger than that of pain on sleep disturbances.1 In fact, a number of laboratory studies of healthy adults also provide causal evidence that sleep deprivation and fragmentation that mimic severe insomnia contribute to greater pain sensitivity.1,5,6 Relatedly, a recent meta-analysis of clinical trials of cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) also suggests that CBT-I improves not only sleep, but also chronic pain.7
Emerging evidence suggests that eveningness or evening type (i.e., circadian preference for staying up late and waking up late), may also serve an important role in chronic pain development and progression, independent from sleep disturbances.8 Heikkala and colleagues9 found that even after controlling for sleep duration, evening types were more likely to report disabling pain among 4,961 individuals with musculoskeletal pain residing in Finland. The same research group published a 15-year follow-up study using the same cohort data suggesting that evening types, compared to morning types, were more likely to show persistent multisite musculoskeletal pain between ages 31 and 46 years.10 Furthermore, Heikkala and colleagues also found that evening types report a higher reduction in health-related quality of life when experiencing worsening musculoskeletal pain, compared to morning types.11 Importantly, studies on fibromyalgia and irritable bowel syndrome have shown that greater eveningness is associated with more severe symptom profiles, disease complications, and a lower quality of life.12–14
Emotional distress, which refers to anxiety and depressive symptoms, is considered an important pain-related outcome15,16, as pain can impact one’s emotional well-being.16,17 Although studies that longitudinally examine the effects of insomnia and circadian preference on emotional distress among individuals with chronic pain are scarce8, the deleterious effects of both insomnia and eveningness on emotional distress, and vice versa, in the general population are well-established. Overall, insomnia and eveningness are associated with development and maintenance of various mental health conditions, including mood and anxiety disorders.18–23
Despite the fact that mechanisms underlying insomnia and eveningness are closely intertwined (e.g., evening types are more likely to experience misalignment between the internal biological rhythm and the societal rhythm than morning or intermediate types, which can cause significant sleep loss and insomnia)24,25 and each may play a unique role in pain and emotional distress, their co-evaluation is scant in the literature. Also, to our knowledge, none of the previous studies have examined the effects of both insomnia severity and evening type (compared to morning and intermediate types) on pain-related outcomes (i.e., pain severity, pain interference, and emotional distress)15,26 prospectively among individuals with chronic pain. The present study tries to address these important gaps in the literature with use of a large dataset that longitudinally examined the effects of insomnia and eveningness on pain-related outcomes over 21 months. We hypothesized that individuals with greater insomnia severity and eveningness will exhibit worsened pain-related outcomes both in medium- (at 9-months) and long-term (at 21-months) follow-ups (see Figure 1). Although some studies suggested that evening-type insomniacs present greater levels of depression, lower positive affect, and blunted neural function in brain regions associated with positive affect compared to morning- or intermediate-types27,28, other studies showed no significant moderating effects of sleep disturbance in the association between eveningness and musculoskeletal pain.9,10 Given these mixed previous findings, we also explored the effects of an interaction between eveningness and insomnia severity on outcomes.
Figure 1.
Hypothesized Path Model
Note. e = error term. Double-headed arrows indicate correlation/covariance between variables. Baseline levels of outcomes (autocorrelations) are controlled in the model. Covariates include age, gender, ethnicity, income, and education.
Methods
The present study is a secondary data analysis from a parent study. The parent study aimed to investigate relationships between emotion regulation and pain-related experiences in adults with chronic pain, and the main outcome papers are previously published.29,30 We would like to note that the aims of the current study do not overlap with those in the parent study. In addition, the present study also incorporates the newly collected Time 4 data (described below) that stemmed from additional funding (i.e., PM&R Seed Grant from the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation at the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine) that we received after we published the main outcome papers.
Participants
Participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) at four time points throughout the study: Time 1 (04/22/2020 – 05/15/2020), Time 2 (07/29/2020 – 08/05/2020), Time 3 (05/07/2021 – 05/26/2021), and Time 4 (04/26/2022 – 06/02/2022). Cloud Research, a third-party data collection company, was used to recruit MTurk “workers” (they are not employees of the company Amazon) for the study. Previous studies have found that MTurk responses exhibit strong overall validity and reliability.31
An initial pain duration screening item was used to determine preliminary study eligibility. Those who reported experiencing pain more than half of the week, every week, over the past 3 months were invited to complete an additional screening questionnaire to determine definitive eligibility. Inclusion criteria were: (a) age ≥18 years; (b) average past week pain severity of ≥3/10; (c) U.S. residence; (d) English proficiency; and (e) willingness to participate in follow-up assessments. The following data quality standards based upon Meade and Craig32 were implemented to maximize the strength and reliability of survey responses: (1) inclusion of workers with ≥95% approval ratings from other MTurk requesters; (2) exclusion of participants who failed one or more attention check items out of three (e.g., “Please select Never True”); (3) exclusion of participants who took substantially greater (>60 minutes) or less than (<16 minutes) the average completion time of 35 minutes based upon pilot data.
A total of 30,096 workers responded to the initial one-item screening question. Of this group, 10,308 (34.3%) indicated the presence of chronic pain, 2,153 met the inclusion criteria, and 1,809 (84.0%) initiated the additional screening survey. A total of 1,484 (82%) individuals met the data quality standards. Of these, we identified 31 duplicate cases which were removed. As a result, the final Time 1 sample size was 1,453. These participants were invited to participate in Time 2 (3 months), 3 (12 months), and 4 (24 months) follow-up assessments. Among 1,453 participants, 884 (61%) were retained in Time 2, 813 (56%) were retained in Time 3, and 784 (54%) were retained in Time 4. As compared to retention rates of other longitudinal MTurk studies33, rates for the present study were marginally higher.
Note that in the present study, we focus on Time 2, 3, and 4 data, as the circadian preference measure was available from Time 2. Hence, the final sample used for the current study was N = 884 (those who completed Time 2 survey). Among 884 participants, 614 (70%) and 586 (66%) participants completed Time 3 (9-month follow-up from Time 2) and Time 4 (21-month follow-up from Time 2) surveys, respectively. In the present study, we use the term “baseline” to refer to Time 2. In sum, as the baseline of the present study starts from Time 2, the follow-up assessments were 9 (Time 3) and 21 months (Time 4) apart from Time 2.
Procedures
MTurk workers who met eligibility criteria were sent a link to complete self-report questionnaires. Participants were compensated $5–7 (approximately at a rate of $12/hour) for survey completion at each time point. All study procedures were approved by the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine (JHSOM) Institutional Review Board. In compliance with the JHSOM IRB policy for exempt applications, which asserts that the identity of human subjects must be well-protected, each participant was provided with a study outline, contact information for the principal investigator, and the JHSOM IRB number prior to consenting to participate.
Measures
Socio-demographics.
At baseline, participants reported their age, gender, race, ethnicity, education, income, marital status, and duration of chronic pain. The original items that were used to measure socio-demographic characteristics are available as online supplement material. Note that for statistical analyses, the present study focused on the gender binary (i.e., male and female), given that a very small proportion (0.5%) of individuals identified as non-binary and/or genderqueer.
Insomnia Severity.
The Insomnia Severity Index (ISI)34 assessed perceived severity of insomnia. ISI is a well-validated measure for insomnia and has been widely used in studies with individuals who have various types of chronic pain conditions.4 Each of the 7 items is rated on a scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). A total score with higher scores indicating greater insomnia severity was used in the present study. Total score can range from 0 to 28. Although some suggested clinical cutoffs of ISI exist, we elected to use the total score as a primary variable because (1) binary categorization of continuous variable can lead to substantial loss of statistical power to detect effects35; and (2) ISI is not a gold standard measure to diagnose individuals for insomnia disorder. Cronbach’s alpha at baseline was .90. Note that ISI was measured across all time points, but given that the present study was focusing on examining the medium- and long-term effects of baseline insomnia levels, we only focused on using the baseline ISI.
Circadian Preference.
We would like to note that morningess and eveningness are often inaccurately referred to as ‘chronotype.’ However, chronotype reflects a behavior (i.e., sleep midpoint) rather than a preference.36 Morningness and eveningness are an individual’s self-reported preference, and thus, using the term ‘circadian preference’ rather than chronotype is more accruate.36 The 19-item Horne-Östberg Morningness and Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ)37 was used to determine individuals’ circadian preference. The MEQ is well-validated and one of the most widely used self-report circadian preference measures.38 The MEQ features both Likert-type and time-scale items, with each section of the scale being scored from 1–5 to compute the global score. Once global scoring for the scale is determined, participants are categorized into one of the following three groups and corresponding scores: (1) morning type (>58); (2) intermediate (neither) type (scores between 42–58); and (3) evening type (<42).39 Evening type was used as a reference group in the present study. Cronbach’s alpha at baseline was .79.
Pain Severity and Interference.
The Brief Pain Inventory–Short Form40 was used to assess pain severity and pain interference at all time points (i.e., baseline, 9 months follow-up, and 21 months follow-up). For pain severity, participants rated their current pain, as well as least, worst, and average pain in the prior 24 hours based upon a scale of 0 (no pain) to 10 (pain as bad as you can imagine). A composite of these 4 items was created by taking the average. For pain interference, participants were asked to indicate the extent to which pain interfered with general activity, mood, walking ability, work, relations with others, sleep, and enjoyment of life based up on a scale of 0 (does not interfere) to 10 (completely interferes). A composite of these 7 items was created by taking the average. Cronbach’s alphas across time ranged from .90 to .91 for pain severity, and .92 to .93. for pain interference.
Depression and Anxiety Symptoms.
The 4-item PROMIS Emotional Distress-Depression and Anxiety scales41 assessed the severity of depressive and anxiety symptoms among participants over the past week. Each item was rated on a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always). Total scores were transformed into T-scores with a score of 50 reflecting the average for the general U.S. population. T-scores typically range from 20 to 80. These scales have demonstrated good internal reliability and validity within the chronic pain population.41 Cronbach’s alphas across time ranged from .92 to .94 for PROMIS Emotional Distress-Depression, and .90 to .91. for PROMIS Emotional Distress-Anxiety.
Power Analysis
As mentioned above, this is a secondary data analyses of a parent longitudinal study. Hence, we computed the sensitivity to detect an effect in a multiple regression model based upon the Time 2 sample size. G*Power showed that in a multiple regression model with 10 predictors, a sample of 884 with alpha level of .05 (two-tailed) can produce a statistical power of .80 to detect even very small effects (Cohen’s f2 = .009). Note that Cohen’s f2 values of .02, .15, and .35 indicate small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.
Data Analytic Strategy
First, descriptive statistics were computed to summarize the characteristics of the current sample. Second, attrition analyses were conducted on key sociodemographic variables, as well as predictor and outcome variables at baseline. Third, associations among circadian preferences, insomnia severity, and outcomes at baseline were examined. We conducted an ANOVA to explore mean differences in insomnia severity and outcomes measured at baseline among different circadian preferences. Pearson bi-variate correlations were conducted to examine relationships between insomnia severity and outcomes measured at baseline. Fourth, using the Mplus statistical software (Version 8.3), we conducted two main path models that examined: (1) predictive effects of insomnia severity and evening type (compared to morning and intermediate types) on pain severity, pain interference, and emotional distress (i.e., depressive and anxiety symptoms) assessed at 9-month follow-up; and (2) the same outcomes measured at 21-month follow-up. Given that circadian preference is a categorical predictor, we created two dummy variables by setting the evening type as a priori reference group. For both models, select socio-demographic variables (i.e., age, gender, ethnicity, income, and education) that are known to be related to pain-related outcomes42–44 were included as covariates. Note that race was not included as a covariate because our previous studies using the present data did not reveal any significant association with pain-related outcomes.45,46 Both education and income variables were dichotomized to aid the interpretability of the findings. Education was coded 0 = “Less than College Education” and 1 = “College or Above.” Income was coded 0 = “$49,999 or below” and 1 = “$50,000 or above.” In addition, as recommended previously47, we included the baseline measure of a given outcome (e.g., pain severity at baseline) as a covariate in these models. This approach also allowed for a change score interpretation associated with the outcome. Lastly, we examined insomnia severity and circadian preference interaction effects in these two models. Note that path model is an extension of linear regression in multivariate modeling. Hence, the path estimates can be interpreted the same way as regression estimates (e.g., B = .50 indicates that one unit change of independent variable results in .50 increase in the outcome).
To determine model fit of the path models, we employed several fit indices including the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; critical value ≥ .9048), Standardized Root Mean Residual (SRMR; critical value, ≤ .1049), and the Root Mean Squared Estimate of Approximation (RMSEA; critical value ≤ .0850). Missing data were handled by the full information maximum likelihood (FIML) method, which provides unbiased parameter estimates under the missing at random (MAR) assumption (i.e., the missingness is related to some of the observed data). In terms of effect sizes, we reported η2 (eta-squared) for ANOVA (small effect = .01, medium effect = 0.06, and large effect = 0.14) and standardized betas for path models. Lastly, a number of sensitivity analyses (i.e., excluding the sleep disturbance item from the pain interference measure, dichotomizing ISI variable based upon clinical cutoff score, and re-running analysis using total score of the MEQ) were conducted to further demonstrate the robustness of study findings.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Table 1 presents a summary of the participant characteristics. The mean age of participants was 44.2 years, and the majority of them were women (65.4%), White (87.3%), had at least some college education (87.8%), were working part- or full-time (63%), and were married (46.4%). The median income category range was $50,000-$74,999. In terms of the clinical characteristics, participants reported an overall moderate level of pain severity (M=3.8), pain interference (M=4.5), and depressive (M=56.2) and anxiety (M=58.1) symptoms at baseline. About 22 percent of the participants were categorized as evening type.
Table 1.
Characteristics of the Study Sample
Variables | M (SD) or N (%) |
---|---|
| |
Age (years) | 44.2 (13.3) |
Gender | |
Female | 576 (65.4%) |
Male | 305 (34.6%) |
Race | |
White | 757 (87.3%) |
Black/African American | 58 (6.7%) |
Asian/Asian American | 29 (3.3%) |
American Indian/Alaska Native | 5 (0.6%) |
Prefer not to answer | 3 (0.3%) |
Don’t know | 1 (0.1%) |
Other race | 14 (1.6%) |
Ethnicity | |
Hispanic | 67 (7.6%) |
Non-Hispanic | 812 (92.4%) |
Education | |
9th to 12th grade; no high school diploma | 1 (0.1%) |
GED or high school diploma | 106 (12.0%) |
Some college, no degree | 229 (25.9%) |
Associate’s degree (2-year degree) | 138 (15.6%) |
Bachelor’s degree/college degree | 241 (27.3%) |
Some graduate work, no degree | 30 (3.4%) |
Graduate degree | 138 (15.6%) |
Prefer not to answer | 1 (0.1%) |
Employment Status | |
Working full-time | 430 (48.6%) |
Working part-time | 127 (14.4%) |
Unemployed or laid off | 93 (10.5%) |
Looking for work | 20 (2.3%) |
Keeping house or raising children full-time | 65 (7.4%) |
Retired | 82 (9.3%) |
Other | 67 (7.6%) |
Income | |
Less than $5,000 | 29 (3.3%) |
$5,000–$11,999 | 37 (4.2%) |
$12,000–$15,999 | 31 (3.5%) |
$16,000–$24,999 | 89 (10.1%) |
$25,000–$34,999 | 118 (13.3%) |
$35,000–$$49,999 | 138 (15.6%) |
$50,000–$74,999 | 202 (22.9%) |
$75,000–$99,999 | 106 (12.0%) |
$100,000 and greater | 126 (14.3%) |
Prefer not to answer | 5 (0.6%) |
Don’t know | 3 (0.3%) |
Marital Status | |
Married | 410 (46.4%) |
Divorced | 130 (14.7%) |
Separated | 13 (1.5%) |
Widowed | 18 (2.0%) |
Single | 302 (34.2%) |
Prefer not to answer | 11 (1.2%) |
Chronic Pain Conditions | |
Low Back or Neck Pain | 566 (64.0%) |
Osteoarthritis | 295 (33.4%) |
Migraine | 273 (30.9%) |
Irritable Bowel Syndrome | 149 (16.9%) |
Fibromyalgia | 85 (9.6%) |
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome | 84 (9.5%) |
Rheumatoid Arthritis | 59 (6.7%) |
Temporomandibular Joint Disorder | 34 (3.8%) |
Endometriosis | 3 (3.4%) |
Pain Severity (0–10 NRS) | 3.8 (1.7) |
Pain Interference (0–10 NRS) | 4.5 (2.4) |
Depressive Symptoms (T-score) | 56.2 (10.1) |
Anxiety Symptoms (T-score) | 58.1 (9.8) |
Insomnia Severity (0–28) Circadian Preference |
13.0 (6.7) |
Evening type | 189 (22.2%) |
Intermediate type | 518 (60.7%) |
Morning type | 146 (17.1%) |
Attrition Analyses
We conducted a series of attrition analyses (i.e., t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables) based upon select socio-demographic variables (i.e., age, gender, race, ethnicity, income, education, and working status), baseline predictor, and outcome variables (i.e., insomnia severity, circadian preference, pain severity, pain interference, depressive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms). Results showed that only age was significantly associated with missingness at 21-month follow-up (p < .001), such that participants of a younger age were more likely to drop out at 21-month follow-up. These findings lend further support that the missingness in the present study is likely to meet the MAR assumption. Hence, use of FIML to handle the missing data is justified.
Associations Among Insomnia Severity, Circadian Preference, and Outcomes at Baseline
Table 2 shows the results of the ANOVA employed to examine mean differences in baseline insomnia severity and baseline outcome levels across the three circadian preferences (i.e., Evening, Intermediate, and Morning types). Insomnia severity (p < .001; η2 = .04), pain interference (p = .043; η2 = .01), depressive symptoms (p < .001; η2 = .05), and anxiety symptoms (p < .001; η2 = .03) significantly differed by circadian preferences. The Evening type had the highest level of insomnia severity, pain interference, and depressive and anxiety symptoms. There were no significant circadian preference differences in the levels of pain severity at baseline. In terms of bi-variate correlations, results showed that insomnia severity was moderately and positively correlated with all outcome variables at baseline: (1) r = .32 with pain severity; (2) r = .45 with pain interference; (3) r = .47 with depressive symptoms; and (4) r = .47 with anxiety symptoms.
Table 2.
Mean differences in insomnia severity and pain-related outcomes at baseline by circadian preference
Evening Type |
Intermediate Type |
Morning Type |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
Variables | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Test Statistic (F score) |
p-
value |
Effect Size (eta-squared) |
| |||||||||
Insomnia Severity | 14.33 a |
6.7 5 |
13.31b | 6.58 | 10.29a, b | 6.3 6 |
16.78 | < .001 | .038 |
Pain Severity | 3.69 | 1.6 9 |
3.85 | 1.71 | 3.64 | 1.7 4 |
1.25 | .287 | .003 |
Pain Interference | 4.68 | 2.4 5 |
4.58 | 2.41 | 4.06 | 2.4 2 |
3.15 | .043 | .007 |
Depressive | 58.91 | 9.8 | 56.58d | 10.1 | 51.60c, d | 8.9 | < .001 | .052 | |
Symptoms | c | 7 | 6 | 7 | 23.22 | ||||
Anxiety | 59.59 | 9.8 | 58.50f | 9.90 | 54.49e, f | 8.7 | < .001 | .029 | |
Symptoms | e | 4 | 8 | 12.78 |
Note. Matching superscripts indicate significant (p <.05) differences in Tukey’s post-hoc comparisons.
Effects of Insomnia Severity and Circadian Preference on Pain-Related Outcomes at 9 Months
The model fit of this path model was good overall, RMSEA = .07, CFI = .98, and SRMR = .03. Table 3 shows the detailed parameter estimates of this model. Even after controlling for baseline levels of the outcomes and socio-demographic covariates, insomnia severity at baseline was significantly associated with changes in pain severity (B = .03, SE = .01, p = .003), pain interference (B = .05, SE = .01, p < .001), depressive symptoms (B = .18, SE = .04, p < .001), and anxiety symptoms (B = .21, SE = .05, p < .001) at 9-month follow-up. In terms of interpretability of the path estimates, for every one unit increase of insomnia severity (from a total score that ranges 0–28), pain severity increased by .03 (from a 0–10 scale), pain interference increased by .05 (from a 0–10 scale), depressive symptoms increased by .18 (from T-score that typically ranges from 20 to 80), and anxiety symptoms increased by .21 (from T-score that typically ranges from 20 to 80). Individuals with greater levels of insomnia severity at baseline reported worsening pain-related symptoms at 9-month follow-up. Dummy variables that compared Evening vs. Intermediate and Evening vs. Morning types demonstrated no significant mean differences in the change of pain-related outcomes at 9-month follow-up. The model (RMSEA = .07, CFI = .98, and SRMR = .03) that examined insomnia severity and circadian preference moderation revealed that there were no significant interactions (p-values ranging from .07 to .95) across all four outcomes.
Table 3.
Insomnia Severity and Circadian Preference Predicting Pain-Related Outcomes at 9-Month Follow-Up
Outcome | Predictors | B | SE | 95% CI | Standardized β |
P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
Pain Severity | Insomnia Severity | .03 | .01 | [.01, .04] | .10 | .003 | ||
Evening Type vs. Morning Type | .04 | .18 | [−.32, .40] | .01 | .81 | |||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | .02 | .14 | [−.25, .29] | .01 | .90 | |||
Baseline Pain Severity | .58 | .03 | [.52, .64] | .57 | < .001 | |||
Age | .001 | .01 | [−.01, .01] | .01 | .82 | |||
Gender (1 = Female) | .22 | .12 | [−.01, .46] | .06 | .07 | |||
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | .08 | .22 | [−.36, .52] | .01 | .73 | |||
Income (1 = High) | −.13 | .11 | [−.35, .09] | −.04 | .24 | |||
Education (1 = High) | −.24 | .12 | [−.47, −.01] | −.07 | .04 | |||
| ||||||||
Pain Interference |
Insomnia Severity | .05 | .01 | [.02, .07] | .12 | < .001 | ||
Evening Type vs. Morning Type | .24 | .26 | [−.28, .75] | .04 | .37 | |||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | .05 | .20 | [−.34, .44] | .01 | .81 | |||
Baseline Pain Interference | .55 | .03 | [.49, .61] | .53 | < .001 | |||
Age | −.001 | .01 | [−.01, .01] | −.01 | .89 | |||
Gender (1 = Female) | .41 | .17 | [.07, .74] | .08 | .02 | |||
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −.26 | .32 | [−.89, .37] | −.03 | .42 | |||
Income (1 = High) | −.16 | .16 | [−.48, .16] | −.03 | .33 | |||
Education (1 = High) | −.31 | .17 | [−.63, .02] | −.06 | .06 | |||
| ||||||||
Depressive | Insomnia Severity | .18 | .04 | [.10, .27] | .12 | <.001 | ||
Symptoms | Evening Type vs. Morning | −.39 | .86 | [−.2.08, | −.02 | .65 | ||
Type | 1.29] | |||||||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | −.72 | .64 | [−.1.98, .54] | −.03 | .26 | |||
Baseline Pain Depression | .70 | .03 | [.65, .75] | .69 | < .001 | |||
Age | −.05 | .02 | [−.09, −.01] | −.07 | .02 | |||
Gender (1 = Female) | .24 | .56 | [−.85, 1.33] | .01 | .66 | |||
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −.20 | 1.03 | [−.2.22, 1.82] |
−.01 | .85 | |||
Income (1 = High) | −.1.43 | .53 | [−.2.47, −.39] |
−.07 | .01 | |||
Education (1 = High) | −.11 | .54 | [−.1.16, .94] | −.01 | .84 | |||
| ||||||||
Anxiety | Insomnia Severity | .21 | .05 | [.12, .30] | .14 | < .001 | ||
Symptoms | Evening Type vs. Morning | −.47 | .91 | [−.2.26, | −.02 | .60 | ||
Type | 1.31] | |||||||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | −.80 | .68 | [−.2.14, .54] | −.04 | .24 | |||
Baseline Anxiety | .65 | .03 | [.59, .70] | .63 | < .001 | |||
Age | −.09 | .02 | [−.14, −.05] | −.12 | <.001 | |||
Gender (1 = Female) | .91 | .60 | [−.27, 2.08] | .04 | .13 | |||
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −.05 | 1.11 | [−.2.22, 2.13] |
−.001 | .97 | |||
Income (1 = High) Education (1 = High) |
−.75 .33 |
.56 .57 |
[−.1.85, .35] [−.79, 1.45] | −.04 .02 |
.18 .57 |
Effects of Insomnia Severity and Circadian Preference on Pain-Related Outcomes at 21 Months
The model fit of this long-term outcome model was also good overall, RMSEA = .06, CFI = .98, and SRMR = .03. Table 4 shows the detailed parameter estimates of this model. Even controlling for baseline levels of the outcomes and socio-demographic covariates, insomnia severity at baseline was significantly associated with changes in pain interference (B = .05, SE = .01, p < .001), depressive symptoms (B = .17, SE = .05, p = .001), and anxiety symptoms (B = .23, SE = .05, p < .001), but not pain severity, at 21-month follow-up. Dummy variables that compared Evening vs. Intermediate and Evening vs. Morning types revealed no significant mean differences in the change of pain-related outcomes at 21-month follow-up, apart from a significant difference between Evening and Intermediate types in terms of depressive symptoms (B = −2.4, SE = .74, p = .001). Individuals who were Evening types showed worsening depressive symptoms than those who were Intermediate types. The model (RMSEA = .06, CFI = .98, and SRMR = .03) that examined insomnia severity and circadian preference moderation demonstrated that there were no significant interactions (p-values ranging from .23 to .96) across all four outcomes.
Table 4.
Insomnia Severity and Circadian Preference Predicting Pain-Related Outcomes at 21-Month Follow-Up
Outcome | Predictors | B | SE | 95 % CI | Standardized β |
p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Pain Severity | Insomnia Severity | .01 | .01 | [−..01, .03] | .04 | .23 |
Evening Type vs. Morning Type | −..11 | .19 | [−..49, .26] | −..02 | .55 | |
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | −..11 | .14 | [−..39, .17] | −..03 | .45 | |
Baseline Pain Severity | .60 | .03 | [.54, .66] | .58 | < .001 | |
Age | .001 | .01 | [−..01, .01] | .01 | .81 | |
Gender (1 = Female) | .25 | .12 | [.01, .49] | .07 | .04 | |
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −..30 | .23 | [−..75, .15] | −..05 | .19 | |
Income (1 = High) | .03 | .12 | [−..20, .26] | .01 | .78 | |
Education (1 = High) | −..17 | .12 | [−..40, .07] | −..05 | .17 | |
| ||||||
Pain Interference |
Insomnia Severity | .05 | .01 | [.02, .08] | .14 | < .001 |
Evening Type vs. Morning Type | −..17 | .27 | [−..70, .35] | −..03 | .52 | |
Evening Type vs. Intermediate Type | −..25 | .20 | [−..64, .15] | −..05 | .22 | |
Baseline Pain Interference | .54 | .03 | [.48, .60] | .53 | < .001 | |
Age | −..01 | .01 | [−..02, .01] | −..03 | .46 | |
Gender (1 = Female) | .29 | .17 | [−..05, .62] | .06 | .10 | |
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −..41 | .32 | [1.05, .23] | −..04 | .21 | |
Income (1 = High) | −..01 | .17 | [−..34, .32] | −..002 | .95 | |
Education (1 = High) | −..03 | .17 | [−..37, .30] | −..01 | .85 | |
| ||||||
Depressive | Insomnia Severity | .17 | .05 | [.07, .18] | .11 | .001 |
Symptoms | Evening Type vs. Morning | −.1.74 | .99 | [−. | −..07 | .08 |
Type | 3.67, .19] | |||||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate | −.2.36 | .74 | [−.3.81, | −..12 | .001 | |
Type | −..91] | |||||
Baseline Depression | .58 | .03 | [.53, .64] | .59 | < .001 | |
Age | −..06 | .03 | [−..11, −..01] | −..08 | .02 | |
Gender (1 = Female) | −..19 | .64 | [−.1.44, 1.05] |
−..01 | .76 | |
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | −..31 | 1.19 | [−.2.64, 2.02] |
−..01 | .80 | |
Income (1 = High) | −.1.90 | .61 | [−.3.10, −..70] |
−..10 | .002 | |
Education (1 = High) | .51 | .62 | [−..70, 1.72] |
.03 | .41 | |
| ||||||
Anxiety | Insomnia Severity | .23 | .05 | [.13, .33] | .16 | < .001 |
Symptoms | Evening Type vs. Morning | −..20 | 1.02 | [−.2.20, | −..01 | .85 |
Type | 1.81] | |||||
Evening Type vs. Intermediate | −..87 | .77 | [−. | −..04 | .26 | |
Type | 2.38, .63] | |||||
Baseline Pain Anxiety | .54 | .03 | [.48, .60] | .54 | < .001 | |
Age | −..09 | .03 | [−..14, −..04] | −..12 | .001 | |
Gender (1 = Female) | .54 | .66 | [−..76, 1.83] |
.03 | .42 | |
Ethnicity (1 = Hispanic) | .44 | 1.24 | [−.1.99, 2.87] |
.01 | .72 | |
Income (1 = High) | −.1.69 | .63 | [−.2.93, −..45] |
−..09 | .01 | |
Education (1 = High) | −..01 | .64 | [−.1.26, 1.25] |
< .001 | .99 |
Sensitivity Analysis
The BPI Pain Interference subscale includes an item relating to pain’s interference with sleep. It is possible that the robust longitudinal association between insomnia severity and pain interference is mainly driven by this sleep interference item in the BPI. Hence, we created a composite of the pain interference variable, excluding this sleep interference item, and re-analyzed the main path models. The sensitivity analysis revealed that even after excluding the sleep interference item from the pain interference composite, insomnia severity at baseline significantly predicted both 9-month (B = .04, SE = .01, p = .001) and 21-month (B = .05, SE = .01, p = .001) follow-up pain interference, while controlling for covariates that were mentioned above. This demonstrates that the sleep interference item did not drive the significant association between insomnia severity and pain interference.
We elected to use total ISI scores in the present study due to a methodological advantage (i.e., better statistical power than when dichotomizing a continuous variable) and potential false diagnosis of insomnia disorder using a clinical cutoff for ISI. However, some may still be interested in whether clinical level of insomnia (rather than insomnia severity) predicts pain-related outcomes longitudinally. Hence, we conducted an additional sensitivity analysis by creating a binary ISI variable (i.e., clinical-level insomnia vs. not clinical-level insomnia) using ISI cutoff score of 14.51,52 Based upon this cutoff score, we found that 41% of our participants are categorized as having clinical level insomnia. We re-analyzed all of the main models switching the continuous ISI variable to this binary ISI variable. Results showed that all of the findings were replicated using the dichotomized ISI variable. The summary of these sensitivity analyses is available as online supplement Table S1 and S2.
It is possible that one of the main reasons that we could not observe the effects of eveningness on pain-related outcomes is due to reduction in statistical power that is caused by categorization of a continuous variable (i.e., the MEQ). We re-analyzed models by including a total MEQ score instead of dummy MEQ variables. However, consistent with the dummy variables, the total score of MEQ did not significantly predict both 9- and 21-month pain-related outcomes, while controlling for all other covariates including the insomnia severity.
Discussion
Growing evidence suggests that insomnia and evening type are associated with both pain-specific and emotional health-related outcomes. However, there has been a dearth of studies that consider both insomnia and circadian preference in the context of chronic pain maintenance and progression. The present study is one of the first to investigate prospective effects of both insomnia severity and evening type on pain-related outcomes using a large dataset that followed individuals with chronic pain longitudinally. Contrary to our expectations, only insomnia severity was significantly associated with changes in both medium- and long-term pain-related outcomes. In addition, we found no significant interaction effects between insomnia severity and circadian preference on pain-related outcomes.
Consistent with previous observational and well-controlled laboratory-based sleep disruption studies1,5,6, our findings demonstrate that insomnia may serve an important role in the progression of chronic pain. Greater insomnia severity at baseline was associated with worsening pain interference and emotional distress over nearly two years. However, unlike pain interference, we found that insomnia severity was not significantly associated with long-term (21 months) changes in pain severity. It is unlikely that insomnia severity is more strongly associated with pain interference because of the inclusion of the sleep interference item in the BPI. As we demonstrated from our sensitivity analysis above, even after excluding this item from pain interference composite, we found significant predictive effects of insomnia severity on pain interference at both 9 and 21 months. In fact, a recent meta-analysis of CBT-I on chronic pain showed that the effect size of CBT-I on pain severity (standardized mean difference = 0.19) is much smaller than that on pain interference (standardized mean difference = 0.75).7 Also, at least two previous studies revealed that the CBT-I improved only pain interference, not pain severity.53,54 We speculate that the negative impact of insomnia on chronic pain is more likely manifested by difficulties in ‘coping’ with pain (e.g., increase in pain catastrophizing55,56 and negative affect57), as opposed to a significant elevation in pain severity, thus resulting in greater pain interference and emotional distress. Future studies that investigate the underlying biobehavioral mechanisms linking insomnia and the progression of chronic pain are strongly warranted.
Similar to previous cross-sectional studies9,12–14,58, we found that evening types showed greater pain interference and emotional distress at baseline compared to intermediate and morning types. However, we found that evening types were not at a higher risk of experiencing worsening pain-related outcomes, while controlling for the effects of baseline insomnia severity, than intermediate and morning types. Furthermore, there were no significant moderating effects of insomnia severity and circadian preference on pain-related outcomes, such that evening types with greater insomnia severity did not show worsening pain-related outcomes compared to other types with lower insomnia severity. Overall, our findings suggest no clinically meaningful associations between evening types and an exacerbation of pain-related outcomes over time, and levels of insomnia severity do not modulate its effects on the outcomes.
Yet, our results should be replicated and further extended in future studies with use of more sophisticated biobehavioral circadian markers and by employing a well-controlled laboratory design. Assessment of objective circadian markers via actigraphy, dim light melatonin onset (DLMO), and/or 6-sulfatoxymelatonin (aMT6s) in urine that provides reliable measures of endogenous circadian rhythms59,60, may help us better understand how circadian misalignment or blunted circadian rhythms may be associated with maintenance and exacerbation of chronic pain.61 Notably, a number of animal studies have consistently demonstrated that experimental disruption of circadian rhythms (e.g., exposure to dim light at night, mistimed feeding, and simulated jet lag) cause amplification of pain sensitivity.62–64 Human laboratory-based chronobiological studies, such as employing inverted sleep schedules, simulated night shifts, or forced desynchrony protocols65, may also shed further light on understanding the effects of circadian misalignment on pain-related outcomes.8
A few clinical and research implications can also be gleaned from the findings of the current study. First, our results suggest that untreated insomnia can facilitate maintenance and progression of chronic pain over a long period of time. Hence, symptoms of insomnia should be routinely examined in clinical settings when working with individuals with chronic pain. Second, as shown in a recent meta-analysis7, CBT-I is efficacious in improving not only sleep, but also pain-related outcomes, particularly pain interference. However, consistent with findings of the present study that showed overall significant but small magnitude of effects of insomnia on pain-related outcomes, the meta-analysis suggests that the overall effect size of CBT-I on pain-related outcomes are small to moderate at most.7 Hence, a sensible future goal may be to examine whether a combination of CBT-I and another evidence-based intervention for chronic pain (e.g., Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction, Acceptance Commitment Therapy) can produce a more powerful and enduring treatment effects for chronic pain.
Strengths and Limitations
The present study had a number of strengths, including the use of a large sample, long-term (21 months) follow-up, and integration of both insomnia and circadian preferences in the same model. However, there were also several limitations. First, circadian preference was not assessed at Time 1, and thus the final sample was limited to those who completed Time 2 assessments. There may have also been a sample selection bias, potentially limiting the generalizability of our findings. Second, the data of our study are based upon an online crowdsourcing (i.e., MTurk) sample and the sample did not represent minoritized individuals well, especially those who identify as Black/African American and Hispanic/Latino/a/x. Hence, it may be difficult to generalize findings of our study to the chronic pain population in the U.S. in general and minoritized individuals in particular. Future studies should incorporate a better sampling strategy, such as using random digit dialing to collect data from a nationally representative sample or oversampling minoritized racial and ethnic groups. Third, although we assessed both insomnia and circadian preference using well-validated self-report measures, we did not collect any objective markers of sleep or insomnia diagnoses evaluated by a structured clinical interview. Also, we did not assess social jet lag (i.e., discrepancy between biological and social clocks)66, which may have shown more powerful association with pain-related outcomes. These data could have provided us with greater insight into our current findings. Lastly, we did not assess whether participants received any treatment for chronic pain before and/or during the observational period.
Conclusion
The present study suggest that the severity of insomnia is relatively more important in predicting changes in pain-related outcomes in both medium- and long-term when compared to eveningness. Greater insomnia severity is robustly associated with not only worsening pain severity and interference, but also emotional distress. Therefore, insomnia remains an important clinical target for chronic pain management. However, to better understand potentially unique roles of sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances in chronic pain maintenance and progression, future studies should assess sleep and circadian rhythm using more sophisticated biobehavioral markers. In addition, conducting human laboratory studies may shed further light on the effects of circadian misalignment on chronic pain.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Greater insomnia symptoms were associated with worsening of pain-related outcomes.
Evening types were not at higher risk for worsening pain-related symptoms over time.
No significant insomnia and evening type moderation effects were found.
Footnotes
Disclosures:
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose. Funding for this research was provided by National Institutes of Health grants (F32DA049393, R01MD00906, and K23HD104934) and PM&R Seed Grant from Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation at the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Finan PH, Goodin BR, Smith MT. The association of sleep and pain: an update and a path forward. J Pain. 2013;14(12):1539–1552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tang NKY, Wright KJ, Salkovskis PM. Prevalence and correlates of clinical insomnia co-occurring with chronic back pain. J Sleep Res. 2007;16(1):85–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Finan PH, Smith MT. The comorbidity of insomnia, chronic pain, and depression: dopamine as a putative mechanism. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17(3):173–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sun Y, Laksono I, Selvanathan J, et al. Prevalence of sleep disturbances in patients with chronic non-cancer pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2021:101467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kundermann B, Krieg J-C, Schreiber W, Lautenbacher S. The effects of sleep deprivation on pain. Pain Res Manag. 2004;9(1):25–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lautenbacher S, Kundermann B, Krieg J-C. Sleep deprivation and pain perception. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(5):357–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Selvanathan J, Pham C, Nagappa M, et al. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia in patients with chronic pain-A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;60:101460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mun CJ, Burgess HJ, Sears DD, et al. Circadian rhythm and pain: A review of current research and future implications. Curr Sleep Med Reports. 2022;8:114–123. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Heikkala E, Oura P, Korpela T, Karppinen J, Paananen M. Chronotypes and disabling musculoskeletal pain: A Finnish birth cohort study. Eur J Pain. 2022;26(5):1069–1078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Heikkala E, Merikanto I, Tanguay-Sabourin C, Karppinen J, Oura P. Eveningness is associated with persistent multisite musculoskeletal pain: A 15-year follow-up study of Northern Finns. J Pain. 2023;24:679–688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Heikkala E, Paananen M, Merikanto I, Karppinen J, Oura P. Eveningness intensifies the association between musculoskeletal pain and health-related quality of life. Pain. 2022;163:2154–2161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kantermann T, Theadom A, Roenneberg T, Cropley M. Fibromyalgia syndrome and chronotype: late chronotypes are more affected. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27(2):176–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Türkoğlu G, Selvi Y. The relationship between chronotype, sleep disturbance, severity of fibromyalgia, and quality of life in patients with fibromyalgia. Chronobiol Int. 2020;37(1):68–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chakradeo PS, Keshavarzian A, Singh S, et al. Chronotype, social jet lag, sleep debt and food timing in inflammatory bowel disease. Sleep Med. 2018;52:188–195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dworkin RH, Turk DC, Farrar JT, et al. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain. 2005;113(1–2):9–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fisher K, Johnston M. Emotional distress and control cognitions as mediators of the impact of chronic pain on disability. Br J Health Psychol. 1998;3(3):225–236. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Banks SM, Kerns RD. Explaining high rates of depression in chronic pain: A diathesis-stress framework. Psychol Bull. 1996;119(1):95–110. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Riemann D, Voderholzer U. Primary insomnia: a risk factor to develop depression? J Affect Disord. 2003;76(1–3):255–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Baglioni C, Battagliese G, Feige B, et al. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: a meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. J Affect Disord. 2011;135(1–3):10–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.van Mill JG, Vogelzangs N, van Someren EJW, Hoogendijk WJG, Penninx BWJH. Sleep duration, but not insomnia, predicts the 2-year course of depressive and anxiety disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;75(2):119–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Neckelmann D, Mykletun A, Dahl AA. Chronic insomnia as a risk factor for developing anxiety and depression. Sleep. 2007;30(7):873–880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Au J, Reece J. The relationship between chronotype and depressive symptoms: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2017;218:93–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kivelä L, Papadopoulos MR, Antypa N. Chronotype and psychiatric disorders. Curr sleep Med reports. 2018;4(2):94–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rosenwasser AM. Functional neuroanatomy of sleep and circadian rhythms. Brain Res Rev. 2009;61(2):281–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mongrain V, Carrier J, Dumont M. Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness–eveningness. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(2):162–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Turk DC, Dworkin RH, Allen RR, et al. Core outcome domains for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain. 2003;106(3):337–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hasler BP, Germain A, Nofzinger EA, et al. Chronotype and diurnal patterns of positive affect and affective neural circuitry in primary insomnia. J Sleep Res. 2012;21(5):515–526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ong JC, Huang JS, Kuo TF, Manber R. Characteristics of insomniacs with self-reported morning and evening chronotypes. J Clin sleep Med. 2007;3(3):289–294. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Aaron RV, Mun CJ, McGill LS, Finan PH, Campbell CM. The longitudinal relationship between emotion regulation and pain-related outcomes: Results from a large, online prospective study. J Pain. 2022;23(6):981–994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aaron RV, McGill LS, Finan PH, Wegener ST, Campbell CM, Mun CJ. Determining profiles of pain-specific and general emotion regulation skills and their relation to 12-month outcomes among people with chronic pain. J Pain. 2023;24:667–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6:3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Meade AW, Craig SB. Identifying careless responses in survey data. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(3):437–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Strickland JC, Stoops WW. The use of crowdsourcing in addiction science research: Amazon Mechanical Turk. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2019;27(1):1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001;2(4):297–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple correlation/regression analysis for the behavioral sciences. UK Taylor Fr. 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bauducco S, Richardson C, Gradisar M. Chronotype, circadian rhythms and mood. Curr Opin Psychol. 2020;34:77–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Horne JA, Östberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cavallera GM, Giudici S. Morningness and eveningness personality: A survey in literature from 1995 up till 2006. Pers Individ Dif. 2008;44(1):3–21. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chan JWY, Lam SP, Li SX, et al. Eveningness and insomnia: independent risk factors of nonremission in major depressive disorder. Sleep. 2014;37(5):911–917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cleeland CS, Ryan KM. Pain assessment: global use of the Brief Pain Inventory. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 1994;23(2):129–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kroenke K, Yu Z, Wu J, Kean J, Monahan PO. Operating characteristics of PROMIS four-item depression and anxiety scales in primary care patients with chronic pain. Pain Med. 2014;15(11):1892–1901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Campbell CM, Edwards RR. Ethnic differences in pain and pain management. Pain Manag. 2012;2(3):219–230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Janevic MR, McLaughlin SJ, Heapy AA, Thacker C, Piette JD. Racial and socioeconomic disparities in disabling chronic pain: Findings from the health and retirement study. J Pain. 2017;18(12):1459–1467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mun CJ, Davis MC, Molton IR, et al. Personal resource profiles of individuals with chronic pain: Sociodemographic and pain interference differences. Rehabil Psychol. 2019;64(3):245–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mun CJ, Campbell CM, McGill LS, Aaron RV. The early impact of COVID-19 on chronic pain: A cross-sectional investigation of a large online sample of individuals with chronic pain in the United States, April to May, 2020. Pain Med. 2021;22(2):470–480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mun CJ, Campbell CM, McGill LS, Wegener ST, Aaron RV. Trajectories and individual differences in pain, emotional distress, and prescription opioid misuse during the COVID-19 pandemic: A one-year longitudinal study. J Pain. 2022;23(7):1234–1244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mattes A, Roheger M. Nothing wrong about change: the adequate choice of the dependent variable and design in prediction of cognitive training success. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2020;20(1):1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kline RB. Software review: Software programs for structural equation modeling: Amos, EQS, and LISREL. J Psychoeduc Assess. 1998;16(4):343–364. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Steiger JH. A note on multiple sample extensions of the RMSEA fit index. Struct Equ Model. 1998;5(4):411–419. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gagnon C, Bélanger L, Ivers H, Morin CM. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index in primary care. J Am Board Fam Med. 2013;26(6):701–710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Seow LSE, Abdin E, Chang S, Chong SA, Subramaniam M. Identifying the best sleep measure to screen clinical insomnia in a psychiatric population. Sleep Med. 2018;41:86–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tang NKY, Goodchild CE, Salkovskis PM. Hybrid cognitive-behaviour therapy for individuals with insomnia and chronic pain: a pilot randomised controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50(12):814–821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jungquist CR, O’Brien C, Matteson-Rusby S, et al. The efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia in patients with chronic pain. Sleep Med. 2010;11(3):302–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Campbell CM, Buenaver LF, Finan P, et al. Sleep, pain catastrophizing, and central sensitization in knee osteoarthritis patients with and without insomnia. Arthritis Care Res. 2015;67(10):1387–1396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mun CJ, Davis MC, Campbell CM, Finan PH, Tennen H. Linking nonrestorative sleep and activity interference through pain catastrophizing and pain severity: An intraday process model among individuals with fibromyalgia. J Pain. 2020;21(5–6):546–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Kothari DJ, Davis MC, Yeung EW, Tennen HA. Positive affect and pain : mediators of the within-day relation linking sleep quality to activity interference in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2015;156:540–546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kim S, Park W-J, Cho S, et al. The relationship between chronotypes and musculoskeletal problems in male automobile manufacturing workers. Ann Occup Environ Med. 2021;33(1):1–13. doi: 10.35371/aoem.2021.33.e26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kräuchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A. Alteration of internal circadian phase relationships after morning versus evening carbohydrate-rich meals in humans. J Biol Rhythms. 2002;17(4):364–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Youngstedt SD, Elliott JA, Kripke DF. Human circadian phase–response curves for exercise. J Physiol. 2019;597(8):2253–2268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Youngstedt SD, Elliott J, Patel S, et al. Circadian acclimatization of performance, sleep, and 6-sulfatoxymelatonin using multiple phase shifting stimuli. Front Endocrinol. 2022;13:964681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bumgarner JR, Walker II WH, Liu JA, Walton JC, Nelson RJ. Dim light at night exposure induces cold hyperalgesia and mechanical allodynia in male mice. Neuroscience. 2020;434:111–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Xu F, Zhao X, Liu H, et al. Misaligned feeding may aggravate pain by disruption of sleep–awake rhythm. Anesth Analg. 2018;127(1):255–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Das V, Kc R, Li X, et al. Pharmacological targeting of the mammalian clock reveals a novel analgesic for osteoarthritis-induced pain. Gene. 2018;655:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Vetter C Circadian disruption: what do we actually mean? Eur J Neurosci. 2020;51(1):531–550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Roenneberg T, Pilz LK, Zerbini G, Winnebeck EC. Chronotype and social jetlag: a (self-) critical review. Biology (Basel). 2019;8(3):54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.