Abstract
This scientific commentary refers to ‘Path integration deficits are associated with phosphorylated tau accumulation in the entorhinal cortex’, by Koike et al. (https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcad359).
This scientific commentary refers to ‘Path integration deficits are associated with phosphorylated tau accumulation in the entorhinal cortex’, by Koike et al. (https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcad359).
Spatial navigation—determining and maintaining a trajectory from one place to another by recognition of spatial position by accumulating self-motion cues—is essential for independent daily living. Spatial navigation requires continuous switching between body-centred (egocentric) and object-to-object (allocentric) strategies.1 Whereas the basal ganglia and parietal lobe support egocentric spatial representations, allocentric representations depend on the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex in the medial temporal lobe. Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex play a crucial role in path integration by facilitating the use of self-motion cues to estimate distance travelled and angular displacement and, therefore, position in the environment. These cells are innervated by the visual cortex and send projections to place cells in the CA1 subfield of the hippocampus. In the context of Alzheimer’s disease, it is important to note that the entorhinal cortex is the first brain area to develop tau-containing neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) lesions.2
Impaired judgement of distances and formation of mental maps results in difficulties in spatial orientation and navigation. Spatial disorientation is often experienced by patients with pre-symptomatic Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment,3 but navigational skills may also be impaired in otherwise cognitively intact elderly subjects who find themselves in a non-familiar environment4; this is supposedly due to greater bias towards egocentric versus allocentric strategies and translating between them. Despite evidence that impediments in spatial navigation are specific to Alzheimer’s disease,5 most clinical assessments are still largely based on episodic memory, a measure that is not specific to Alzheimer’s disease.6
The protracted and insidious nature of Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and unlikelihood of its reversibility, makes it futile to presently contemplate ‘cures’ for the disease. Rather, timely pharmacological and non-pharmacological intervention is a more realistic goal; but this goal can only be achieved if pathology can be predicted or detected sufficiently early. To this end, the last two decades has seen the development of stand-alone or combined objective markers of incipient Alzheimer’s disease based on neuropsychological assessment, neuroimaging, genomics and fluid biomarker screening. However, consensus on the ‘perfect’ individual or set of early predictors is still lacking.
Recent studies have begun to explore the potential of digital biomarkers for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease through the use of head-mounted immersive virtual reality (iVR), devices7-9 (defined as ‘systems that encompass the user’s visual field and where virtual movement replicates actual head or bodily movement’10). One of these showed that iVR can discriminate between healthy aged subjects and patients with mild cognitive impairment8 and observed that ‘path integration impairment in early Alzheimer’s disease is not an extension of healthy aging’ (cf. Li and King4).
The work by Koike et al.7 in this issue of Brain Communications supports the notion that detection of deficits in path integration may help identify persons likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease. Given that spatial navigation abilities begin to decline during asymptomatic and prodromal stages of Alzheimer’s disease, the authors suggest that cohort selection and post-treatment assessment based on objective and quantifiable criteria in future clinical trials are more likely to generate clearer results than those obtained to date.
Importantly, Koike et al.7 show that their iVR paradigm distinguishes between entorhinal cortex-dependent path integration and hippocampus-dependent spatial memory and that errors in path integration increase with age; by extrapolation from the Braak age-related classification, significant declines in path integration ability would start from around 50 years of age. This observation is consistent with the authors’ assertion that preventative or therapeutic interventions for Alzheimer’s disease should be initiated at Braak stages I and II when NFT are confined to the entorhinal cortex2 if they are to be efficacious.
While the authors7 did not directly measure NFT in their subjects, they ingeniously ‘borrowed’ from Braak’s neuropathological staging of Alzheimer’s disease as a function of age and appearance of NFT in the entorhinal cortex.2 Supporting their deductions, they showed that chemogenetic inactivation of entorhinal cortex neurons disrupted path integration in healthy young adult mice. In addition, using a modified L-maze, they showed that path integration is impaired in middle-aged tau-overexpressing transgenic mice, coinciding with the appearance of NFT in the entorhinal cortex. From comparison of the performance of mice in the Morris water maze versus the L-maze, the authors confirmed that NFT accumulation in the entorhinal cortex interferes with path integration but not spatial memory. The latter is consistent with the conclusions of one of the other iVR-based studies,8 namely, that deficits in path integration appear in at-risk individuals before conversion to pre-clinical Alzheimer’s disease. An interesting question to be addressed in future research concerns the nature (e.g. connectivity) of the influence of NFT in the entorhinal cortex (allocentric behaviour) on basal ganglia (egocentric behaviour) since these brain regions inter-dependently contribute to the efficacy of path integration.
Despite the limitations of this initial research by Koike et al.7 (also see Newton et al.8 and Castegnaro et al.9), the work deserves credit for bringing iVR-based assessment of spatial navigation for early detection of Alzheimer’s disease closer to reality. However, much work is still needed before their findings can be translated into a robust diagnostic tool. For the purpose of diagnosis, the findings will require replication and validation in larger cohorts. Since a multiplicity of factors place individuals at risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease, subjects must be screened and selected on the basis of objective neuropsychological, neuroimaging, genetic and fluid biomarker criteria, with less reliance on self-reports of physical, mental and cognitive health status. Further, besides age, data should be stratified to include a broader set of demographic characteristics—for example, the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease is higher in women than in men (and there are sex differences in spatial and other cognitive functions), and higher socioeconomic and educational status are reported to have a mitigating effect on the time of disease onset. Lastly, to avoid biases in cohort selection, investigators need to consider that volunteers’ motivation to enrol in clinical studies may be confounded by factors such as family history of dementia, education level and general stress, mood and anxiety levels.
There is broad consensus that proof of the success of new preventative and ameliorating therapies for Alzheimer’s disease depends on early detection of the disease by robust analytical tools that are sensitive, accurate, standardizable, generalizable and affordable. At least until sufficient data become available, diagnostic approaches should ideally include analysis of areas of the brain in which neuropathology originates and spreads from. While PET represents the gold standard for monitoring late-stage Alzheimer’s disease when tau proteinopathy is present (the so-called T2 category of biomarkers; Braak stages III and VI), there is presently no suitable approach for monitoring pre-tangle tau (T1) in situ. The development of better early diagnostics clearly demands even greater multi-disciplinarity effort.
The introduction of iVR to the test battery for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease has the potential to greatly advance the search for effective therapies. As reviewed in detail,10 iVR gives both insight into cognitive function in a quantifiable manner and information about the processing and integration of sensory cues. Several features of iVR make its use widely applicable whether in clinics, practices or homes: the instrumentation is cheap and easy to use, and the software can be standardized to enable longitudinal follow-up examinations and comparison of data across centres. Importantly, iVR yields ecologically valid data, allowing testing in large landscapes without demands on space. From the patient perspective, iVR technology is intuitive and well-tolerated by elderly persons. Many older people reportedly find iVR to be a rewarding experience, although a minority are susceptible to cybersickness or may be anxious about new technologies.
In conclusion, the recent work by Koike et al.7 and others8,9 opens a promising new phase in the diagnosis and management of Alzheimer’s disease—from sufficiently early detection to the development of properly targeted treatments.
Competing interests
The author reports no competing interests.
References
- 1. Lithfous S, Dufour A, Després O. Spatial navigation in normal aging and the prodromal stage of Alzheimer's disease: Insights from imaging and behavioral studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12(1):201–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Braak H, Braak E. Staging of Alzheimer's disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16(3):271–278. discussion 278-284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Serino S, Cipresso P, Morganti F, Riva G. The role of egocentric and allocentric abilities in Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;16:32–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Li AWY, King J. Spatial memory and navigation in ageing: A systematic review of MRI and fMRI studies in healthy participants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103:33–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Tu S, Wong S, Hodges JR, Irish M, Piguet O, Hornberger M. Lost in spatial translation—A novel tool to objectively assess spatial disorientation in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2015;67:83–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Coughlan G, Laczó J, Hort J, Minihane AM, Hornberger M. Spatial navigation deficits—Overlooked cognitive marker for preclinical Alzheimer disease? Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14:496–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Koike R, Soeda Y, Kasai A, et al. Path integration deficits are associated with phosphorylated tau accumulation in the entorhinal cortex. Brain Comm. 2024:fcad359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Newton C, Pope M, Rua C, et al. Path integration selectively predicts midlife risk of Alzheimer's disease. bioRxiv. 2023. 10.1101/2023.01.31.526473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 9. Castegnaro A, Ji Z, Rudzka K, Chan D, Burgess N. Overestimation in angular path integration precedes Alzheimer's dementia. Curr Biol. 2023;33(21):4650–4661. e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Clay F, Howett D, FitzGerald J, Fletcher P, Chan D, Price A. Use of immersive virtual reality in the assessment and treatment of Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;75(1):23–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]