Abstract
Background and Objectives
Neurodevelopmental effects of fetal antiseizure medication (ASM) exposure on creativity and executive functions are poorly understood. We previously found fetal valproate exposure to adversely affect measures of creativity and executive functions. In this study, we examine fetal exposure of newer ASMs on these functions in children of women with epilepsy (WWE) compared with children of healthy women (HW).
Methods
The Maternal Outcomes and Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs study is a multicenter NIH-funded prospective observational cohort study of WWE and HW enrolled in pregnancy and their offsprings. This report examines blindly assessed creativity and executive functions in 4.5-year-old children of WWE vs HW. In addition, exposure-dependent ASM effects during the third trimester were examined in children of WWE, using a ratio of maximum observed ASM concentrations and ratio of defined daily dose (ratio DDD). For polytherapy, ratios were summed across ASMs. Linear regression models adjusted for multiple potential confounding factors were conducted for all analyses. The primary outcome for 4.5-year-old children was the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural Creativity Index. Secondary outcomes included the Global Executive Composite Score from the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version and subscales and other indexes of both measures.
Results
The primary analysis included 251 children of WWE and 73 of HW. No differences in creativity or executive function were found between children of WWE vs HW. No ASM exposure-dependent effects were found for the creativity measures, but exposure-dependent effects for executive function were present for ratio ASM concentration and ratio DDD.
Discussion
Our findings at 4.5 years show no differences in creative thinking between children of WWE vs HW (−3.2 [−9.0 to 2.7], p = 0.286) or associations with fetal exposure to ASMs (−2.6 [−11.0 to 5.7], p = 0.530). Secondary analyses revealed fetal exposure-dependent effects for executive function in children of WWE (7.0 [2.9–11.2], p = 0.001), which are most marked for levetiracetam (12.9 [4.2–21.6], p = 0.004). Our findings suggest that even for relatively safe ASMs, dosing needs to be adjusted to concentrations that prevent seizures, but balance risks to the fetus that high concentrations may pose.
Trial Registration Information
The study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov as NCT01730170.
Introduction
Creativity is conceptualized as the ability to produce original and worthwhile content1 and is a major force for positive innovations in our society. The neural mechanisms of creativity are uncertain, but likely involve dynamic interactions of large-scale brain systems. Although creativity is related to IQ, executive functions, and other cognitive functions, it is an independent construct.2-4
Fetal exposure to antiseizure medications (ASMs) can produce neurodevelopmental effects and congenital malformations.5 IQ and specific cognitive abilities can be adversely affected by fetal exposure to some ASMs. Few studies have examined the effects of fetal ASM exposure on creative and executive abilities. In our prior study, the Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs investigation examining the neurodevelopmental effects of fetal ASM exposure to monotherapies of carbamazepine, lamotrigine, phenytoin, and valproate, we found that valproate impaired creative fluency and originality at 4.5 years old as measured by the Torrance Thinking Creatively in Action and Movement (TCAM),6,7 which can be predictive of personal and public achievement.8 In this new cohort of the Maternal Outcomes and Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs (MONEAD) study, ASM prescribing patterns have changed,9,10 and we examined the effects of fetal ASM exposure on creativity functions in 4.5-year-old children of mothers with and without epilepsy, as well as fetal ASM exposure effects in children of women with epilepsy. We also investigated fetal ASM exposure effects on executive functions at 4.5 years old and the relationship of creativity functions to executive functions and IQ.
Methods
Study Design
The MONEAD study is an ongoing multicenter NIH-funded prospective observational parallel-group cohort study of pregnant women with epilepsy (WWE) and pregnant healthy women (HW) without epilepsy and their children and a cohort of non-pregnant women with epilepsy. Enrollment occurred from December 2012 through January 2016. Inclusion criteria were female patients aged 14–45 years. Exclusion criteria included IQ <70,11 drug/alcohol abuse within the prior year, history of psychogenic non-epileptic spells, progressive cerebral disease, other major medical illness, planned surgical intervention for epilepsy, or switching ASMs during pregnancy before enrollment (WWE). Additional exclusion criteria for pregnant women were >20 weeks gestational age (WGA), exposure to other known teratogens, detection of fetal congenital malformations before enrollment, or known genetic disorder. Twenty epilepsy centers across the United States with a specialty focus on treatment of WWE were enrollment sites.
Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents
Signed informed consent was obtained from all adult participants before participation. This study was approved by the individual site institutional review boards and is registered on ClinicalTrials.gov as NCT01730170. The central site institutional review board is Stanford University.
Outcome Measures
The primary outcome for 4.5-year-old children was the Torrance7 Test of Creative Thinking—Figural (TTCT-F) Creativity Index by a blinded assessor. Secondary outcomes included the TTCT-F subscales and the Global Executive Composite Score, Inhibitory Self Control Index, Flexibility Index, and Emergent Metacognition Index from the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version (BRIEF-P),12 completed by the child's mother/father at age 4.5 years. Both evaluations were scored by blinded assessors to the child study group. Note that higher scores are better for TTCT-F and worse for BRIEF-P.
Primary Objectives
The primary objective of the study was (1) to compare 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index between children of WWE and children of HW and (2) to assess the association between 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index and in utero third-trimester ASM blood concentration for children of WWE on ASM. To standardize ASM blood concentrations across ASMs, the ratio of the ASM concentration was calculated for each ASM by dividing the mother's ASM concentration by the upper limit of the suggested therapeutic ranges, mostly taken from published ranges,13 or clinical laboratory references used by Stanford University clinic, as was done in our age 3 neurocognitive analyses.14 The sum of the ratio ASM concentrations across all ASMs was used for mothers on polytherapy. The maximum observed ratio ASM concentration during the third trimester (including delivery) was used for analysis.
Risk Factors
Potential risk factors affecting 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index scores assessed in secondary analyses included mother’s breastfeeding status, periconceptual folate use and folate dose (none, >0–0.4, >0.4–1.0, >1.0–4.0, >4.0 mg), average maternal anxiety (Beck Anxiety Inventory),15 depression (Beck Depression Inventory-2),16 and perceived stress (Perceived Stress Scale-14)17 during pregnancy and/or after birth through the child's 4.5-year-old visit, and average maternal sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index)18 during pregnancy and/or postpartum. Additional potential risk factors for children of WWE on ASM included maximum third-trimester ASM dose, ASM group (monotherapy vs polytherapy), ASM category (lamotrigine monotherapy, levetiracetam monotherapy, other monotherapy, lamotrigine + levetiracetam polytherapy, or other polytherapy), and specific ASM. ASM dose was standardized across ASMs by dividing the prescribed daily dose for a specific ASM by its World Health Organization–defined daily dose value19 or derived from package inserts to calculate the mother's ratio of defined daily dose (ratio DDD). The sum of the ratio DDDs across all ASMs was used for mothers on polytherapy, and the maximum ratio DDD in the third trimester was used for analysis.
Statistical Analysis
Analysis Populations
Primary analysis 1 was restricted to the subset of children with non-missing TTCT-F Creativity Index scores, and primary analysis 2 was further restricted to children of WWE on ASM during the third trimester with available blood concentrations.
Primary Analyses
Unadjusted and adjusted linear regression models were used to compare TTCT-F Creativity Index scores between children of WWE and children of HW for primary analysis 1 and to assess the association between TTCT-F Creativity Index scores and mothers' maximum observed third-trimester ratio ASM concentration for primary analysis 2. Mother's IQ11 was included in both adjusted models a priori. Additional covariates were chosen using separate stepwise selection algorithms for each primary analysis. Akaike information criterion was used to compare models. The significance level for variable inclusion was p = 0.10 and that for staying in the model was p = 0.15. Potential maternal covariates consisted of age, WGA, employment status, education level, and household income at enrollment, planned and/or welcomed pregnancy, lifetime history of mood and/or anxiety disorders, any major depressive episode, smoking, alcohol use, and illicit substance use during pregnancy, and the risk factors described above. Additional maternal covariates for primary analysis 2 included epilepsy type (focal, generalized, unclassified/mixed), >5 convulsive seizures during pregnancy, number of convulsions during pregnancy, ASM group, and ASM category. Potential child-related covariates were WGA at birth, birth weight, sex, race, ethnicity, small for gestational age, and major congenital malformations. Variables with >5% missingness were excluded from consideration. Assumptions for the linear regression models were assessed graphically. Significance was considered at the p < 0.05 level (see eMethods for additional details of statistical analyses).
Secondary Analyses
Secondary analyses for risk factors and additional outcomes used similar linear regression models as the primary analyses and incorporated the same covariates in the adjusted models. The BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score was used as the primary measure of executive function, and the other BRIEF-P Indexes were subsequently analyzed. Interaction models were used to assess whether the association between the TTCT-F or BRIEF-P outcomes and ASM concentration or ASM dose in the third trimester differed by ASM group or ASM category. Correlations between the 4.5-year-old TTCT-F creativity outcomes, the BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score, and the 3-year-old Differential Abilities Scale II (DAS-II) General Conceptual Ability Standard Score were measured using Pearson correlation coefficient. Age 3 years was used because DAS-II was not given at age 4.5 years.
Exploratory Analyses
An exploratory analysis was run to assess whether the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic affected the TTCT-F Creativity Index. Scores before and after the start of the COVID-19 shutdown, defined as April 1, 2020, were compared for all children and separately in children of WWE on ASM in the third trimester.
Sensitivity Analyses
Sensitivity analyses were run to compare demographic variables, risk factors, covariates, and outcomes between children included in the primary analyses and those who were excluded. The impact of twins on the primary analyses was assessed by rerunning the primary analyses excluding 1 twin, excluding both twins, and using generalized estimating equations to account for the correlation between twin pairs. Primary analysis 1 was rerun removing children of WWE whose mothers were not on ASM during pregnancy. As a post hoc sensitivity analysis, the primary adjusted analyses were rerun including age at TTCT-F assessment as an additional covariate because of our sample consisting of mostly children younger than 5 years and the TTCT-F normalized scores being defined for 5-year-old children.
Data Availability
All data included in these analyses will be shared as deidentified data by request from any qualified investigator.
Results
Of 345 children born to WWE and 106 children born to HW, 251 and 73 were included in primary analysis 1, respectively. Primary analysis 2 consisted of 228 children of WWE whose mothers had third-trimester ASM blood concentration data. The most prevalent third-trimester ASM regimen for WWE in primary analysis 1 was monotherapy (72.5%), followed by polytherapy (21.9%), while mothers not on ASM during pregnancy were only a minority (5.2%). One mother was not on ASM during the third trimester because of delivery in the second trimester. Lamotrigine and levetiracetam were the most common monotherapies (44.5% and 37.4% of all monotherapies, respectively), and lamotrigine + levetiracetam was the most common polytherapy (41.8% of all polytherapies) (see eTable 1 for the full list of individual ASMs). More information on individual ASM doses and blood concentrations are available in our prior publication.14 A flowchart of enrollment and exclusions is provided (eFigure 1). Demographics, baseline characteristics, and risk factors for children of WWE vs HW from primary analysis 1 are summarized in Table 1 and for children of WWE from primary analysis 2 in eTables 2 and 3.
Table 1.
Demographic, Baseline Characteristics, and Risk Factors for Children of WWE vs HW in Primary Analysis 1 (N = 324)
Categorical variables | Children of WWE (N = 251), n (%) | Children of HW (N = 73), n (%) | p Valuea |
Child's sex: male | 112 (44.6) | 38 (52.1) | 0.262 |
Child's race | 0.011 | ||
White | 204 (81.3) | 49 (67.1) | |
Black or African American | 12 (4.8) | 10 (13.7) | |
Other/unknown | 35 (13.9) | 14 (19.2) | |
Child's ethnicity: Hispanic or Latino | 52 (20.7) | 17 (23.3) | 0.637 |
Small for gestational age | 16 (6.5) | 5 (6.8) | 1.000 |
Missing | 6 | 0 | |
Major congenital malformations | 10 (4.0) | 1 (1.4) | 0.467 |
Any breastfeeding | 194 (77.6) | 65 (89.0) | 0.031 |
Missing | 1 | 0 | |
Periconceptional folate use | 223 (88.8) | 48 (65.8) | <0.001 |
Periconceptional folate dose | <0.001 | ||
0 mg | 28 (11.3) | 25 (34.7) | |
>0–0.4 mg | 20 (8.1) | 13 (18.1) | |
>0.4–1 mg | 46 (18.5) | 27 (37.5) | |
>1–4 mg | 138 (55.6) | 7 (9.7) | |
>4 mg | 16 (6.5) | 0 (0.0) | |
Missing | 3 | 1 | |
Pregnancy planned/welcomed | 0.300 | ||
Planned | 181 (72.1) | 46 (63.0) | |
Unplanned/welcome | 63 (25.1) | 25 (34.2) | |
Unplanned/unwelcome | 7 (2.8) | 2 (2.7) | |
Major depressive episode during pregnancyb | 11 (4.4) | 2 (2.7) | 0.740 |
Mother's education | 0.245 | ||
College degree (advanced) | 69 (27.5) | 26 (35.6) | |
College degree (not advanced) | 112 (44.6) | 25 (34.2) | |
No college degree | 70 (27.9) | 22 (30.1) | |
Mother's employment | 0.767 | ||
Employed full-time | 143 (57.0) | 40 (54.8) | |
Employed part-time | 36 (14.3) | 13 (17.8) | |
Unemployedc | 72 (28.7) | 20 (27.4) | |
Household income | 0.950 | ||
Subject desires not to answer | 10 (4.0) | 4 (5.5) | |
$24,999 or less | 45 (17.9) | 14 (19.2) | |
$25,000–49,999 | 23 (9.2) | 7 (9.6) | |
$50,000–74,999 | 37 (14.7) | 8 (11.0) | |
$75,000–99,999 | 46 (18.3) | 12 (16.4) | |
$100,000 and up | 90 (35.9) | 28 (38.4) | |
Smokingd | 13 (5.2) | 5 (6.8) | 0.567 |
Alcohol used | 58 (23.1) | 25 (34.2) | 0.055 |
Illicit substance used | 7 (2.8) | 4 (5.5) | 0.276 |
Lifetime history of mood disordere | 42 (16.9) | 9 (13.0) | 0.436 |
Missing | 3 | 4 | |
Lifetime history of anxiety disordere | 41 (16.5) | 11 (15.9) | 0.907 |
Missing | 3 | 4 |
Continuous variables | N | Mean (95% CI) | Median (IQR) | Min–max | N | Mean (95% CI) | Median (IQR) | Min–max | p Valuef |
WGA at birth | 251 | 38.5 (38.3–38.8) | 39 (38–40) | 24–42 | 73 | 38.5 (38.0–38.9) | 39 (38–40) | 26–41 | 0.831 |
WGA at enrollment | 251 | 13.2 (12.6–13.8) | 13 (10–17) | 4–29 | 73 | 15.3 (14.4–16.3) | 16 (13–19) | 5–23 | <0.001 |
Birth weight (kg) | 246 | 3.24 (3.16–3.31) | 3.3 (2.9–3.6) | 0.6–4.9 | 73 | 3.28 (3.13–3.42) | 3.3 (3.0–3.5) | 1.1–5.0 | 0.606 |
Mother's age at enrollment | 251 | 31.1 (30.5–31.7) | 31 (28–34) | 18–46 | 73 | 29.8 (28.6–31.0) | 31 (27–33) | 15–38 | 0.056 |
Mother's IQ | 251 | 98.5 (96.9–100.0) | 100 (90–107) | 62–123 | 73 | 104.6 (101.6–107.7) | 106 (96–114) | 72–129 | <0.001 |
Father's IQg | 178 | 103.9 (102.0–105.9) | 105 (95–113) | 58–139 | 45 | 103.2 (99.2–107.2) | 101 (95–114) | 75–123 | 0.753 |
Maternal relative's IQg | 102 | 100.9 (97.7–104.1) | 102 (93–113) | 51–133 | 25 | 102.2 (98.0–106.5) | 103 (97–109) | 74–123 | 0.693 |
BDI-IIh | |||||||||
Enrollment: 4.5-y-old visit | 251 | 6.00 (5.35–6.66) | 4.9 (2.2–7.6) | 0.2–34.0 | 73 | 5.16 (4.21–6.12) | 4.7 (2.3–6.3) | 0.0–26.1 | 0.449 |
Pregnancy | 251 | 6.95 (6.22–7.67) | 5.3 (3.3–8.7) | 0.0–38.7 | 73 | 5.97 (5.07–6.87) | 5.0 (3.0–8.3) | 0.0–17.7 | 0.419 |
Post-birth | 251 | 5.63 (4.92–6.34) | 4.3 (1.7–7.5) | 0.0–32.9 | 73 | 4.75 (3.66–5.85) | 3.9 (1.6–6.0) | 0.0–30.6 | 0.415 |
BAIh | |||||||||
Enrollment: 4.5-y-old visit | 251 | 4.93 (4.32–5.53) | 3.3 (1.9–6.6) | 0.0–34.2 | 73 | 3.79 (2.76–4.82) | 3.0 (1.3–4.6) | 0.0–26.8 | 0.028 |
Pregnancy | 251 | 6.29 (5.56–7.02) | 4.7 (2.3–8.0) | 0.0–38.3 | 73 | 5.40 (4.25–6.55) | 4.0 (2.3–7.7) | 0.0–29.3 | 0.309 |
Post-birth | 251 | 4.38 (3.74–5.02) | 2.6 (1.2–5.5) | 0.0–32.4 | 73 | 3.08 (2.00–4.15) | 1.9 (0.6–3.7) | 0.0–25.7 | 0.006 |
PSS-14h | |||||||||
Enrollment: 4.5-y-old visit | 251 | 18.05 (17.24–18.86) | 18.0 (13.0–22.3) | 5.0–39.2 | 73 | 17.35 (16.20–18.51) | 17.4 (12.9–20.6) | 8.1–28.7 | 0.494 |
Pregnancy | 251 | 17.98 (17.07–18.90) | 17.7 (12.0–23.0) | 2.7–40.0 | 73 | 16.65 (15.28–18.03) | 16.3 (12.7–21.0) | 3.3–28.7 | 0.272 |
Post-birth | 251 | 18.10 (17.25–18.95) | 18.1 (13.2–22.6) | 2.1–40.3 | 73 | 17.64 (16.38–18.89) | 16.9 (12.8–21.6) | 8.9–33.0 | 0.639 |
PSQIg,i | |||||||||
Pregnancy + postpartum | 200 | 5.66 (5.31–6.02) | 5.6 (3.7–7.0) | 0.1–13.0 | 56 | 5.47 (4.82–6.11) | 5.0 (3.9–6.7) | 1.2–12.3 | 0.605 |
Pregnancy | 229 | 5.75 (5.37–6.12) | 5.3 (3.6–7.6) | 0.1–14.0 | 65 | 4.87 (4.23–5.52) | 4.3 (3.4–5.8) | 1.0–11.8 | 0.029 |
Postpartum | 202 | 5.60 (5.22–5.98) | 5.1 (3.6–7.0) | 0.0–13.6 | 56 | 5.89 (5.15–6.62) | 6.1 (3.7–7.7) | 0.8–12.5 | 0.484 |
Abbreviations: BAI = Beck Anxiety Inventory; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II; HW = healthy women; IQR = interquartile range; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PSS-14 = Perceived Stress Scale 14; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural; WGA = weeks gestational age; WWE = women with epilepsy.
Missing not used in calculation of percentages.
For categorical variables: p values from the χ2 test except for small for gestational age, major congenital malformations, major depressive episode during pregnancy, smoking, and illicit substance, which used the Fisher exact test.
Depression based on a SCID mood module at any depression assessment during pregnancy.
Unemployed includes stay-at-home parents without outside job, unemployed because of disability, full-time student, and looking for work.
Self-reported smoking, alcohol use, or illicit substance use (including marijuana) at any time during pregnancy.
Diagnosis from SCID.
p Values from t test (WGA at birth, WGA at enrollment, birth weight, age, IQ scores, and PSQI scores) and Wilcoxon rank-sum test (BDI, BAI, and PSS scores).
Father's IQ, maternal relative's IQ, and PSQI scores not assessed for inclusion in the regression model because of extensive missing data.
Average of all assessments completed during the time period. Enrollment: 4.5-year-old visit: enrollment through the 4.5-year-old visit; pregnancy: enrollment through the day of delivery; post-birth: after delivery through the 4.5-year-old visit.
Weighted monthly average of PSQI assessments completed during the time period. At most 1 PSQI score per day was used in the calculation. Pregnancy: enrollment through the day before delivery; postpartum: 6 weeks through 9 months after delivery; pregnancy + postpartum: assessments completed during either period (only women with scores in both periods included).
Primary Analysis 1
There were no statistically significant differences found between children of WWE vs HW in the mean 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index standard score (unadjusted mean [95% CI] 90.2 [87.4–93.0] vs 94.9 [89.8–100.1]; adjusted least squares mean [95% CI] 90.5 [87.8–93.2] vs 93.7 [88.6–98.8]) (Figure 1). Higher maternal IQ was significantly associated with higher 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index in the adjusted model (Table 2). Education level and illicit substance use during pregnancy were also selected as additional covariates for the adjusted model, but were not significant at the 0.05 level in the adjusted model.
Figure 1. Age 4.5 Years TTCT-F Creativity Index by Mother's Study Group—Children of WWE vs HW (N = 324).
Adjusted least squares mean scores (95% CI) 90.5 (87.8–93.2) for children of WWE vs 93.7 (88.6–98.8) for children of HW (nonsignificant). HW = healthy women; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural; WWE = women with epilepsy.
Table 2.
Age 4.5 Years TTCT-F Creativity Index Scores: Full Model Summaries for Primary Analyses
Model parameter | Parameter estimate (95% CI) | p Value |
Children of WWE vs HW (N = 324)a | ||
Mother's study group: WWE vs HW | −3.2 (−9.0 to 2.7) | 0.286 |
Mother's IQ | 0.3 (0.0 to 0.5) | 0.024 |
Education level | 0.062 | |
College degree (advanced) | Ref | — |
College degree (not advanced) | −5.2 (−11.1 to 0.8) | 0.087 |
No college degree | −8.9 (−16.4 to −1.4) | 0.020 |
Mother's illicit substance use during pregnancy: any vs noneb | −12.3 (−25.6 to 1.0) | 0.070 |
Children of WWE with third-trimester ASM blood concentrations (N = 228)c | ||
Mother's third trimester max observed ratio ASM concentrationd | −2.6 (−11.0 to 5.7) | 0.530 |
Mother's IQ | 0.3 (0.1 to 0.6) | 0.007 |
Abbreviations: ASM = antiseizure medication; HW = healthy women; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural; WWE = women with epilepsy.
Variables selected into adjusted models using a stepwise selection algorithm. Mother's IQ and study group or third-trimester ratio ASM concentration included in the model a priori. Akaike information criterion was used to compare models. Significance level for covariate entry was set to p = 0.01, and significance level to remain in the model was set to p = 0.15.
N = children of WWE and HW with age 4.5 years TTCT-F Creativity Index. Full model R2: 0.091.
Self-reported illicit substance use (including marijuana) at any time during pregnancy.
N = children of WWE with age 4.5 years TTCT-F Creativity Index and mothers with third-trimester ASM concentrations. Full model R2: 0.036.
Ratio ASM concentration calculated as the of ratio the upper limit for therapeutic range. For mothers on polytherapy, ratio ASM concentration calculated by summing the ratio ASM concentration for each ASM. Maximum observed value during the third trimester, including day of delivery.
Primary Analysis 2
No significant relationship was found between 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index and mother's third-trimester maximum observed ratio ASM concentration (unadjusted parameter estimate [95% CI] −4.1 [−12.5 to 4.3]; adjusted parameter estimate [95% CI] −2.6 [−11.0 to 5.7]) (Table 2 and Figure 2). Higher maternal IQ was significantly related to greater creativity in the adjusted model. No additional covariates were selected into the adjusted analyses.
Figure 2. Age 4.5 Years TTCT-F Creativity Index Score vs Third-Trimester Maximum Observed Ratio ASM Concentration in Children of WWE With Third-Trimester Blood Concentrations (N = 228).
Adjusted parameter estimate (95% CI) −2.6 (−11.0 to 5.7) (nonsignificant). ASM = antiseizure medication; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural.
Secondary Analyses
Children of WWE and HW did not differ on any subscales of the 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index (eTable 4). The associations between third-trimester ASM concentration with 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index subscales were not significant (Table 3) by either ASM group or ASM category (eTables 5–8). The results with ASM dose were similar (eTables 9–14).
Table 3.
Association Between Age 4.5 Years TTCT-F Subscales and BRIEF-P Scores and Maximum Observed Ratio ASM Concentrationa in the Third Trimester for Children of WWE With Third-Trimester Blood Concentrations
N | Unadjusted analysisb | Adjusted analysisc | |||
Parameter estimate (95% CI) | p Value | Parameter estimate (95% CI) | p Value | ||
TTCT-F Fluency Standard Score | 228 | −1.6 (−9.1 to 6.0) | 0.684 | −1.0 (−8.6 to 6.6) | 0.799 |
TTCT-F Originality Standard Score | 228 | −4.8 (−14.3 to 4.6) | 0.315 | −3.4 (−12.8 to 6.0) | 0.483 |
TTCT-F Elaboration Standard Score | 228 | −2.4 (−7.6 to 2.8) | 0.356 | −1.1 (−6.2 to 4.0) | 0.668 |
TTCT-F Abstractness of Titles Standard Score | 228 | −0.0 (−11.8 to 11.7) | 0.993 | 1.8 (−10.0 to 13.5) | 0.766 |
TTCT-F Resist to Closure Standard Score | 228 | −8.2 (−20.1 to 3.7) | 0.176 | −7.6 (−19.6 to 4.3) | 0.211 |
BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score T-score | 230 | 7.6 (3.5 to 11.8) | <0.001 | 7.0 (2.9 to 11.2) | 0.001 |
BRIEF-P Inhibitory Self-Control Index T-score | 230 | 7.4 (3.3 to 11.5) | <0.001 | 7.1 (3.0 to 11.2) | <0.001 |
BRIEF-P Flexibility Index T-score | 230 | 7.0 (3.0 to 11.1) | <0.001 | 6.6 (2.5 to 10.6) | 0.002 |
BRIEF-P Emergent Metacognition Index T-score | 230 | 7.1 (3.4 to 10.9) | <0.001 | 6.7 (2.9 to 10.4) | <0.001 |
Abbreviations: ASM = antiseizure medication; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version; HW = healthy women; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural; WWE = women with epilepsy.
N = children of WWE with non-missing outcome score and mothers with max observed ratio ASM concentration in the third trimester (includes day of delivery).
Note positive parameter estimates indicated association of higher ratio ASM concentrations, which are better for TTCT-F scores but worse for BRIEF-P scores.
Ratio ASM concentration calculated as the ratio of the upper limit for the therapeutic range. For mothers on polytherapy, ratio ASM concentration calculated by summing the ratio ASM concentration for each ASM. Maximum observed value recorded during the third trimester, including the day of delivery used for analysis.
Linear regression model.
Linear regression model adjusted for mother's IQ.
Executive Function
The mean T-scores of children of WWE and HW did not differ on any BRIEF-P indexes (eTable 4). The BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score at age 4.5 years was associated with third-trimester ASM concentration in adjusted analyses, as were all the other indexes (Table 3). Worse outcomes of executive functions (i.e., higher scores on the BRIEF-P) were observed with increasing third-trimester ASM exposure. Analyses of BRIEF-P scores stratified by ASM category and group were conducted (Table 4 and eTable 15). Higher blood concentrations of levetiracetam were associated with poorer performance on the BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score, Inhibitory Self Control Index, and Emergent Metacognition Index, and higher concentrations of lamotrigine + levetiracetam were associated with poorer performance on the Flexibility Index and Emergent Metacognition Index (Table 4 and eFigure 2). The results with ASM dose were similar (eTable 12–14). Lamotrigine had no exposure-dependent effects, and samples sizes for other ASMs were inadequate to assess individually.
Table 4.
Association Between Age 4.5 Years BRIEF-P Scores and Maximum Observed Ratio ASM Concentrationa in the Third Trimester by ASM Category, Children of WWE With Third-Trimester Blood Concentrations
Third-trimester ASM categoryd | N | Unadjusted analysisb | Adjusted analysisb,c | ||
Parameter estimate (95% CI) | p Value | Parameter estimate (95% CI) | p Value | ||
BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score T-score | |||||
Lamotrigine | 79 | 5.2 (−7.9 to 18.4) | 0.432 | 5.4 (−7.6 to 18.4) | 0.415 |
Levetiracetam | 70 | 12.5 (3.7 to 21.3) | 0.005 | 12.9 (4.2 to 21.6) | 0.004 |
Other monotherapy | 33 | −4.7 (−22.1 to 12.7) | 0.598 | −5.1 (−22.3 to 12.1) | 0.562 |
Lamotrigine + levetiracetam | 24 | 12.0 (0.3 to 23.8) | 0.045 | 11.6 (−0.1 to 23.2) | 0.052 |
Other polytherapy | 24 | 5.6 (−7.9 to 19.1) | 0.416 | 5.4 (−8.0 to 18.7) | 0.429 |
p Value for interaction terme | — | — | 0.435 | — | 0.395 |
BRIEF-P Inhibitory Self-Control Index T-score | |||||
Lamotrigine | 79 | 5.3 (−7.6 to 18.3) | 0.421 | 5.4 (−7.6 to 18.3) | 0.415 |
Levetiracetam | 70 | 9.9 (1.2 to 18.5) | 0.026 | 10.0 (1.4 to 18.7) | 0.024 |
Other monotherapy | 33 | −2.9 (−20.1 to 14.2) | 0.738 | −3.1 (−20.3 to 14.0) | 0.720 |
Lamotrigine + levetiracetam | 24 | 10.6 (−1.0 to 22.2) | 0.073 | 10.4 (−1.2 to 22.0) | 0.079 |
Other polytherapy | 24 | 6.1 (−7.2 to 19.4) | 0.367 | 6.0 (−7.3 to 19.3) | 0.375 |
p Value for interaction terme | — | — | 0.708 | — | 0.697 |
BRIEF-P Flexibility Index T-score | |||||
Lamotrigine | 79 | 8.3 (−4.5 to 21.1) | 0.201 | 8.4 (−4.3 to 21.1) | 0.193 |
Levetiracetam | 70 | 7.0 (−1.5 to 15.6) | 0.108 | 7.3 (−1.2 to 15.8) | 0.093 |
Other monotherapy | 33 | 1.8 (−15.1 to 18.7) | 0.832 | 1.5 (−15.3 to 18.3) | 0.862 |
Lamotrigine + levetiracetam | 24 | 12.5 (1.1 to 24.0) | 0.032 | 12.1 (0.7 to 23.5) | 0.037 |
Other polytherapy | 24 | 5.8 (−7.3 to 18.9) | 0.381 | 5.7 (−7.4 to 18.7) | 0.393 |
p Value for interaction terme | — | — | 0.867 | — | 0.872 |
BRIEF-P Emergent Metacognition Index T-score | |||||
Lamotrigine | 79 | 4.5 (−7.2 to 16.2) | 0.449 | 4.6 (−7.0 to 16.2) | 0.435 |
Levetiracetam | 70 | 14.2 (6.3 to 22.0) | <0.001 | 14.5 (6.7 to 22.2) | <0.001 |
Other monotherapy | 33 | −9.8 (−25.3 to 5.6) | 0.211 | −10.2 (−25.5 to 5.2) | 0.192 |
Lamotrigine + levetiracetam | 24 | 11.0 (0.5 to 21.4) | 0.040 | 10.6 (0.2 to 21.0) | 0.046 |
Other polytherapy | 24 | 4.2 (−7.7 to 16.2) | 0.486 | 4.1 (−7.8 to 15.9) | 0.501 |
p Value for interaction terme | — | — | 0.075 | — | 0.061 |
Abbreviations: ASM = antiseizure medication; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version; WWE = women with epilepsy.
N = children of WWE with non-missing outcome score and mothers with maximum observed ratio ASM concentration in the third trimester (includes the day of delivery).
Ratio ASM concentration calculated as the ratio of the upper limit for the therapeutic range. For mothers on polytherapy, ratio ASM concentration calculated by summing the ratio ASM concentration for each ASM. Maximum observed value recorded during the third trimester, including the day of delivery.
Parameter estimate and p value from a linear regression model including mother's max observed ratio ASM concentration in the third trimester, ASM category, and their interaction. Note that higher BRIEF scores are worse, so a positive parameter estimate indicates worse score with higher ASM concentration.
Adjusted for mother's IQ.
Based on ASM regimen taken at the time of blood draw for maximum observed ratio ASM concentration during the third trimester (includes the day of delivery).
p Value for the interaction between max observed ratio ASM concentration in the third trimester and ASM category. If significant, the association between max observed ratio ASM concentration in the third trimester and age 4.5-year-old score differs by mother's ASM category.
Risk Factors
No risk factors were found to be significantly associated with the 4.5-year-old TTCT-F Creativity Index in adjusted models in either analysis population (eTables 16–21). Mother's ASM group, ASM category, and specific ASM were also not related to creativity in the primary analysis 2 population (eTable 22).
Exploratory Analyses
There were no differences in the TTCT-F Creativity Index in children evaluated before vs after the COVID-19 shutdown (eTables 23 and 24).
Sensitivity Analyses
Comparisons of children included vs excluded in the primary analyses are presented in eTables 25–31. Conclusions from primary analysis 1 remained the same after removing children of WWE whose mothers were not on ASM (eTables 32 and 33). The presence of twins did not affect the results of either primary analysis (eTables 34 and 35). Age was significantly associated with creativity, but adjusting for age in the primary analysis models did not affect the main results of the analysis (eTables 36 and 37).
Correlative Analyses
Correlations between various measures of the 4.5-year-old TTCT-F with the 4.5-year-old BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score and the 3-year-old DAS-II General Conceptual Ability Standard Score were significant but low, ranging from −0.16 through −0.02 and 0.21 through 0.38, respectively (Table 5). The correlation between the BRIEF-P and DAS-II measures was −0.28.
Table 5.
Correlation Between Age 4.5 TTCT-F Subscales, Age 4.5 Years BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score T-Score, and Age 3 Years DAS-II General Conceptual Ability Standard Score, Children of WWE and HW (N = 449)
Outcome | Summary statistics | Pearson correlation | |||||||
4.5-y-old BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite score T-score | 3-y-old DAS-II General Conceptual Ability Standard Score | ||||||||
N | Mean (95% CI) | Median (min–max) | n | ρ | p Value | n | ρ | p Value | |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F Creativity Index | 324 | 91.2 (88.8–93.7) | 94.0 (46–137) | 314 | −0.13 | 0.027 | 303 | 0.38 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F Fluency Standard Score | 324 | 84.6 (82.3–86.9) | 85.0 (40–147) | 314 | −0.10 | 0.066 | 303 | 0.32 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F Originality Standard Score | 324 | 85.8 (83.0–88.6) | 89.0 (40–141) | 314 | −0.08 | 0.139 | 303 | 0.32 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F elaboration standard score | 324 | 83.7 (82.2–85.3) | 84.0 (53–138) | 314 | −0.16 | 0.006 | 303 | 0.37 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F Abstractness of Titles Standard Score | 324 | 88.3 (84.9–91.7) | 91.0 (40–149) | 314 | −0.12 | 0.030 | 303 | 0.30 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old TTCT-F Resist to Closure Standard Score | 324 | 90.7 (87.3–94.2) | 96.0 (40–147) | 314 | −0.02 | 0.785 | 303 | 0.21 | <0.001 |
4.5-y-old BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite Score T-score | 327 | 47.2 (45.8–48.5) | 44.0 (33–95) | — | — | — | 301 | −0.28 | <0.001 |
3-y-old DAS-II General Conceptual Ability Standard Score | 361 | 105.4 (103.8–107.0) | 106.0 (42–155) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Abbreviations: ASM = antiseizure medication; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version; DAS-II = Differential Abilities Scale II; HW = healthy women; TTCT-F = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural; WWE = women with epilepsy.
N = children of WWE and HW with non-missing outcome measure; n = children of WWE and HW with non-missing measures for both outcomes in correlation.
Discussion
Our study demonstrates that there are no significant differences in measures of creativity or parent ratings of executive functions at age 4.5 years between children of WWE vs HW. Furthermore, no ASM exposure-dependent effects were found for the creativity measure, although exposure-dependent effects for executive function were present for ASM concentrations and ASM doses. Analyses of risk factors and sensitivity analyses support these findings.
The absence of ASM effects may be because of the young age of the children as the TTCT-F is only standardized down to age 5 years. However, we previously demonstrated adverse effects of valproate vs carbamazepine and lamotrigine on creativity measures in children with a mean age of 4.2 years.6 Furthermore, the specific tasks from the TTCT-F differed from the creativity measure used in our prior study, which used the Torrance TCAM. The TCAM is a tactile and kinesthetic measure that may be more sensitive for capturing creativity in young children compared with the TTCT-F used in this study because young children more easily express their creativity through this modality.7 However, fetal exposure to the currently used ASMs may not affect creativity as valproate does.
Although children of WWE did not differ from children of HW on executive functions, there was an exposure-dependent effect on executive functions, such that increasing third-trimester ASM concentrations were associated with poorer performance on parent ratings of executive functions. This effect seemed to be primarily due to levetiracetam. Lamotrigine had no exposure-dependent effects, and sample sizes for other ASMs were inadequate to assess individually. We observed similar findings for levetiracetam in our prior analyses of cognition at age 3 years and of adaptive and behavioral functioning at 4.5 years.14,20 In addition, a recent retrospective population-based study found that fetal levetiracetam exposure is associated with increased risk of anxiety and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, although ASM concentration and dose effects were not assessed.21 Nevertheless, levetiracetam has low risks of major congenital malformations and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes, so it is considered one of the safest ASMs during pregnancy.22 All ASMs are potential teratogens, and teratogens act in an exposure-dependent manner. Even lamotrigine has shown a small but significant dose-dependent risk of increased malformations.23 Thus, management of WWE during pregnancy requires ASM dosing to be sufficiently high enough to control seizures to protect the mother and the fetus, but at the lowest effective dose to minimize risks to the fetus.5 Furthermore, dosing needs to take into account clearance changes for many ASMs during pregnancy.24
Our creativity measure (TTCT-F at age 4.5 years) was correlated with IQ (DAS-II at age 3 years) and to a lesser degree with executive function (BRIEF-P at age 4.5 years). These correlations are expected based on prior studies.2-4
Strengths of this study include the large prospective design with detailed data for WWE and HW and their children, allowing control for multiple factors. Our study also assessed ASM blood concentrations to control for clearance changes during pregnancy. Despite being one of largest prospective studies of WWE in pregnancy with detailed information on potential confounders, our sample size is limited. Patients were recruited from tertiary centers, so there is a concern for generalizability. Assessments for a portion of our children aged 4.5 years occurred during the COVID-19 epidemic, which could have affected results; however, no differences in TTCT-F Creativity Index scores were found between children evaluated before vs after the COVID-19 shutdown. Finally, because this is an observational study, like all studies of pregnancy outcomes in WWE, there may be unmeasured confounding because of the observational nature of the study design. While adjusted models were run, few potentially confounding variables were selected into these models because of nonsignificant associations with the primary outcome measure, which may have resulted in biased results because of potential unmeasured confounding. Selection bias may also be present because of the lack of randomization of ASM regimens for practical and ethical reasons. Thus, these findings require replication in separate cohorts.
No differences in creativity or executive functions at age 4.5 years were found for children of WWE vs HW. No ASM exposure-dependent effects were found for creativity, but secondary analyses revealed exposure-dependent effects for executive function.
Acknowledgment
The authors acknowledge Eugene Moore and Jordan Seliger for their administrative assistance throughout the MONEAD trial. They thank the patients, subjects, families, and research staff that made this work possible. See online supplemental materials for other members of the MONEAD Investigator Group (eAppendix 1).
Glossary
- ASM
antiseizure medication
- BRIEF-P
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version
- COVID-19
coronavirus disease 2019
- DAS-II
Differential Abilities Scale II
- HW
healthy women
- MONEAD
Maternal Outcomes and Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs
- ratio DDD
ratio of defined daily dose
- TCAM
Thinking Creatively in Action and Movement
- TTCT-F
Torrance Test of Creative Thinking—Figural
- WWE
women with epilepsy
- WGA
weeks gestational age
Appendix 1. Authors
Name | Location | Contribution |
Kimford J. Meador, MD | Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
Morris J. Cohen, EdD | Pediatric Neuropsychology International, Augusta, GA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
David W. Loring, PhD | Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
Abigail G. Matthews, PhD | The Emmes Company, Rockville, MD | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
Carrie A. Brown, PhD | The Emmes Company, Rockville, MD | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; analysis or interpretation of data |
Chelsea Robalino, MS | The Emmes Company, Rockville, MD | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; study concept or design |
Andrea Carmack, MB | The Emmes Company, Rockville, MD | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; analysis or interpretation of data |
Sarah Sumners, PhD | Piedmont University, Athens, GA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content |
Angela K. Birnbaum, PhD | University of Minnesota, Minneapolis | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
Laura A. Kalayjian, MD | University of Southern California, Los Angeles | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Evan Gedzelman, MD | Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Paula E. Voinescu, MD | Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Elizabeth E. Gerard, MD | Northwestern University, Chicago, IL | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Julie Hanna, MD | Minnesota Epilepsy Group, Roseville | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Jennifer Cavitt, MD | University of Cincinnati, OH | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Maria Sam, MD | Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Sean T. Hwang, MD | Northwell Health, Great Neck, NY | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Alison M. Pack, MD | Columbia University, New York, NY | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Jeffrey J. Tsai, MD | University of Washington, Seattle | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data |
Page B. Pennell, MD | University of Pittsburgh, PA | Drafting/revision of the manuscript for content, including medical writing for content; major role in the acquisition of data; study concept or design; analysis or interpretation of data |
Appendix 2. Coinvestigators
Coinvestigators are listed at Neurology.org. |
Footnotes
Editorial, page e209553
Study Funding
Funded by NIH, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke/NICHD U01-NS038455; ClinicalTrials.gov number NCT01730170.
Disclosure
K.J. Meador has received research support from the NIH, Eisai, and Medtronic Inc. The Epilepsy Study Consortium pays his university for his research consultant time related to Eisai, GW Pharmaceuticals, NeuroPace, Novartis, Supernus, Upsher-Smith Laboratories, UCB Pharma, and Vivus Pharmaceuticals. In addition, K.J. Meador is Co-I and Director of Cognitive Core of the Human Epilepsy Project for the Epilepsy Study Consortium, and is on the editorial boards for Neurology, Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, Epilepsy & Behavior, and Epilepsy & Behavior Case Reports. M.J. Cohen receives royalties from Multi Health Systems, Inc. as author of the Children's Memory Scale. D.W. Loring receives research support from the NIH, is a consultant for Medtronic, serves on the editorial boards for Neuropsychology Review and Epilepsia for which he receives editorial stipends, and is on the editorial board for Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. A.K. Birnbaum reports receiving grant support, paid to her institution, from Supernus Pharmaceuticals and Veloxis Pharmaceuticals, holding patent US9770407B2 on parenteral carbamazepine formulation, licensed to Lundbeck, and patent EP12150783A on novel parenteral carbamazepine formulations, licensed to Lundbeck. P.E. Voinescu received speaking honoraria from Physicians' Education Resource and from Philippines League Against Epilepsy. E.E. Gerard has served as site-PI for clinical trials sponsored by Xenon and Sunovion pharmaceuticals as well as a trial sponsored by Eisai and Stanford University, and has been reimbursed for lectures given to GW Pharmaceuticals Staff and Neurology Week. J. Cavitt received research support from National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (MONEAD) and from GW Pharmaceuticals, and advisory board fees from Jazz Pharmaceuticals. M. Sam has received advisory board consulting fees for Aquestive. S. Hwang reports no disclosures. A.M. Pack reports funding from NIH, royalties from Up to Date, and travel reimbursement for AAN and ABPN activities. J.J. Tsai reports grant from Institute of Translational Health Sciences (funded by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the NIH), and has served as site-PI for clinical trials sponsored by Xenon. P.B. Pennell reports grants from NIH, personal fees from NIH for Grant reviews, personal fees from AES and the AAN as speaking honoraria, personal fees from Medical Schools for speaking honoraria and travel, and personal fees from UpToDate, Inc. for royalties outside the submitted work. The other authors report no relevant disclosures. Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.
References
- 1.Sternberg R, Sternberg K. Creativity. In: Cognitive Psychology. 6th ed. Cengage Learning; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Beaty RE, Benedek M, Silvia PJ, Schacter DL. Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends Cogn Sci. 2016;20(2):87-95. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Guilford JP, Christensen PR. The one-way relation between creative potential and IQ. J Creat Behav. 1973;7(4):247-252. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1973.tb01096.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rogers CJ, Tolmie A, Massonnié J, Thomas MS. Complex cognition and individual variability: a mixed methods study of the relationship between creativity and executive control. Front Psychol. 2023;14:1191893. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1191893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Li Y, Meador KJ. Epilepsy and pregnancy. Continuum (Minneap Minn). 2022;28(1):34-54. doi: 10.1212/con.0000000000001056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McVearry KM, Gaillard WD, VanMeter J, Meador KJ. A prospective study of cognitive fluency and originality in children exposed in utero to carbamazepine, lamotrigine, or valproate monotherapy. Epilepsy Behav. 2009;16(4):609-616. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.09.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Torrance EP. Thinking Creatively in Action and Movement. Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Runco MA, Millar G, Acar S, Cramond B. Torrance tests of creative thinking as predictors of personal and public achievement: a fifty-year follow-up. Creat Res J. 2010;22(4):361-368. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2010.523393 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lawal OD, Meador KJ, Hume AL, Wen X. Utilization of antiseizure medications in women of childbearing age with epilepsy and nonepilepsy indications: a retrospective cohort study. Neurology. 2023;101(10):e1083-e1096. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000207580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Meador KJ, Pennell PB, May RC, et al. Changes in antiepileptic drug-prescribing patterns in pregnant women with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2018;84:10-14. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: BRIEF. Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 13.St Louis EK. Minimizing AED adverse effects: improving quality of life in the interictal state in epilepsy care. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2009;7(2):106-114. doi: 10.2174/157015909788848857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Meador KJ, Cohen MJ, Loring DW, et al. Cognitive outcomes at age 3 years in children with fetal exposure to antiseizure medications (MONEAD study) in the USA: a prospective, observational cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 2023;22(8):712-722. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(23)00199-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Beck AT. Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI). Psychological Corporation; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Beck AT, Steer RA. Beck Depression Inventory Manual—Revised. Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385-396. doi: 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Broocks A, Riemann D, Hohagen F. Test–retest reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in primary insomnia. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(3):737-740. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00330-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology. WHO ATC/DDD Index 2021. Updated December 17, 2020. Accessed April 30, 2021. whocc.no/atc_ddd_index/. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cohen MJ, Meador KJ, Loring DW, et al. Behavioral outcomes and neurodevelopmental disorders among children of women with epilepsy. JAMA Neurol. 2024;81(1):19-29. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2023.4315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dreier JW, Bjørk M-H, Alvestad S, et al. Prenatal exposure to antiseizure medication and incidence of childhood-and adolescence-onset psychiatric disorders. JAMA Neurol. 2023;80(6):568-577. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2023.0674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tomson T, Battino D, Bromley R, et al. Executive summary: management of epilepsy in pregnancy: a report from the International League Against Epilepsy Task Force on Women and Pregnancy. Epilepsia. 2019;60(12):2343-2345. doi: 10.1111/epi.16395 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tomson T, Battino D, Bonizzoni E, et al. Comparative risk of major congenital malformations with eight different antiepileptic drugs: a prospective cohort study of the EURAP registry. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17(6):530-538. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30107-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pennell PB, French JA, May RC, et al. Changes in seizure frequency and antiepileptic therapy during pregnancy. N Engl J Med. 2020;383(26):2547-2556. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2008663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data included in these analyses will be shared as deidentified data by request from any qualified investigator.