Skip to main content

This is a preprint.

It has not yet been peer reviewed by a journal.

The National Library of Medicine is running a pilot to include preprints that result from research funded by NIH in PMC and PubMed.

bioRxiv logoLink to bioRxiv
[Preprint]. 2023 Mar 17:2023.03.17.533103. [Version 1] doi: 10.1101/2023.03.17.533103

Uncovering the Morphological Evolution of Language-Relevant Brain Areas

Guillermo Gallardo 1,, Cornelius Eichner 1,, Chet C Sherwood 2, William D Hopkins 3, Alfred Anwander 1, Angela D Friederici 1
PMCID: PMC10055248  PMID: 36993711

Abstract

Human language is supported by a cortical network involving Broca’s area which comprises Brodmann Areas 44 and 45 (BA44, BA45). While cytoarchitectonic homolog areas have been identified in nonhuman primates, it remains unknown how these regions evolved to support human language. Here, we use histological data and advanced cortical registration methods to precisely compare the morphology of BA44 and 45 between humans and chimpanzees. We found a general expansion of Broca’s areas in humans, with the left BA44 enlarging the most, growing anteriorly into a region known to process syntax. Together with recent functional studies, our findings show that BA44 evolved from a purely action-related region to a more expanded region in humans, with a posterior portion supporting action and an anterior portion supporting syntactic processes. Furthermore, our findings provide a solution for the longstanding debate concerning the structural and functional evolution of Broca’s area and its role in action and language.

Introduction

Language processing is a human trait that involves Broca’s area in the inferior frontal gyrus. Previous studies suggest an involvement of this area in the perception, execution, and imitation of actions [1]. Moreover, homologous areas in nonhuman primates have similarly been shown to be involved in actions of the orofacial muscles and upper limbs [2]. However, the relationship between action and language, and how Broca’s area evolved to support them remains elusive. A longstanding debate persists regarding the relationship between language and action, and whether they share the same neural basis. Within this debate, there are currently two opposing views. The first view states that language emerged from action expressed in communicative gestures, and thus they share a common basis [24]. The second view sees language as a cognitive ability independent of action [5,6].

Both views - favoring and opposing a shared basis for language and action - built their arguments on theoretical and empirical grounds [24]. At the theoretical level, the debate focuses on the (di)similarity between the hierarchical structure of linguistic syntax, as a core aspect of human language, and that of goal-directed sequential actions. While some argue that action can be described using a hierarchical structure of subgoals [7,8], others claim that such a description does not meet the definition of hierarchy in language [5]. In this way, the theoretical arguments are stalled. Meanwhile, at the empirical level, several studies in humans have found action to recruit Broca’s area [9,10], an area primarily related to language, thus suggesting a functional co-dependence between action and language [1,2,11]. However, these studies did not compare actions versus language, nor tried to identify their exact location, thus making it hard to understand if the same regions activate for both processes.

To date, there are only two studies directly comparing the neural underpinning of action and language in humans. One is a meta-analysis comparing peak activations of syntactic tasks against motor-related ones [12], and the other is a functional imaging study comparing tool use with language in a within-subject design [13]. Both of these studies found that language and action recruit non-overlapping areas of Broca’s area, with language being processed more anterior than action. Moreover, the meta-analysis shows that Brodmann Area 44 (BA44), the cytoarchitectonic defined posterior division of Broca’s area [14,15], is functionally divided in two regions. In BA44, language recruits the anterior part and action recruits the posterior one [12].

To help settle the debate on the language/action relationship we can turn to our close evolutionary relatives. Anthropoid primates, such as chimpanzees and macaques, possess a cytoarchitectonically similar Broca’s area homolog that, as in humans, functionally responds to action [10]. Moreover, there is evidence that great apes can master some aspects of language using augmentative or alternative communication systems such as sign language or visual graphic symbols [16]. However, only humans possess the faculty of language when defined by the development of syntax and the ability to create multiword utterances following a set of grammatical rules. Hence, a cross-species comparison between the linguistic human brain and one of our closest living relatives, namely chimpanzees, may shed light on the neural basis of action and language.

Earlier cross-species comparisons have shown that the prefrontal cortex is a region that allometrically scales to increase at a disproportionate rate across primates, leading to a relatively large size in the human brain [17,18]. Particularly, the cytoarchitectural regions BA44 and BA45 were found to be up to 6.6-fold larger in humans than in chimpanzees (1.3-fold and 1.4-fold larger than expected, respectively, after correcting for overall cortical enlargement) [19]. Furthermore, based on histological studies, it has been shown that Broca’s subregions BA44 and BA45 differ between humans and chimpanzees in terms of their asymmetry. Human BA45 reaches its leftward volumetric asymmetry by the age of 5 years during development. Human BA44 only reaches its asymmetry by the age of 11 years [20] when children acquire full proficiency in semantic and syntactic knowledge [21]. In contrast, in chimpanzees, neither BA44 nor BA45 develop volumetric asymmetry [19].

Here, we examined the evolution of Broca’s area by comparing cytoarchitectural segmentations of BA44 and BA45 in humans and chimpanzees, derived from published histological data. Leveraging advanced cortical registration methods [22,23], we aligned the brains of chimpanzee and human, enabling us to perform a direct comparison of the segmentations across species. Our analysis confirms that Broca’s area expanded in humans, with left BA44 being the subregion that enlarged the most. Furthermore, we show that when mapped into the human brain, the chimpanzee left BA44 only covers the posterior region related to action, while having virtually no overlap with the anterior syntax regions. Our results provide strong evidence that BA44 evolved from a pure action region, as found in our closet living ape relatives, to a bipartite system recruiting action posteriorly, and syntax anteriorly. In this way, we provide a complete picture of Broca’s area evolution and offer a solution to the longstanding debate of BA44’s role in language and action.

Results

Symmetry of Broca’s Area Homolog in Chimpanzee and a Surface Probabilistic Atlas

Through a semi-supervised pipeline (summarized in Fig. 1a) we precisely reconstructed the cortical surface of nine chimpanzee brains from their structural MRI data. Having their surface representation, we projected both BA44 and BA45 volumetric histological segmentations [19] to each individual surface. We examined all individuals for evidence of surface area asymmetry of both histologically defined regions (BA44 and BA45) using a Wilcoxon signed-ranks test. Although there was considerable asymmetry in some individuals (see Fig. 1b) both BA44 and BA45 showed no asymmetry at the population level (BA44 T=16, p=0.49; BA45 T=10, p=0.16.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

(A) Reconstruction pipeline for the cytoarchitectonic surface maps. First, the raw MRI data were cleaned using noise reduction and contrast inversion. Next, the individual surfaces were reconstructed in FreeSurfer. The individual maps of BA44 and BA45 are displayed in black and yellow, respectively. Finally, the individual surfaces and cytoarchitectural maps were registered to the JUNA template surface (B) Probabilistic atlas of regions BA44 and BA45 in the chimpanzee brain, derived from the individual maps, alongside the lateralization index for each individual brain.

To enable comparison across subjects, we co-registered the individual brain surfaces to the surface reconstruction of the JUNA [24] chimpanzee template (see Fig. 1a). On the JUNA surface we averaged all the individual segmentations, deriving a high-quality probabilistic atlas of BA44 and BA45 homologs in the chimpanzee brain (Fig 1b). The resulting atlas is open access and available for direct download (see Data and Code Availability Statement).

Comparison Between Human Broca’s Area and its Chimpanzee Homolog

Leveraging advanced surface registration [22,23], we co-registered the JUNA surface to the surface reconstruction of the MNI-2009c human template (Fig. 2a) [25]. This enabled us to compare the human BA44 and BA45 histological atlases derived by Amunts et al. [15], with our probabilistic atlas of the chimpanzee homolog (Fig. 2b).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

(A) Two-step surface registration, in the first step we align gross anatomical landmarks. This first alignment is then used to start a more granular one, based on sulcal depth. (B) Side-by-side comparison of our chimpanzee probabilistic atlas with the human population overlap of Amunts et al. [15] in the human brain template. Left BA44 is the area that grew the most and shows a large anterior expansion which is not present in Right BA44.

After projecting the chimpanzee segmentations to the human brain, we computed their volumes using the MNI template’s cortical thickness. We found the chimpanzee BA44 to have an average size of 2714 mm3 (SD: 1059) in the left hemisphere, and 2179 mm3 (SD: 1184) in the right hemisphere. In contrast, Amunts et al. [15] reported the human BA44 to have an average size of 3839 mm3 (SD: 2277) in the left hemisphere, and 2527 mm3 (SD: 1597) in the right hemisphere. This means that, when scaled and projected to a same surface template, the human BA44 is 1.42 times larger in the left hemisphere than in the chimpanzee, and 1.16 times larger in the right hemisphere. Moreover, Figure 2b shows that such enlargement is likely the result of a large anterior expansion, not present in the right BA44.

For the chimpanzee BA45, the average size after projecting to the human brain (Fig. 2b) was 3168 mm3 (SD: 1212) and 2347 mm3 (SD: 1435) for the left and right hemispheres, respectively. For the same region in humans, Amunts et al. [15] reported an average size of 3242 mm3 (SD: 1149) and 3173 mm3 (SD: 1637) for the left and right hemispheres, respectively [15]. In comparison, the human BA45 was only 1.02 times larger in the left hemisphere than the chimpanzee’s homolog area, while being 1.35 times larger in the right hemisphere.

Comparing the Projection of Chimpanzee BA44 with Human Functional Maps

To better understand the behavioral role of the observed expansion, we computed the overlap of the projected chimpanzee BA44 with sub-divisions of human BA44 related to action and syntax [12,2628]. To compare only with the core chimpanzee BA44, we thresholded the projected atlas at the 0.5 level. We found that the chimpanzee BA44 overlapped most with the regions involved in action [12,26] (Fig. 3 left, Table 1, and Sup. Fig. 1). The highest overlap was found with the area Clos 4, associated with action imagination [26], of which 34% was contained by the chimpanzee’s BA44. Following this were the regions Clos 1 (26% contained, associated with phonology and overt speech tasks [26]), Clos 5 (20%, associated with phonology and semantics [26]), Papitto’s region (18%, associated with action execution/imitation [12]), and Clos 2 (7%, associated with semantics, orthography, and covert speech [26]). In contrast, the region Clos 3, associated with basic syntactic operations [2628], had only a 3% overlap with the chimpanzee BA44. Similar results were obtained when comparing across different levels of thresholding (see Sup. Fig. 1).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Percentage of overlap between the chimpanzee BA44 and functional subdivisions of the human BA44 [12,2628]. Action-related regions present the highest overlap with action-related areas and virtually no overlap with the syntax area. The chimpanzee BA44 atlas was thresholded at 0.5 to maintain only its core area. The functions being reported are those with the highest P (Activation | Domain) as reported by Clos et al. [26] and Papitto et al. [12], excepting Clos 1, which was originally reported to be a syntax area, but further studies did not find to be involved in basic syntactic operations [26,27].

Table 1.

Anatomical overlap between functional areas [12,26] with the projected BA44 chimpanzee thresholded at 0.5.

Region Area Contained by BA44 Associated Functions

Clos 4 34% Action Imagination
Clos 1 26% Phonology, Syntax*
Clos 5 20% Phonology, Semantics
Papitto 18% Action Execution / Imitation
Clos 2 7% Orthography, Working Memory
Clos 3 3% Syntax, Phonology

The functional role of each area, as reported by their authors, is stated in the third column. For Clos regions, we report the functions with the highest P (Activation | Domain).

*:

It is important to notice that, even though Clos was originally reported to be a syntax area, further studies did not find it to be involved in basic syntactic operations [27,28].

Discussion

In this study, we leveraged comparable cytoarchitectonic segmentations of BA44 and 45 from chimpanzees and humans to study evolutionary changes in their size and distribution on the cortical surface. Particularly, we used advanced algorithms [22,23] to align human and chimpanzee brains, enabling a direct comparison of their (a)symmetry, size, alignment, and functional relevance.

BA44 Became Increasingly Left Lateralized in Evolution

We tested for asymmetry in surface area for both BA44 and BA45 in chimpanzee brains. In consistency with Schenker et al. [19] volumetric analysis, we found both regions to show no statistical difference in size, thus being symmetric at the population level. This result is in clear contrast with the strongly left lateralized BA44 in humans [15]. Since both chimpanzee and human segmentation were obtained through similar histological procedures, the results support the conclusion that the asymmetry of BA44 developed after humans diverged from our last common ancestor with chimpanzees. Sample sizes are relatively small for histological analyses in both humans and chimpanzees and this conclusion should be tested further with a larger number of subjects.

BA44 Expanded the Most in Humans Relative to Chimpanzees, Extending Anteriorly

We projected the BA44 and BA45 of each chimpanzee to the surface of a human cortical template and computed an average volume using the template’s cortical thickness. Assuming the MNI template is representative of the Amunts et al. [15] population, their reported volumes for humans are directly comparable with our scaled-up volumes for chimpanzee. Our comparison revealed that BA44 enlarged by a factor of 1.42 and 1.16 in the left and right hemispheres beyond the amount of overall cortical expansion, respectively, in this cross-species comparison. Meanwhile, BA45 enlarged only in the right hemisphere, by a factor of 1.35. Our results show that Broca’s area enlargement is remarkable in context of the evolution of human prefrontal cortex size [17,19], while showing a clear anterior expansion of left BA44, the area that enlarged the most.

The Chimpanzee BA44 Overlaps with Human Areas Related to Action and not Syntax

We directly compared the chimpanzee BA44 atlas, projected to the human cortical surface, with subdivisions of Broca’s area derived from human functional imaging studies. We found that the core chimpanzee BA44 overlapped solely with areas related to action, with the greatest overlap found (in descending order) with regions related to action imagination, phonology, and action execution/imitation [26] [12]. Indeed, we found almost no overlap between the core BA44 chimpanzee homolog and the Broca’s subdivision involved in basic syntax operations (Clos et al. [26], Zaccarella et al. [27,28]). These results were consistent across multiple levels of thresholding for the chimpanzee BA44 probabilistic map.

BA44 Evolved into a Bipartite Region to Support both Action and Syntax in Humans

Recent functional imaging studies found that both language and action recruit non-overlapping subdivisions of Broca’s area in the human brain, with language being processed more anteriorly than action [12,13,2628]. Moreover, it has been found that particularly left BA44 segregates action and language processing in two distinct sub-regions, with language recruiting its anterior part and action the posterior one [12,2628].

In this study, we have found strong evidence that the left human BA44 evolved to accommodate syntax through an anterior expansion of action-related regions in the inferior frontal cortex. Particularly, we have shown that this region enlarged more than the rest of Broca’s area subdivisions, and that, when mapped into the human brain, its chimpanzee homolog only corresponded to cortical regions known to support action. Hence, our results support the theory of a bipartite left BA44, with an anterior segment serving syntax and a distinct posterior segment serving action. Further, we have shown that this bipartite nature is likely the result of an evolutionary process. A process in which the pure action precursor of left BA44 expanded anteriorly to give rise to the core component of our human language – syntax.

Materials and Methods

Cytoarchitecture Segmentation of Broca’s Area in Human Brains

We downloaded the publicly available data from Amunts et al. [15], in which the left and right BA44 and BA45 were manually segmented on 10 subjects following histological procedures. While the individual maps are not available, the Julich institute has released the probabilistic cytoarchitectural map of both areas derived from the Amunts’ dataset.

Cytoarchitecture Segmentation of Broca’s Area Homolog in Chimpanzee Brains

Our chimpanzee cytoarchitectural data comes from a previous study, in which both BA44 and BA45 were bilaterally delineated and guided by the same cytoarchitectonic criteria defined for humans by Amunts et al. [19]. Whole-brain MRI data were acquired ex-vivo for all chimpanzees (Supplementary Information). From the population of 12 chimpanzees, we discarded 3 based on inadequate corresponding MRI data quality, retaining 9 subjects (Pan troglodytes, 5/4 males/females, age = 32.8 ± 11.8 years, age range = 12–44.5 years). The demographics of the included chimpanzees are summarized in Supplementary Table S1. For additional information on the data acquisition please refer to the original publication [19].

Broca’s Homolog in the Chimpanzee: Deriving a Probabilistic Atlas and Studying Population Symmetry

We derived a probabilistic atlas of Broca’s area homolog from the individual cytoarchitectonic segmentations of BA44 and BA45, and their associated ex-vivo MRI scans. We performed all the analysis on the reconstructed cortical surfaces, as surface analysis better captures and aligns brains based on their gyrification, thus being more robust than volumetric analysis [29].

The procedure can be summarized in five steps: (I) reconstruct 3D brain surfaces from the ex-vivo MRI scans using FreeSurfer, (II) project each chimpanzee’s BA44 and BA45 volumetric segmentation to their corresponding surfaces, (III) register all surfaces to a common template, namely the JUNA chimpanzee brain template [24], (IV) map the individual cytoarchitectural regions to the JUNA template, and (V) aggregate them to derive a high-quality probabilistic atlas. See Fig. 1a. for a graphical explanation, and the Supplementary Methods for a detailed explanation of each step. The processing scripts for the computation of the open access atlas are readily available for download (see Data and Code Availability Statement).

We further leveraged the individual reconstructions to study the surface-area asymmetry of Broca’s homolog in the chimpanzee brain. For this, we computed the areas of BA44 and BA45 on each individual chimpanzee, and tested their bilateral symmetry through a Wilcoxon sign-ranks test.

Mapping Chimpanzee Cytoarchitectural Maps to the Human Brain

To enable cross-species comparison, we aligned the cortical reconstruction of JUNA template [24] to that of the human MNI template (ICBM152 9c Asymmetric) [25]. Given the differences in brain shape and volume, we opted to use surface-based registration algorithms, which have been proven successful in aligning the brains of chimpanzees and humans [23].

Based on the work of Eichert et al. [23] we performed the surface-based registration in two stages. In the first stage, we performed a first alignment of the brain templates using gross anatomical regions. Specifically, we aligned the brains based on their inferior frontal gyrus, as defined by the Desikan atlas (Fig. 2a) [30]. Starting from that rough alignment, we then carried a more granular registration based on the sulcal patterns. For a detailed explanation of each stage please refer to the Supplementary Methods as well as the open access processing script (see Data and Code Availability Statement).

Expansion of BA44 & BA45 in Humans Relative to Chimpanzees

In their histological study, Amunts et al. [15] report the average gray-matter volume for human BA44 and BA45. Since our chimpanzee regions stem from similar histological procedures, we can study how much BA44 and BA45 expanded through evolution by mapping them to a common space, and comparing their size across species.

Having morphed chimpanzee BA44 and BA45 to the human template, we computed their individual volumes using the MNI template cortical thickness. In this way, we obtained volumes for chimpanzee Broca’s area subregions that are scaled up and projected onto the template human cortical surface. Assuming the MNI template is representative of the Amunts et al. [15] population, their reported volumes for humans are directly comparable with our scaled-up volumes for chimpanzee.

Functional Aspects of the BA44 Homolog in the Human Brain

We aimed to understand the relation between function and the location of the projected chimpanzee Broca’s area homolog - with a particular interest in language and action. For this, we projected the functional subdivisions of human BA44 defined by Papitto et al. [12] and Clos et al. [26] to the MNI cortical surfaces. There, we compared them to the core chimpanzee BA44, obtained by thresholding the atlas at the 0.5 level, i.e. the points in the surface where the majority of the chimpanzee population had their BA44 located. Particularly, for each functional region we computed their overlap with the chimpanzee BA44, defined as how much of the functional area was contained by the chimpanzee BA44.

Supplementary Material

Supplement 1

Acknowledgment

This study was funded by the Max Planck Society under the inter-institutional funds of the president of the Max Planck Society for the project “Hominoid Brain Connectomics” (M.IF.A.XXXX8103). This work was supported, in part, by NIH grants AG-067419, NS-42867, and NS-092988 to WDH and CCS. All aspects of this research conformed to existing US and NIH federal policies on the ethical use of chimpanzees in research. We thank Hannah Gerbeth for her support during the FreeSurfer white matter segmentation.

Data and Code Availability

The probabilistic chimpanzee atlases of BA44 and 45, alongside the respective processing scripts used to generate them are publicly available for download in our repository: https://github.com/gagdiez/chimpanzee-broca.

References

  • 1.Fadiga L, Craighero L, D’Ausilio A. Broca’s area in language, action, and music. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169: 448–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA. Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21: 188–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tomasello M. Origins of Human Communication. The MIT Press, Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pulvermüller F, Fadiga L. Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11: 351–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Moro A. On the similarity between syntax and actions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18: 109–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Berwick RC, Friederici AD, Chomsky N, Bolhuis JJ. Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17: 89–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Greenfield PM. Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behav Brain Sci. 1991;14: 531–551. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Maffongelli L, D’Ausilio A, Fadiga L, Daum MM. The ontogenesis of action syntax. Collabra Psychol. 2019;5: 21. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Binkofski F, Buccino G. Motor functions of the Broca’s region. Brain Lang. 2004;89: 362–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pulvermüller F. The syntax of action. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18: 219–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Pulvermüller F. Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Prog Neurobiol. 2018;160: 1–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Papitto G, Friederici AD, Zaccarella E. The topographical organization of motor processing: An ALE meta-analysis on six action domains and the relevance of Broca’s region. Neuroimage. 2020;206: 116321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Thibault S, Py R, Gervasi AM, Salemme R, Koun E, Lövden M, et al. Tool use and language share syntactic processes and neural patterns in the basal ganglia. Science. 2021;374: eabe0874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brodmann K. Beiträge zur histologischen Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde: VI. Die Cortexgliederung des Menschen. 1908. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Amunts K, Schleicher A, Bürgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HB, Zilles K. Broca’s region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. J Comp Neurol. 1999;412: 319–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lyn H. Apes and the Evolution of Language: Taking Stock of 40 Years of Research. In: Shackelford TK, Vonk J, editors. Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 356–378. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Passingham RE, Smaers JB. Is the prefrontal cortex especially enlarged in the human brain allometric relations and remapping factors. Brain Behav Evol. 2014;84: 156–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Donahue CJ, Glasser MF, Preuss TM, Rilling JK, Van Essen DC. Quantitative assessment of prefrontal cortex in humans relative to nonhuman primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: E5183–E5192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schenker NM, Hopkins WD, Spocter MA, Garrison AR, Stimpson CD, Erwin JM, et al. Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping, asymmetry, and comparison to humans. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20: 730–742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Amunts K, Schleicher A, Ditterich A, Zilles K. Broca’s region: cytoarchitectonic asymmetry and developmental changes. J Comp Neurol. 2003;465: 72–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Skeide MA, Friederici AD. The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17: 323–332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Robinson EC, Garcia K, Glasser MF, Chen Z, Coalson TS, Makropoulos A, et al. Multimodal surface matching with higher-order smoothness constraints. Neuroimage. 2018;167: 453–465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Eichert N, Verhagen L, Folloni D, Jbabdi S, Khrapitchev AA, Sibson NR, et al. What is special about the human arcuate fasciculus? Lateralization, projections, and expansion. Cortex. 2019;118: 107–115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vickery S, Hopkins WD, Sherwood CC, Schapiro SJ, Latzman RD, Caspers S, et al. Chimpanzee brain morphometry utilizing standardized MRI preprocessing and macroanatomical annotations. Elife. 2020;9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.60136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fonov VS, Evans AC, McKinstry RC, Almli CR, Collins DL. Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage. 2009;47: S102. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Clos M, Amunts K, Laird AR, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Tackling the multifunctional nature of Broca’s region meta-analytically: co-activation-based parcellation of area 44. Neuroimage. 2013;83: 174–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zaccarella E, Friederici AD. Merge in the Human Brain: A Sub-Region Based Functional Investigation in the Left Pars Opercularis. Front Psychol. 2015;6: 1818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Zaccarella E, Schell M, Friederici AD. Reviewing the functional basis of the syntactic Merge mechanism for language: A coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80: 646–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fischl B, Rajendran N, Busa E, Augustinack J, Hinds O, Yeo BTT, et al. Cortical folding patterns and predicting cytoarchitecture. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18: 1973–1980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31: 968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Robinson EC, Jbabdi S, Glasser MF, Andersson J, Burgess GC, Harms MP, et al. MSM: a new flexible framework for Multimodal Surface Matching. Neuroimage. 2014;100: 414–426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hopkins WD, Li X, Crow T, Roberts N. Vertex- and atlas-based comparisons in measures of cortical thickness, gyrification and white matter volume between humans and chimpanzees. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222: 229–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement 1

Data Availability Statement

The probabilistic chimpanzee atlases of BA44 and 45, alongside the respective processing scripts used to generate them are publicly available for download in our repository: https://github.com/gagdiez/chimpanzee-broca.


Articles from bioRxiv are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Preprints

RESOURCES