Abstract
When divergent populations interbreed, their alleles are brought together in hybrids. In the initial F1 cross, most divergent loci are heterozygous. Therefore, F1 fitness can be influenced by dominance effects that could not have been selected to function well together. We present a systematic study of these F1 dominance effects by introducing variable phenotypic dominance into Fisher's geometric model. We show that dominance often reduces hybrid fitness, which can generate optimal outbreeding followed by a steady decline in F1 fitness, as is often observed. We also show that “lucky” beneficial effects sometimes arise by chance, which might be important when hybrids can access novel environments. We then show that dominance can lead to violations of Haldane's Rule (reduced fitness of the heterogametic F1) but strengthens Darwin's Corollary (F1 fitness differences between cross directions). Taken together, results show that the effects of dominance on hybrid fitness can be surprisingly difficult to isolate, because they often resemble the effects of uniparental inheritance or expression. Nevertheless, we identify a pattern of environment‐dependent heterosis that only dominance can explain, and for which there is some suggestive evidence. Our results also show how existing data set upper bounds on the size of dominance effects. These bounds could explain why additive models often provide good predictions for later‐generation recombinant hybrids, even when dominance qualitatively changes outcomes for the F1.
Keywords: Darwin's corollary, Fisher's geometric model, heterosis, haldane's rule, optimal outbreeding, speciation
During hybridization, alleles from diverged genomes can be expressed together for the first time. The interactions between these alleles will help to determine the outcome of the hybridization. If hybrids are sufficiently fit, for example, then the hybridization might lead to ongoing gene flow (e.g. Lee et al., 2013; Rieseberg et al., 1999), or to a hybrid swarm, potentially adapted to a new niche (e.g. Taylor et al., 2006). If hybrids are unfit, by contrast, the lineages will remain reproductively isolated, and may be subject to reinforcement selection. All hybrids trace their ancestry to a first‐generation, or “F1” hybrid, and so the fitness of the F1 has a special importance. Ongoing gene flow, for example, is only possible if the F1 are both viable and fertile (Blair, 1955; Butlin, 1987; Dobzhansky, 1937; Fisher, 1930; Wallace, 1889). Moreover, the F1 are often easier to form than later generation crosses (Muller, 1940; Vetukhiv, 1954), and so have been more extensively studied.
Decades of data on F1 fitness show that all possible patterns are sometimes observed. In some cases, as illustrated in Figure 1a, the F1 are fitter than both of their parents (best‐parent heterosis); while in other cases, the F1 are less fit (Fig. 1b), or intermediate (Fig. 1c; Arnold & Hodges, 1995; Coyne & Orr, 2004; Wellhausen, 1952; Edmands, 2007; Hatfield & Schluter, 1999; Fraïsse et al., 2016, Table S1; Coughlan & Matute, 2020; Favre et al., 2017; Gramlich et al., 2022; Thompson & Schluter, 2022). Moreover, as illustrated in Figure 1d, the direction of the cross may be important, with strong fitness differences between the reciprocal F1 (i.e., female‐male vs. male‐female cross directions of the same parental lines). This pattern, known as “Darwin's Corollary” (Kölreuters, 1766; Darwin, 1859, Ch. 8; Turelli & Moyle, 2007) is very widely observed in plant (Tiffin et al., 2001), fungus (Dettman et al., 2003), and animal systems (Bolnick et al., 2008; Brandvain et al., 2014), including simultaneous hermaphrodites (Bouchemousse et al., 2016; Escobar et al., 2008; Fraïsse et al., 2016; Sato et al., 2014). Finally, the outcomes are often environment‐dependent, with hybrids fitter than parents in some conditions, and less fit in others (e.g. Wang et al., 1997).
Despite this heterogeneity, two factors have been shown to predict the fitness of F1 hybrids with reasonable consistency. The first factor is the level of genetic divergence between the parents. Patterns like Fig. 1a are most often observed with closely related lines, but with the level of heterosis increasing with divergence (Birchler, 2013). By contrast, Fig. 1b is most commonly observed between more distantly related lines, with F1 fitness often decreasing steadily with divergence, and so forming an “F1 clock” (Coughlan & Matute, 2020; Edmands, 2002). The combined result is a pattern of “optimal outbreeding,” where the F1 are fittest between parents of intermediate genetic distance (Bateson, 1978; Butlin, 1987; Dagilis et al., 2019; Waser, 1993; Wei & Zhang, 2018).
A second key factor that often predicts the fitness of F1 hybrids is their sex, such that one sex is as fit or fitter than the parents (Fig. 1a), while the other sex is inviable or infertile (Fig. 1b). When such a difference is observed, and sex chromosomes are heteromorphic, the heterogametic sex is almost always inferior to the homogametic sex. This pattern is well‐known as Haldane's Rule (Haldane, 1922; Coyne & Orr, 2004, Ch. 8; Schilthuizen et al., 2011).
What remains unclear is whether these disparate patterns in F1 fitness needs to be explained in a piecemeal way, or whether some common properties of gene interaction—shared by disparate animals, plants and fungi—might help to explain them all. Suggestive of the second possibility, a class of fitness landscapes based on Fisher's geometric model (Fisher, 1930; Orr, 1998) has been successful in generating many of these empirical patterns (Barton, 2001; Chevin et al., 2014; Fraïsse et al., 2016; Mani & Clarke, 1990; Simon et al., 2018; Schneemann et al., 2020). However, there are also reasons to be suspicious. In particular, while the model always allows for dominance in fitness, most studies have assumed phenotypic additivity—that is, they assume that the phenotypic effect of a homozygous allele is exactly double its heterozygous effect. By contrast, empirical studies indicate that variable levels of phenotypic dominance are common for loci affecting quantitative traits, including fitness components (Lynch & Walsh, 1998, p.485; Clo et al., 2021). Given its high heterozygosity, the F1 will be especially affected by this variable dominance. There are, moreover, reasons to expect dominance effects to evolve in a qualitatively different way from additive effects. Additive effects act together in homozygous genotypes, and so they will often be co‐adapted—that is, selected to function well together in the parental environments. By contrast, the dominance effects will act together only in early generation hybrids, and so lack any tendency to be co‐adapted.
Here, building on previous work (Schneemann et al., 2020), we systematically explore the effects of variable phenotypic dominance on hybrid fitness, with a particular focus on the F1. Our global aim, as summarized in Table 1, is to ask how various empirically observed patterns are affected by phenotypic dominance, and whether they provide evidence for, or against, its importance for hybrid fitness. We find that some of the well‐established patterns provide upper bounds on the typical size of dominance effects in nature, while not ruling out their presence altogether. Other patterns have multiple possible explanations, making it difficult to establish their true cause(s). Nevertheless, we identify a single pattern that only dominance can explain.
Table 1.
Strictly biparental inheritance/expression | Some uniparental inheritance/expression | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Without | With | Without | With | |||||||
phenotypic | phenotypic | phenotypic | phenotypic | |||||||
Observation | Section | dominance | dominance | dominance | dominance | |||||
1. | Reciprocal F1 both fitter than both parents | 1, 2.2, 3, Fig 1a |
|
|
|
|
||||
2. | Reciprocal F1 both less fit than both parents | 2, 4.1, Fig 1b | ✗ |
|
|
|
||||
3. | Optimal outbreeding and F1 clock | 2.1, 4.1 | ✗ |
|
|
|
||||
4. | Reciprocal F1 of locally adapted parents both fitter in one parental environment | 3.2 |
|
|
|
|
||||
5. | Homogametic F1 fitter than heterogametic F1 (Haldane's rule) | 4.2 | – | – |
|
|
||||
6. | Heterogametic F1 fitter than homogametic F1 (Anti‐Haldane's rule) | 4.2 | – | – |
|
|
||||
7. | Fitness asymmetry between reciprocal F1 (Darwin's corollary) | 4.3, Fig 1d | ✗ | ✗ |
|
|
||||
8. | Reciprocal F1 both fitter than both parents in one environment, and less fit in another | 3.1, Fig 1a and Fig 1b | ✗ |
|
✗ |
|
Note: 1. Only if parents are phenotypically distinct; 2. Only if parental environments have different selection regimes; 3. Only if , where x is the proportion of divergent sites that are uniparentally inherited; 4. Only with X silencing in females (or Z silencing in males); 5. Only if (see main text); 6. Increasingly unlikely at high divergence, d.
Model
Fisher's (1930) geometric model assigns fitnesses to genotypes using a simple model of n quantitative traits under optimizing selection. The parameter n, which is sometimes called “organismal complexity,” is also the dimensionality of the fitness landscape. An individual's phenotype is represented by a point in this landscape, with its trait values collected in the n‐dimensional vector, . The relative fitness of the individual is denoted as w, and depends on the Euclidean distance of the phenotype from some optimum , whose position is determined by the current environmental conditions. Here, we will use a simple quadratic model, where log fitness declines with the squared distance to the optimum:
(1) |
(2) |
Substitutions are modeled as n‐dimensional vectors of change to the phenotype. Thus, any two phenotypes can be connected by a chain of vectors that represent the divergent alleles accrued since their most recent common ancestor. This is illustrated in Figure 2a, where we show a chain of genomic differences connecting two homozygous parental lines, labelled P1 and P2. In the illustration, each population fixed equal numbers of substitutions, but this is not an assumption of the analyses. What is important, however, is that all substitution effects are defined relative to the P1 genotype. This means that none of our results depend on knowing whether the P1 alleles are ancestral or derived.
Any given hybrid between P1 and P2 will contain some combination of the d vectors. Under phenotypic additivity, the effects of these vectors sum together both between and within loci. This means that an allele carried as a heterozygote will have the same orientation, but half the length, of the allele carried as a homozygote. This is shown by the yellow arrow in Figure 2a inset. To model phenotypic dominance, we relax the assumption of additivity within loci, using dominance coefficients . This is shown by the dark red arrow in Figure 2a inset. Non‐zero values of the can alter both the length of the vector, and its orientation if different traits show different levels of dominance.
We note that the very simple model described above can be derived, either exactly or approximately, from a large family of more complex fitness functions (Martin, 2014; Martin & Lenormand, 2006). In that case, few if any of the n traits correspond to real quantitative traits, so that n becomes a phenomenological parameter of the fitness landscape (Martin & Lenormand, 2006; Orr, 2000; Welch & Waxman, 2003).
Results
To explore the various effects of dominance on the fitness of F1 hybrids, we consider several different scenarios and patterns. As such, throughout the results section, we present the major results without derivation, and relegate the full derivations to Appendix 1. Simulations are used solely to illustrate the analytical results.
THE F1 UNDER ADDITIVITY AND BIPARENTAL INHERITANCE
Let us start with a simple model of strictly biparental inheritance and expression. Although such a model cannot generate Haldane's Rule or Darwin's Corollary (rows 5–7 Table 1), it allows us to isolate the effects of dominance from other factors.
With strictly biparental inheritance, F1 hybrids contain all d of the divergent alleles as heterozygotes, and so under phenotypic additivity the F1 phenotype matches the midparental phenotype, obtained by averaging the parental values for all n traits. This midparent is illustrated by the yellow point labelled MP in Figure 2b. The fitness of the midparental phenotype, w MP, therefore depends solely on the fitnesses of the parental lines, w P1 and w P2, and their relative positions in the n‐dimensional phenotypic space. This is characterized by θ, the angle in radians between the parental phenotypes (see Fig. 2b). Indeed, from the definition of cosine similarity, we find:
(3) |
where and are respectively the arithmetic and geometric mean log fitness of the parents. Since varies between −1 and 1, it follows that . Therefore, with phenotypic additivity, the F1 will always be at least as fit as the average of the fitnesses of the two parental lines (Barton, 2001; Fraïsse et al., 2016). Moreover, as long as the parental lines are not phenotypically identical, F1 fitness may exceed that of both parents, generating the pattern shown in Fig. 1a (row 1 Table 1).
To understand the consequences of this result, let us consider a concrete situation, in which the two parental lines remain well adapted to a fixed phenotypic optimum (effective stabilizing selection) but diverge from their common ancestor largely via “system drift” (Schiffman & Ralph, 2021; Schneemann et al., 2020). In the initial stages of divergence the parental lines fix different alleles, and so they wander slightly in phenotypic space while remaining near the optimum. This wandering tends to increase θ, bringing the midparent closer to the optimum and thereby increasing the potential for heterosis (Equation 3). After this initial divergence, heterosis remains at roughly constant levels because selection prevents the parental lines from wandering any further and hence the phenotypic distance between them has no further tendency to increase. This is confirmed by individual‐based simulations of Fisher's geometric model, which are described in detail in Appendix 2 and shown by the yellow lines in Figure 2c. With a fixed optimum and additive phenotypes, F1 heterosis shows a brief initial increase before settling at a constant positive value.
This prediction of heterosis at all levels of genetic divergence implies that, with additivity and strictly biparental inheritance, the pattern shown in Figure 1b could never arise, and so neither could the patterns of optimal outbreeding or the F1 clock (see rows 2–3 Table 1). Furthermore, even if we considered a different type of fitness landscape in which fitness declines more rapidly with distance to the optimum, this could lead to loss of midparental heterosis, but the F1 could never have lower fitness than both of its parents.
THE DELETERIOUS EFFECTS OF PHENOTYPIC DOMINANCE
The principal effect of variable phenotypic dominance is to push the F1 phenotype away from the midparent, usually reducing its fitness (Schneemann et al., 2020). To see this, let us first define the total dominance deviation on trait i, including contributions from all d substitutions.
(4) |
Here, is the additive effect of substitution j on trait i, and is its dominance coefficient. These quantities are illustrated in the inset panels in Figure 2a and 2b. With this definition, the fitness of the F1 can be written as:
(5) |
Here, without loss of generality, we have rotated the trait axes such that the midparent falls short of the optimum only for trait 1 (as newly defined), but is optimal for the remaining traits. This is shown in Figure 2b.
An important point about Equation 5 is that the joint effects of dominance across divergent sites, the , are first expressed in hybrids. This is because the heterozygous effect of an allele at one locus is only expressed during its segregation, and becomes hidden from selection once it fixes. So, whereas the additive effects are expressed jointly in the P2 genotype, the dominance effects are unlikely to be under selection together during divergence, especially allopatric divergence. As such, the joint effects of dominance across divergent sites, captured by the will show no tendency to be co‐adapted or remain close to any optimum, even when selection acts on the individually. This means that similar scenarios of parental divergence, with similar trajectories for the parental phenotypes, can nonetheless yield very different and thus very different F1 phenotypes. This is illustrated by the cloud of dark red points in Figure 2b, which show possible F1 that might have resulted from the same divergence scenario between P1 and P2.
To predict F1 fitness, we must average over these possible evolutionary histories. Let us first consider the scenario discussed above, where parental populations remain well adapted to the same constant environment. In this case, we can think of the as undergoing a random walk away from the midparent, so that the set of possible F1 form a growing cloud (Schneemann et al., 2020). This yields:
(6) |
(7) |
where is the variance, across divergence histories, of the the total dominance deviations (see Equations 4 and 27); while is the equivalent variance in dominance effects of single substitutions (i.e., the d increments of the random walk; Equations 4 and 28). As with the additive model, Equations 6‐7 predict a transient increase in F1 fitness in the early stages of divergence, as long as the second terms remain small. But as the number of substitutions (d) increases, the second terms start to grow, causing a steady fitness decline (ultimately giving rise to the pattern shown in Fig. 1b). To illustrate this, we repeated our simulations after allowing for variable phenotypic dominance among the new mutations (see Appendix 2 for full details). The results, shown as dark red curves in Figure 2c, show a clear pattern of transient heterosis—optimal outbreeding—followed by a linear decline in log fitness: the F1 clock (see row 3 Table 1). Note that heterosis is greater with phenotypic dominance (compare peaks of yellow and dark red curve in Fig. 2c) because more highly recessive deleterious mutations now segregate (i.e. mutations that are also phenotypically recessive).
We can also think of this result as exemplifying two regimes. In the early stages of divergence, the phenotypic distance between the parents increases faster than the cloud of possible F1. This means that most F1 trait values will be intermediate between the parental values. Later, the parents stop diverging phenotypically (because of effective stabilizing selection acting on their traits), but the cloud of possible F1 continues to expand. This leads to transgressive trait variation, with the F1 values lying outside of the range of the parental values. It is notable that both of these regimes have been observed in quantitative traits from F1 hybrids: transgressive variation sometimes decreases with d, and sometimes increases with d (Stelkens & Seehausen, 2009, ; see also Appendix 1 and Figure S2 for more details).
The tick rate of the F1 clock
Equation 7 gives the tick rate of the F1 clock as , which is also the rate of expansion of the cloud of possible F1 phenotypes. This quantity summarizes the distribution of factors fixed, which, as previous work has shown, will depend on many different aspects of the system's biology (e.g. Charlesworth, 1992; Griswold, 2006; Matuszewski et al., 2014; Orr, 1998; Schneemann et al., 2020; Yamaguchi & Otto, 2020; Yeaman & Whitlock, 2011).
One important determinant of the tick rate is likely to be the distribution of dominance coefficients of new mutations. In Figure 2c, we used an empirically motivated model, in which larger‐effect mutations tended to have extreme levels of dominance (see Appendix 2; Orr, 1991; Manna et al., 2011), however, the effects of mutational dominance are easier to see with a simpler model, where they can vary independently of the additive effects. As such, we repeated our simulations using the model of Manna et al. (2011), where the dominance coefficient of each new mutation on each trait was drawn from a shifted beta distribution, with a vanishing mean (such that mutations were semi‐dominant on average) and variance . We simulated under seven different variances, including the extremes of (phenotypic additivity) and , such that new mutations were either fully dominant or fully recessive with a 50:50 probability. The seven distributions we used are illustrated in Figure 3a.
Figure 3b shows clouds of F1 phenotypes for sets of 100 replicate simulations, each with for visualization. In all cases, the cloud of possible F1 is centred on the optimum, but grows in width with . Figure 3c shows the consequence of this F1 cloud expansion for the F1 clock. Here, to match Fig. 2c, we simulated with traits, but with a larger population size, so that parental populations remained well adapted and thus had little potential for F1 heterosis (see Figure S3 for other parameter regimes). While this may not hold under all possible regimes, all our simulation results show that the F1 tick rate increases steadily with the variance in mutational dominance, . This is because the heterozygous effects of the substitutions become more variable with . To see this, we can isolate the contribution of dominance to that will be captured by the key quantity , defined as
(8) |
where is the variance of the fixed additive effects, averaged over traits (Equation 29), and captures the variability in dominance coefficients of fixed effects, as well as any potential correlations between fixed additive effects and their associated dominance coefficient (see section 1.1.1 of Appendix 1). Figure 3d (circles) shows that for these simulations the quantity is very close in value to the mutational variance in dominance coefficients, i.e. that (see also Schneemann et al. 2020).
Directional selection and directional dominance
Results above assume that dominance deviations have no tendency to point in any phenotypic direction, such that the cloud of possible F1s is centered on the midparent, and all (see Fig. 2b). However, it is well known that directional selection can lead to directional dominance, as with Haldane's Sieve (Billiard et al., 2021; Crnokrak & Roff, 1995; Frankham, 1990; Haldane, 1924, 1927), especially if adaptation is from new mutations (Orr & Betancourt, 2001). This is because populations evolving in a particular direction preferentially fix alleles that are dominant in that direction.
To understand the effects of directional dominance for F1 fitness, let us consider the most extreme case, where both populations adapt independently to identical environmental change, so that all of the dominance deviations point in the same direction. In this case, the cloud of possible F1 becomes shifted in the direction of the past evolutionary change. This is illustrated in Figure 3e, where a bout of directional selection on trait 1 leads to . To understand why this happens, let us consider the evolution of trait 1 during the bout of adaptation. If we denote the F1 trait as z F1, 1, and the midparental value as z MP, 1, then Equations 2 and 5 imply that:
(9) |
(10) |
The important point about Equation 9 is that it contains both positive and negative terms. This is because, during the adaptation phase, the directional dominance can increase F1 fitness, by taking its trait value closer to the still‐distant optimum. But as shown by Equation 10, once the new optimum is approached, the directional dominance leads to a permanent and deleterious “overshoot” of this optimum (see also Figure S4, and Ono et al. (2017) for a related phenomenon shown in yeast).
If the adaptation to the new optimum required d dir substitutions, the expected log fitness of the F1 after this period (i.e., once system drift at the new optimum has begun) is:
(11) |
where is the variance in dominance coefficients of the adaptive substitutions, and is the log fitness of the maladapted ancestor. Comparing eq. 11 to earlier results (Equations 6 and 7), we see that the overshoot adds a new term. The history of directional selection leads to a non‐zero intercept for the F1 clock.
Complete simulations of this scenario are reported in Figure 3f. Comparison of Figure 3f and 3c shows clearly the transient increase in F1 fitness (Equation 9), and the deleterious overshoot once the new optimum is approached (Equation 10). After the initial period, the F1 clock continues to tick at the same rate as before (see triangles in Figure 3d), but with a permanent intercept (Equation 11). Fitting linear regressions to the F1 clocks in Figure 3f after excluding the first 50 substitutions allowed us to calculate this intercept. Figure 3g (triangles) confirms that the intercept is also affected by the input of new mutations, such that .
Results above concern directional selection in a common direction, but they generalize readily. For example, if each parental population underwent directional adaptive change on a different phenotypic trait, the resulting directional dominance would lead to an F1 with a mixture of the derived traits of the two parental lines. This is described as “trait mismatch” by Thompson et al. (2021). In all cases, with variable phenotypic dominance, a history of past directional selection can lead to an additional loss of fitness for the F1.
THE LUCKY BENEFICIAL EFFECTS OF DOMINANCE
So far, we have considered the typical effects of dominance in the F1, by averaging over the possible evolutionary histories. These effects are generally deleterious, and any heterosis tends to be transient. Nevertheless, even when effects are deleterious on average, by chance alone, some realisations of the evolutionary process will take the F1 closer to the optimum. In Fisher's geometric model, these “lucky” outcomes are far more likely when the number of phenotypic traits, n, is small. This is because random changes are more likely to go in the right direction when the dimensionality of the landscape is low (Fisher, 1930; Orr, 2000).
To see this, let us first consider again the scenario illustrated in Figures 2c and 3b‐c, where parental populations remain well adapted to a single fixed optimum. We assume that the have vanishing means (i.e., no directional dominance), and are approximately normally distributed. The normality is justified by the central‐limit‐like behaviour arising from the sum in eq. 4. With these assumptions, the coefficient of variation in log F1 fitness is approximately
(12) |
Equation 12 implies that, when n is large, the F1 clock will tick in a relatively deterministic way. When n is small, by contrast, increasing the divergence between the parental lines may lead to the chance re‐appearance of heterosis, even after many generations of low fitness F1. This is confirmed by simulations reported in Figure S5a‐b.
Lucky beneficial effects in a novel environment
The lucky beneficial effects of dominance may be particularly consequential when hybrids are formed in novel environments, to which one or both parental lines are severely maladapted. In the additive model, F1 heterosis can only appear when the parents are maladapted in different ways (Schneemann et al., 2020; Simon et al., 2018; Yamaguchi & Otto, 2020). For example, hybrids between parents adapted to low and high altitudes might thrive at intermediate altitudes (Wang et al., 1997). With phenotypic dominance, however, hybrid advantage might appear under a broader range of conditions (row 1 Table 1). Consider, for example, the situation shown in Figure 4a, where two genomically divergent, but phenotypically similar parental lines hybridize in a novel habitat. In this case, the midparent matches the parental phenotypes (), and so the effects of dominance will be deleterious on average (Equation 6). Nevertheless, for some divergence histories, the F1 will be fitter by chance. The probability of this lucky heterosis can be derived by noting that Equation 5 has a non‐central chi‐squared distribution if the are normal. The probability therefore depends on n and the ratio , which compares the sizes of the dominance deviations to the maladaptation of the parents. Figure 4b plots this probability for a range of values, and we also have the approximation:
(13) |
(see Appendix 1). As shown in Figure 4b, the maximum probability of heterosis is , and it applies when dominance effects are small compared to the maladaptation of the parents such that (toward the left of Fig. 4b). This implies that any fitness gain due to heterosis would also be very small, and simply reproduces Fisher's result that very small changes have a 50% chance of being beneficial (Fisher, 1930). Conversely when the dominance effects are very large, such that (toward the right of Fig. 4b), the F1 are almost certain to overshoot the new optimum. Therefore, the area of interest concerns (toward the centre of Fig. 4b), and in this regime, the probability of heterosis declines rapidly with n (see also Fig. S6). Only when n is small is there a non‐negligible chance that dominance effects are both substantial and beneficial.
Hybrids between locally adapted parents
Now let us consider the situation shown in Figure 4c. This panel represents the outcome of divergent selection (Schluter, 2000), where each parental line is well adapted to a different habitat, characterized by different phenotypic optima. In this case, the midparent lies midway between the optima, such that, in either habitat , with denoting the fitness of the maladapted parental line. In this scenario, results in Figure 4b now describe the probability of the F1 being fitter than the midparent in one of the two parental habitats. In most cases, the F1 will be fitter in one of the habitats than the other, and in this “preferred habitat”, its expected log fitness is:
(14) |
Equation 14 neatly separates the negative and positive effects of dominance in its second and third terms, and shows that the deleterious effects—but not the beneficial effects—grow with n. Equation 14 also shows that the fitness benefits of dominance will be greatest at intermediate values of , and therefore, at intermediate levels of divergence (Equations 6‐7). This means that, in contrast to the scenario shown in Figure 3e–f, F1 fitness in one of the two habitats, might continue to increase even after two parental populations have adapted to their new optima. At this intermediate level of divergence, when the fitness benefit is greatest, we find:
(15) |
so, at suitable levels of divergence, phenotypic dominance can yield a substantial fitness increase for the F1 over the midparent. However, these lucky effects of dominance occur only in one of the two habitats and only when n is small. All of these results are confirmed in Figure 4d, which shows simulation results with (see also Figure S7). Results confirm that on average (solid lines), F1 fitness with dominance initially increases with d, before reaching a maximum approximated by Equation 15, and then decreasing. However, Equation 12 still applies, and so for any single realization of the divergence history (faint lines in Figure 4d) the fitness of F1 with phenotypic dominance is highly erratic.
Taken together, these results imply that phenotypic dominance can cause an asymmetry in F1 fitness between the two parental habitats (see row 4 Table 1, and top row Fig. S8). This is a form of “dominance drive” (Barton, 1992; Mallet & Barton, 1989), and could lead to asymmetrical gene flow between locally adapted populations after secondary contact. The results also imply that the gene flow, which by definition reduces d, may have an unpredictable effect on F1 fitness. In some cases, homogenisation of the parental genomes will increase the fitness of their F1, but in other cases, it will lead to a switch in the direction of the gene flow (as the F1 becomes adapted to the other parental habitat), or even to a substantially lower F1 fitness. In the latter case, the outcome would resemble reinforcement selection, albeit via a completely different route.
THE INTERACTION OF PHENOTYPIC DOMINANCE WITH UNIPARENTAL INHERITANCE
Let us now expand the model to include the uniparental inheritance or expression of some of the divergent alleles, for example on sex chromosomes or mitochondrial genomes (Fraïsse et al., 2016; Simon et al., 2018). Like phenotypic dominance, uniparental inheritance leads to unpredictable deviations in hybrid phenotype. This is because the complete set of additive effects may be co‐adapted in the parental lines, for example, due to compensatory changes. But when hybrids inherit some parts of their genome from only one parent, they may lack some co‐adapted alleles from the other parent. The one key difference from dominance is that, under uniparental inheritance, the direction of the deviation from the midparent is opposite for the two cross directions. This is illustrated in Figure 5a.
In this figure, and throughout this section, we assume a concrete example of uniparental effects, although results generalize easily to other cases. Our example involves sex chromosomes, where the heterogametic sex are effectively XO (i.e. where males carry only the maternal X, and the Y is either missing or highly degenerate). We further assume a common form of dosage compensation where X‐linked alleles have identical effects in homozygous and hemizygous state, as found in e.g. Diptera and Hemiptera (Deng et al., 2011; Gu & Walters, 2017; Mank et al., 2011). This last assumption ensures that non‐hybrid offspring of both sexes are phenotypically identical (i.e., that male and female offspring from a P1 × P1 cross are equally fit). It also means the homogametic sex still experience dominance effects at X‐linked loci, such that we can study the interaction between uniparental and dominance effects. The consequences of some alternative forms of dosage compensation are addressed in Fraïsse et al. (2016), and will be discussed briefly below.
In the case described, the absence of the paternal X causes the heterogametic F1 hybrid to deviate from the midparent, even under phenotypic additivity. These deviations are equal and opposite for the two cross directions (see the blue and green arrows in Fig. 5a). If there is phenotypic dominance for alleles on the autosomes, then this leads to further deviations that apply identically to both cross directions (see the dark red arrows in Fig. 5a). In the sections below, we show how uniparental effects and phenotypic dominance combine to determine F1 fitness.
The F1 clock under dominance and uniparental inheritance
If we consider any single F1, then uniparental effects and phenotypic dominance have essentially the same consequences. As such, they represent alternative, but non‐exclusive explanations of the F1 clock and optimal outbreeding (row 3 Table 1). To see this, let us consider the expected log fitness of an XO F1 hybrid. If we assume that a fraction, x, of the d divergent alleles are X‐linked, then from eq. 5 and published results (Fraïsse et al., 2016; Simon et al., 2018), we find:
(16) |
Equation 16 still predicts optimal outbreeding, but there are now two terms that reduce F1 fitness. The second term captures the dominance deviations from the autosomes, and the third term captures the lack of co‐adaptation between paternal autosomes and the absent paternal X.
Figure 5b illustrates these results. We returned to our simulated data, and formed heterogametic F1 in the way described. Results show that optimal outbreeding and the F1 clock now appear under the additive model too (yellow curves; Fraïsse et al., 2016; see also Fig. S5c‐d). Phenotypic dominance simply accelerates the fitness decline (dark red curves).
Haldane's rule
If we consider fitness differences between heterogametic and homogametic hybrids, then the uniparental effects and phenotypic dominance tend to push in different directions. If the parents are well adapted, then the additive model predicts lower fitness for the heterogametic sex, in accord with Haldane's Rule (row 5 Table 1; Haldane, 1922; Barton, 2001; Fraïsse et al., 2016; Simon et al., 2018). This is due to the loss of co‐adaptation, described above (see the third term of eq. 16). However, phenotypic dominance has the opposite effect, because it tends to makes heterozygosity deleterious, and heterogametic F1, being hemizygous for the X, lack the deleterious heterozygosity on the X (to see this, compare the second terms of eqs. 7 and 16).
To see how these two effects balance, let us consider the difference in log fitness between heterogametic and homogametic F1. Using Equations (7), (8), and 16, we find:
(17) |
Haldane's Rule holds on average if eq. 17 is negative. If parents are well adapted, or divergence is substantial, then the second term in Equation 17 dominates and we expect Haldane's Rule on the condition that
(18) |
Equation 18 confirms that Haldane's Rule will always hold under additivity (when ), but that variable phenotypic dominance can lead to violations (i.e., ‘anti‐Haldane's Rule'), especially when the dominance coefficients are highly variable, or if the X is very large (rows 5–6 Table 1). This is confirmed by the simulations shown in Figure 6a–b. These plots use the simulation runs from Fig. 3b, for which the dominance coefficients match the mutational input (). The results show that Haldane's Rule is indeed violated when .
Darwin's corollary
Let us now consider the fitness differences between the F1 cross directions, that is a cross where P1 is dam (P1♀× P2♂) compared to a cross where P1 is sire (P2♀× P1♂). In stark contrast to Haldane's Rule, phenotypic dominance works together with uniparental effects to generate this pattern. To see this, we will use the following measure of log fitness asymmetry, where the absolute difference between the cross directions is normalized by the absolute mean:
(19) |
Here, ♀♂ and ♂♀ denote the two cross directions, and the statistic is bounded at .
Fraïsse et al. (2016) studied F1 asymmetry under the additive version of Fisher's model, and showed that asymmetry could appear only if the midparental phenotype was maladapted. The reason is clear from Figure 5a. When the midparent matches the optimum, the equal and opposite deviations (blue and green arrows), will lead to identical fitness loss.
Furthermore, even if the midparent is maladapted, its distance from the optimum must be large relative to the deviations for substantial asymmetry to appear. But in a stable environment, the parents (and therefore the midparent) remain close the optimum, while the deviations grow with d (e.g. eq. 7). As such, the F1 in both cross directions are expected to become less fit, and the difference between their fitnesses is expected to become smaller. This implies that fitness asymmetry between the cross directions will decline with d under the additive model. This is confirmed by the yellow curves in Figure 6c–d.
Adding phenotypic dominance to the model qualitatively changes this result. Because the dominance deviations also grow with d, levels of asymmetry can remain large, even at high divergence (row 7 Table 1). An illustrative case is shown in Figure 5a. Moreover, with dominance, we find that to a rough approximation:
(20) |
so the normalized asymmetry in log fitness is predicted to remain roughly constant at all levels of divergence, and to depend solely on the number of traits, n. This is confirmed by the simulations with dominance shown in Figure 6c–d. For all of the parameter values we simulated, as long as there was phenotypic dominance (), then asymmetry levels remained roughly constant, and were close to the prediction of eq. 20 (see also Figure S5e– f).
The arguments in this section show that under Fisher's geometric model, Darwin's Corollary can be explained as another “lucky” beneficial effect of dominance, where the good luck for one cross direction is balanced by bad luck for the other. The consequence is that – with phenotypic dominance, but not with additivity – Darwin's Corollary is predicted at high levels of genomic divergence (row 7 Table 1).
Discussion
This paper has investigated the effects – both negative and positive – of variable phenotypic dominance on F1 hybrid fitness. We have shown that these effects are often governed by a quantity , which captures the variability in the dominance coefficients of the fixed differences between hybridizing populations, but with the variability defined across realisations of the divergence history (see eq. 8, and section 1.1.1 in Appendix 1 for a formal definition). As a result, the importance of phenotypic dominance to F1 hybrid fitness will usually depend on the typical size of in nature.
Can we measure directly?
In principle, might be estimated using a quantitative genetics approach (Clo et al., 2021; Hill, 1982; Lynch, 1991), either applied to fitness (De Sanctis et al., 2022; Schneemann et al., 2020) or to the underlying traits, if their relationships to fitness are known (though see Martin, 2014). However, as describes the variance across replicates of the divergence process, its estimation would require very large amounts of data.
Another, indirect approach to estimating uses the properties of new mutations. We have shown that, in some cases, reflects the mutational input, such that (Fig. 3d). Manna et al. (2011) showed that the regression slope of homozygous and heterozygous selection coefficients carries information about (see their eq. 13). Their re‐analysis of several mutation accumulation experiments estimated this slope to be on average 0.27 (see also Charlesworth & Charlesworth, 1998; Lynch & Walsh, 1998; Szafraniec et al., 2003), which corresponds to . However, the same slope could also arise without phenotypic dominance () if the wildtypes were maladapted.
While therefore appears very difficult to estimate, existing data on F1 hybrid fitness might nonetheless provide bounds on its typical value.
What data might place a lower bound on ?
One of the goals of this work, as summarized in Table 1, was to identify patterns in the F1 fitness data that only dominance might generate. Observing these patterns would thereby demonstrate that must be greater than zero. However, such patterns were surprisingly hard to find. This is mainly because the effects of phenotypic dominance so often resemble the effects of uniparental inheritance or expression—with both acting to displace the F1 phenotype from the midparent in an unpredictable direction (Fig. 5b,c; Fraïsse et al., 2016). Moreover, the ubiquity of such uniparental effects is evident in the ubiquity of Darwin's Corollary—fitness differences between the cross directions (Fig. 1d; Kölreuters, 1766; Darwin, 1859, Ch. 8; Turelli & Moyle, 2007; Muller, 1942).
Nevertheless, as summarized in Table 1, there are several patterns in the data that do imply a role for phenotypic dominance. These patterns include (i) F1 heterosis for the reciprocal F1 of phenotypically identical parents (Fig. 4a,b); (ii) reciprocal F1 between locally adapted parents being much fitter in one of the two parental environments (Fig. 4c); (iii) violations of Haldane's Rule due to deleterious heterozygosity (Fig. 6a,b); and (iv) the appearance of Darwin's Corollary at very large genetic distances (Fig. 6c,d).
Unfortunately, in each case, there are complications that make it difficult to conclude that the observations really are due to phenotypic dominance. For example, regarding (i), it is very difficult to know that parental lines are truly identical for all of the relevant traits (Martin, 2014); regarding (ii) parental habitats may be under different forms or strengths of selection, such that phenotypically intermediate F1 are nevertheless much fitter in one environment (see Fig. S8); regarding (iii) violations of Haldane's Rule may appear for other reasons, notably silencing of sex chromosomes in the homogametic sex (Fraïsse et al., 2016), and this does seem plausible in the best‐studied examples (Teleogryllus crickets: Simon et al., 2018; Moran et al., 2017; but see also Rayner et al., 2021; marsupial mammals: Watson & Demuth, 2012; Drosophila: Sawamura, 1996; and Xenopus: Malone et al., 2007); finally, regarding (iv) levels of fitness asymmetry can vary erratically with genetic distance (see Fig. S5e,f), and moreover the relevant measure of asymmetry (Equation 19), can be difficult to obtain at low divergences—where parental fitness cannot serve as a proxy for the optimal fitness—and at very high divergences, where zeros cannot be log transformed.
As a result of these complications, our work has identified only one pattern in F1 fitness data from which the action of phenotypic dominance can be confidently inferred. This pattern—described in row 8 of Table 1—is a particular form of environment‐dependent heterosis, where both F1 cross directions outperform both parents in one environment (as in Fig. 1a), while both parents outperform both F1 in a second environment (as in Fig. 1b). (The second environment might involve the presence of novel stressors, to which the parental lines are not normally exposed). The F1 heterosis in the novel environment would appear either as a “lucky” effect (Figs. 4a,b; Fig. S7), or as a predictable consequence of Haldane's Sieve and past adaptation (Fig. 3e,f). How common, then, is this pattern? While there are many excellent examples of environment‐dependent heterosis, they rarely match the pattern described exactly—either because F1 fitness is intermediate in one of the environments (e.g., Crespel et al., 2012, 2012; Martins et al., 2019; Munaro et al., 2011; Vetukhiv & Beardmore, 1959; Walter et al., 2020; Wu & Campbell, 2006; Wang et al., 2022), or because F1 fitness was not available for the individual cross directions (e.g., Benyi & Gall, 1981; Dittrich‐Reed, 2013; Domgínguez & Albornoz, 1987; Harrison, 1962; Maynard Smith, 1956; Stojanova et al., 2021; Wagner et al., 2021). As such we have found only two possible examples of the complete pattern. The first, from Pereira et al. (2014), shows temperature‐dependent F1 heterosis in the copepod Tigriopus californicus; the second, from Hahn and Rieseberg (2017), shows drought‐dependent F1 heterosis between particular populations of common ragweed Ambrosia artemisiifolia. The relevant data from these studies are reproduced in Figure S9. While there are caveats in each case (slightly different measures of fitness or large confidence intervals) data of this kind are the strongest evidence for the importance of phenotypic dominance to F1 fitness.
What data might place upper bounds on ?
In contrast to the previous section, data placing upper bounds on in nature are relatively easy to find. These data suggest that dominance effects—even if they are present—cannot be very large.
The first relevant observation is the paucity of exceptions to Haldane's Rule – especially once we exclude taxa with X or Z silencing in the homogametic sex (Haldane, 1922; Coyne & Orr, 2004, Ch. 8; Schilthuizen et al., 2011). We have shown that Haldane's Rule is predicted only when , where x is the proportion of divergent loci that are X‐linked (eq. 18; Fig. 6a‐b). Moreover, the in this equation applies to dominance effects at X‐linked loci, which are predicted to be more variable than those on the autosomes (Charlesworth et al., 1987), suggesting a tighter upper bound for genome‐wide . In addition, data from multiple Drosophila species show an increase in the strength of Haldane's rule with x (Turelli & Begun, 1997). It follows from Equation 17, that such an increase is predicted only on the condition that , further tightening the bound.
The second observation that provides an upper bound for is hybrid breakdown—the fact that later‐generation hybrids are very often less fit than the initial F1 (Fraïsse et al., 2016; Muller, 1940; Vetukhiv, 1954, Table S1). This is attested both by experimental crosses, and by the relative absence of recombinant hybrids in wild tension zones that are dominated by viable and fertile F1 (e.g., Milne, 2019). An explanation for this hybrid breakdown is that later‐generation hybrids (just like recombinant haploid hybrids) suffer from segregation of parental alleles that breaks up co‐adapted gene complexes. Consequently, for the F1 to be fitter than later‐generation hybrids, the cost of dominance experienced to a greater extent by the F1, must be less than the cost of segregation experienced by these later‐generation hybrids. A simple generalization of Equation 18 (eq. 53) shows that hybrid breakdown between well adapted parental lines will occur only if:
(21) |
where h is the hybrid index (the proportion of divergent alleles that come from one the parental lines) and p 12 is the interpopulation heterozygosity. It follows that F2 breakdown will occur whenever , since for an F2. And the same weak bound follows from observations of selection for increased heterozygosity in recombinant hybrids (Lindtke et al., 2012; Simon et al., 2018; Thompson et al., 2022). Breakdown among backcross hybrids sets a stronger bound because it occurs only if (since for any backcross), and on average for the first backcross.
While the upper bounds on are not tight, existing data are at least consistent with a world where dominance effects are non‐zero but small (i.e., where ). In this case, simple additive phenotypic models would yield good predictions for later‐generation recombinant hybrids (Fraïsse et al., 2016; Simon et al., 2018), even when dominance qualitatively affects outcomes for the F1.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
A.D.M. and K.A.T. conceived the original idea of the study, performed simulations, and interpreted results. J.J.W. and H.S. expanded these with analytical predictions and individual‐based simulations. All authors discussed the results, gave critical feedback, and contributed to the final manuscript.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
DATA ARCHIVING
All simulation data, code used to generate this data, and scripts used for processing and generating figures can be found at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2bvq83bt9.
1.
Associate Editor: L. Fromhage
Handling Editor: T. Chapman
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
H.S. acknowledges funding from the Wellcome Trust PhD programme in Mathematical Genomics and Medicine (RG92770). A.D.M. was supported by a UBC International Work Learn Award, and Karen McKellin International Leader of Tomorrow Award. K.A.T. was supported by an NSERC Canada Graduate Scholarship (CGS‐D), a UBC Four Year Fellowship, a Killam Doctoral Scholarship, and a British Columbia Graduate Scholarship.
REFERENCES
- Arnold, M.L. & Hodges, S.A. (1995) Are natural hybrids fit or unfit relative to their parents? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 10(2), 67–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barton, N.H. (1992) On the spread of new gene combinations in the third phase of Wright's shifting‐balance. Evolution, 46(2), 551–557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barton, N.H. (2001) The role of hybridization in evolution. Molecular Ecology, 10(3), 551–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateson, P. (1978) Sexual imprinting and optimal outbreeding. Nature, 273, 659–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benyi, K. & Gall, G. A.E. (1981) Genotype‐environment interaction effects on reproductive performance in Tribolium castaneum . Theoretical and Applied Genetics, 59(2), 123–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billiard, S. , Castric, V. & Llaurens, V. (2021) The integrative biology of genetic dominance. Biological Reviews, 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birchler, J.A. (2013) Genetic rules of heterosis in plants. In Polyploid and hybrid genomics (pp. 313–321). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Blair, W.F. (1955) Mating Call and Stage of Speciation in the Microhyla olivacea‐M. carolinensis Complex. Evolution, 9(4), 469–480. [Google Scholar]
- Bolnick, D.I. , Turelli, M. , López‐Fernández, H. , Wainwright, P.C. & Near, T.J. (2008) Accelerated mitochondrial evolution and “Darwin's corollary”: Asymmetric viability of reciprocal F1 hybrids in entrarchid fishes. Genetics, 178(2), 1037–1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouchemousse, S. , Lévêque, L. , Dubois, G. & Viard, F. (2016) Co‐occurrence and reproductive synchrony do not ensure hybridization between an alien tunicate and its interfertile native congener. Evolutionary Ecology, 30, 69–87. [Google Scholar]
- Brandvain, Y. , Pauly, G.B. , May, M.R. & Turelli, M. (2014) Explaining Darwin's corollary to Haldane's rule: the role of mitonuclear interactions in asymmetric postzygotic isolation among toads. Genetics, 197, 743–747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butlin, R. (1987) Speciation by reinforcement. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 2(1), 8–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth, B. (1992) Evolutionary rates in partially self‐fertilizing species. The American Naturalist, 140(1), 126–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth, B. & Charlesworth, D. (1998) Some evolutionary consequences of deleterious mutations. Genetica, 102(3), 3–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth, B. , Coyne, J.A. & Barton, N.H. (1987) The relative rates of evolution of sex chromosomes and autosomes. The American naturalist, 130, 113–146. [Google Scholar]
- Chevin, L.‐M. , Decorzent, G. & Lenormand, T. (2014) Niche dimensionality and the genetics of ecological speciation. Evolution, 68(5), 1244–1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clo, J. , Ronfort, J. & Gay, L. (2021) Fitness consequences of hybridization in a predominantly selfing species: insights into the role of dominance and epistatic incompatibilities. Heredity, 127(4), 393–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coughlan, J.M. & Matute, D.R. (2020) The importance of intrinsic postzygotic barriers throughout the speciation process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1806), 20190533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coyne, J.A. & Orr, H.A. (2004) Speciation. Sinauer Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Crespel, A. , Audet, C. , Bernatchez, L. & Garant, D. (2012) Effects of rearing environment and strain combination on heterosis in brook trout. North American Journal of Aquaculture, 74(2), 188–198. [Google Scholar]
- Crnokrak, P. & Roff, D.A. (1995) Dominance variance: Associations with selection and fitness. Heredity, 75(5), 530–540. [Google Scholar]
- Dagilis, A.J. , Kirkpatrick, M. & Bolnick, D.I. (2019) The evolution of hybrid fitness during speciation. PLOS Genetics, 15(5), e1008125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. (1859) On the origin of species. London: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- De Sanctis, B. , Schneemann, H. & Welch, J.J. (2022) How does the mode of evolutionary divergence affect reproductive isolation? bioRxiv.
- Deng, X. , Hiatt, J.B. , Nguyen, D.K. , Ercan, S. , Sturgill, D. , Hillier, L.W. , Schlesinger, F. , Davis, C.A. , Reinke, V.J. , Gingeras, T.R. , Shendure, J. , Waterston, R.H. , Oliver, B. , Lieb, J.D. & Disteche, C.M. (2011) Evidence for compensatory upregulation of expressed X‐linked genes in mammals, Caenorhabditis elegans and Drosophila melanogaster . Nature Genetics, 43(12), 1179–1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dettman, J.R. , Jacobson, D.J. & Taylor, J.W. (2003) A multilocus genealogical approach to phylogenetic species recognition in the model eukaryote Neurospora . Evolution, 57(12), 2703–2720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dittrich‐ Reed, D.R. (2013) Hybridization as a stimulus for adaptation to a novel environment. PhD thesis, University of Tennessee, Knoxville. [Google Scholar]
- Dobzhansky, T. (1937) Genetics and the origin of species. New York: Colombia University. [Google Scholar]
- Domgínguez, A. & Albornoz, J. (1987) Environment‐dependent heterosis in Drosophila melanogaster . GSE, 19(1), 37–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edmands, S. (2002) Does parental divergence predict reproductive compatibility? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 17(11), 520–527. [Google Scholar]
- Edmands, S. (2007) Between a rock and a hard place: evaluating the relative risks of inbreeding and outbreeding for conservation and management. Molecular Ecology, 16(3), 463–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Escobar, J.S. , Nicot, A. & David, P. (2008) The different sources of variation in inbreeding depression, heterosis and outbreeding depression in a metapopulation of Physa acuta. Genetics, 180, 1593–1608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Favre, A. , Widmer, A. & Karrenberg, S. (2017) Differential adaptation drives ecological speciation in campions (Silene): evidence from a multi‐site transplant experiment. New Phytologist, 213(3), 1487–1499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher, R. (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. [Google Scholar]
- Frankham, R. (1990) Are responses to artificial selection for reproductive fitness characters consistently asymmetrical? Genetical Research, 56(1), 35–42. [Google Scholar]
- Fraïsse, C. , Gunnarsson, P.A. , Roze, D. , Bierne, N. & Welch, J.J. (2016) The genetics of speciation: Insights from Fisher's geometric model. Evolution, 70(7), 1450–1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gramlich, S. , Liu, X. , Favre, A. , Buerkle, C.A. & Karrenberg, S. (2022) A polygenic architecture with habitat‐dependent effects underlies ecological differentiation in Silene . New Phytologist. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Griswold, C.K. (2006) Gene flow's effect on the genetic architecture of a local adaptation and its consequences for QTL analyses. Heredity, 96, 445–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu, L. & Walters, J.R. (2017) Evolution of sex chromosome dosage compensation in animals: A beautiful theory, undermined by facts and bedeviled by details. Genome Biology and Evolution, 9(9), 2461–2476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn, M.A. & Rieseberg, L.H. (2017) Genetic admixture and heterosis may enhance the invasiveness of common ragweed. Evolutionary Applications, 10(3), 241–250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haldane, J.B.S. (1922) Sex ratio and unisexual sterility in hybrid animals. Journal of Genetics, 12(2), 101–109. [Google Scholar]
- Haldane, J.B.S. (1924) A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection, Part I. Transactions of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 23, 19–41. [Google Scholar]
- Haldane, J.B.S. (1927) A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection, Part V: selection and mutation. Mathematical Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 28, 838–844. [Google Scholar]
- Harrison, G.A. (1962) Heterosis and adaptability in the heat tolerance of mice. Genetics, 47(4), 427–434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield, T. & Schluter, D. (1999) Ecological speciation in sticklebacks: environment‐dependent hybrid fitness. Evolution, 53(3), 866–873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill, W.G. (1982) Dominance and epistasis as components of heterosis. Zeitschrift für Tierzüchtung und Züchtungsbiologie, 99(1‐4), 161–168. [Google Scholar]
- Kölreuters, J. (1766) Dritte Fotsetzung. Technical report.
- Lee, Y. , Marsden, C.D. , Norris, L.C. , Collier, T.C. , Main, B.J. , Fofana, A. , Cornel, A.J. & Lanzaro, G.C. (2013) Spatiotemporal dynamics of gene flow and hybrid fitness between the M and S forms of the malaria mosquito, Anopheles gambiae . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(49), 19854–19859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindtke, D. , Buerkle, C.A. , Barbará, T. , Heinze, B. , Castiglione, S. , Bartha, D. & Lexer, C. (2012) Recombinant hybrids retain heterozygosity at many loci: new insights into the genomics of reproductive isolation in Populus . Molecular Ecology, 21(20), 5042–5058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch, M. (1991) The genetic interpretation of inbreeding depression and outbreeding depression. Evolution, 45(3), 622–629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch, M. & Walsh, B. (1998) Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sinauer Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Mallet, J. & Barton, N.H. (1989) Inference from clines stabilized by frequency‐dependent selection. Genetics, 122, 967–976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malone, J.H. , Chrzanowski, T.H. & Michalak, P. (2007) Sterility and gene expression in hybrid males of Xenopus laevis and X. muelleri . PLOS ONE, 2(8), e781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mani, G. & Clarke, B. (1990) Mutational order: a major stochastic process in evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 240, 29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mank, J.E. , Hosken, D.J. & Wedell, N. (2011) Some inconvenient truths about sex chromosome dosage compensation and the potential role of sexual conflict. Evolution, 65(8), 2133–2144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manna, F. , Martin, G. & Lenormand, T. (2011) Fitness landscapes: An alternative theory for the dominance of mutation. Genetics, 189, 923–937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, G. (2014) Fisher's geometrical model emerges as a property of complex integrated phenotypic networks. Genetics, 197, 237–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, G. & Lenormand, T. (2006) A general multivariate extension of Fisher's geometrical model and the distribution of mutation fitness effects across species. Evolution, 60(5), 893–907. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins, N. , Pearson, G.A. , Gouveia, L. , Tavares, A.I. , Serrão, E.A. & Bartsch, I. (2019) Hybrid vigour for thermal tolerance in hybrids between the allopatric kelps Laminaria digitata and L. pallida (Laminariales, Phaeophyceae) with contrasting thermal affinities. European Journal of Phycology, 54(4), 548–561. [Google Scholar]
- Matuszewski, S. , Hermisson, J. & Kopp, M. (2014) Fisher's geometric model with a moving optimum. Evolution, 68(9), 2571–2588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. (1956) Acclimatization to high temperatures in inbred and outbred Drosophila subobscura . Journal of Genetics, 54(1), 497–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milne, R.I. (2019) A look at hybrid zones in Rhododendron, with particular reference to F1‐dominated hybrid zones (F1DZs). Rhododendron International, 3, 98–117. [Google Scholar]
- Moran, P.A. , Ritchie, M.G. & Bailey, N.W. (2017) A rare exception to Haldane's rule: Are X chromosomes key to hybrid incompatibilities? Heredity, 118(6), 554–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller, H. (1940) Bearing of the Drosophila work on systematics. In The New Systematics (pp. 185–268). Oxford: Clarendon Press. edited by J. Huxley. [Google Scholar]
- Muller, H. (1942) Isolating mechanisms, evolution and temperature. Biological Symposium, 6, 71–125. [Google Scholar]
- Munaro, E.M. , Eyhérabide, G.H. , D'Andrea, K.E. , Cirilo, A.G. & Otegui, M.E. (2011) Heterosis‐environment interaction in maize: What drives heterosis for grain yield? Field Crops Research, 124(3), 441–449. [Google Scholar]
- Ono, J. , Gerstein, A.C. & Otto, S.P. (2017) Widespread genetic incompatibilities between first‐step mutations during parallel adaptation of Saccharomyces cerevisiae to a common environment. PLOS Biology, 15(1), e1002591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, H.A. (1991) A test of Fisher's theory of dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 88, 11413–11415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, H.A. (1998) The population genetics of adaptation: the distribution of factors fixed during adaptive evolution. Evolution, 52(4), 935–949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, H.A. (2000) Adaptation and the cost of complexity. Evolution, 54(1), 13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr, H.A. & Betancourt, A.J. (2001) Haldane's sieve and adaptation from the standing genetic variation. Genetics, 157(2), 875–884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pereira, R.J. , Barreto, F.S. & Burton, R.S. (2014) Ecological novelty by hybridization: experimental evidence for increased thermal tolerance by transgressive segregation in tigriopus californicus . Evolution, 68(1), 204–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, J.G. , Hitchcock, T.J. & Bailey, N.W. (2021) Variable dosage compensation is associated with female consequences of an X‐linked, male‐beneficial mutation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 288(1947), 20210355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieseberg, L.H. , Whitton, J. & Gardner, K. (1999) Hybrid zones and the genetic architecture of a barrier to gene flow between two sunflower species. Genetics, 152(2), 713–727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato, A. , Shimeld, S.M. & Bishop, J.D.D. (2014) Symmetrical reproductive compatibility of two species in the Ciona intestinalis (Ascidiacea) species complex, a model for marine genomics and developmental biology. Zoological Science, 31(6), 369–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawamura, K. (1996) Maternal effect as a cause of exceptions for Haldane's rule. Genetics, 143(1), 609–611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffman, J.S. & Ralph, P.L. (2021) System drift and speciation. Evolution, 76(2), 236–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilthuizen, M. , Giesbers, M.C.W.G. & Beukeboom, L.W. (2011) Haldane's rule in the 21st century. Heredity, 107(2), 95–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluter, D. (2000) The ecology of adaptive radiation. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schneemann, H. , De Sanctis, B. , Roze, D. , Bierne, N. & Welch, J.J. (2020) The geometry and genetics of hybridization. Evolution, (74), 2575–2590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon, A. , Bierne, N. & Welch, J.J. (2018) Coadapted genomes and selection on hybrids: Fisher's geometric model explains a variety of empirical patterns. Evolution Letters, 2(5), 472–498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stelkens, R. & Seehausen, O. (2009) Genetic distance between species predicts novel trait expression in their hybrids. Evolution, 63(4), 884–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stojanova, B. , Münzbergová, Z. & Pánková, H. (2021) Inbreeding depression and heterosis vary in space and time in the serpentinophyte perennial Minuartia smejkalii . Preslia, 93(2), 149–168. [Google Scholar]
- Szafraniec, K. , Wloch, D.M. , Sliwa, P. , Borts, R.H. & Korona, R. (2003) Small fitness effects and weak genetic interactions between deleterious mutations in heterozygous loci of the yeast Saccharomyces cerevisiae . Genetics Research, 82(1), 19–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, E.B. , Boughman, J.W. , Groenenboom, M. , Sniatynski, M. , Schluter, D. , & Gow, J.L. (2006) Speciation in reverse: morphological and genetic evidence of the collapse of a three‐spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) species pair. Molecular Ecology, 15(2), 343–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, K.A. , Peichel, C.L. , Rennison, D.J. , McGee, M.D. , Albert, A. Y.K. , Vines, T.H. , Greenwood, A.K. , Wark, A.R. , Brandvain, Y. , Schumer, M. & Schluter, D. (2022) Analysis of ancestry heterozygosity suggests that hybrid incompatibilities in threespine stickleback are environment dependent. PLOS Biology, 20(1), e3001469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, K.A. & Schluter, D. (2022) Heterosis counteracts hybrid breakdown to forestall speciation by parallel natural selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1974), 20220422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, K.A. , Urquhart‐Cronish, M. , Whitney, K.D. , Rieseberg, L.H. & Schluter, D. (2021) Patterns, predictors, and consequences of dominance in hybrids. The American Naturalist, 197(3), E72–E88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffin, P. , Olson, S. & Moyle, L.C. (2001) Asymmetrical crossing barriers in angiosperms. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1469), 861–867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turelli, M. & Begun, D.J. (1997) Haldane's rule and X chromosome size in Drosophila . Genetics, 147, 1799–1815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turelli, M. & Moyle, L.C. (2007) Asymmetric postmating isolation: Darwin's corollary to Haldane's rule. Genetics, 176, 1059–1088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetukhiv, M. (1954) Integration of the genotype in local populations of three species of Drosophila . Evolution, 8(3), 241–251. [Google Scholar]
- Vetukhiv, M.O. & Beardmore, J.A. (1959) Effect of environment upon the manifestation of heterosis and homeostasis in Drosophila pseudoobscura . Genetics, 44(5), 759–768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, M.R. , Tang, C. , Salvato, F. , Clouse, K.M. , Bartlett, A. , Vintila, S. , Phillips, L. , Sermons, S. , Hoffmann, M. , Balint‐Kurti, P.J. & Kleiner, M. (2021) Microbe‐dependent heterosis in maize. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(30), e2021965118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace, A. (1889) Darwinism. Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Walter, G.M. , Richards, T.J. , Wilkinson, M.J. , Blows, M.W. , Aguirre, J.D. , & Ortiz‐Barrientos, D. (2020) Loss of ecologically important genetic variation in late generation hybrids reveals links between adaptation and speciation. Evolution Letters, 4(4), 302–316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, H. , Bruce, T.J. , Su, B. , Li, S. , Dunham, R.A. & Wang, X. (2022) Environment‐dependent heterosis and transgressive gene expression in reciprocal hybrids between the channel catfish Ictalurus punctatus and the blue catfish Ictalurus furcatus . Biology, 11(1), 117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, H. , McArthur, E.D. , Sanderson, S.C. , Graham, J.H. & Freeman, D.C. (1997) Narrow hybrid zone between two subspecies of big sagebrush (Artemisia tridentata: Asteraceae). IV. Reciprocal transplant experiments. Evolution, 51(1), 95–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waser, N.M. (1993) Population structure, optimal outbreeding, and assortative mating in angiosperms. In The natural history of inbreeding and outbreeding (pp. 173–199). Section: 9. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, E.T. & Demuth, J.P. (2012) Haldane's rule in marsupials: What happens when both sexes are functionally hemizygous? Journal of Heredity, 103(3), 453–458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei, X. & Zhang, J. (2018) The optimal mating distance resulting from heterosis and genetic incompatibility. Science Advances, 4(11), eaau5518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welch, J.J. & Waxman, D. (2003) Modularity and the cost of complexity. Evolution, 57(8), 1723–1734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellhausen, E.J. (1952) Heterosis in a new population. In Heterosis (pp. 418–450). Ames, Iowa: Iowa State University. [Google Scholar]
- Wu, C.A. & Campbell, D.R. (2006) Environmental stressors differentially affect leaf ecophysiological responses in two Ipomopsis species and their hybrids. Oecologia, 148(2), 202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi, R. & Otto, S.P. (2020) Insights from Fisher's geometric model on the likelihood of speciation under different histories of environmental change. Evolution, 74, 1603–1619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeaman, S. & Whitlock, M.C. (2011) The genetic architecture of adaptation under migration‐selection balance. Evolution, 65(7), 1897–1911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All simulation data, code used to generate this data, and scripts used for processing and generating figures can be found at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2bvq83bt9.