Abstract
Studies of neurodegenerative disease risk in gout are contradictory. Relationships with neuroimaging markers of brain structure, which may offer insights, are uncertain. Here we investigated associations between gout, brain structure, and neurodegenerative disease incidence. Gout patients had smaller global and regional brain volumes and markers of higher brain iron, using both observational and genetic approaches. Participants with gout also had higher incidence of all-cause dementia, Parkinson’s disease, and probable essential tremor. Risks were strongly time dependent, whereby associations with incident dementia were highest in the first 3 years after gout diagnosis. These findings suggest gout is causally related to several measures of brain structure. Lower brain reserve amongst gout patients may explain their higher vulnerability to multiple neurodegenerative diseases. Motor and cognitive impairments may affect gout patients, particularly in early years after diagnosis.
Subject terms: Dementia, Gout
The potential association between neurodegenerative disease risk and gout is not fully understood. Here the authors showed that gout is causally related to several measures of brain structure which may explain their higher vulnerability to dementia.
Introduction
Gout is the most common inflammatory arthritis affecting ~1–4% of the population1. The clinical syndrome is characterised by acute flares of joint pain and swelling, resulting from deposition of monosodium urate crystals in joints and peri-articular tissues, initiating an acute inflammatory cascade. In contrast to multiple other organ systems2, classically the brain is not thought to be affected. However, emerging studies have cited contradictory associations between hyperuricaemia and neurodegenerative disease. Observational studies have reported a lower risk of dementia, particularly Alzheimer’s disease, in hyperuricaemia3–5. Antioxidant properties of uric acid have been proposed as a potential mechanism for this neuroprotection6. Mendelian randomisation studies, which can offer insights into causal relationships, have offered conflicting results in Alzheimer’s disease7,8. Hyperuricaemia and gout have also been linked to higher stroke risk9. Clarifying the impact on the brain is vital given that hyperuricaemia is a treatable target.
Insights into relationships between gout and neurodegenerative disease could result from examining links with brain structure, as yet unexplored. MRI markers provide quantitative, sensitive intermediate endophenotypes for neuropsychiatric disease10. A few studies have investigated associations between serum urate and a handful of biomarkers for stroke and dementia. Most have found no association11,12. However, to date, no studies have examined gout. Urate analyses have been small (n < 2500), not accounted for many potential confounding variables, and explored only a few aspects of brain structure, while using solely observational approaches.
We performed the first investigation of neuroimaging markers in patients with gout. Observational and Mendelian randomisation (MR) approaches were combined for stronger causal inference. Furthermore, we explored relationships between gout and relevant neurodegenerative diseases. The purpose of this study was to assess whether associations between gout and brain structure would provide insights into relationships with neurodegenerative disease risk.
Results
Participant characteristics
In total, 11,735 participants (1165 within the imaging subset) had a diagnosis of gout. Medical professionals made the majority of diagnoses (31.1% primary care, 18.5% hospital admission). A minority (15.2%) were solely self-reported. Overall, 30.8% of gout patients reported current use of urate-lowering therapy (ULT) at assessment. Gout sufferers were older and comprised a greater proportion of males. Relationships between baseline urate and demographic variables differed by sex (Supplementary Data 1). In males, urate was positively correlated with alcohol intake and lower socioeconomic status. This was not the case in females. Mean baseline serum urate amongst asymptomatic individuals was 305.0 ± 76.7 μmol/L. During follow-up 3126 participants developed dementia and 16,422 individuals died. Deaths amongst gout patients were more than double those of controls (11% vs. 5%).
Observational analyses
Gout and higher urate were associated with multiple measures of brain structure (Fig. 1). There were no marked sex differences in associations. Urate inversely associated with global brain volume (beta = −1.66E-4, T = 3.60, P = 2.30E-4), and also grey and (separately) white matter volumes, with corresponding higher cerebrospinal fluid volumes (beta=4.93E-4, T = 4.94, P = 7.84E-4). To contextualise the effect size, we related the differences to the cross-sectional effects of age (discounting non-linear effects of age). The effect of gout on the global grey matter was equivalent to a 2-year greater age, assuming all other potentially confounding factors were held constant.
There were highly significant differences in regional grey matter volumes, particularly of mid- and hindbrain structures, such as cerebellum (beta = −9.91E-04, T = −9.26, P = 2.25E-20), pons (beta = −5.63E-04, T = −6.23, P = 4.95E-10) and midbrain (beta = −4.00E-04, T = −5.15, P = 2.67E-07) in gout and high urate (Fig. 2). Cerebellar differences were focused on the posterior lobe, particularly inferiorly. Subcortical differences centred on the nucleus accumbens, putamen and caudate. Significant differences were also observed in white matter tract microstructure in the fornix, the major output of the hippocampus. These included lower fractional anisotropy (beta = −2.81E-4, T = −2.83, P = 4.73E-03) and higher mean diffusivity. Notably, there were no significant associations with hippocampal volume or WMH volumes. Gout and higher urate significantly associated with markers of higher iron deposition (lower T2* and higher magnetic susceptibility) of several basal ganglia structures, including bilateral putamen (beta=9.50E-04, T = 9.25, P = 2.38E-20) and caudate (7.62E-4, T = 7.12, P = 1.13E-12). Whilst associations with urate were generally robust after additional adjustment for urate-lowering therapy and consequences of hyperuricaemia, many with gout were not. Further exploration of these models clarified that blood pressure was responsible for a loss of association with cerebellar and striatal volumes, whereas markers of renal function (creatinine and cystatin C) reduced associations with basal ganglia QSM susceptibility measures. Higher urate (but not gout) was positively associated with cuneal and frontal gyrus volumes in model 1, but not in model 2 or in stratified analyses amongst solely the highest earners (Supplementary Data 2).
Gout associated with a higher incidence of dementia (average over study HR = 1.60 [1.38–1.85]). The risk was time-varying, highest in the first 3 years after gout diagnosis (HR = 7.40 [4.95–11.07]) and then decreasing (Fig. 3). We explored possible reasons for this temporal gradient. Accounting for the competing risk lowered hazard ratios but did not alter the pattern of associations (first 3 years HR = 3.53 [2.31–5.38]) (Supplementary Data 3). Excluding controls with asymptomatic hyperuricaemia (average HR = 1.59 [1.42–1.78]) or additionally controlling for possible consequences of hyperuricaemia (average HR = 1.54 [1.27–1.87]) made little impact. Receiving a gout diagnosis later in the study (independently from age) was associated with a slightly higher dementia incidence (average HR = 1.03 [1.01–1.05]). ULT at baseline assessment was not linked to dementia incidence (HR −0.25 [0.59–1.04]).
Risks were higher for vascular dementia (average HR = 2.41 [1.93–3.02]) compared to all-cause dementia, but not for Alzheimer’s disease (average HR = 1.62 [1.30–2.02]). Again, there was a strong time dependence to the risks, particularly for vascular dementia. Amongst asymptomatic individuals, in the linear model, there was an inverse association between urate and dementia incidence (HR = 0.85, 95% CI: 0.80–0.89), with no time dependence. Using restricted cubic splines a non-linear relationship between urate and dementia incidence was observed (P for non-linearity <0.0001). Both low and high urate levels associated with a higher dementia incidence (Fig. 2).
Given the associations with cerebellum and striatum measures, we examined relevant phenotypes post hoc. Gout was associated with a higher incidence of both Parkinson’s disease (HR = 1.43 [1.15–1.79]) and probable essential tremor (HR = 6.75 [5.59–8.00]). Amongst asymptomatic individuals, there was a linear inverse relationship between urate and incident Parkinson’s disease (HR = 0.89 [0.84–0.95], P for non-linearity=0.3), but not with probable essential tremor (HR = 0.95 [0.89–1.02]).
Genetic analyses
Four of the urate-associated SNPs and one of the gout-associated SNPs were unavailable in the outcome datasets. Proxy SNPs were found for three urate SNPs (Supplementary Data 4). Genetic associations mostly mirrored observational ones. Both genetically predicted gout and serum urate significantly associated with regional grey matter volumes, including cerebellar (gout IVW beta = −0.05 [−0.08 to −0.03], P = 1.50E-04; urate IVW beta = −0.05 [−0.09 to −0.01], P = 7.71E-03), midbrain (gout IVW beta = −0.07 [−0.12 to −0.03], P = 1.55E-03; urate IVW beta = −0.06 [−0.10 to −0.02], P = 7.31E-03), pons (gout IVW beta = −0.11 [−0.16 to −0.06], P = 3.01E-05; urate IVW beta = −0.07 [−0.11 to −0.02], P = 2.10E-03) and brainstem (gout IVW beta = −0.11 [−0.16 to −0.06], P = 3.78E-05; urate IVW beta = −0.07 [−0.10 to −0.02], P = 2.48E-03). There were also significant associations with higher magnetic susceptibility in the putamen and caudate, markers of higher iron (Fig. 4). However, whilst genetically predicted gout was significantly associated with global grey matter volume (IVW beta = −0.05 [−0.08 to −0.01], P = 7.75E-03), urate did not IVW beta = −0.05 [−0.29 to −0.20], P = 0.71]. All associations survived FDR correction for multiple testing. Pons, brainstem and the magnetic susceptibility associations additionally survived Bonferroni correction. Robust MR methods including the weighted median and mode gave broadly consistent estimates for gout (Supplementary Data 5). However, for urate only IVW estimates were significant. Whilst there was little evidence of horizontal pleiotropy on the MR Egger intercept test, there was significant heterogeneity between IVW estimates, particularly for urate.
Discussion
In this UK prospective cohort study, participants with a history of gout had smaller global and regional brain volumes and higher brain iron. MR analyses suggested gout was causally related to brain structure. Gout was associated with a higher incidence of several neurodegenerative diseases, particularly in the first three years after diagnosis. Lower brain reserve in gout patients may explain their vulnerability to dementia.
To our knowledge, there are no previous neuroimaging studies of gout. Investigations examining urate are small and, with two exceptions, examined solely markers of cerebrovascular disease13. One study reported higher white matter atrophy with hyperuricaemia14, and another a null association with hippocampal volume11. We replicated these findings, but did not duplicate reported positive relationships between urate and WMH15. Methodological differences may be responsible. In UKB, WMH volumes were calculated automatically whereas previous studies used visual ratings. We found associations with global grey matter and cerebellar (motor and non-motor regions16), brainstem and striatal brain volumes. Gout patients had a higher incidence of probable essential tremor. The neurobiological correlates of essential tremor are unclear, although links have been made with cerebellar atrophy17,18. As this likely comprises a highly heterogeneous category we are cautious in our interpretations of these results. Associations with markers of higher iron in basal ganglia with hyperuricaemia mirror findings in aging, Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases19,20, and recently alcohol consumption21. Our MR findings provide support for causal relationships underlying the associations. Taken together, these brain findings lend further support to recent claims of a causal impact of hyperuricaemia on Alzheimer’s7,22.
The mechanism(s) underlying how gout affects brain volume is unclear. Hyperuricaemia has been linked to arterial stiffness23, and associated with brain microvascular damage24, which may improve with allopurinol treatment25. Alternatively, toxic metabolic pathways may be responsible26,27. Interestingly, we found that controlling for blood pressure reduced associations between gout and brain volumes, suggesting this as a potential mediating mechanism. Whilst urate associated with measures of white matter microstructure, no relation was observed with WMHs. One explanation is that due to selection biases, the healthier UKB imaging subset may not have yet manifested WMHs. Of note, the strongest associations of hyperuricaemia were with posterior brain regions, but the calculation of WMH volume only includes the anterior circulation. It is unclear whether, at least in healthy individuals, urate can cross the blood brain barrier28. High levels of iron in basal ganglia could result from inflammatory processes in gout29,30 or poorer urinary iron excretion as suggested by our observed reduction in association after controlling for renal function. Conversely, higher ferritin (a blood measure of iron) secondary to diet, could lead to higher urate levels29.
In this sample gout associated with higher incidence of dementia, a finding at odds with previous claims of protective effects in observational studies13,31. A recent small meta-analysis found no evidence for an association with gout overall, but cited a possible protective effect on Alzheimer’s disease based on two studies5. To our knowledge, there have been just two MR studies of gout and dementia, both reporting no significant associations with Alzheimer’s disease22,32. However, low statistical power, driven by weak instrument bias, limits their interpretation. Causal relationships with other dementia subtypes, such as vascular dementia, has been limited by the lack of available large-scale GWAS. Whilst we found gout was associated with smaller global brain volumes, associations were not observed with classical imaging markers of Alzheimer’s disease33 or vascular dementia34 such as hippocampal volume35 or white matter hyperintensities. Instead as gout played a causative role in multiple neurodegenerative pathologies, we propose a brain vulnerability model (Fig. 5). A similar phenomenon is well recognised with delirium36. Global brain volumes are commonly conceived as a proxy for brain reserve, structural characteristics of the brain that allow some people to better cope with brain pathology before cognitive changes emerge37. The strong temporal gradient in risk of neurodegenerative disease after gout is interesting. We propose three hypotheses. First, death is a competing risk. Second, there may be a selective detection bias. Receiving a gout diagnosis may result in more frequent medical review initially, when cognitive problems could be noticed. Third, the inflammatory response in a gout flare may induce a stepwise global decline that may precipitate acute cognitive decline.
The relationship between baseline urate and dementia incidence was non-linear. As others have previously raised, associations with a single urate measurement, which may not reflect long-term exposure38, should be interpreted cautiously. Reduced risks of dementia with hyperuricaemia have been previously reported in observational studies4, including in an earlier analysis of UKB3. Whilst earlier Mendelian randomisation studies using genetically proxied urate, reflective of lifelong exposure, were not significant8,39, two studies published this year found support for a harmful causal effect on Alzheimer’s disease7,22. Differing choice of SNPs as instrumental variables and outcome GWAS between studies may explain the contradictory results. Drawing together the recent MR studies and the consistent brain associations we observed in urate and gout, we hypothesise collider bias may be causing a spurious protective association for urate40. Both higher urate (in affluent individuals), and absence of dementia increase likelihood of UKB recruitment.
Taken together, these results lend support not only for a causal role of gout in Alzheimer’s disease, but more widely in neurodegenerative disease. Furthermore, the brain associations offer a potential mechanism through which this risk is conferred. This is an area ripe for further exploration in the future with mediation MR, when large-scale GWAS of dementia subtypes become available. These findings are relevant for gout patients, clinicians and researchers. There are also wider implications for our understanding of mechanistic pathways in neurodegenerative disease that could lead to novel intervention targets. Prophylaxis for gout flares may be justified on the basis of brain health in addition to joint and cardiovascular health, although interventional trials will be needed to test this. Patients with gout should be monitored for cognitive and motor symptoms of neurodegenerative disease given their increased risk, especially in the early period after diagnosis.
Strengths and limitations
The large size and statistical power enabled triangulation of observational and MR approaches. Consistent directions of association between different methods strengthen causal inference41. A comprehensive range of brain metrics were examined, whilst adjusting for multiple confounds and multiple testing. Other key strengths include the prospective cohort, examination of relevant clinical phenotypes in the same sample and time-to-event analyses accounting for competing risks.
This study has several limitations. First, UKB is a self-selective cohort42. Second, the measurement of serum urate was cross-sectional and may not reflect chronic exposure43. Third, MRI data was at a single time point so we cannot infer changes in brain structure. Fourth, case identification may be subject to ascertainment bias, likely skewed to more severe cases, which could bias associations towards the null. Fifth, there may be diagnostic confounding between Parkinson’s disease and essential tremor. Sixth, gout covaries with obesity and alcohol. However, we controlled for these factors, performed MR analyses, and we have previously reported brain structure associations with alcohol in a markedly different spatial distribution in the same sample44. Seventh, MR relies on a set of unverifiable assumptions, which we tried to assess where possible. Whilst we found no evidence of horizontal pleiotropy (untestable Inside assumption), there was heterogeneity of MR estimates. This may be due to failure of the assumptions of homogeneity of the genetic association with the risk factor or linearity of the effect of the risk factor on the outcome, or plausibly, differing biological causal mechanisms between variants. A minority of genetic variants used to instrument urate may have suffered from weak instrument bias. However, post hoc choice of instruments based on measured F-statistics can exacerbate bias45. Eighth, the strongest genetic associations were between gout and brain markers rather than urate. Sample overlap between gout and brain imaging GWAS may bias genetic associations towards observational associations. The interpretation of MR estimates with binary exposures needs care. The underlying latent continuous trait (urate), although alone is insufficient to cause gout, may influence the brain independently of gout and therefore bias MR estimates [exclusion restriction assumption]46. As such, IV estimates from the gout MR analyses should be interpreted as an estimate for genetic compliers (likely to be a small subgroup of the population). Finally, this was not a bespoke study with optimal measures for testing cerebellar function, for example, measured gait speed or eye movements.
In this population-based sample, participants with gout had smaller global and regional grey matter volumes. Lower brain reserve may explain the vulnerability of gout patients to neurodegenerative disease. Clinicians should be vigilant for motor and cognitive problems in gout patients.
Methods
Study population
Participants were from UK Biobank (UKB) study47, which recruited volunteers aged 40–69 years in 2006–2010. UK Biobank has approval from the North West Multi-centre Research Ethics Committee (MREC) as a Research Tissue Bank (RTB) approval. All participants gave informed consent. A subset underwent imaging which included brain MRI (~50,000 scanned to date). Participants underwent imaging at three centres (Newcastle, Stockport or Reading) with identical Siemens Skyra 3T scanners (software VD13) using a standard 32-channel head coil. Details of image pre-processing and quality control pipelines are described elsewhere48. Participants with complete data were included (Supplementary Fig. S1).
Exposure measurements
Gout diagnoses were algorithmically defined from UKB’s baseline assessment data collection, linked data from Hospital Episode Statistics, primary care and death records (https://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/refer.cgi?id=460). Individuals who developed gout after dementia were excluded to minimise reverse causation. Serum urate (in μmol/L) was measured using a Beckman Coulter AU5800 at study baseline in all participants.
Outcome measurements
Brain markers
We used 2138 summary image-derived phenotypes (IDPs) representing distinct measures of brain structure from the following modalities: T1-weighted and T2-weighted-FLAIR structural imaging, susceptibility-weighted MRI, diffusion MRI. These are outputs of a dedicated processing pipeline and are available from UKB49. IDPs included: whole brain and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) volumes, cortical volumes, surface area, thickness, T2-FLAIR white matter hyperintensities (WMH) volumes50, brain iron deposition metrics (T2* and magnetic susceptibility51), white matter microstructural measures (Supplementary Methods).
The precise spatial distribution of associations between gout and grey matter volume (T1-weighted images) across the brain was investigated using FSL-VBM52 (Supplementary Methods), a method to compare grey matter volume in each 3D volume element (voxel) of the structural image, after adjusting between individuals for estimated total intracranial volume.
Neurodegenerative disease
Cases were algorithmically defined from UKB’s baseline assessment data collection (including self-report), linked data from Hospital Episode Statistics, primary care and death records (https://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/showcase/refer.cgi?id=460). Incident cases (diagnosed any time after baseline) were included. Prevalent cases (diagnosis before baseline) and those who developed neurodegenerative disease prior to gout were excluded, to lessen reverse causation. The primary outcome was all-cause dementia. In planned subgroup analyses, Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia were examined.
Covariates
Selected a priori confound covariates were identified on the basis of literature supporting their impact on both gout and MRI markers: all image-related confounds, age, age2, sex (self-reported/NHS records), educational qualifications, Townsend Deprivation Index, household income, historical job code, waist-hip-ratio, alcohol, smoking, and diuretic use (Model 1). Additional adjustments for potential consequences of hyperuricaemia (potentially on the causal pathway) included: blood pressure, cholesterol, cystatin C, creatinine, diabetes mellitus, urate-lowering therapy (ULT), and chronic kidney disease (Model 2). Supplementary Methods describes measurement details.
Genetic variants
Mendelian randomisation was performed for two related exposures: (1) serum urate (two-sample), and (2) gout (one-sample). Genetic instruments for urate (n = 183) were identified based on the sentinel variants at genome-wide significant loci (P < 5 × 10−8) reported in the largest publicly available genome-wide association study53 (Supplementary Data 4). Genetic instruments for gout (n = 13) were identified from the largest GWAS of gout versus asymptomatic hyperuricaemia (aiming to distinguish high urate from inflammatory components of gout)54. Clumping was performed in the original GWAS, using a 1-Mb locus. Genetic associations with brain phenotypes were obtained from the largest GWAS of brain imaging phenotypes55. Proxy SNPs were selected (LD R2 > 0.9) where available.
Statistical analysis
All analyses were performed in R (version 4.1.2) unless otherwise stated.
Observational analyses
Multivariable linear regression models assessed the relationship between gout/urate and each of 2138 IDPs as outcomes. Brain measures and urate were quantile normalised resulting in Gaussian distributions with mean zero and standard deviation one. To account for multiple testing, Bonferroni and (separately) 5% false discovery rate (FDR) multiple comparison corrections were applied (2138 tests). Effect sizes were compared with those of age (Supplementary Methods). For VBM, the Big Linear Model toolbox (Supplementary Methods) was used to perform mass univariate ordinary least squares regression (parametric inference) voxelwise.
Cox proportional hazards models were used to estimate associations between gout/urate and neurodegenerative disease incidence. The length of follow-up was calculated as the interval between the origin and either date of event (first disease code) or censoring (date of death or last date of data collection—January 2, 2022). Origin was the baseline date, except for participants who developed gout after baseline, where the gout diagnosis date was used. The impact of time to gout diagnosis on neurodegenerative disease incidence was explored by including the interval between study baseline and gout diagnosis date as a fixed covariate. Relationships between the urate level and dementia were assessed using linear (fixed effects) and non-linear regression models. For the latter urate was categorised into quintiles and restricted cubic splines (5 knots) applied56. Non-linearity was formally tested (H0: β2 = β3 = …=βk − 1 = 0) with an F test. Assumptions were checked as described in Supplementary Methods. Associations between gout and neurodegenerative disease incidence accounting for the competing risk of death were assessed using the subdistribution method57. In sensitivity analyses, participants with asymptomatic hyperuricaemia (defined by serum urate levels >357 μmol/L in females and >428 μmol/L in males58) were excluded from the control group in gout analyses.
Genetic analyses
We used MR to investigate whether causal relationships could underpin the observational associations we found with brain structure (Supplementary Methods for further details, including power calculation). One- (gout) and two-sample (urate) linear MR analyses using summary statistics from European participants were conducted using R packages MendelianRandomization (version 0.5.1) and TwoSampleMR (version 0.5.6). Variant harmonisation ensured that association between SNPs and exposures, and between SNPs and outcomes, reflected the same allele. Several robust MR methods were used to evaluate the consistency of the causal inference. To adjust for multiple testing, Bonferroni and false discovery rate (FDR, 5%) corrected p values were calculated across 20 tests.
Post hoc analyses
To investigate whether positive associations between urate and a few IDPs could be the result of residual confounding, models were stratified by income. After finding associations between gout and cerebellum, midbrain and striatum, additional relevant clinical outcomes were also examined post hoc: incidence of Parkinson’s disease and incidence of other extrapyramidal movement disorders (ICD10 G25). The latter is likely dominated by essential tremor given its prevalence but also includes drug-induced tremor, myoclonus, and akathisia. We refer to this diagnosis henceforth as a probable essential tremor, although we recognise the heterogeneous nature of this entity.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank Maria Christodoulou and the University of Oxford Statistics Consulting service for providing advice on the survival model analyses. We also thank the Wellcome Trust—216462/Z/19/Z (A.T.), 225790/Z/22/Z (S.Bu.), 206589/Z/17/Z (A.W.), 215573/Z/19/Z (S.S.), 202788/Z/16/Z (K.Mi.), 100309/Z/12/Z (T.N.); the UK MRC—G1001354 (K.P.E.), G1001354 (K.P.E.); the European Commission—Horizon 2020 732592 (K.P.E.); the British Heart Foundation—RG/16/4/32218 (S.Be.); the NIH—R01EB026859 (T.N.); EPSRC—EP/S02428X/1 (I.H.); and the NIHR—BRC-1215-20014 (S.Be.); NIHR203312 (S.Bu.); BRC-1215-20014 (T.N.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Source data
Author contributions
A.T. conceived of the study and carried out imaging and MR analyses. C.W., F.A.A. and K.Mi. created relevant imaging data and confounds. T.N. and S.S. co-supervised the imaging analyses. S.Bu. and S.Be. co-supervised the MR analyses. K.Ma., A.W., K.P.E., I.H., S.S. and T.N. contributed to the interpretation of results. A.T. wrote the paper. All authors revised the paper.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Chang-Fu Kuo, Johnathan Cooper-Knock and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Data availability
Full pseudonymized participant data cannot be openly shared under the material transfer agreement with UK Biobank and ethics approval. Other researchers can apply for UK Biobank data to answer specific research questions. Further information about applying for data access can be obtained from the UK Biobank website (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk) or by emailing UK Biobank (ukbiobank@ukbiobank.ac.uk). Genetic summary statistics for serum urate, gout and brain imaging measures are freely available. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
No custom computer code or algorithm was used to generate the results reported in this paper R version 4.1.2 was used for most analyses (standard packages—survival v3.5-5, cmprsk v2.2-11, MendelianRandomization v0.7.0, TwoSampleMR v0.4.26, forester 0.5.0, ggforestplot available here: https://github.com/NightingaleHealth/ggforestplot). For VBM analyses, BLM was used (details in Supplementary Methods). Code for the VBM analyses performed is available at https://github.com/TomMaullin/BLMM.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-023-38602-6.
References
- 1.Kuo C-F, Grainge MJ, Zhang W, Doherty M. Global epidemiology of gout: prevalence, incidence and risk factors. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 2015;11:649–662. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2015.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Borghi C, et al. Hyperuricaemia and gout in cardiovascular, metabolic and kidney disease. Eur. J. Intern. Med. 2020;80:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ejim.2020.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cao Z, et al. Associations of BMI and serum urate with developing dementia: a prospective cohort study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2020;105:e4688–e4698. doi: 10.1210/clinem/dgaa638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Scheepers LE, et al. Urate and risk of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: a population-based study. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2019;15:754–763. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pan S-Y, Cheng R-J, Xia Z-J, Zhang Q-P, Liu Y. Risk of dementia in gout and hyperuricaemia: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. BMJ Open. 2021;11:e041680. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-041680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ames BN, Cathcart R, Schwiers E, Hochstein P. Uric acid provides an antioxidant defense in humans against oxidant-and radical-caused aging and cancer: a hypothesis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1981;78:6858–6862. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.11.6858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Şanlı, B. A. et al. Unbiased metabolome screen links serum urate to risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging120, 167–176 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 8.Efstathiadou A, Gill D, McGrane F, Quinn T, Dawson J. Genetically determined uric acid and the risk of cardiovascular and neurovascular diseases: a Mendelian randomization study of outcomes investigated in randomized trials. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2019;8:e012738. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.012738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Seminog OO, Goldacre MJ. Gout as a risk factor for myocardial infarction and stroke in England: evidence from record linkage studies. Rheumatology. 2013;52:2251–2259. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/ket293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Braskie, M. N., Ringman, J. M. & Thompson, P. M. Neuroimaging measures as endophenotypes in Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Alzheimer’s Dis.2011, 490140 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Latourte A, et al. Uric acid and incident dementia over 12 years of follow-up: a population-based cohort study. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 2018;77:328–335. doi: 10.1136/annrheumdis-2016-210767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kim JW, et al. Serum uric acid, Alzheimer-related brain changes, and cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2020;12:160. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Latourte A, Dumurgier J, Paquet C, Richette P. Hyperuricemia, gout, and the brain—an update. Curr. Rheumatol. Rep. 2021;23:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s11926-021-01050-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Verhaaren BF, et al. The relation of uric acid to brain atrophy and cognition: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;41:29–34. doi: 10.1159/000346606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tang, X., Song, Z.-H., Cardoso, M. A., Zhou, J.-B. & Simó, R. The relationship between uric acid and brain health from observational studies. Metabolic Brain Dis.37, 1989–2003 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 16.Guell X, Gabrieli JD, Schmahmann JD. Triple representation of language, working memory, social and emotion processing in the cerebellum: convergent evidence from task and seed-based resting-state fMRI analyses in a single large cohort. Neuroimage. 2018;172:437–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bagepally BS, et al. Decrease in cerebral and cerebellar gray matter in essential tremor: A voxel‐based morphometric analysis under 3T MRI. J. Neuroimaging. 2012;22:275–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2011.00598.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pietracupa, S., Bologna, M., Tommasin, S., Berardelli, A. & Pantano, P. The contribution of neuroimaging to the understanding of essential tremor pathophysiology: a systematic review. Cerebellum21, 1–23 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 19.Ayton S, et al. Brain iron is associated with accelerated cognitive decline in people with Alzheimer pathology. Mol. Psychiatry. 2020;25:2932–2941. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0375-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Thomas GEC, et al. Brain iron deposition is linked with cognitive severity in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2020;91:418–425. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2019-322042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Topiwala A, et al. Associations between moderate alcohol consumption, brain iron, and cognition in UK Biobank participants: observational and Mendelian randomization analyses. PLoS Med. 2022;19:e1004039. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1004039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ou, Y.-N. et al. The association of serum uric acid level, gout, and Alzheimer’s disease: a bidirectional Mendelian randomization study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis.89, 1–11 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 23.Albu A, Para I, Porojan M. Uric acid and arterial stiffness. Ther. Clin. risk Manag. 2020;16:39. doi: 10.2147/TCRM.S232033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mitchell GF, et al. Arterial stiffness, pressure and flow pulsatility and brain structure and function: the Age, gene/environment susceptibility–Reykjavik study. Brain. 2011;134:3398–3407. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ng KP, et al. Allopurinol is an independent determinant of improved arterial stiffness in chronic kidney disease: a cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e91961. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Pierce DR, Williams DK, Light KE. Purkinje cell vulnerability to developmental ethanol exposure in the rat cerebellum. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 1999;23:1650–1659. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04057.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Manto M. Toxic agents causing cerebellar ataxias. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2012;103:201–213. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-51892-7.00012-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.McFarland NR, Burdett T, Desjardins CA, Frosch MP, Schwarzschild MA. Postmortem brain levels of urate and precursors in Parkinson’s disease and related disorders. Neurodegener. Dis. 2013;12:189–198. doi: 10.1159/000346370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fatima T, et al. The relationship between ferritin and urate levels and risk of gout. Arthritis Res. Ther. 2018;20:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s13075-018-1668-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Yuan S, Larsson S. Causal associations of iron status with gout and rheumatoid arthritis, but not with inflammatory bowel disease. Clin. Nutr. 2020;39:3119–3124. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2020.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Min KH, Kang SO, Oh SJ, Han JM, Lee KE. Association between gout and dementia in the elderly: a nationwide population-based cohort study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2021;29:1177–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2021.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lee YH. Gout and the risk of Alzheimer′ s disease: a Mendelian randomization study. Int. J. Rheum. Dis. 2019;22:1046–1051. doi: 10.1111/1756-185X.13548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McKhann GM, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011;7:263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wardlaw JM, Valdés Hernández MC, Muñoz‐Maniega S. What are white matter hyperintensities made of? Relevance to vascular cognitive impairment. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2015;4:e001140. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.114.001140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Duguid J, De La Paz R, DeGroot J. Magnetic resonance imaging of the midbrain in Parkinson’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1986;20:744–747. doi: 10.1002/ana.410200618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fong TG, Davis D, Growdon ME, Albuquerque A, Inouye SK. The interface between delirium and dementia in elderly adults. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:823–832. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00101-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stern Y, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2020;16:1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Merriman TR, Joosten LA. CHIP and gout: trained immunity? Blood J. Am. Soc. Hematol. 2022;140:1054–1056. doi: 10.1182/blood.2022017212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Yuan H, Yang W. Genetically determined serum uric acid and Alzheimer’s disease risk. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018;65:1259–1265. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Munafò MR, Tilling K, Taylor AE, Evans DM, Davey Smith G. Collider scope: when selection bias can substantially influence observed associations. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2018;47:226–235. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Munafò MR, Davey Smith G. Robust research needs many lines of evidence. Nature. 2018;553:399–401. doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-01023-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fry A, et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2017;186:1026–1034. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Shaffer A, Rahn E, Saag K, Mudano A, Gaffo A. Variation in serum urate levels in the absence of gout and urate lowering therapy. BMC Rheumatol. 2021;5:1–7. doi: 10.1186/s41927-021-00202-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Topiwala, A., Ebmeier, K. P., Maullin-Sapey, T. & Nichols, T. E. Alcohol consumption and MRI markers of brain structure and function: cohort study of 25,378 UK Biobank participants. NeuroImage Clin.35, 103066 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Burgess S, Thompson SG, collaboration, C.C.g. Avoiding bias from weak instruments in Mendelian randomization studies. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2011;40:755–764. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Burgess S, Labrecque JA. Mendelian randomization with a binary exposure variable: interpretation and presentation of causal estimates. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2018;33:947–952. doi: 10.1007/s10654-018-0424-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Allen, N. E., Sudlow, C., Peakman, T., Collins, R. & biobank, UK. UK biobank data: come and get it. Sci. Transl. Med.6, 224ed224 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 48.Miller KL, et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1523–1536. doi: 10.1038/nn.4393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Alfaro-Almagro F, et al. Image processing and Quality Control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. Neuroimage. 2018;166:400–424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Griffanti L, et al. BIANCA (Brain Intensity AbNormality Classification Algorithm): a new tool for automated segmentation of white matter hyperintensities. Neuroimage. 2016;141:191–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Wang, C. et al. Phenotypic and genetic associations of quantitative magnetic susceptibility in UK Biobank brain imaging. Nat. Neurosci.25, 818–831 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 52.Douaud G, et al. Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain. 2007;130:2375–2386. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tin A, et al. Target genes, variants, tissues and transcriptional pathways influencing human serum urate levels. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:1459–1474. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0504-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sandoval-Plata G, Morgan K, Abhishek A. Variants in urate transporters, ADH1B, GCKR and MEPE genes associate with transition from asymptomatic hyperuricaemia to gout: results of the first gout versus asymptomatic hyperuricaemia GWAS in Caucasians using data from the UK Biobank. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 2021;80:1220–1226. doi: 10.1136/annrheumdis-2020-219796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Smith SM, et al. An expanded set of genome-wide association studies of brain imaging phenotypes in UK Biobank. Nat. Neurosci. 2021;24:737–745. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00826-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Herndon JE, Harrell FE., Jr The restricted cubic spline as baseline hazard in the proportional hazards model with step function time‐dependent covariables. Stat. Med. 1995;14:2119–2129. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780141906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fine JP, Gray RJ. A proportional hazards model for the subdistribution of a competing risk. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1999;94:496–509. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1999.10474144. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Li Q, et al. Diagnosis and treatment for hyperuricemia and gout: a systematic review of clinical practice guidelines and consensus statements. BMJ Open. 2019;9:e026677. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Full pseudonymized participant data cannot be openly shared under the material transfer agreement with UK Biobank and ethics approval. Other researchers can apply for UK Biobank data to answer specific research questions. Further information about applying for data access can be obtained from the UK Biobank website (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk) or by emailing UK Biobank (ukbiobank@ukbiobank.ac.uk). Genetic summary statistics for serum urate, gout and brain imaging measures are freely available. Source data are provided with this paper.
No custom computer code or algorithm was used to generate the results reported in this paper R version 4.1.2 was used for most analyses (standard packages—survival v3.5-5, cmprsk v2.2-11, MendelianRandomization v0.7.0, TwoSampleMR v0.4.26, forester 0.5.0, ggforestplot available here: https://github.com/NightingaleHealth/ggforestplot). For VBM analyses, BLM was used (details in Supplementary Methods). Code for the VBM analyses performed is available at https://github.com/TomMaullin/BLMM.