Skip to main content
Evolutionary Psychology logoLink to Evolutionary Psychology
. 2022 Jul 29;20(3):14747049221112657. doi: 10.1177/14747049221112657

Le Petit Machiavellian Prince: Effects of Latent Toxoplasmosis on Political Beliefs and Values

Robin Kopecky 1, Lenka Příplatová 1, Silvia Boschetti 1, Konrad Talmont-Kaminski 1, Jaroslav Flegr 2,
PMCID: PMC10303488  PMID: 35903902

Abstract

Humans infected by Toxoplasma gondii express no specific symptoms but manifest higher incidence of many diseases, disorders and differences in personality and behavior. The aim of this study was to compare the political beliefs and values of Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free participants. We measured beliefs and values of 2315 responders via an online survey (477 Toxoplasma-infected) using the Political Beliefs and Values Inventory (PI34). This study showed Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free participants of our cross-sectional study differed in three of four factors of PI34, scoring higher in Tribalism and lower in Cultural liberalism and Anti-Authoritarianism. We found sex differences in political beliefs associated with Toxoplasma infection. Infected women scored higher in tribalism and lower in cultural liberalism, compared with the Toxoplasma-free control group, while infected men scored higher in economic equity. These results fit with sexual differences in behavior and attitude observed after toxoplasmosis infection. Controlling for the effect of worse physical health and mental health had little impact, suggesting that impaired health did not cause these changes. Rather than adaptation to prevalence of parasites, as suggested by parasite-stress theory, the differences might be side-effects of long-term mild inflammatory reaction. However, to get clear picture of the mild inflammation effects, more research focused on different infectious diseases is needed.

Keywords: Toxoplasma gondii, manipulation hypothesis, political beliefs, stress, infectious diseases, parasite threat, pathogen avoidance

Introduction

Toxoplasma gondii, the parasitic protozoon of cats, infects about one-third of the human population in both developed and developing countries (Tenter et al., 2000). Infected humans express no specific symptoms of the life-long infection, however, they differ from non-infected subjects by a higher incidence of many diseases and disorders (Flegr & Escudero, 2016; Flegr, Klapilová, et al., 2014; Flegr, Prandota, et al., 2014) and also by specific changes in personality (Flegr, 2013; Khademvatan et al., 2013; Lindová et al., 2012) and behavior (Fekadu et al., 2010; Houdek, 2017). These behavioral differences are mostly considered to be products or by-products of the manipulative activity of Toxoplasma aimed to increase the chance of transmitting from the infected intermediate host, which can be any warm-blooded animal, to its definitive host, a cat (Webster, 2007). However, Toxoplasma infected animals, including humans, express many signs of the activated immune system. They have increased levels of some proinflammatory cytokines, especially IL-6 (Kaňková et al., 2010). It is known that inflammation, and independently also resulting stress have very strong impacts on various motivational, emotional, and behavioral processes, for recent reviews, see, e.g., Moieni & Eisenberger, 2018; Lasselin et al., 2018; Gassen & Hill, 2019. It was suggested (Lindova et al., 2010; Martynowicz et al., 2019; Tedford & McConkey, 2017) that a large part of the observed toxoplasmosis-associated changes are not the products of the manipulative activity of Toxoplasma but a result of mild chronic stress associated with long-term parasitic infection and resulting health problems. It is known that many toxoplasmosis-associated changes go in opposite directions in the infected men and women (Lindová et al., 2006), and also that men and women cope with stress in diametrically opposed ways (Matud, 2004; Tamres et al., 2016). The stress-coping hypothesis suggests that many toxoplasmosis-associated changes, e.g., higher extroversion in infected women and lower in men, or lower suspiciousness of infected women and higher in suspiciousness of infected men (all in comparison to non-infected peers) can be interpreted as gender-dependent stress coping reaction.

Additionally, the immune response to chronic stress due to parasite infection may elicit a different immune response in the two sexes and lead to different behavioral changes. The research focused on the role of cytokines in the inflammatory process of the CNS showed the presence of a difference between males and females (Bernardi et al., 2020; Lefèvre et al., 2012). The increment of cytokines during toxoplasmosis infection can affect the dopaminergic function (Felger & Miller, 2012) and provoke the behavioral change (Kamerkar & Davis, 2012). Indeed, many of the behavioral changes associated with toxoplasmosis are correlated with the dopaminergic system in humans and in rats (Flegr, 2007; Skallová et al., 2006). Therefore, the behavioral differences observed in infected men and women can be a consequence of differences in the immune system, related to the production of cytokines and the function of the dopaminergic system.

Among the traits affected by toxoplasmosis are also such that could affect the political beliefs of infected subjects. For example, infected men have lower superego strength, i.e., the tendency to disregard the rules of society, while the opposite tendency was observed in infected women (Flegr, 2007). Both infected men and women have lower conscientiousness (Lindová et al., 2012), lower generosity (Lindova et al., 2010) and have lower novelty-seeking (Flegr et al., 2003; Skallová et al., 2005). Men express lower and women higher altruism in the dictatorship game (Lindova et al., 2010). Infected subjects express higher entrepreneurship activity and have a higher tendency for risky behavior (Johnson et al., 2018), which might explain a higher risk of traffic accidents in the infected subjects (Flegr et al., 2002; Kocazeybek et al., 2009). Toxoplasma-infected women have also higher aggression (Cook et al., 2015) and self-aggression (Mathai et al., 2016; Postolache et al., 2021).

Although certain effects of infection by a parasitic disease on political views ought to be expected based on the research summarized above, to our knowledge, no relation of toxoplasmosis (or other parasitic diseases) and political beliefs and values has been reported to date. Moreover, studies focused on possible causal connection between infectious diseases (except for toxoplasmosis) and changes in personality profiles are also few and far between, and focused mainly on diseases causing profound changes in brain, e.g. AIDS (Meehan & Brush, 2016) or neurocysticercosis (El-Kady et al., 2021).

Regarding the connection between parasite stress and political views, there exists extensive literature describing changes in traits that differ with varied geographical parasite stress levels. All published studies, however, are population-based studies, searching for the difference between populations exposed to different intensity of the parasite stress. While it is possible to consider our data in the context of this parasite-stress theory and it will be done so in the Discussion, this study primarily focuses on association between Toxoplasma status and political beliefs on an individual level. For this purpose, we measured political beliefs and values of 2315 respondents of an internet survey (477 Toxoplasma-infected) using the Political Beliefs and Values Inventory (PI34). To test the hypothesis that the observed changes in three of the factor (Tribalism, Cultural Liberalism, and Anti-authoritarianism), but not the fourth (Economic Equity), are related to stress-coping strategies of men and women, we ran the analyses separately for each sex and to find out whether the observed differences are mediated by impaired health of the infected subjects we also analysed a complex model controlling not only for age, sex, size of place of residence, and level of education but also for physical health and mental health.

Results

The final data set of 2315 respondents, consisted of 467 men (mean age 35.54, s.d. 12.8) and 1848 women (mean age 35.95, s.d. 11.5); the difference in age was not significant (t = −0.69, df. = 2313, p = .493). In men, 90 men (mean age 35.54, s.d. 12.9) were infected with Toxoplasma and 377 (mean age 35.53, s.d. 12.4) were Toxoplasma-free; again, the difference in age was not significant (t = −0.69, df. = 2313, p = .493). In contrast, the difference in age between 518 Toxoplasma-infected women (mean age 38.39, s.d. 12.8) and 1330 Toxoplasma-free women (mean age 35.01, s.d. 11.2) was highly significant (t = −5.72, df. = 1846, p < .0001). Prevalence of toxoplasmosis was lower in men (19.27%) than in women (28.03%) (Chi2 = 14.8, p = .0001). Prevalence of toxoplasmosis decreased with size of place of residence from 32.9% in smallest villages to 20.9% in the cities over 500,000 inhabitants (Partial Tau controlled for age and sex = −0.070, p < .0001). No association was observed between toxoplasmosis and education (Partial Tau controlled for age and sex = −0.013, p = .368), however, the absence of the association in the whole population was caused by the existence of positive association between toxoplasmosis and achieved level of education in men (Partial Tau controlled for age and sex = 0.083, p = .007) and negative association between the toxoplasmosis and achieved level of education in women (partial Tau controlled for age and sex = −0.037, p = .017).

Toxoplasma-infected women expressed worse mental and physical health, and Toxoplasma-infected men worse physical health than their Toxoplasma-free peers (Figure 1). MANCOVA computed with the physical and mental health problems as the dependent variables, toxoplasmosis, sex, and sex-toxoplasmosis interaction as the factors, and age and size of place of living as covariates showed significant effects of age (F2,1843 = 70.02, p < <0.0001, partial eta2 = 0.07) and toxoplasmosis (F2,1843 = 7.34, p = .0007, partial eta2 = 0.008). Results of corresponding ANCOVA analyses computed separately for physical and mental health problems and men, women, and men and women together are shown in Table 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Association of toxoplasmosis and sex on health. Note. The vertical bars show 95% C.I., the mean values computed for covariates (age and size of place of living) at their means.

Table 1.

the Associations Between Physical and Mental Health and Toxoplasmosis.

Physical health problems Mental health problems
All F1,1846 p Partial eta2 F1,2301 p Partial eta2
Intercept 24.86 0.0000 0.013 10.89 0.0010 0.005
Age 64.94 0.0000 0.034 35.39 0.0000 0.015
Size of place of living 3.50 0.0615 0.002 0.51 0.4749 0.000
Toxoplasmosis 22.11 0.0000 0.012 6.64 0.0100 0.003
Men F1,353 p Partial eta2 F1,462 p Partial eta2
Intercept 9.99 0.0017 0.028 0.00 0.9539 0.000
Age 23.44 0.0000 0.062 6.75 0.0097 0.014
Size of place of living 2.16 0.1423 0.006 2.10 0.1484 0.005
Toxoplasmosis 5.95 0.0153 0.017 0.03 0.8681 0.000
Women F1,1489 p Partial eta2 F1,1835 p Partial eta2
Intercept 15.88 0.0001 0.011 11.67 0.0006 0.006
Age 43.40 0.0000 0.028 28.36 0.0000 0.015
Size of place of living 2.15 0.1423 0.001 0.17 0.6824 0.000
Toxoplasmosis 16.19 0.0001 0.011 6.32 0.0120 0.003

Note. The table shows the effects of toxoplasmosis, age, and size of place of living computed by ANCOVA for all subjects, men, and women. Statistically significant results were printed in bold.

Factor analysis performed on data from the Political Inventory revealed four factors with an eigenvalue higher than 1.0. According to loading of these factors by particular questions of the inventory (Table 2), it was possible to identify these factors as Tribalism (11.5% of variability), Economic equity preference (7.8% of variability), Cultural liberalism (5.1% of variability), and Anti-authoritarianism (3.0% of variability). We computed values of these four variables for each respondent as scores in particular factors.

Table 2.

Loadings of Four Factors Identified by Factor Analysis of 34 Questions of the Political Inventory.

Tribalism Economic Equity Cultural Liberalism Anti-authoritarianism
1 reduce income differences −0.029 0.768 −0.053 0.000
2 same sex together −0.255 −0.036 0.504 0.113
3 right to protest −0.062 0.004 0.082 0.466
4 to regulate corporation −0.072 0.289 0.057 −0.063
5 good for corp. is good for society 0.190 −0.156 −0.008 −0.069
6 minimize differences in status −0.007 0.800 −0.075 0.001
7 pornography −0.051 −0.058 0.351 0.302
8 citizens over foreigners 0.674 0.049 −0.086 0.099
9 minorities are threat 0.715 0.056 −0.128 0.004
10 class not nationality −0.228 0.172 0.121 0.018
11 rich are taxed too much 0.108 −0.416 0.032 0.047
12 religion not to be disgraced −0.088 0.128 −0.060 −0.428
13 decriminalize marihuana −0.108 0.006 0.186 0.264
14 euthanasia 0.170 0.037 0.644 0.149
15 parents can spank children 0.388 −0.100 0.019 0.064
16 there are no barbarians −0.309 0.131 0.123 −0.130
17 cancel borders and nations −0.466 0.194 0.094 0.098
18 immigrants won't assimilate 0.624 0.021 −0.128 0.034
19 no work no social benefits 0.448 −0.137 0.075 −0.042
20 to each according needs 0.056 0.363 0.046 −0.081
21 mothers stay at home 0.380 0.071 −0.305 −0.072
22 free speech 0.043 −0.097 0.080 0.554
23 no abortion 0.065 0.066 −0.601 −0.101
24 respect authority 0.368 −0.013 0.002 −0.242
25 proud of history 0.267 −0.030 −0.100 −0.032
26 poor but equal 0.108 0.664 −0.082 −0.104
27 free market is free society 0.110 −0.367 0.105 0.223
28 home-school −0.184 −0.030 −0.079 0.209
29 no to internet censorship 0.025 −0.067 0.096 0.453
30 an eye for an eye 0.610 0.074 0.164 −0.008
31 my life for the nation 0.244 −0.037 −0.180 0.047
32 capital punishment 0.635 0.062 0.228 −0.122
33 right to firearm 0.356 −0.178 −0.034 0.360
34 deport foreign criminals 0.412 0.023 −0.048 0.049

To disclose the effect of Toxoplasma infection on political beliefs, we computed the associations of the toxoplasmosis with Tribalism, economic equity preference, Cultural liberalism, and Anti-authoritarianism using partial Kendall correlation controlled to age, sex, and size of place of residence. The results showed that being Toxoplasma-infected correlated positively with Tribalism and negatively with Cultural liberalism, and Anti-authoritarianism, see Table 3. All three associations retain statistical significance after the correction for multiple tests. Table 3 also shows the results of the same analyses performed separately for men and women. The most conspicuous sexual differences were the absence of the positive association of toxoplasmosis with Tribalism in men – here the partial Tau was even negative, −0.014, p = .760 in men vs. 0.040, p = .009 in women, and the presence of the positive association with the preference for economic equity in men 0.064, p = .040 but not in women Tau = 0.010, p = .526, see Figure 2.

Table 3.

Correlation of Political Beliefs with Toxoplasmosis and Potential Confounding Variables.

Tribalism Economic Equity Cultural liberalism Anti-authoritarianism Tribalism Economic Equity Cultural liberalism Anti-authoritarianism
ALL ALL
Toxo 0.029 0.020 −0.042 −0.034 0.023 0.013 −0.046 −0.032
Age 0.024 −0.039 −0.104 −0.033 0.028 −0.023 −0.092 −0.022
sex 0.003 0.032 0.062 −0.234 0.003 0.031 0.054 −0.234
living −0.089 −0.054 0.063 0.027 −0.076 −0.045 0.058 0.026
education −0.098 −0.080 0.023 −0.008 −0.097 −0.076 0.024 −0.008
physical health 0.043 0.027 −0.002 −0.041 0.044 0.022 −0.014 −0.054
mental health −0.001 0.070 0.031 0.025 −0.010 0.063 0.033 0.030
WOMEN WOMEN
Toxo 0.040 0.010 −0.038 −0.030 0.030 −0.001 −0.042 −0.027
Age 0.022 −0.034 −0.105 −0.027 0.029 −0.021 −0.098 −0.022
living −0.092 −0.053 0.055 0.017 −0.079 −0.047 0.050 0.020
education −0.124 −0.084 0.025 −0.020 −0.121 −0.079 0.027 −0.022
physical health 0.058 0.036 −0.003 −0.043 0.057 0.028 −0.022 −0.034
mental health 0.013 0.073 0.056 0.001 0.000 0.065 0.060 0.004
MEN MEN
Toxo −0.015 0.064 −0.060 −0.055 −0.009 0.065 −0.071 −0.059
Age 0.022 −0.045 −0.079 −0.037 0.024 −0.028 −0.081 −0.025
living −0.080 −0.050 0.073 0.041 −0.073 −0.043 0.071 0.040
education −0.061 −0.074 0.020 0.012 −0.062 −0.072 0.020 0.014
physical health 0.022 0.015 −0.001 −0.041 0.021 0.016 −0.002 −0.079
mental health −0.024 0.066 −0.008 0.060 −0.028 0.061 −0.007 0.067

Note. The left half of the table shows partial Kendall Tau controlled for age, sex, size of place of residence, and the right half Tau controlled for age, sex, size of place of residence, physical health, mental health, and achieved education level. A variable that was used as the dependent variable) was removed from the list of covariates to be controlled for. Statistically significant results were printed in bold.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The violin plots show the means and the distribution computed as the kernel probability density for male and female, toxoplasma-infected and toxoplasma-free participants of the study.

Toxoplasmosis had a strong association with human health and the size of the place of residence. At the same time, political beliefs had a strong association with health, size of place of residence, and achieved level of education (Table 3, left part). It could be therefore speculated that the association between toxoplasmosis and health is just the side effects of the associations of toxoplasmosis with these confounding variables. To test this possibility, we repeated the partial Kendall analyses, this time controlled not only for sex, age, and size of place of residence but also for physical and mental health and achieved level of education. The results were nearly identical as those obtained with the simpler model (Table 3, right part). Therefore, the effect of toxoplasmosis on political beliefs was not mediated by the effect of toxoplasmosis on health or education.

Discussion

The present study showed that Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free participants of our cross-sectional study differed in three of four factors measured with Political inventory, namely scored higher in Tribalism and lower in Cultural liberalism and Anti-authoritarianism. These results are in line with previous broad research, showing that individuals in parasite affected areas are more likely to be conservative and authoritarian (Murray et al., 2013).

Furthermore, we observed sex differences in the studied factors associated with the Toxoplasma infection. Indeed, Toxoplasma-infected men scored higher in Economic Equity, showing a preference for a more equal and less competitive society, while women infected with toxoplasmosis scored higher in tribalism and lower in cultural liberalism. These associations were not reduced when the effect of worse physical health and mental health were controlled, suggesting that impaired health of infected subjects is not the cause of changes in political beliefs. The same conclusion was also supported by the fact that the changes go in the same direction in men and women, because stress coping-associated behavioral and personality changes mostly go in different directions in men and women.

It was suggested by Lindová et al, (2006, 2012) that these associations might be the results of a mild chronic stress caused by the toxoplasmosis infection instead of the result of the toxoplasmosis itself. The presence of a chronic stress not only explain the presence of behavioral and political differences with the non-infected control group, but also the presence of sex differences in these behavior and ideologies as different responses to chronic stress in the two sexes, involving differences in the immune system response and in the coping strategies used. Many of the behavioral changes observed in toxoplasmosis infected people correlate with the function of dopamine in the brain and they may have more broad implications, including political ideologies. In line with our results, previous studies (Flegr et al., 2003, Skallová et al., 2005) showed that infected subjects scored lower in novelty seeking, a factor that contributes to a conservative and political opinion (Carney et al., 2008). Indeed, in our sample the infection was associated with higher tribalism and lower cultural liberalism, specifically in women. While we expected differences in the political ideologies of infected men and women, we did not expect a higher score in economic equity in infected men. Typically, men affected with toxoplasmosis showed higher risk propensity and higher entrepreneurial activities (Johnson et al., 2018) more compatible with a competitive type of economy. The association of toxoplasmosis and the preference for an egalitarian economy in men needs to be better explored in future works.

Several studies have found that societies that are more affected by infectious pathogens also exhibit higher levels of conservative political attitudes such as xenophobia and traditionalism (Bennett & Nikolaev, 2020; Murray et al., 2013; Nikolaev & Salahodjaev, 2017; Thornhill et al., 2009). Similar results have also been found in our study performed on the individual level. The hypothesis that has been proposed is that the attitudes exhibited are connected to pathogen avoidance behaviors aimed at minimizing contact with outsiders (intergroup effect) (Aarøe et al., 2017) who may be carrying new pathogens as well as the maintenance of social traditions that may serve to help protect against pathogens (intragroup effect) (Fincher & Thornhill, 2012), with evidence from a recent cross-national study favoring the intragroup effect (Tybur et al., 2016). Significantly, however, both the effects – and the intragroup effect in particular – seem potentially open to the interpretation that they are generalized responses to stress, rather than to a pathogen (Brown et al., 2016; Currie & MacE, 2012; Hruschka & Henrich, 2013; Ma, 2020).

While the present study examines differences found in association between a parasitic infection and political values in context of increased stress at individual level, the results can be seen as an alternative to the parasite stress theory (Fincher et al., 2008; Thornhill & Fincher, p. 2014) for the following two main reasons: Parasite-stress theory aims to be the ultimate evolutionary explanation of changes in traits that differ with varied geographical parasite stress levels, yet this study focuses directly on the difference between actual infected and non-infected subjects in one small region, where the intensity of the parasite stress is mostly constant (and low). Second reason is that it has been shown that primarily non-zoonotic diseases have a relation to human personality and societal values (Thornhill et al., 2010). However, toxoplasmosis is primarily a zoonotic disease with very specific and limited spread between people – the only intrapersonal route of infection was suggested from male to female or between two male partners through sexual transmission (Flegr, Klapilová, et al., 2014; Flegr, Prandota, et al., 2014; Hlaváčová et al., 2021; Kaňková et al., 2020).

This being said, there might be a possible connection between the present study and the parasite-stress theory after all. An extensive body of research confirms association between infectious (and in most cases parasitic) diseases and changes in personality profile of animals from molluscs (Seaman & Briffa, 2015) and minnows (Kekäläinen et al., 2013) to migratory birds (Marinov et al., 2017) and mammals (Boyer et al., 2010) including men (Webster, 2001). There are also well studied associations between personality traits and political views, e.g. (Furnham & Fenton-O’Creevy, 2018; Harell et al., 2021; Verhulst et al., 2010; Wang, 2016). While the direction of causality needs to be studied further (Bakker et al., 2021) and while the human-centred field of parasite induced changes in personality traits is regrettably understudied and quite complex (Friedman, 2008), we might expect at least some effect of infectious diseases on political attitudes caused by shifts of personality traits. A possibility thus exists, that at least part of the reported difference in political attitudes in countries with different parasitic disease burdens is not caused by parasite-avoidance but results from a significant part of the population being infected with one or multiple pathogens. This hypothesis is supported by studies that linked a change in personality traits with clear connection to political attitudes (e.g., conservatism) with chronic diseases, although not the infectious ones (Mendelsohn et al., 1995; Sutin et al., 2013). On the other hand, some results suggest stronger prediction of personality traits by historical prevalence of diseases rather than by the current situation, suggesting parasite-avoidance as a factor with greater importance in personality shifts (Schaller & Murray, 2008).

Since the available body of literature discussing possible causal relationships between infectious diseases and political beliefs and values is very sparse, this direction of research might provide interesting and important insight into the changing political climate in certain countries. Studies focused on a wider range of infectious diseases besides toxoplasmosis and severe debilitating illnesses such as neurocysticercosis or AIDS would be especially valuable.

The present study showed a 19% prevalence of toxoplasmosis in men and 28% in women. The most recent Czech epidemiological study performed between 2014–2015 (Flegr, 2017) showed the prevalence of 25% in men and 36% in women aged 30–39 years. It is known, however, that prevalence of toxoplasmosis decreases relatively quickly in most developed countries, including the Czech Republic. For example, a large epidemiological study performed on Czech male soldiers 20 years ago found a prevalence of 35% for the age strata 30–35 years (Kolbekova et al., 2007). It is, therefore, possible that the observed seroprevalence reflects the actual situation in the Czech general population.

Limitations

The main limitation of the present study was the fact that the participants of the study were self-selected. Their subpopulation probably represents a specific (more altruistic and more curious) segment of the Czech population, rather than a random sample of the Czech internet population. In addition, people with impairments or severe diseases as well as those from the lowest socioeconomic strata were unable to participate. It is not therefore clear to which extent the results can be generalized to the general Czech (or the World) population.

Another limitation of the study is the moderate number of Toxoplasma-infected men (90). The reason of imbalanced sex ratio was the fact that women are often tested for toxoplasmosis during pregnancy and therefore a larger fraction of women than men know their toxoplasmosis status. Due to the lower number of men, the associations of toxoplasmosis with Cultural liberalism and Anti-authoritarianism were not significant in men, despite being stronger in men than in the more numerous (518) women.

Since this study dealt with the effect of pathogen-caused stress, there is a possible interference of the global COVID-19 pandemics. However, only 6% of the respondents participated in the period between April 2020 when the first infection in Czechia was observed, and the end of data collection in April 2021.

In the present study, we calculated aggregate indices of physical and mental health and used these indices in our statistical models. In future studies, it will be valuable to analyse the effects of individual health-related variables to disentangle the complex relationships between toxoplasmosis, mental and physical health, psychological traits and political beliefs. Such research could also answer important questions related to the causal direction of the observed correlations.

The associations found in the present study are based on correlations and we cannot infer the direction of causality. It cannot be ruled out that the explanation of the effect is in the opposite direction, that e.g., higher tribalism itself, by an unknown mechanism, increases the chance of being infected by Toxoplasma gondii. Like nearly all past studies, also this one was cross-sectional in nature. It is very difficult to study the relationship between Toxoplasma infection and personality using a longitudinal design. The frequency of the Toxoplasma infection in adulthood is low, and thousands of participants should be recruited to find several dozens of subjects who will acquire the infection during the study. Until such a study is performed, any conclusion about the causality behind the correlation between the infection and human personality must be based only on analogies with animal models (Skallová et al., 2006; Hodková et al., 2007) or on the existence of a correlation between length of infection and observed personality trait changes (Flegr et al., 1996, 2000) and must be therefore considered only provisional.

Further research is also needed to better clarify the extension and the implication of the associations we found between toxoplasmosis infection and political ideologies, and to clarify the role of sex differences.

Material and Methods

Subjects

The questionnaire was distributed via the internet as an online Qualtrics survey. Participants were invited to partake in the study using a social media-based snowball method (Kankova et al., 2015). Potential volunteers, mostly members of the “Lab bunnies” community, an 18,000-member group of Czech and Slovak nationals willing to take part in evolutionary psychology experiments, were invited to participate in an anonymous study about “mystical thinking, superstitions, prejudices, religion and the relation between various environmental factors and health and wellbeing.” The online questionnaire was also promoted in various types of electronic and printed media and TV. Toxoplasmosis and political belief were not explicitly mentioned in the information provided to potential participants in order not to affect the participants’ responses; this procedure was approved by the IRB, see below. They were, however, informed about that the data anonymously collected in the research will be health- and beliefs-related and gave their informed consent with participation in the experiment. Responders were not monetarily compensated for their participation in this study; nevertheless, after finishing the approximately 80 min questionnaire, they were provided with information about the results of related studies and their own results of several tests that were part of the questionnaire.

At the beginning of the survey, the participants were given the following information and were asked to provide their informed consent to participate in the study by pressing a special button: “The study is anonymous and obtained data will be used exclusively for scientific purposes. Your cooperation in the project is voluntary and you can terminate it at any time by closing this web page. You can also skip any uncomfortable questions; however, complete data is most valuable. If you agree to participate in the research press the “Next” button”. Only the subjects who provided their informed consent (95.2% of the visitors of the web page) were allowed to participate in the study. Between 27th May 2016 and 24th April 2021, 14,902 responders took part in the study.

All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. The project, including the method of obtaining electronic informed consent to participate in this anonymous study from all participants, was approved by the IRB anonymized.

Questionnaire

The electronic survey consisted of several parts that concerned various unrelated projects on evolutionary psychology and psychiatry. In the present study, we inspected and analysed only the data from the Political Inventory (see below), Toxoplasma seropositivity. Regarding toxoplasmosis, participants were asked whether they were laboratory tested for toxoplasmosis, what was the result of the laboratory test, and what was the reason for their test. Only participants who were tested due to pregnancy (62% of female participants), health reasons (28% male and 20% female participant), or as part of other research (60% male and 35% female participants) and were, therefore, informed about their toxoplasmosis status by a medical or research professional were included in the study. In the Czech Republic, IgG ELISA is always used in clinical diagnostic, however, in our research, the participants were tested with both ELISA and complement fixation test, therefore a certain fraction of participants were tested with both tests. To control for potential confounding variables, the responders were also asked about their sex, age, education (ordinal scale 1–8: 1– elementary, 8 – Ph.D.), and the size of the communities where they currently live (ordinal variable urbanization: 0 – less than 1000 inhabitants, 1: 1–5 thousand inhabitants, 2: 5–50 thousand inhabitants, 3: 50–100 thousand inhabitants, 4: 100–500 thousand inhabitants, 5: more than 500 thousand inhabitants).

In the anamnestic part of the questionnaire, the participants were also asked to respond to a set of questions concerning their physical health and mental health. They were asked how many different prescription drugs they were taking currently, how many different non-prescription drugs they were taking currently (“how many different herbs, food supplements, multivitamins, superfoods, etc. do you currently take per day”), how many times they visited their primary care doctor in past 365 days (“not for prevention”), how many times they used antibiotics in the past 365 days, and how many different medical specialists they visited (not for prevention) in the past five years. The physical health problems score was calculated as a mean of Z-scores of the last five variables. The responders were also requested to rate how much they suffer from anxieties, phobias, depression, mania, obsessions, auditory hallucinations, and visual hallucinations using a 0–100 scale. We also counted the number of diagnosed and the number of undiagnosed mental health disorders that the responders checked on a list of 25 mental health disorders and epilepsy. The mental health problems score was calculated as a mean of Z-scores of these 9 variables.

Political beliefs and values of the responders were measured with the Political Inventory (PI34). This instrument consisted of 34 questions (see English translation in appendix) in randomized order. The respondents rated the strength of agreement with statements on a scale of 1–5 (1 – strongly disagree, 2 – rather disagree, 3 – neither agree nor disagree, 4 – rather agree, 5 – strongly agree). The ratings were inversed for questions marked with r. Tribalism is calculated as the arithmetic means of responses to questions 8, 9, 15, 16r, 17r, 18, 19, 21, 24, 25, 30, 32, 34; Economic equity as the arithmetic mean of responses to questions 1, 4, 6, 11r, 20, 26, 27r; Cultural liberalism as the arithmetic mean of responses to questions 2, 7, 14, 21r, 23r; and Anti-authoritarianism as the arithmetic mean of responses to questions 3, 12r, 13, 22, 24r, 29, 33. Rather than examining actual partisanship related to established political parties or established ideologies, we assembled the Political Inventory based on contemporary public discourse in the Czech society as a culturally specific questionnaire was needed. Selection of topics for PI34 was inspired mainly by 10 Items in the ideological Consistency Scale by PEW based on the using set of questions since 1994 (Dimock et al., 2014) and the 12-item political and social conservatism scale (Everett, 2013).

Statistics

The raw data file contained the records of 14,902 participants, some of them, however, did not complete the final parts of the questionnaire with the political inventory. We filtered out all subjects younger than 18 and older than 90 years. We also filtered out all records with suspicious data, e.g., those from subjects reporting too low (< 130 cm) or high (> 215 cm) body height and too high number of reported diagnosed or undiagnosed mental health disorders (>10). This data set was used for computing physical health problem scores, mental health score (see above), and for factor analyses. In the main analyses, we used the factors (scores) computed with the Maximum likelihood factors extraction method after varimax rotation, however, the results were very robust; a nearly identical structure of factors was obtained with another method of factor extraction and other methods of factor rotation. In fact, all four factors (plus four other factors) were identified even by the principal component analysis.

For analysing the association between toxoplasmosis and political beliefs we used a core data set consisting of 2315 responders who had been tested for toxoplasmosis in the past and were able to provide us with the information about the results of their laboratory test. About 39% of participants were tested in our laboratory during other scientific studies, 29% during pregnancy, 22% because of their health problems, and 10% for other reasons, e.g., curiosity, blood donation, their partner's pregnancy, etc. Some output variables had asymmetric distribution; therefore, we performed all focal analyses with a partial Kendall test that is not sensitive to such irregularity in data. The correction for multiple tests was performed with the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure with a False Discovery Rate set to 0.1 (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Partial Kendall tests were performed in R 3.3.1 (R Core Team, 2018) using the package ppcor (Kim, 2015). All other tests, factor analyses, and PCA analyses were performed with the statistical package Statistica v. 10.0.

Conclusions

Beside worse mental health in Toxoplasma-infected women and worse physical health of Toxoplasma-infected participants of both sexes, our study showed sex-dependent differences in Tribalism, Cultural liberalism, and Anti-authoritarianism between Toxoplasma-positive and Toxoplasma-negative subjects. Specifically, we have found positive association of toxoplasmosis and Tribalism in women but not in men, and a positive association of toxoplasmosis and preference for social equity in men but not in women, in both sexes, there were also negative associations with Cultural liberalism and Anti-Authoritarianism. While our results are in agreement with the parasite-stress theory that predicts higher Authoritarianism and Conservatism in areas with higher prevalence of parasitic diseases, the results cannot be interpreted simply in the framework of this theory, i.e., as a result of enhanced behavioral immunity (parasite avoidance). Rather, the direct effect of long-term mild stress caused by an inflammatory reaction to the presence of a parasite may be at play, shifting hosts’ personalities and thus also their political attitudes. Considering the substantial lack of literature discussing effects of mild infectious diseases on personality traits of infected individuals as well as effects of being infected by a parasite on political beliefs and values, further research should be encouraged.

Appendix A

Political beliefs and values inventory Pi34

  1. The gap between high and low incomes should be reduced.

  2. It is acceptable for a same sex couple to live together.

  3. Everyone should have the right to publicly protest against or for something.

  4. Because we cannot trust corporations that they will respect environmental protection voluntarily, it is necessary to regulate them by law.

  5. What is good for big corporations is in consequence good for society.

  6. Differences in wealth and social status should be as small among people as possible.

  7. If I were married, I wouldn’t mind my partner watching pornography alone.

  8. My government should value the lives of its citizens more than the lives of foreigners.

  9. Minorities of different nationalities are a threat for our culture.

  10. People are divided more by their socioeconomic status than by their nationality.

  11. People with high incomes are paying overly high taxes.

  12. It should be forbidden to publicly insult someone's religion.

  13. Growing marihuana for one's own use should not be criminalized.

  14. People should have the right to end their life with the help of a physician if they ask for it.

  15. Parents should be allowed to physically punish their children.

  16. There are no savages, barbarians or primitive cultures in the world, just different kinds of culture.

  17. I wish that nations and state borders cease to exist, so we become members of one big group.

  18. The first generation and the majority of the second generation of immigrants will not completely integrate in their new country.

  19. For those who are able but not willing to work, there should be no social benefits.

  20. It should be held true: “From each according to their ability, to each according to their needs.”

  21. Mothers of children can have their own career; however, their primary role is to take good care of children and the household.

  22. Free speech is more important than the possibility of someone feeling deeply offended.

  23. If a mother’s life is not endangered, abortion should be prohibited by law.

  24. It is necessary to teach children to respect authorities and superiors.

  25. I am proud of the history of my country.

  26. A society that is poor but equal is better than a rich society that is stratified in income and wealth.

  27. The freer the market the freer the society.

  28. It is acceptable that parents prefer their children to be educated at home instead of sending them to school.

  29. The state shouldn't have the right to censor the internet.

  30. It should be held true: “An eye for an eye, a tooth for a tooth.”

  31. I would sacrifice my life for my nation!

  32. Capital punishment should be re-introduced for exceptionally serious crimes.

  33. Adults with clean criminal records should have a right to own a firearm.

  34. Foreigners who commit a criminal offense in our country should be deported to their country of origin.

1 2 3 4 5
strongly disagree rather disagree neither agree nor disagree rather agree strongly agree

Scales (Arithmetic Mean of Responses):

Tribalism: 8, 9, 15, 16r, 17r, 18, 19, 21, 24, 25, 30, 32, 34;

Economic equity: 1, 4, 6, 11r, 20, 26, 27r;

Cultural liberalism: 2, 7, 14, 21r, 23r;

Anti-authoritarianism: 3, 12r, 13, 22, 24r, 29, 33.

r: Reversed scoring (scale 5–1, not 1–5).

Author Contribution: Conceptualization RK, LP, JF; methodology RK, LP, SB, KTK, JF; data curation JF; statistical analysis JF; writing - original draft and reviewing and editing RK, LP, SB, KTK, JF. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by the Grantová Agentura České Republiky, (grant number 21-31059S).

Institutional Review Board Statement: This study was approved by the IRB of the Faculty of Science, Charles University (Komise pro práci s lidmi a lidským materiálem Přírodovědecké Fakulty Univerzity Karlovy)—No. 2015/07

Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability

Dataset available on Figshare.com https://figshare.com/s/fb78539b38e11db6f73f

References

  1. Aarøe L., Petersen M. B., Arceneaux K. (2017). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: Why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration. American Political Science Review, 111(2), 277–294. 10.1017/S0003055416000770 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bakker B. N., Lelkes Y., Malka A. (2021). Reconsidering the link between self-reported personality traits and political preferences. American Political Science Review, 115(4), 1–17. 10.1017/S0003055421000605 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. 10.1111/J.2517-6161.1995.TB02031.X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bennett D. L., Nikolaev B. (2020). Historical disease prevalence, cultural values, and global innovation. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 45(1), 145–174. 10.1177/1042258720914506 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bernardi S., Toffoli B., Tonon F., Francica M., Campagnolo E., Ferretti T., Comar S., Giudici F., Stenner E., Fabris B. (2020). Sex differences in proatherogenic cytokine levels. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 21(11), 3861. 10.3390/IJMS21113861 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boyer N., Réale D., Marmet J., Pisanu B., Chapuis J.-L. (2010). Personality, space use and tick load in an introduced population of siberian chipmunks tamias sibiricus. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79(3), 538–547. 10.1111/J.1365-2656.2010.01659.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown G. D. A., Fincher C. L., Walasek L. (2016). Personality, parasites, political attitudes, and cooperation: A model of how infection prevalence influences openness and social group formation. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 98–117. 10.1111/TOPS.12175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carney D. R., Jost J. T., Gosling S. D., Potter J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29(6), 807–840. 10.1111/J.1467-9221.2008.00668.X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cook T. B., Brenner L. A., Cloninger C. R., Langenberg P., Igbide A., Giegling I., Hartmann A. M., Konte B., Friedl M., Brundin L., Groer M. W., Can A., Rujescu D., Postolache T. T. (2015). “Latent” infection with Toxoplasma gondii: Association with trait aggression and impulsivity in healthy adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 60, 87–94. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Currie T. E., MacE R. (2012). Analyses do not support the parasite-stress theory of human sociality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 83–85. 10.1017/S0140525X11000963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. El-Kady A. M., Allemailem K. S., Almatroudi A., Abler B., Elsayed M. (2021). Psychiatric disorders of neurocysticercosis: Narrative review. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 17, 1599–1610. 10.2147/NDT.S306585 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Everett J. A. C. (2013). The 12 item social and economic conservatism scale (SECS). PLOS ONE, 8(12), e82131. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0082131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fekadu A., Shibre T., Cleare A. J. (2010). Toxoplasmosis as a cause for behaviour disorders - overview of evidence and mechanisms. Folia Parasitologica, 57(2), 105–113. 10.14411/fp.2010.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Felger J. C., Miller A. H. (2012). Cytokine effects on the basal ganglia and dopamine function: The subcortical source of inflammatory malaise. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 33(3), 315–327. 10.1016/J.YFRNE.2012.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fincher C. L., Thornhill R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 61–79. 10.1017/S0140525X11000021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fincher C. L., Thornhill R., Murray D. R., Schaller M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1640), 1279–1285. 10.1098/RSPB.2008.0094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Flegr J. (2007). Effects of toxoplasma on human behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 757–760. 10.1093/schbul/sbl074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Flegr J. (2013). Influence of latent toxoplasma infection on human personality, physiology and morphology: Pros and cons of the toxoplasma–human model in studying the manipulation hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 216(1), 127–133. 10.1242/JEB.073635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Flegr J. (2017). Predictors of toxoplasma gondii infection in Czech and Slovak populations: The possible role of cat-related injuries and risky sexual behavior in the parasite transmission. Epidemiology & Infection, 145(7), 1351–1362. 10.1017/S095026881700019X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Flegr J., Escudero D. Q. (2016). Impaired health status and increased incidence of diseases in toxoplasma-seropositive subjects - an explorative cross-sectional study. Parasitology, 143(14), 1974–1989. 10.1017/S0031182016001785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Flegr J., Havlícek J., Kodym P., Malý M., Smahel Z. (2002). Increased risk of traffic accidents in subjects with latent toxoplasmosis: A retrospective case-control study. BMC Infectious Diseases, 2(1), 11. 10.1186/1471-2334-2-11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Flegr J., Klapilová K., Kaňková S. (2014). Toxoplasmosis can be a sexually transmitted infection with serious clinical consequences. Not all routes of infection are created equal. Medical Hypotheses, 83(3), 286–289. 10.1016/J.MEHY.2014.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Flegr J., Kodym P., Tolarová V. (2000). Correlation of duration of latent toxoplasma gondii infection with personality changes in women. Biological Psychology, 53(1), 57–68. 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00034-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Flegr J., Prandota J., Sovičková M., Israili Z. H. (2014). Toxoplasmosis – A global threat. Correlation of latent toxoplasmosis with specific disease burden in a set of 88 countries. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e90203. 10.1371/journal.pone.0090203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Flegr J., Preiss M., Klose J., Havlíček J., Vitáková M., Kodym P. (2003). Decreased level of psychobiological factor novelty seeking and lower intelligence in men latently infected with the protozoan parasite toxoplasma gondii dopamine, a missing link between schizophrenia and toxoplasmosis? Biological Psychology, 63(3), 253–268. 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00075-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Flegr J., Zitková Š, Kodym P., Frynta D. (1996). Induction of changes in human behaviour by the parasitic protozoan toxoplasma gondii. Parasitology, 113(1), 49–54. 10.1017/S0031182000066269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Friedman H. S. (2008). The multiple linkages of personality and disease. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 22(5), 668–675. 10.1016/J.BBI.2007.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Furnham A., Fenton-O’Creevy M. (2018). Personality and political orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 129, 88–91. 10.1016/J.PAID.2018.03.020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harell A., Hinckley R., Mansell J. (2021). Valuing liberty or equality? Empathetic personality and political intolerance of harmful speech. Frontiers in Political Science, 3, 64. 10.3389/FPOS.2021.663858 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gassen J., Hill S. E. (2019). Why inflammation and the activities of the immune system matter for social and personality psychology (and not only for those who study health). Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(6), e12471. 10.1111/spc3.12471 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hlaváčová J., Flegr J., Řežábek K., Calda P., Kaňková Š. (2021). Male-to-Female presumed transmission of toxoplasmosis between sexual partners. American Journal of Epidemiology, 190(3), 386–392. 10.1093/AJE/KWAA198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hodková H., Kodym P., Flegr J. (2007). Poorer results of mice with latent toxoplasmosis in learning tests: Impaired learning processes or the novelty discrimination mechanism? Parasitology, 134(10), 1329–1337. 10.1017/S0031182007002673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Houdek P. (2017). Puppet master: Possible influence of the parasite toxoplasma gondii on managers and employees. Academy of Management Perspectives, 31(1), 63–81. 10.5465/amp.2015.0163 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hruschka D. J., Henrich J. (2013). Institutions, parasites and the persistence of in-group preferences. PLOS ONE, 8(5), e63642. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0063642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Johnson S. K., Fitza M. A., Lerner D. A., Calhoun D. M., Beldon M. A., Chan E. T., Johnson P. T. J. (2018). Risky business: Linking toxoplasma gondii infection and entrepreneurship behaviours across individuals and countries. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1883), 20180822. 10.1098/RSPB.2018.0822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kamerkar S., Davis P. H. (2012). Toxoplasma on the brain: Understanding host-pathogen interactions in chronic CNS infection. Journal of Parasitology Research, 2012(1), 1–10. 10.1155/2012/589295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaňková Š, Hlaváčová J., Flegr J. (2020). Oral sex: A new, and possibly the most dangerous, route of toxoplasmosis transmission. Medical Hypotheses, 141, 109725. 10.1016/J.MEHY.2020.109725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaňková Š, Holáň V., Zajícová A., Kodym P., Flegr J. (2010). Modulation of immunity in mice with latent toxoplasmosis—the experimental support for the immunosuppression hypothesis of toxoplasma-induced changes in reproduction of mice and humans. Parasitology Research, 107(6), 1421–1427. 10.1007/s00436-010-2013-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kankova S., Flegr J., Calda P. (2015). The influence of latent toxoplasmosis on women’s reproductive function: Four cross-sectional studies. Folia Parasitologica, 62(1), 056. 10.14411/fp.2015.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kekäläinen J., Lai Y.-T., Vainikka A., Sirkka I., Kortet R. (2013). Do brain parasites alter host personality? — experimental study in minnows. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68(2), 197–204. 10.1007/S00265-013-1634-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Khademvatan S., Khajeddin N., Saki J., Izadi-Mazidi S. (2013). Effect of toxoplasmosis on personality profiles of Iranian men and women. South African Journal of Science, 109(1–2), 1–4. 10.1590/sajs.2013/0017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kim S. (2015). Ppcor: An R package for a fast calculation to semi-partial correlation coefficients. Communications for Statistical Applications and Methods, 22(6), 665–674. 10.5351/CSAM.2015.22.6.665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kocazeybek B., Oner Y. A., Turksoy R., Babur C., Cakan H., Sahip N., Unal A., Ozaslan A., Kilic S., Saribas S., Aslan M., Taylan A., Koc S., Dirican A., Uner H. B., Oz V., Ertekin C., Kucukbasmaci O., Torun M. M., … (2009). Higher prevalence of toxoplasmosis in victims of traffic accidents suggest increased risk of traffic accident in toxoplasma-infected inhabitants of Istanbul and its suburbs. Forensic Science International, 187(1–3), 103–108. 10.1016/J.FORSCIINT.2009.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kolbekova P., Kourbatova E., Novotna M., Kodym P., Flegr J. (2007). New and old risk-factors for toxoplasma gondii infection: Prospective cross-sectional study among military personnel in the Czech Republic. Clinical Microbiology and Infection, 13(10), 1012–1017. 10.1111/j.1469-0691.2007.01771.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lasselin J., Lekander M., Axelsson J., Karshikoff B. (2018). Sex differences in how inflammation affects behavior: What we can learn from experimental inflammatory models in humans. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 50, 91–106. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lefèvre N., Corazza F., Duchateau J., Desir J., Casimir G. (2012). Sex differences in inflammatory cytokines and CD99 expression following in vitro lipopolysaccharide stimulation. Shock (Augusta, Ga.), 38(1), 37–42. 10.1097/SHK.0B013E3182571E46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lindova J., Kubena A. A., Sturcova H., Krivohlava R., Novotna M., Rubesova A., Havlicek J., Kodym P., Flegr J. (2010). Pattern of money allocation in experimental games supports the stress hypothesis of gender differences in Toxoplasma gondii-induced behavioural changes. Folia Parasitologica, 57(2), 136–142. 10.14411/fp.2010.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lindová J., Novotná M., Havlíček J., Jozífková E., Skallová A., Kolbeková P., Hodný Z., Kodym P., Flegr J. (2006). Gender differences in behavioural changes induced by latent toxoplasmosis. International Journal for Parasitology, 36(14), 1485–1492. 10.1016/j.ijpara.2006.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lindová J., Příplatová L., Flegr J. (2012). Higher extraversion and lower conscientiousness in humans infected with toxoplasma. European Journal of Personality, 26(3), 285–291. 10.1002/per.838 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ma M. Z. (2020). Testing the parasite-stress theory of sociality based on the circular model of human values: A multilevel analysis approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110277–110277. 10.1016/J.PAID.2020.110277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Marinov M. P., Marchetti C., Dimitrov D., Ilieva M., Zehtindjiev P. (2017). Mixed haemosporidian infections are associated with higher fearfulness in Yellow Wagtail (Motacilla flava). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 95(6), 405–410. 10.1139/CJZ-2016-0121 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Martynowicz J., Augusto L., Wek R. C., Boehm S. L., Sullivan W. J. (2019). Guanabenz reverses a key behavioral change caused by latent toxoplasmosis in mice by reducing neuroinflammation. MBio, 10(2), 2. 10.1128/mBio.00381-19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mathai A. J., Lowry C. A., Cook T. B., Brenner L. A., Brundin L., Groer M. W., Peng X., Giegling I., Hartmann A. M., Konte B., Friedl M., Fuchs D., Rujescu D., Postolache T. T. (2016). Reciprocal moderation by Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and blood phenylalanine – tyrosine ratio of their associations with trait aggression. Pteridines, 27(3–4), 77–85. 10.1515/PTERID-2016-0006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Matud M. P. (2004). Gender differences in stress and coping styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(7), 1401–1415. 10.1016/J.PAID.2004.01.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Meehan R. A., Brush J. A. (2016). An overview of AIDS dementia complex. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementiasr, 16(4), 225–229. 10.1177/153331750101600411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mendelsohn G. A., Dakof G. A., Skaff M. (1995). Personality change in Parkinson’s disease patients: Chronic disease and aging. Journal of Personality, 63(2), 233–257. 10.1111/J.1467-6494.1995.TB00809.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Moieni M., Eisenberger N. I. (2018). Effects of inflammation on social processes and implications for health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1428(1), 5–13. 10.1111/nyas.13864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Murray D. R., Schaller M., Suedfeld P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLOS ONE, 8(5), e62275. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0062275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Nikolaev B., Salahodjaev R. (2017). Historical prevalence of infectious diseases, cultural values, and the origins of economic institutions. Kyklos, 70(1), 97–128. 10.1111/KYKL.12132 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pew Research Center, Dimock M., Kiley J., Keeler S., Doherty C. (2014). Political polarization in the American public - Appendix A: The Ideological Consistency Scale . Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2014/06/12/appendix-a-the-ideological-consistency-scale/
  61. Postolache T. T., Wadhawan A., Rujescu D., Hoisington A. J., Dagdag A., Baca-Garcia E., Lowry C. A., Okusaga O. O., Brenner L. A. (2021). Toxoplasma gondii, suicidal behavior, and intermediate phenotypes for suicidal behavior. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 806. 10.3389/FPSYT.2021.665682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing . R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.r-project.org/.
  63. Schaller M., Murray D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 212–221. 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Seaman B., Briffa M. (2015). Parasites and personality in periwinkles (Littorina littorea): Infection status is associated with mean-level boldness but not repeatability. Behavioural Processes, 115, 132–134. 10.1016/J.BEPROC.2015.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Skallová A., Kodym P., Frynta D., Flegr J. (2006). The role of dopamine in Toxoplasma-induced behavioural alterations in mice: An ethological and ethopharmacological study. Parasitology, 133(5), 525–535. 10.1017/S0031182006000886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Skallová A., Novotná M., Kolbeková P., Gašová Z., Veselý V., Sechovská M., Flegr J. (2005). Decreased level of novelty seeking in blood donors infected with toxoplasma. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 26(5), 480–486. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sutin A. R., Zonderman A. B., Ferrucci L., Terracciano A. (2013). Personality traits and chronic disease: Implications for adult personality development. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 68(6), 912–920. 10.1093/GERONB/GBT036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tamres L. K., Janicki D., Helgeson V. S. (2016). Sex differences in coping behavior: A meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(1), 2–30. 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tedford E., McConkey G. (2017). Neurophysiological changes induced by chronic Toxoplasma gondii infection. Pathogens (Basel, Switzerland), 6(2), 19. 10.3390/pathogens6020019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tenter A. M., Heckeroth A. R., Weiss L. M. (2000). Toxoplasma gondii: From animals to humans. International Journal for Parasitology, 30(12–13), 1217–1258. 10.1016/S0020-7519(00)00124-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Thornhill R., Fincher C. L. (2014). The parasite-stress theory of sociality, the behavioral immune system, and human social and cognitive uniqueness. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 257–264. 10.1037/EBS0000020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Thornhill R., Fincher C. L., Aran D. (2009). Parasites, democratization, and the liberalization of values across contemporary countries. Biological Reviews, 84(1), 113–131. 10.1111/J.1469-185X.2008.00062.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Thornhill R., Fincher C. L., Murray D. R., Schaller M. (2010). Zoonotic and non-zoonotic diseases in relation to human personality and societal values: Support for the parasite-stress model. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(2), 151–169. 10.1177/147470491000800201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tybur J. M., Inbar Y., Aarøe L., Barclay P., Barlow F. K., de Barra M., Becker D. V., Borovoi L., Choi I., Choi J. A., Consedine N. S., Conway A., Conway J. R., Conway P., Adoric V. C., Demirci D. E., Fernández A. M., Ferreira D. C. S., Ishii K., … I. Žeželj, (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 12408–12413. 10.1073/PNAS.1607398113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Verhulst B., Hatemi P. K., Martin N. G. (2010). The nature of the relationship between personality traits and political attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(4), 306–316. 10.1016/J.PAID.2009.11.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wang C. H. (2016). Personality traits, political attitudes and vote choice: Evidence from the United States. Electoral Studies, 44, 26–34. 10.1016/J.ELECTSTUD.2016.07.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Webster J. P. (2001). Rats, cats, people and parasites: The impact of latent toxoplasmosis on behaviour. Microbes and Infection, 3(12), 1037–1045. 10.1016/S1286-4579(01)01459-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Webster J. P. (2007). The effect of Toxoplasma gondii on animal behavior: Playing cat and mouse. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 752–756. 10.1093/schbul/sbl073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Dataset available on Figshare.com https://figshare.com/s/fb78539b38e11db6f73f


Articles from Evolutionary Psychology are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES