Abstract
Alterations of the limbic system may be present in the chronic phase of SARS-CoV-2 infection. Our aim was to study the long-term impact of this disease on limbic system–related behaviour and its associated brain functional connectivity, according to the severity of respiratory symptoms in the acute phase. To this end, we investigated the multimodal emotion recognition abilities of 105 patients from the Geneva COVID-COG Cohort 223 days on average after SARS-CoV-2 infection (diagnosed between March 2020 and May 2021), dividing them into three groups (severe, moderate or mild) according to respiratory symptom severity in the acute phase. We used multiple regressions and partial least squares correlation analyses to investigate the relationships between emotion recognition, olfaction, cognition, neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional brain networks. Six to 9 months following SARS-CoV-2 infection, moderate patients exhibited poorer recognition abilities than mild patients for expressions of fear (P = 0.03 corrected), as did severe patients for disgust (P = 0.04 corrected) and irritation (P < 0.01 corrected). In the whole cohort, these performances were associated with decreased episodic memory and anosmia, but not with depressive symptoms, anxiety or post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuroimaging revealed a positive contribution of functional connectivity, notably between the cerebellum and the default mode, somatosensory motor and salience/ventral attention networks. These results highlight the long-term consequences of SARS-Cov-2 infection on the limbic system at both the behavioural and neuroimaging levels.
Keywords: post-COVID syndrome, emotion, neuropsychological deficits, MRI, functional connectivity
Thomasson et al. reported that patients performed poorly on multimodal emotion recognition 6–9 months after SARS-CoV-2 infection. This poor performance was associated with functional MRI markers, particularly in structures within the limbic and cerebellar systems. Taken together, the results may provide evidence of long-term damage by SARS-CoV-2 to the CNS.
See A. Matias-Guiu and Díez-Cirarda (https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcad189) for a scientific commentary on this article.
Graphical Abstract
See A. Matias-Guiu and Díez-Cirarda (https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcad189) for a scientific commentary on this article.
Introduction
Behavioural and neuroimaging data point to alterations of the limbic system in the chronic phase of COVID-19.1–6 Regarding commonly reported sequelae other than the well documented olfactory7 and episodic memory disorders,1,2,8–12 evidence has emerged suggesting diminished emotion recognition abilities.1,13 Authors have recently shown that some patients who had moderate or severe symptoms in the acute phase of COVID-19 exhibit a general reduction in their ability to recognize emotional stimuli 6–9 months later.2,8 This suggests an effect of SARS-CoV-2 infection on affective recognition abilities that goes beyond the impact of hospitalization in an intensive care unit (ICU),14 but to date, no study has sought to determine whether these results are driven by specific emotions. Neuroimaging studies in patients with COVID-19 have used a variety of methods, including fluorodeoxyglucose (18FDG) PET and MRI, to highlight hypometabolism or hypoconnectivity in cortical–subcortical brain regions and networks,1,3–5,13,15 many belonging to the limbic system, as well as alterations in the cerebellum.16 Moreover, a recent study highlighted a reduction in grey-matter thickness in the orbitofrontal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus, as well as tissue damage in regions that are functionally connected to the primary olfactory cortex—regions that are also known to form part of the limbic system.17 However, only a small number of studies have correlated cognitive and neuropsychiatric variables with functional neuroimaging data,18 and studies of emotion recognition abilities have so far focused solely on behavioural data.2,8 None have explored the relationships between multimodal emotion recognition and structural and functional neuroimaging data.
Although previous results suggest that the virus directly (or more likely indirectly) attacks the CNS, and presumably the limbic system in particular, the potential effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms on long-term limbic-related functions has yet to be investigated. Many neuropsychiatric disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), anxiety and depressive symptoms, have been described following SARS-CoV-2 infection, as well as fatigue and sleep disturbances,19 and are well known to have an impact on emotion recognition abilities.20–24 To the best of our knowledge, no study has yet assessed the impact of relevant secondary neuropsychiatric variables on the recognition of individual multimodal emotions, as a function of the severity of respiratory symptoms in the acute phase. In this context, tasks assessing emotion processing abilities could be of interest. Only two studies have behaviourally assessed long-term emotion recognition abilities following SARS-CoV-2 infection, highlighting reduced performance in hospitalized versus non-hospitalized groups of patients.2,8 That said, these studies were carried out on overall scores and did not assess performances for individual emotions.
According to recent models of emotions, particularly the process model of emotions, each emotion has distinct properties and involves five distinct functions (appraisal, automatic physiology, action tendencies, motor expression and subjective feeling).25 The limbic circuits, involving the orbitofrontal cortex, insula and amygdala, inferior temporal lobe and subcortical regions, have historically been described as forming the neural base of emotional processes, as well as memory functions.26,27 Nevertheless, recent evidence suggests that there is not one limbic system, but several differentiated limbic systems for emotional processes, involving anterior structures of the limbic system, orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in emotion processing, reward assessment and decision-making.28 Meanwhile, memory functions are underpinned by the hippocampus and the limbic structures to which it is connected, including the posterior cingulate cortex and the fornix–mammillary body–anterior thalamus–posterior cingulate circuit.28 Finally, these processes also seem to be underpinned by cerebellar structures, highlighting the close interaction between the limbic systems and the cerebellum.29
In this context, the present study was conducted to examine the impact of the severity of respiratory symptoms in the acute phase of COVID-19 on emotion recognition abilities 6–9 months post-infection (aim 1) and to explore the influence of secondary behavioural variables (e.g. olfaction, memory or depressive symptoms) (aim 2), as well as functional brain connectivity (aim 3). To this end, we assessed multimodal emotion recognition in 105 patients 223.07 ± 41.69 days following SARS-CoV-2 infection. We ran regression analyses to investigate the potential predictive value of neuropsychological functions sustained by the limbic system (verbal and visual episodic memory), neuropsychiatric manifestations (PTSD, anxiety, depressive symptoms, fatigue and sleep disorders) and olfactory performances. Finally, 45 of these 105 patients underwent functional MRI, and exploratory partial least squares correlation (PLSC) analyses were performed to identify associations between multimodal emotion recognition abilities and functional brain networks.
Materials and methods
Participants
Data from 105 patients were extracted from the COVID-COG database of Geneva University Hospitals (HUG).1,2,8,13,30 For each patient, we carried out a medical file review, followed by a telephone call inviting the patient to take part in the study, if all the eligibility criteria were met. Exclusion criteria were a history of neurological issues, neuropsychiatric disorders, cancer, neurodevelopmental pathologies, pregnancy and age above 80 years.
The patients had all been diagnosed with SARS-CoV-2 infection between March 2020 and May 2021, either by positive PCR from a nasopharyngeal swab and/or by positive serology. Patients were included in the study 223.07 ± 41.69 days post-infection and divided into the following 3 groups: 24 patients who had been in ICU during the acute phase of the infection (severe), 39 patients who had been hospitalized but did not require mechanical ventilation (moderate) and 42 patients who had tested positive but had not been hospitalized (mild). During the screening–inclusion process, because of the limited number of patients who had been in ICU and who met our inclusion criteria, the mild and moderate groups were matched with the severe group for age, sociocultural level, gender and clinical variables (except for sleep apnoea and chronic kidney disease). All descriptive data are provided in Table 1.
Table 1.
Mild n = 44 |
Moderate n = 42 |
Severe n = 24 |
P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean age in years (± SD) | 55.45 (± 8.76) | 56.13 (± 10.30) | 61.48 (± 11.92) | 0.110 |
Mean education level (1–3) (± SD) | 2.77 (± 0.42) | 2.63 (± 0.59) | 2.52 (± 0.59) | 0.192 |
Gender (% men) | 65.91 | 63.15 | 73.91 | 0.420 |
Handedness (% right handed) | 97.70 | 92.90 | 95.80 | 0.553 |
Mean days of hospitalization (± SD) | – | 11.38 (± 12.60) | 40.52 (± 32.72) | – |
Diabetes (%) | 2.30 | 10.50 | 21.70 | 0.075 |
Smoking (%) | 13.60 | 2.60 | 4.30 | 0.138 |
History of respiratory disorders (%) | 13.50 | 10.50 | 26.10 | 0.242 |
History of cardiovascular disorders (%) | 13.60 | 15.80 | 21.70 | 0.691 |
History of neurological disorders (%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
History of psychiatric disorders (%) | 4.5a | 0a | 4.30a | 0.416 |
History of cancer (%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
History of severe immunosuppression (%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
History of developmental disorders (%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 |
Chronic kidney disease (%) | 0 | 0 | 8.3 | 0.026* |
Sleep apnoea syndrome (%) | 9.10 | 10.50 | 30.40 | 0.043* |
Statistical analysis performed: Kruskal–Wallis or chi2.
SD, standard deviation.
Treated depression more than 10 years prior to COVID-19.
Standard protocol approvals, registrations and patient consents
All participants gave their written informed consent, and the study was approved by the cantonal ethics committee of Geneva (CCER-02186).
Measures
For the purpose of the present study, we extracted olfactory, multimodal emotion recognition, memory and neuropsychiatric data,1,8,13,30 as well as neuroimaging data1,13 from the COVID-COG project database.9
Multimodal emotion recognition
We used the short form of the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test (GERT-S31), a validated multimodal emotion recognition task adapted from the full 83-item GERT. GERT-S is a performance-based test that measures individual differences in people’s ability to recognize others’ emotions expressed in the face, voice or body. During this task, participants watched 42 short video clips with sound (duration: 1–3 s), in which 14 different emotions extracted from the validated GEMEP database32 were displayed (see Fig. 1). Of the 14 emotions, 12 could be contrasted with respect to valence and arousal: emotions of joy, amusement, pride (high arousal/positive valence), pleasure, relief, interest (low arousal/positive valence), anger, fear, despair (high arousal/negative valence) and irritation, anxiety and sadness (low arousal/negative valence). The remaining two emotions were disgust and surprise. Each emotion was expressed in three different video clips, in which actors expressed the emotion through facial, gestural or vocal cues. The vocal stimuli were meaningless speech (pseudowords), to avoid semantic aspects. Responses were scored as correct (1) or incorrect (0), such that the total possible GERT-S score ranged from 0 to 42. For the purpose of the study, multimodal emotion recognition data that had not previously been analysed (except for the total score) were extracted from the COVID-COG database.
Secondary variables
Episodic memory
Verbal episodic memory was assessed with Grober and Buschke’s French-language 16-item Free and Delayed Recall Test (RL/RI-16).33 Based on short-term and long-term recall, this test allows users to distinguish between the different memory processes of encoding, retrieval and storage. Visual episodic memory was assessed with the delayed recall of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test.34 This test is divided into two phases: (i) a copy phase, which measures visuoconstructive function, and (ii) a recall phase, performed 3 and 20 min later (i.e. delayed recall 3′′ and delayed recall 20′′), which measures the extent and quality of recall of the original figure from short- or long-term spatial memory. If patients failed to produce a copy of Rey’s figure, we did not administer the delayed recall,35 as the assessment of visual memory would otherwise have been biased by visuoconstructive deficits. For this reason, 11 patients did not perform the memory recall, but all patients were included in the other analyses.
Olfaction
We administered the validated Sniffin’ Sticks test battery, where participants must identify 16 odours. For each odour, they can choose between four possible answers.36
Neuropsychiatric measures
All neuropsychiatric data were collected online (Qualtrics, 2010) in the week preceding the neuropsychological assessment, via standardized and computerized questionnaires specially designed for the study. More specifically, depressive symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory-Second edition,37 anxiety with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,37 apathy and its distinct subtypes with the Apathy Motivation Index,38 PTSD with the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5,39 manic symptoms with the Goldberg Mania Inventory,40 dissociative symptoms in the patient’s daily life with the Dissociative Experience Scale,41 current stress perception with the Perceived Stress Scale-14 items,42 emotion regulation with the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire43 and susceptibility to others’ emotions with the Emotion Contagion Scale.44
Fatigue and sleep disorders
Finally, fatigue was assessed with the French version of the Fatigue Impact Scale,45 potential sleeping disorders with the Insomnia Severity Index46 and symptoms of sleepiness in daily life with the Epworth Sleepiness Scale.47
Neuroimaging data
Anatomical T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (T1w MPRAGE) and resting-state functional (rs-fMRI) MRI data were extracted from the COVID-COG dataset for 45 participants (mild, n = 17; moderate, n = 19; severe, n = 9). Within this subset, no significant differences were found between the groups on either sociocultural level (mild, 2.71 ± 0.47; moderate, 2.76 ± 0.44; severe, 2.72 ± 0.47; P = 0.93), handedness (one left-handed participant in both the mild and severe groups) or age (mild, 54.47 ± 9.17 years; moderate, 57.06 ± 11.60 years; severe, 58.00 ± 11.87 years; P = 0.75), whereas gender ratios differed significantly, as there were no women in the severe group (mild, 41.18% women; moderate, 35.29% women; severe, 0% women; P = 0.04). In addition, no differences were observed on the intervals between infection and MRI (mild, 253.81 ± 44.37 days; moderate, 276.88 ± 51.94 days; severe, 284.00 ± 50.46 days; P = 0.37) and between neuropsychological testing and MRI (mild, 33.06 ± 22.17 days; moderate, 35.71 ± 25.14 days; severe, 52.27 ± 27.42 days; P = 0.15).
The image acquisition procedure is described elsewhere1 (details and parameters listed in Supplementary Information 1, Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Preprocessing was performed using fMRIPrep 20.2.3,48 which is based on Nipype 1.6.1.49 In addition, the fMRI timeseries were detrended, low-frequency discrete cosine-basis regressors (≤0.005 Hz) were removed from the signal, a low-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 0.15 Hz was applied, each fMRI volume was spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum = 4 mm), and frames with framewise displacement >0.7 mm were excluded.
Resting-state functional connectivity was computed as the Fisher-transformed Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the mean fMRI timecourses within 156 regions of interest associated with 19 resting-state networks50 (100 cortical,51 34 cerebellar52 and 22 subcortical regions53).
Statistical analysis
First, we performed intergroup comparisons between the severe, moderate and mild groups on the GERT-S total score and subscores, using nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis analyses of variance and (if significant) Mann–Whitney U-tests. Bilateral P-values were corrected using the Bonferroni criterion. In addition, to establish baseline emotion recognition performances within our cohort, we compared the performances of our three groups of patients with that of healthy control participants from a previous study where the GERT-S was administered to a normative population (German-speaking part of Switzerland) in March 2020 (during lockdown).54 The raw scores were converted to standardized scores (z scores, with a pathological cut-off score of −1.6), in accordance with the guidelines of the Swiss Association of Neuropsychology.55,56
Second, we performed forward stepwise multiple regressions on the significant emotion scores, adding verbal and visual episodic memory, neuropsychiatric, fatigue and olfactory scores as predictors. All the statistical analyses were run on TIBCO Statistica™ 14.0.0.
Third, to find associations between neuroimaging and behavioural data, we extracted multivariate components using a data-driven approach, performing exploratory group PLSC analysis on the whole cohort of patients.57–60 The resulting latent components were defined as linear combinations of whole-brain functional connectivity patterns and GERT performances (total score, fear, disgust and irritation emotion scores) with maximum covariances across patients and within each group. Additionally, the procedure was repeated separately for each group, to estimate the coherence of the group PLSC analysis. The significance of the multivariate correlations was assessed with permutation testing (1000 permutations) and the reliability of the features’ contributions (i.e. loadings) to the correlation components with bootstrapping (500 resamples). To control for the effect of memory processes on emotion recognition, we included the RL/RI 16-delayed free recall as a measure of verbal episodic memory, along with age, gender and sociocultural level as covariates. PLSC analyses were performed using the myPLS toolbox (https://github.com/danizoeller/myPLS).
To probe for structural alterations that might be associated with the functional patterns we observed, we ran voxel-based morphometry analyses, by computing the proportions of white- and grey-matter voxels in the whole brain or within parcels of our custom fMRI atlas. We used analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) to assess statistical differences between the groups, with age, gender and sociocultural level as covariates.
Data availability
Nonsensitive COVID-COG data are available in open access on a dedicated platform (https://yareta.unige.ch/home, dataset DOI: 10.26037/yareta:56vcowyr7fdgxfikm5wycsc47a). The code for the PLSC analyses and figure generation is available in a GitHub repository (https://github.com/Cionkito/PLS-COVID_emotions).
Results
Multimodal emotion recognition 6-9 months post-infection according to symptom severity in the acute phase
Results revealed a significant difference between the three groups (mild, moderate and severe) on the total emotion recognition score 6–9 months post-infection (H = 10.9, P < 0.01) (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Table 3). More specifically, mild patients performed better than both moderate (Cohen’s d = 3.33, η2 = 0.74, z = 2.9, P = 0.01) and severe (Cohen’s d = 2.89, η2 = 0.68, z = 2.6, P = 0.03) patients. This effect on the total score was mainly driven by performances for expressions of fear, irritation and disgust: mild patients performed better than moderate patients on fear expression recognition (Cohen’s d = 3.33, η2 = 0.74, z = 2.6, P = 0.03) and also performed better than severe patients on the recognition of expressions of irritation (Cohen’s d = 2.88, η2 = 0.68, z = 3.4, P < 0.01) and disgust (Cohen’s d = 0.67, η2 = 0.68, z = 2.5, P = 0.04).
When we compared the performances of our three patient groups with the performance of healthy control participants, we observed deficits for mild (9.52%), moderate (30.71%) and severe patients (45.83%) (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for more details).
Clinical predictors of multimodal emotion recognition 6–9 months post-infection
The variance inflation factor, which measures the correlations and the strength of these correlations, between predictor variables in a regression model, was calculated for each clinical predictor (Fig. 3). Analyses revealed values ranging from 1.00 to 2.72, indicating an acceptable level of multicollinearity in our regression model.61,62
For the GERT total score, the best fit was achieved using the following variables: RL/RI 16-delayed free recall (R2 = 0.32, P < 0.01), sniff test (anosmia) (R2 = 0.08, P < 0.01), Rey figure-delayed recall 3′′ (R2 = 0.06, P < 0.01), RL/RI 16-sum of three free recalls (R2 = 0.04, P < 0.01) and RL/RI 16-immediate recall (R2 = 0.02, P = 0.04).
For the GERT fear score, the best fit was achieved using the Rey figure-delayed recall 3′′ (R2 = 0.13, P < 0.01) and RL/RI 16-delayed free recall (R2 = 0.06, P = 0.01).
For the GERT disgust score, the best fit was achieved using the RL/RI 16-delayed total recall (R2 = 0.15, P < 0.01), RL/RI 16-delayed free recall (R2 = 0.05, P = 0.02) and Fatigue Impact Scale (physical fatigue) (R2 = 0.04, P = 0.04).
For the GERT irritation score, the best fit was achieved using the RL/RI 16-delayed free recall (R2 = 0.17, P < 0.01), sniff test (anosmia) (R2 = 0.04, P = 0.02) and RL/RI 16-sum of three total recalls (R2 = 0.04, P = 0.03).
Functional and structural brain networks associated with emotion recognition performances
The group PLSC analyses identified one significant component that survived false discovery rate correction (P = 0.004) (Fig. 4). This explained 35.63% of the covariance between emotion recognition scores and functional connectivity (Fig. 4A). The neuroimaging component revealed positive contributions of functional connections between the cerebellum and subcortical and cortical networks such as the default mode, somatosensory motor and salience/ventral attention networks (Fig. 4B and C). The same functional connectivity pattern was associated with two different behaviours in the patient group: higher total, fear, disgust and irritation recognition scores for mild patients and lower total, fear and irritation recognition scores for moderate patients. Moreover, the multivariate correlation was associated with younger age and female gender for mild participants and older age as well as lower sociocultural level for moderate patients. Finally, poorer verbal episodic memory, as measured by delayed total recall, remained stable for the mild and moderate groups. Results were comparable when verbal episodic memory was not included in the model (Supplementary Fig. 2). Separate PLSC analyses were performed for each group. They only yielded significant results for moderate patients (P = 0.021), and these did not survive false discovery rate correction (Supplementary Fig. 3). Even so, the multivariate pattern specific to moderate participants was congruent with the PLSC results (Supplementary Fig. 3). Finally, no structural differences were observed in the anatomical images that could be associated with the functional patterns (Supplementary Table 4).
Discussion
The aim of the present study was 3-fold: first, to explore the impact of the severity of respiratory symptoms in the acute phase of COVID-19 on multimodal emotion recognition performances 6–9 months later; second, to determine whether memory, neuropsychiatric or olfactory variables are associated with these performances; and third, to identify the functional brain networks that are associated with them.
With respect to the first aim, we observed that both moderate and severe patients had significantly lower emotion recognition scores than mild patients. This result on the total score is consistent with previous results obtained with a smaller cohort7 and in a previous study performed on the COVID-COG cohort.2 However, for the first time to our knowledge, our results revealed poorer scores not just across the board, but for specific emotions (fear, irritation and disgust). These results support the hypothesis that the long-term neuropsychological effects of COVID-19 go beyond the well-known impact of hospitalization in ICU on cognitive functions, as attested by the performances displayed by the moderate group. However, this does not exclude an effect of conventional hospitalization.1,8,9 Thus, the ability to recognize emotions may be impaired following SARS-CoV-2 infection. Interestingly, using a transconnectome-based network diffusion model, Parsons et al.63 reported changes in thalamocortical connectivity in patients with COVID-19 that may have disrupted their regulation of consciousness and arousal, possibly affecting or interacting with emotion recognition.64
Regarding the second aim, multiple regression analyses revealed that the best predictors of multimodal emotion recognition were performances on verbal and visual episodic memory, as well as olfactory recognition abilities and self-reported physical fatigue. Surprisingly, neuropsychiatric symptoms, such as anxiety, depressive symptoms and PTSD, were not associated with emotion recognition abilities, despite showing high heterogeneity. Olfactory deficits (and potential damage to the olfactory system),7 as well as short- and long-term memory impairment post-infection,1,8,11–13 have previously been described following SARS-CoV-2 infection,1,3,4,13,17 but we are the first to demonstrate associations between these disorders and emotion recognition. Given the overlapping brain substrates underlying memory, olfactory and emotion recognition mechanisms,65 notably via the insula, amygdala and hippocampus,66,67 our observation of these associations is particularly relevant and is probably consistent with the idea of limbic system alterations following SARS-CoV-2 infection. Interestingly, this hypothesis is partially corroborated by the neuroimaging findings of the present study and of previous studies in other neuroimaging modalities (e.g. FDG-PET).
Regarding the third aim, we found significant correlations between multimodal emotion recognition performances and functional connectivity patterns involving cortico–subcortical–cerebellar networks overlapping the so-called limbic system. Although we identified a positive correlation between emotion recognition scores and functional connectivity in the mild group, there was a negative correlation in the moderate group and no correlation in the severe group. Our interpretation is that moderate to severe symptoms in the acute phase may induce a compensatory response in the limbic system and lead to the patterns of altered functional connectivity that can be observed at 6–9 months post-infection. Whatever the underlying processes, which remain to be explored in future studies, these results are consistent with our behavioural results, as well as with previous observations in the literature for the long-term consequences of SARS-CoV-2 infection using both fMRI1,5,13 and PET,3,4,68 and underline the involvement of the limbic system (probably the emotional and memory pathways) following SARS-CoV-2 infection.1 Interestingly, limbic system alteration following viral infections has already been reported, with an increased number of enzymes involved in inflammatory responses within limbic system and associated brain structures, such as the hippocampus and basal ganglia.69,70
Limitations
It is important to note that the present study had five limitations. First, the smaller number of patients in the severe group who agreed to undergo an MRI (n = 9 patients) may explain the absence of significant correlations in the PLSC analyses. Nevertheless, the functional connectivity data of the severe patients were not particularly heterogeneous, compared with those of the mild and moderate groups (Supplementary Table 2). Further studies are needed to confirm the neuroimaging findings with a larger sample size. Second, by enrolling volunteers, we may have selected the most severe cases, even though a significant proportion of our sample did not report any complaints, as confirmed by the very low mean scores on the self-report cognitive complaint questionnaires, as previously observed in this cohort of patients.1,2 Third, between March 2020 and November 2020, the criteria for hospitalization may have changed, and some patients may therefore have been hospitalized for non-respiratory problems associated with COVID-19. That said, patient management was generally comparable, and all the patients included had the initial SARS-CoV-2 strain, as the variants only emerged later. Fourth, we did not include a control group, as the aim of the present study was to investigate differences in multimodal emotion recognition abilities and brain connectivity according to the severity of the acute infection. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the mild group would have exhibited reduced scores in comparison with a control group, as has been described in the literature. Finally, our moderate and severe groups may not have been representative of the population of hospitalized SARS-CoV-2 patients, owing to their lack of comorbidities (an inclusion criterion).
Conclusion
Taken together, the present results may provide further evidence of long-term damage by SARS-CoV-2 to the CNS and more specifically to the limbic system.3,4,13 This damage may not be solely explained by ICU effects (as attested by the performances displayed by the moderate group) nor by neuropsychiatric disorders that may arise from the pandemic context and/or personal history of infection such as anxiety, depressive symptoms or PTSD. In the light of the evidence provided in this study, interventions tailored to lessen cognitive impairment induced by SARS-CoV-2 infection could be developed.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge the CIBM Centre for Biomedical Imaging for its support in MRI data acquisition. CIBM is funded and supported by Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), University of Lausanne (UNIL), École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), University of Geneva (UNIGE) and Geneva University Hospitals (HUG). We would like to thank the patients for contributing their time to this study.
Abbreviations
- BSR =
bootstrap sampling ratio
- FDG =
fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG)
- GERT =
Geneva Emotion Recognition Test
- ICU =
intensive care unit
- PLSC =
partial least squares correlation
- PTSD =
post-traumatic stress disorder
Contributor Information
Marine Thomasson, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Philippe Voruz, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland.
Alexandre Cionca, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Isabele Jacot de Alcântara, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Anthony Nuber-Champier, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Gilles Allali, Leenaards Memory Centre, Lausanne University Hospital and University of Lausanne, Lausanne 1205, Switzerland.
Lamyae Benzakour, Psychiatry Department, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Patrice H Lalive, Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland.
Karl-Olof Lövblad, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology Department, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Olivia Braillard, Division and Department of Primary Care Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Mayssam Nehme, Division and Department of Primary Care Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Matteo Coen, Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals and Geneva University, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Jacques Serratrice, Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals and Geneva University, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Jean-Luc Reny, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals and Geneva University, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Jérôme Pugin, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Intensive Care Department, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Idris Guessous, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Division and Department of Primary Care Medicine, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Basile N Landis, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Rhinology-Olfactology Unit, Otorhinolaryngology Department, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Alessandra Griffa, Leenaards Memory Centre, Lausanne University Hospital and University of Lausanne, Lausanne 1205, Switzerland; Institute of Bioengineering, Centre for Neuroprosthetics, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne 1015, Switzerland; Department of Radiology and Medical Informatics, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Dimitri Van De Ville, Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland; Institute of Bioengineering, Centre for Neuroprosthetics, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne 1015, Switzerland.
Frederic Assal, Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Geneva 1011, Switzerland.
Julie A Péron, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva 1205, Switzerland; Neurology Department, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Geneva University Hospitals, Geneva 1205, Switzerland.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.
Funding
The present research was supported by Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) grants to J.A.P. (PI) and F.A. (Co-PI) within the framework of the COVID-19 National Research Program (NRP 78; grant no. 407840_198438, RNP 78). The funders had no role in data collection, discussion of content, preparation of the manuscript or decision to publish.
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
References
- 1. Voruz P, Cionca A, de Alcântara IJ, et al. . Functional connectivity underlying cognitive and psychiatric symptoms in post-COVID-19 syndrome: Is anosognosia a key determinant? Brain Commun. 2022;4(2):fcac057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Voruz P, Cionca A, de Alcântara IJ, et al. . Brain functional connectivity alterations associated with neuropsychological performance 6–9 months following SARS-CoV-2 infection. Human Brain Mapp. 2023;44(4):1629–1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Guedj E, Million M, Dudouet P, et al. . 18 F-FDG brain PET hypometabolism in post-SARS-CoV-2 infection: Substrate for persistent/delayed disorders? Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2021;48(2):592–595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Guedj E, Campion J, Dudouet P, et al. . 18 F-FDG brain PET hypometabolism in patients with long COVID. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imag. 2021;48(9):2823–2833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Esposito F, Cirillo M, De Micco R, et al. . Olfactory loss and brain connectivity after COVID-19. Hum Brain Mapp. 2022;43(5):1548–1560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Meyer PT, Hellwig S, Blazhenets G, Hosp JA. Molecular imaging findings on acute and long-term effects of COVID-19 on the brain: A systematic review. J Nucl Med. 2022;63(7):971–980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Mutiawati E, Fahriani M, Mamada SS, et al. . Anosmia and dysgeusia in SARS-CoV-2 infection: Incidence and effects on COVID-19 severity and mortality, and the possible pathobiology mechanisms—A systematic review and meta-analysis. F1000Res. 2021;10:40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Voruz P, Allali G, Benzakour L, et al. . Long COVID neuropsychological deficits after severe, moderate, or mild infection. Clin Transl Neurosci. 2022;6(2):9. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Voruz P, de Alcântara IJ, Nuber-Champier A, et al. . Frequency of abnormally low neuropsychological scores in post-COVID-19 syndrome: The Geneva COVID-COG Cohort. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2023;38(1):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Alemanno F, Houdayer E, Parma A, et al. . COVID-19 cognitive deficits after respiratory assistance in the subacute phase: A COVID-rehabilitation unit experience. PLoS One 2021;16(2):e0246590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Almeria M, Cejudo JC, Sotoca J, Deus J, Krupinski J. Cognitive profile following COVID-19 infection: Clinical predictors leading to neuropsychological impairment. Brain Behav Immun Health. 2020;9:100163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Jaywant A, Vanderlind WM, Alexopoulos GS, Fridman CB, Perlis RH, Gunning FM. Frequency and profile of objective cognitive deficits in hospitalized patients recovering from COVID-19. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021;46(13):2235–2240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Voruz P, Cionca A, Jacot de Alcântara I, et al. . Brain functional connectivity alterations associated with neuropsychological performance 6–9 months following SARS-CoV-2 infection. Human Brain Mapping, 2022;44(4):1629-1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Sakusic A, O'Horo JC, Dziadzko M, et al. . Potentially modifiable risk factors for long-term cognitive impairment after critical illness: A systematic review. Elsevier; 2018:68–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Fu Z, Tu Y, Calhoun VD, et al. . Dynamic functional network connectivity associated with post-traumatic stress symptoms in COVID-19 survivors. Neurobiol Stress. 2021;15:100377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Shaikh AG, Manto M, Mitoma H. 2 Years into the pandemic: What did we learn about the COVID-19 and cerebellum? Springer; 2022:1–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Douaud G, Lee S, Alfaro-Almagro F, et al. . SARS-CoV-2 is associated with changes in brain structure in UK Biobank. Nature 2022;604(7907):697–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Díez-Cirarda M, Yus M, Gómez-Ruiz N, et al. . Multimodal neuroimaging in post-COVID syndrome and correlation with cognition. Brain 2023;146(5):2142–2152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Schou TM, Joca S, Wegener G, Bay-Richter C. Psychiatric and neuropsychiatric sequelae of COVID-19—A systematic review. Brain Behav Immun. 2021;97:328–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Dalili MN, Penton-Voak IS, Harmer C, Munafò MR. Meta-analysis of emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. 2015;45(6):1135–1144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Passardi S, Peyk P, Rufer M, Wingenbach TS, Pfaltz MC. Facial mimicry, facial emotion recognition and alexithymia in post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2019;122:103436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Demenescu LR, Kortekaas R, den Boer JA, Aleman A. Impaired attribution of emotion to facial expressions in anxiety and major depression. PLoS One 2010;5(12):e15058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Oldershaw A, Hambrook D, Rimes K, et al. . Emotion recognition and emotional theory of mind in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychol Health. 2011;26(8):989–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. de Almondes KM, Júnior FWNH, Alves NT. Sleep deprivation and implications for recognition and perception of facial emotions. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2016;14(1):13–22. [Google Scholar]
- 25. Sander D, Grandjean D, Scherer KR. A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Netw. 2005;18(4):317–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. MacLean PD. Psychosomatic disease and the “visceral brain”: Recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosom Med. 1949;11(6):338–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. MacLean PD. Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain). Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1952;4(4):407–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Rolls ET. Limbic systems for emotion and for memory, but no single limbic system. Cortex 2015;62:119–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Schmahmann JD. The cerebellum and cognition. Neurosci Lett. 2019;688:62–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Nuber-Champier A, Voruz P, de Alcântara IJ, et al. . Monocytosis in the acute phase of SARS-CoV-2 infection predicts the presence of anosognosia for cognitive deficits in the chronic phase. Brain Behav Immun Health. 2022;26:100511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Schlegel K, Scherer KR. Introducing a short version of the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test (GERT-S): Psychometric properties and construct validation. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48(4):1383–1392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bänziger T, Mortillaro M, Scherer KR. Introducing the Geneva multimodal expression corpus for experimental research on emotion perception. Emotion 2012;12(5):1161–1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Van der Linden M, Coyette F, Poitrenaud J, et al. . Ii. L’épreuve de rappel libre/rappel indicé à 16 items (RL/RI-16). In: Van der Linden M, editor. L’Evaluation des troubles de la mémoire. Solal; 2004. p. 25–47. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Meyers JE, Meyers KR. Rey complex figure test and recognition trial professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Osterrieth PA. Le test de copie d'une figure complexe; contribution a l'etude de la perception et de la memoire. Arch Psychol (Geneve). 1944;30:206–356. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Oleszkiewicz A, Schriever V, Croy I, Hähner A, Hummel T. Updated Sniffin’Sticks normative data based on an extended sample of 9139 subjects. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;276(3):719–728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manuel de l'inventaire d'anxiété état-trait forme Y (STAI-Y) [inventory of state-trait anxiety manual]. Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Ang Y-S, Lockwood P, Apps MA, Muhammed K, Husain M. Distinct subtypes of apathy revealed by the apathy motivation index. PLoS One 2017;12(1):e0169938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Ashbaugh AR, Houle-Johnson S, Herbert C, El-Hage W, Brunet A. Psychometric validation of the English and French versions of the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). PLoS One 2016;11(10):e0161645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Goldberg IK. Questions & answers about depression and its treatment: A consultation with a leading psychiatrist. Charles Press Pub; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Carlson EB, Putnam FW. Dissociative experiences scale. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1986;174(12):727-735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Lesage F-X, Berjot S, Deschamps F. Psychometric properties of the French versions of the Perceived Stress Scale. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2012;25(2):178–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Gross J, John O. Emotion regulation questionnaire. NeuroImage 2003;48(10):9–9. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Doherty RW. The emotional contagion scale: A measure of individual differences. J Nonverbal Behav. 1997;21(2):131–154. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Debouverie M, Pittion-Vouyovitch S, Louis S, Guillemin F. Validity of a French version of the fatigue impact scale in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J. 2007;13(8):1026–1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. Guilford press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1991;14(6):540–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Blair RW, et al. . fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods. 2019;16(1):111–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Gorgolewski K, Burns CD, Madison C, et al. . Nipype: A flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in python. Front Neuroinform. 2011;5:13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, et al. . The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–1165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, et al. . Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(9):3095–3114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, Ramnani N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 2009;46(1):39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Amunts K, Lepage C, Borgeat L, et al. . Bigbrain: An ultrahigh-resolution 3D human brain model. Science 2013;340(6139):1472–1475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Schlegel K, von Gugelberg HM, Makowski LM, Gubler DA, Troche SJ. Emotion recognition ability as a predictor of well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2021;12(7):1380–1391. [Google Scholar]
- 55. Frei A, Balzer C, Gysi F, Leros J, Plohmann A, Steiger G. Kriterien zur Bestimmung des Schweregrades einer neuropsychologischen Störung sowie Zuordnungen zur Funktions-und Arbeitsfähigkeit. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie. 2016;27(2):107–119. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Heaton RK, Grant I, Matthews CG. Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Psychological Assessment Resources; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 57. Krishnan A, Williams LJ, McIntosh AR, Abdi H. Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review. Neuroimage 2011;56(2):455–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. McIntosh AR, Lobaugh NJ. Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: Applications and advances. Neuroimage 2004;23:S250–S263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Kebets V, Holmes AJ, Orban C, et al. . Somatosensory-motor dysconnectivity spans multiple transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86(10):779–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Griffa A, Bommarito G, Assal F, Herrmann FR, Van De Ville D, Allali G. Dynamic functional networks in idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus: Alterations and reversibility by CSF tap test. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42(5):1485–1502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. James G, Witten D, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. An introduction to statistical learning. vol. 112. Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Menard S. Applied logistic regression analysis. Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Parsons N, Outsikas A, Parish A, et al. . Modelling the anatomic distribution of neurologic events in patients with COVID-19: A systematic review of MRI findings. Am J Neuroradiol. 2021;42(7):1190–1195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Aue T. Chapitre 5. Psychophysiologie des émotions. Dunod; 2014:168–199. [Google Scholar]
- 65. Soudry Y, Lemogne C, Malinvaud D, Consoli S-M, Bonfils P. Olfactory system and emotion: Common substrates. Eur Ann Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Dis. 2011;128(1):18–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Cauda F, D’agata F, Sacco K, Duca S, Geminiani G, Vercelli A. Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain. Neuroimage 2011;55(1):8–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Ghaziri J, Tucholka A, Girard G, et al. . Subcortical structural connectivity of insular subregions. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Hosp JA, Dressing A, Blazhenets G, et al. . Cognitive impairment and altered cerebral glucose metabolism in the subacute stage of COVID-19. Brain 2021;144(4):1263–1276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Sartori AC, Vance DE, Slater LZ, Crowe M. The impact of inflammation on cognitive function in older adults: Implications for health care practice and research. J Neurosci Nurs. 2012;44(4):206–217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Raz N, Rodrigue KM. Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2006;30(6):730–748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Nonsensitive COVID-COG data are available in open access on a dedicated platform (https://yareta.unige.ch/home, dataset DOI: 10.26037/yareta:56vcowyr7fdgxfikm5wycsc47a). The code for the PLSC analyses and figure generation is available in a GitHub repository (https://github.com/Cionkito/PLS-COVID_emotions).