Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2023 Aug 23;290(2005):20231338. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2023.1338

Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings

Eleanor S Diamant 1,, Ian MacGregor-Fors 2, Daniel T Blumstein 1, Pamela J Yeh 1,3
PMCID: PMC10445014  PMID: 37608719

Abstract

Following the COVID-19 pandemic, many people around the world stayed home, drastically altering human activity in cities. This exceptional moment provided researchers the opportunity to test how urban animals respond to human disturbance, in some cases testing fundamental questions on the mechanistic impact of urban behaviours on animal behaviour. However, at the end of this ‘anthropause', human activity returned to cities. How might each of these strong shifts affect wildlife in the short and long term? We focused on fear response, a trait essential to tolerating urban life. We measured flight initiation distance—at both individual and population levels—for an urban bird before, during and after the anthropause to examine if birds experienced longer-term changes after a year and a half of lowered human presence. Dark-eyed juncos did not change fear levels during the anthropause, but they became drastically less fearful afterwards. These surprising and counterintuitive findings, made possible by following the behaviour of individuals over time, has led to a novel understanding that fear response can be driven by plasticity, yet not habituation-like processes. The pandemic-caused changes in human activity have shown that there is great complexity in how humans modify a behavioural trait fundamental to urban tolerance in animals.

Keywords: behavioural response, flight initiation distance, urban ecology, behavioural plasticity, anthropause

1. Background

In 2020, many countries in the world went into ‘lockdown' in response to the COVID-19 pandemic. With human mobility suddenly halted, these lockdowns drastically changed the dynamics of our cities and caused what has been coined as the ‘anthropause' [1]. While devastating for human communities, the absence of humans from the landscape provided a unique opportunity to study how animals respond to human activity, from the level of individual behaviour to population dynamics to community composition. Likely because of the direct and indirect effects of human activity, such as vehicular traffic, collisions, light pollution and noise pollution, some wildlife—specifically urban wildlife—adjusted their behaviours and patterns across the globe [16]. For example, during the initial pandemic lockdowns, urban white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) rapidly responded to the reduction in traffic noise by notably changing their songs to more high-performing songs that are otherwise interrupted by urban noise [2]. Lockdowns have sporadically ended and re-occurred in different parts of the world, though human activity has broadly bounced back to pre-pandemic levels. As a result, urban animals are now faced with increased human activity and stressors following a long absence. By assessing their individual and population-level behaviours before, during and after the anthropause, we can begin to understand how animals respond to dynamic human processes and stressors. We can also determine if and how this exceptional event continues to impact wildlife even after humans have returned to the landscape.

Determining how animals cope with urban stressors is essential to predicting wildlife response in the face of strong anthropogenic change [7,8]. Urbanization is a leading cause of habitat loss and biodiversity loss, though some animals manage to survive, adapt and ultimately thrive in cities [912]. Though the underlying causes and associations for urban success vary across species and populations, tolerating humans is essential to urban life [1315]. Indeed, at the population and species levels, we see that urban animals typically have a reduced fear of humans [16]. The mechanisms underlying this behavioural shift are challenging to parse out: in some organisms, this is due to habitat selection wherein individuals that are less fearful choose urban habitats with increased human activity and stress [17]. However, habituation-like processes that may underly within-generational behavioural plasticity—an individual's propensity to shift their behaviour in response to differences in their environments—can also explain this observed phenomenon [18]. Here, individuals exposed to human activity might decrease their fear response with increased exposure. Once in the city, the urban environment might select for individuals that are less fearful. Plasticity itself might be under selection if certain individuals express less fear upon exposure than others and may evolve if this has reproductive consequences [19,20].

COVID-19 lockdowns and reopenings provided the opportunity for us to study the complex nature of how fear is affected by human activity, allowing us to test how plastic the response is in a successful urban bird: the dark-eyed junco (Junco hyemalis). This songbird, native to North America, began breeding in urban habitats in the past 20–40 years in southern Californian cities [2123], likely independently. By assessing fear response across lockdown conditions, we can then determine if and how non-evolutionary mechanisms, such as habituation-like processes that might lead to tolerance and habitat selection whereby tolerant individuals settle around humans while less tolerant ones avoid humans, as well as evolutionary processes like selection on plasticity itself, impact fear. Further, the relatively sudden reintroduction of humans to the landscape provided us with the opportunity to assess if dark-eyed junco behaviour returned to a pre-pandemic ‘normal' or if lockdowns shifted how this urban bird behaves and copes with human presence long term.

We tested individual and population-level fear response in urban dark-eyed juncos before, during and after COVID-19 closures to understand the immediate and longer-term effects of COVID-19 on other animals and to test fundamental questions in urban behavioural adaptation. If lower urban fear response is due to habituation, we expected fear response to lower during the COVID-19 closures and increase following reopening. On the other hand, if lower urban fear response is due to habitat selection and is less plastic, we expected fear response to remain unchanged with respect to the closures. If both habitat selection and habituation play a role, as might be seen if birds that have relatively lower fear response and a plastic habituative response are selected for, we expected fear response to increase during the closures, though not to the level of non-urban birds, and to decrease after reopening at the individual and population level. None of these hypotheses were supported by our findings. Instead, we found that, at the population level and individual level, urban birds did not change their behaviour during the COVID-19 closures but became significantly less fearful of humans following reopenings in comparison to pre-pandemic baselines.

2. Methods

(a) . Study sites

To confirm that urban dark-eyed juncos have lower fearfulness across cities in comparison with non-urban dark-eyed juncos, we conducted fieldwork at urban and non-urban sites in southern California between 2017 and March 2020, representing a pre-pandemic baseline of urban and non-urban differences. We assessed individually colour-ringed birds across three metropolitan areas: Santa Barbara County (Santa Barbara), Los Angeles County (Los Angeles) and San Diego County (San Diego). At each of these metropolitan areas, we sampled birds at the local University of California (University of California Santa Barbara (UCSB), Los Angeles (UCLA) and San Diego (UCSD), respectively). In Los Angeles, specifically, we also sampled birds across the city, at Occidental College and parks of various sizes across the urban core. Dark-eyed juncos likely began breeding in San Diego in the early to mid-1980s, in Los Angeles in the early to mid-2000s, and in Santa Barbara in the early 2010s. These sites were compared to non-urban, mountainous sites that are indicative of their historic breeding range [23,24]: the UC Stunt Ranch Reserve in the Santa Monica Mountains, the UC James San Jacinto Mountain Reserve and the Angeles Forest in the San Gabriel Mountains. Non-urban sites were all university-managed research reserves or federally managed and protected forests, representing ‘wildland' counterparts. We continued to assess birds in Los Angeles at UCLA until 2022. Urban study sites were more artificially built up and exhibited more light pollution in comparison to non-urban sites (electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2).

To investigate individual plasticity of fearfulness in response to human disturbance, we conducted a long-term study at UCLA. Due to logistic constraints, we did not visit other sites during the pandemic. Because we have a longitudinal study site in urban Los Angeles, dark-eyed juncos at UCLA were individually distinguishable by unique coloured leg rings. In March 2020, UCLA closed classes and research, except for essential researchers. UCLA remained remote until autumn 2021, when classes resumed on campus. We conducted human pedestrian surveys at UCLA to confirm that human activity was lower during campus closures than when classes are in session and in person. We previously surveyed 12 points across campus twice per week, once in the morning and once in the afternoon during the anthropause (May–July 2021) and when the campus was ‘back to normal' (March–July 2022). Each survey lasted two minutes and all individuals, vehicles, and dogs crossing the observer's eyeline were counted. Campus closures caused human activity to be approximately 7× lower than ‘normal' in 2021 [25], and therefore even lower during 2020 at the height of Los Angeles lockdowns and UCLA campus restrictions.

(b) . Individual colour ringing

We captured and ringed local dark-eyed juncos at each site at the start of territorial singing—around mid-January in urban sites and April in non-urban sites—to July 2017–2022. We did not ring birds outside of Los Angeles in March–July 2020 due to pandemic-related logistic constraints. Birds were captured between 6.30 and 11.00. They were lured into mist-nets using playback of junco song recorded at UCLA in 2018 or from the MacCaulay Library (Cornell University). Each junco was fitted with three colour rings and one aluminium USGS ring in a unique combination. Birds were aged by moult limits as ‘second year', ‘after second year' or ‘after hatch year' (when age could not be determined) and sexed by cloacal protuberance or brood patch. When birds were not in breeding condition, they were sexed by plumage, which was later confirmed by behaviour (singing or exhibiting nesting behaviour). All birds were released after processing.

(c) . Flight initiation distance assays

We assessed the fear levels of individually identifiable birds using a flight initiation distance (FID) assay repeatedly during the 2018–2022 breeding seasons (January–June/July). We determined FIDs for each bird following established methods [26,27]. All FID tests were conducted by E.S.D. A marker was dropped at first site of the focal bird. The researcher walked at a steady and practiced pace of approximately 0.5 m s−1 towards the bird with the observer's eyeline straight ahead. A second marker was dropped at the point the researcher was when the bird flew or hopped away, and then a third where the bird was when it flew or hopped. We recorded the starting distance (the distance between the first marker and the third), FID (the distance between the second and third). Effort was made to universally assay juncos in instances with low human activity (less than 15 humans walking in the vicinity while assays were conducted, except for 18 data points in areas rarely empty during the academic year) and with no other humans or juncos between the investigator and the focal bird. Trial number per individual was determined for each fear response assay and varied between 1 and 10 trials per individual. We also estimated distance to cover—defined as habitat a bird could hide within or behind, i.e. vegetated cover, trees or artificial cover such as benches—that was reflective of very local habitat structure and potential risk assessment. While visibility can affect fear response, we accounted for potential variation in fog retrospectively by including a time by month covariate as, anecdotally, fog cover in southern California typically occurs earlier in the year and earlier in the day. The time-by-month category also accounted for variation across the breeding season and temporal effects that may be related to seasonal heat. To the best of our knowledge, hourly numeric cloud cover data do not exist for southern California. A small subset of trials were missing data on distance to cover, which were imputed with the median value across all trials.

We conducted FID tests in 2018 and 2019 in non-urban sites. At sites other than UCLA, we conducted FIDs before COVID-19 lockdowns in 2018, 2019 and up to March 2020, as the campuses closed following ‘safer-at-home' measures. At UCLA, we conducted FIDs on birds in 2018, 2019, 2020 (npre-pandemic = 71; n2020 anthropause = 135), 2021 (n = 131) and 2022 (n = 67). Most FIDs were conducted during the pre-breeding and breeding seasons—between January and July—as birds were more conspicuous, easier to find, and not in wintering flocks, which has been found to affect FIDs in other songbirds [28]. We measured FIDs of juncos before COVID-19 restrictions started on 14 March 2020 (i.e. ‘pre-pandemic'). In-person classes were cancelled on that date, and the campus was mostly closed thereafter (i.e. ‘during anthropause'). During this time, we attempted to re-measure FIDs for individuals every two weeks, though this was not possible universally due to the spontaneous nature of FID testing. We re-assessed individuals following COVID-19 lockdowns from January to June 2022 following UCLA returning to in-person instruction (i.e. ‘post anthropause'). We categorized these time periods as ‘pre-pandemic' (2018–14 March 2020), ‘2020 anthropause' (25 March 2020–July 2020), ‘2021 anthropause' and ‘post-anthropause' (2022).

Because we were also tracking reproductive behaviour prior to COVID-19 lockdowns, we know with certainty that at UCLA all chicks were hatched following the cancellation of in-person classes, and thus a significant decrease in human activity. Previously, at UCLA dark-eyed juncos have started breeding in March [29]. All of our nest observations are recorded in the community science database NestWatch [30]. Including our data, the soonest expected potential hatch date across all dark-eyed junco nests reported to NestWatch was 15 March 2020. As such, all birds ringed at UCLA in 2021 and 2022 that were in their second-year plumage were likely to have been hatched and fledged during the COVID-19 lockdown, with minimal human activity relative to older birds on campus. We also followed reproductive activities in 2021 and ringed chicks in the nest, some of which returned to campus and were assayed as second-year birds.

(d) . Measuring human activity

To gauge human activity shifts in the city as a whole, we accessed Google's Community Mobility Reports [31]. This dataset compiling data from smartphones to determine where users spent time across different place categories compares aggregated data to a pre-pandemic baseline for a given community (in this case, Los Angeles) per day. We used the categories: mean change in activity (averaged across categories) and time spent in residential places as variables in our analysis. Because this dataset only began in February 2020, we calculated the mean and standard deviation for pre-pandemic levels (15 February to 14 March 2020) and randomly generated a normal distribution with the calculated mean and standard deviation. We imputed Los Angeles Community Mobility data for each date before 15 February 2020 randomly from this normal distribution.

(e) . Statistical analysis

To test if urban dark-eyed juncos across southern Californian cities have parallel shifts in FID in comparison to non-urban dark-eyed juncos, we fitted a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) using a gamma distribution and an inverse link function. We included city and starting distance as fixed effects and bird ID as a random effect. For this analysis, we only included dark-eyed juncos in Los Angeles (n = 119), San Diego (n = 33) and Santa Barbara (n = 13) that were assayed before the COVID-19 pandemic. We aggregated data (n = 25) across non-urban populations to compare to. We then determined within model contrasts to test if urban populations were different from each other and from non-urban juncos.

To determine which variables were important to include in GLMMs, we first ran a regression tree analysis. Here, our dependent variable was FID (m). Independent variables we included were: sex, month, UCLA's anthropause status + year (2018–2020 ‘pre-pandemic', ‘2020 anthropause’, ‘2021 anthropause', 2022 ‘post-anthropause'), Google's mean change in activity in Los Angeles, Google's time spent in residential places in Los Angeles, distance from closest cover, time, month and starting distance. Based on this analysis, UCLA's anthropause status + year, mean change in community mobility, and starting distance were variables that were found to diagnose differences in FID. Mean change in activity and time spent in residential places were associated with each other, because mean change in activity is calculated using time spent in residential places, in combination with other variables. The regression tree analysis found that mean change in activity was a more important variable than time spent in residential areas. Thus, we included mean change in activity in our GLMM. The regression tree analysis found a split at 9.00. Thus, we categorized time before 9.00 as ‘early' while those at and after 9.00 as ‘late'.

We then fitted a GLMM with a gamma distribution and an inverse link, fitting the distribution of our data. We included UCLA's anthropause status + year, a given individuals' trial number, mean change in community mobility use with the Los Angeles Google Community Mobility Reports data, distance from closest cover, and starting distance as fixed effects, as well as bird identity and time by month as random effects. To determine individual repeatability, we calculated within-model individual repeatability. Here, the model was adjusted where the response variable was log-transformed FID and was fitted with a Gaussian distribution to fit function parameters.

We visualized individual shifts by subsetting juncos who were assessed before and during the anthropause and/or during and after the anthropause to determine if trends at the population level were repeated at the individual level. Due to lower sample sizes for samples within individuals across time periods, we combined 2020 and 2021 anthropause categories into one category. To determine if patterns in these data were driven by habituation-like processes, we fitted the same GLMM, but only considered three anthropause categories (i.e. ‘before', ‘during' and ‘after' the anthropause) instead of differentiating between 2020 and 2021. All other variables remained the same.

To assess early-life effects, we compared four cohorts of second-year juncos' FID at UCLA. Second-year birds assayed in 2019 (n = 15) hatched and were assayed in a high-human-activity environment. Second-year birds assayed in 2020 (n = 34) hatched with high human activity and were assayed in an anthropause environment. Second-year birds assayed in 2021 (n = 11) hatched and were assayed in an anthropause environment. Second-year birds assayed in 2022 (n = 10) hatched in an anthropause environment yet were assayed in a high-human-activity environment. We fitted a GLMM with a gamma distribution and an inverse link function. The fixed effects were ‘year second-year bird was assayed' (pre-pandemic, 2020 anthropause, 2021 anthropause, 2022 post-anthropause) and starting distance. Bird ID was included as a random effect. We calculated within-model contrasts to determine if there were significant differences in FID between groups. We tested the assumptions of all models by testing for normality and linearity of residuals, as well checking for multicollinearity between independent variables. Assumptions were met for all models.

All tests were done in R v. 4.2.2 [32]. Regression trees were built and tested using R packages rsample [33] and rpart [34]. GLMMs were built and analysed using lme4 [35]. The repeatability analysis was calculated with rptR [36]. Within-model contrasts were calculated using R packages gmodels [37] and multcomp [38].

3. Results

Across tested southern California populations, urban juncos have a consistent lowered fear response in comparison to non-urban conspecifics (electronic supplementary material, figures S1–S3).

At the population level, fear response remained relatively unchanged during campus closure in comparison to pre-pandemic levels (N = 404). Upon campus reopening in the 2021–2022 academic year, fear response in the 2022 breeding season was significantly reduced compared to measurements from prior breeding seasons (figure 1). We found that campus closure status (χ2 = 16.37, d.f. = 3, p = 0.001) strongly and significantly affected fear, but relative change in human activity (χ2 = 1.86, d.f. = 1, p = 0.17), trial number (χ2 = 3.57, d.f. = 1, p = 0.06), distance from cover (χ2 = 0.03, d.f. = 1, p = 0.85) and starting distance (χ2 = 0.02, d.f. = 1, p = 0.96) did not. Additionally, there was significant individual variation in fear (s.d. = 0.23, p < 0.0001) and a significant effect of time by month on fear (s.d. = 0.01|0.01, p = 0.007; electronic supplementary material, figure S4). Collectively, individual juncos varied in their fear response, but the reopening and increase of human activity following restrictions significantly lowered fear across the population (p < 0.001 in 2020 anthropause and 2021 anthropause fearfulness compared with post-anthropause fearfulness, and p = 0.047 in pre-pandemic fearfulness compared with post-anthropause fearfulness; figure 1; electronic supplementary material, figure S4, table S1). Individual repeatability in fearfulness was 0.29 ± 0.06 s.e.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Population-level fearfulness remained unchanged during lockdowns but decreased following reopenings. Population-level flight initiation distance (FID) before (n = 71), during (n2020 = 135; n2021 = 131) and after (n = 67) the anthropause. The dark-eyed junco population at University of California Los Angeles (UCLA) did not shift their FID across the anthropause (GLMM contrast: p > 0.05). FID significantly dropped in the 2022 post-anthropause environment in comparison to both years in the anthropause and the pre-pandemic baseline (GLMM contrasts: p < 0.05 for pre-pandemic, 2020 anthropause, and 2021 anthropause compared to 2022 post-anthropause). FID data are log10 transformed for visual aid, but not in the formal statistical analysis. Data points represent mean log10 FID ± standard error for each time period assessed. Groups with the same letter are not statistically significantly different from each other. Groups with different letters are statistically significantly different.

The patterns seen at the population level were repeated at the individual level (figure 2; electronic supplementary material, table S2). A subset of individuals was tested repeatedly before, during and after pandemic closures. While individuals varied in their fear response once lockdowns occurred, individuals nearly universally became less fearful following campus reopening (n = 10 of 11). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that behavioural plasticity in fear response explained the pattern at the population level. While we expected individual birds to become more tolerant to humans during the closures and return to a pre-pandemic fear response following reopening, we found that urban dark-eyed juncos had a surprisingly plastic response to increased human activity, but not to decreased human activity.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Individuals became less fearful after reopenings in comparison to before the pandemic closures. Individual shifts in flight initiation distance (FID) before, during and after anthropause. (a) FID measurements only for individuals that were tested repeatably for at least one time point in each period: before, during and after the campus closures (n = 11). Trends reflect a similar decrease in FID from before the pandemic to the end of the anthropause. Lines are fitted linearly to demonstrate the change from before to after the anthropause. Each line represents one individual. Dashed red vertical lines denote the beginning and end of the anthropause, respectively. A GLMM, only including birds tested across all time periods and accounting for potential habituation to the investigator (by including trial number), and treating the anthropause as a single category, produced similar results. Here, differences in pre-pandemic fearfulness compared with post-anthropause fearfulness were significant (p = 0.05) and differences in anthropause compared to post-anthropause fearfulness were significant (p = 0.02). (b) Mean FID values per individual (grey) in pre-pandemic, anthropause, and following reopening ‘post-anthropause' time periods. The thick black line represents shifts across all individuals sampled repeatedly between the pre-pandemic and anthropause (n = 33) or the anthropause and post-anthropause time periods (n = 24). These demonstrate pairwise shifts in mean fear response for each individual to account for individuals that might not have been tested in one of the time periods. A GLMM accounting for potential habituation (by including trial number), closest cover, time by month, and considering the anthropause as a single category, revealed significant pairwise differences between pre-pandemic and post-anthropause environments (p = 0.02) and between the anthropause and post-anthropause environments (p < 0.001). Though FIDs were not log10 transformed in our analyses (given their gamma distribution), they are log10 transformed here for visual ease.

4. Discussion

We found that fear response did not shift at the height of the COVID-19 closures, in comparison to before the pandemic, but that the re-introduction of humans led to a decrease in fear both at the population level and at the individual level. Thus, this urban bird did not become more like its wildland counterparts without human presence but rather, when faced with more human activity, became even less fearful than pre-pandemic levels, which is already less fearful than wildland birds. That dark-eyed juncos did not increase fearfulness with decreased human activity, even among birds who hatched during the COVID-19 closures (electronic supplementary material, figure S5), suggests that lower urban fear response is not dynamically driven by habituation-like processes—if these processes have a role in explaining tolerance at all. Indeed, habituation-like processes were controlled for at the individual level in all our analyses and do not explain the population-wide pattern we found. We also controlled for potential impacts of time of day and season, which may affect visibility and hormonal variation, and local habitat structure, known covariates of fearfulness in other birds [3941]. Rather, fear response expression is likely more complex and suggests that studying the ontogeny will be particularly illuminating. It could be that urban colonists from non-urban origins might have a lower baseline fear response in comparison to the pre-pandemic non-urban population at large, and potentially decrease their fear response with human activity. We note that in contrast to virtually all other studies of anthropause effects in birds (e.g. [1,3,4,4244]) that used unmarked birds and were unable to focus on individuals, these insights emerged only from a detailed, longitudinal study of individuals.

A population increase coupled with a re-introduced landscape of fear might have led to higher competition for resources in 2022 and thus trade-offs favouring increased foraging despite higher perceived predator (human) risk. Changing patterns of human mobility shifted birds' use of space broadly across lockdowns [5], suggesting that human presence affects the habitability of urban spaces. Entering into COVID-19 lockdowns and reopenings, contrasting shifts in human activity—one increasing human activity and one decreasing human activity—led to contrasting fitness consequences reflecting this shift in the landscape of fear: great tits (Parus major) in an area with lower human activity had higher reproductive output than that with higher human activity [45]. However, there was no evidence for increased fitness by means of increased nestling condition and nest success when comparing 2021 and pre-pandemic 2019 breeding seasons in this dark-eyed junco population [25]. Additionally, there were no changes in aggressive interactions in the population following reopenings in 2022 and pre-pandemic 2019 breeding seasons [25], suggesting that fear response is not a by-product of shifting behavioural strategies due to a different socioecological context or due to indirect effects on predator density during the anthropause. Urban song rapidly shifted during San Francisco's lockdowns in a related species (white-crowned sparrows) potentially because there was a clear communicative signal being interrupted by urban stressors [2]. The relationships that exist between human activity and other urban behaviours appear more nuanced.

Fear responses could vary because ecological conditions changed, altering the trade-offs in escape behaviour following reopenings. Recent drought conditions in southern California might have made urban birds more reliant on anthropogenic food to buffer declines in natural food resources—as was the case in an urban monkey [46]—leading to a higher tolerance of human presence in 2022. However, urban areas act as a buffer to arid conditions because of irrigation, supporting larger populations and diversity of arthropods [47]. Additionally, UCLA is an irrigated and green campus in an affluent area, which in turn is associated with increased irrigation and higher plant and bird diversity relative to non-urban arid conditions [48]. Thus, drought conditions in 2022 might not have caused strong detrimental effects, if any, to local urban resource distribution. Emergency regulations in California limiting turf irrigation only began in June 2022, making this a particularly unlikely explanation for lower fear response in the time period we were sampling, but something that could be accounted for in future studies given recent water-use restrictions. Different forms of pollution have also been found to affect fear response, such that more light at night was associated with a higher fear response in Eurasian curlews (Numenius arquata) [49] and lower fear response was associated with more noise in ground foraging songbirds [50]. Los Angeles juncos have specifically habituated to urban vehicle sounds and adjust their fear response according to vehicular speed, such that fear response increased when an observer played bike or scooter sounds [51]. In Los Angeles, light pollution decreased by 6.91% during lockdowns [52]. Thus, light pollution does not explain the pattern observed. Traffic-associated pollution generally decreased over the pandemic [53], suggesting that sensory pollution also generally decreased over the pandemic. Indeed, in two cities, Google mobility data were associated with pollution in Uganda and Turkey [54,55]. All of these explanations would predict a shift during the pandemic, rather than after reopenings, which was not found.

Fearfulness can and does vary over time. Due to the intensity and challenges of individually monitoring wild animals, individual repeatability of fear response is limited. Nonetheless, studies thus far indicate that FID has high individual repeatability in single-year and multi-year studies (0.75–0.95) [40,56,57]. In other measures of boldness, female Montagu harriers (Circus pygargus) became bolder with exposure to human disturbance over the course of 19 years [58]. Nonetheless, individual repeatability between years was still high (repeatability of fleeing probability at the nest was 0.65). We found an individual repeatability of 0.29, which is relatively low. The low repeatability we found is likely indicative of lockdown-related effects on fear expression and individual variation in plasticity during the anthropause, rather than natural variation in FID as a function of physiological and temporal variation.

Alternatively, we could have been measuring the level at which juncos were distracted by stimuli. Indeed, escape behaviour can vary based on the number of stimuli as prey must divide their attention. With high human density and disturbances, prey can become distracted and either fail to respond as rapidly to an approaching threat or flee more rapidly [59,60]. Here, fear response in juncos would reflect the focal bird being more or less distracted, rather than more or less fearful. If distraction by the sudden increase in humans was responsible for the reduction in FID, we might expect that distracted animals were unable to detect an approaching human and therefore tolerated closer approach. Nonetheless, if fear response were solely modulated by distraction, we would have expected a higher fear response during the anthropause than after reopening; this pattern was not seen. Thus, juncos behaved surprisingly differently following the re-introduction of high human activity, though it could be due to them filtering stimuli differently from before the pandemic.

We propose two novel hypotheses explaining how fear response develops and is modulated in urban-adapted birds: urban bird fear response is either a ratchet or a spring. Birds that hatched during the anthropause mirrored the population as a whole: they became less fearful with increasing human activity following reopenings rather than expressing fearfulness at the same level as second-year birds did before the pandemic, despite differences in their early life environment. Similarly, the population as a whole became less fearful than their pre-pandemic fear levels following reopenings (electronic supplementary material). Thus, the prolonged absence of human activity followed by a rapid increase, rather than recurrent exposures to human activity, could be driving the expression of this fear response.

If fear response acts as a spring that returns to pre-existing baseline with continuous exposure, we would expect that dark-eyed juncos will eventually re-sensitize to human activity and return to a pre-pandemic intermediate baseline. Alternatively, fear responses could change like a ratchet where each burst of rapid increases in human activity an urban bird is exposed to could lead to lower fear response. Rather than dynamically returning to a pre-pandemic baseline, a long absence coupled with a rapid burst of human activity drives an increase in human tolerance. Testing these hypotheses requires on-going study.

5. Conclusion

Collectively, our results suggest that changes in fear responses might not be as predictable as we might expect, and likely depend on which individuals and how their behaviours develop and shift in combination with strong and rapidly shifting collective human behaviours. Only through studies on individual animals tracked over time can we understand the mechanisms underlying population response, which cannot be confirmed from contradictory broadscale patterns found in meta-analyses [42,44]. While the anthropause created much human hardship, it offered a unique opportunity to identify an important new avenue of ontogenetic research that can create insights which will help us better conserve biodiversity in a rapidly changing, human-dominated world.

Preprint statement

This paper has been uploaded as a preprint [61].

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to Samuel Bressler, Marlene Walters, Sara Freimuth, Taylor Kang, and Wilmer Amaya-Mejia for their participation in the ringing effort. We also thank all undergraduate research assistants from 2018 to 2022 who have helped with ringing stations, particularly Christina Cen, Carissa Reulbach, and Julia Lung. We acknowledge the use of imagery from the NASA Worldview application (https://worldview.earthdata.nasa.gov), part of the NASA Earth Observing System Data and Information System (EOSDIS).

Ethics

All animals were cared for and tested according to institutional guideline and approval (IACUC no. ARC-2018-007). Ringing was conducted under the United States Geographic Survey Master Banding Permit no. 23809 and California Department of Fish and Wildlife Specific Collection Permit S-183040004-18313-001.

Data accessibility

Data and code are accessible via Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.21743810 [62]. Supplementary material is available online at [63].

Declaration of AI use

We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.

Authors' contributions

E.S.D.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; I.M.-F.: formal analysis, visualization, writing—review and editing; D.T.B.: methodology, writing—review and editing; P.J.Y.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing.

All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Conflict of interest declaration

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This work was supported by the American Ornithological Society Hesse Award, University of California Los Angeles Stunt Ranch and the La Kretz Center Research Award, Pasadena Audubon Society, Santa Monica Audubon Society and the Western Section of the Wildlife Society.

References

  • 1.Rutz C, et al. 2020. COVID-19 lockdown allows researchers to quantify the effects of human activity on wildlife. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 4, 1156-1159. ( 10.1038/s41559-020-1237-z) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Derryberry EP, Phillips JN, Derryberry GE, Blum MJ, Luther D. 2020. Singing in a silent spring: birds respond to a half-century soundscape reversion during the COVID-19 shutdown. Science 370, 575-579. ( 10.1126/science.abd5777) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schrimpf MB, et al. 2021. Reduced human activity during COVID-19 alters avian land use across North America. Sci. Adv. 7, eabf5073. ( 10.1126/sciadv.abf5073) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sanderfoot OV, Kaufman JD, Gardner B. 2022. Drivers of avian habitat use and detection of backyard birds in the Pacific Northwest during COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns. Sci. Rep. 12, 12655. ( 10.1038/s41598-022-16406-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Warrington MH, Schrimpf MB, Des Brisay P, Taylor ME, Koper N. 2022. Avian behaviour changes in response to human activity during the COVID-19 lockdown in the United Kingdom. Proc. R. Soc. B 289, 20212740. ( 10.1098/rspb.2021.2740) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bates AE, et al. 2021. Global COVID-19 lockdown highlights humans as both threats and custodians of the environment. Biol. Conserv. 263, 109175. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2021.109175) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Alberti M, Correa C, Marzluff JM, Hendry AP, Palkovacs EP, Gotanda KM, Hunt VM, Apgar TM, Zhou Y. 2017. Global urban signatures of phenotypic change in animal and plant populations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 8951-8956. ( 10.1073/pnas.1606034114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Johnson MTJ, Munshi-South J. 2017. Evolution of life in urban environments. Science 358, eaam8327. ( 10.1126/science.aam8327) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McKinney ML. 2006. Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biol. Conserv. 127, 247-260. ( 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.09.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McDonnell MJ, MacGregor-Fors I. 2016. The ecological future of cities. Science 352, 936-938. ( 10.1126/science.aaf3630) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.McKinney M. 2002. Urbanization, biodiversity, and conservation. Bioscience 52, 883-890. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Marzluff JM. 2001. Worldwide urbanization and its effects on birds. In Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world, pp. 19-47. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Evans KL, Chamberlain DE, Hatchwell BJ, Gregory RD, Gaston KJ. 2011. What makes an urban bird? Glob. Chang. Biol. 17, 32-44. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2486.2010.02247.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sol D, Lapiedra O, González-Lagos C. 2013. Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim. Behav. 85, 1101-1112. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ritzel K, Gallo T. 2020. Behavior change in urban mammals: a systematic review. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 576665. ( 10.3389/fevo.2020.576665) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lowry H, Lill A, Wong BBM. 2013. Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments. Biol. Rev. 88, 537-549. ( 10.1111/brv.12012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rebolo-Ifrán N, Carrete M, Sanz-Aguilar A, Rodríguez-Martínez S, Cabezas S, Marchant TA, Bortolotti GR, Tella JL. 2015. Links between fear of humans, stress and survival support a non-random distribution of birds among urban and rural habitats. Sci. Rep. 5, 13723. ( 10.1038/srep13723) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Vincze E, Papp S, Preiszner B, Seress G, Bókony V, Liker A. 2016. Habituation to human disturbance is faster in urban than rural house sparrows. Behav. Ecol. 27, 1304-1313. ( 10.1093/beheco/arw047) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Caspi T, Johnson JR, Lambert MR, Schell CJ, Sih A. 2022. Behavioral plasticity can facilitate evolution in urban environments. Trends Ecol. Evol. 37, 1092-1103. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2022.08.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Szulkin M, Munshi-South J, Charmantier A. 2020. Urban evolutionary biology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. ( 10.1093/oso/9780198836841.001.0001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yeh PJ, Price TD. 2004. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the successful colonization of a novel environment. Am. Nat. 164, 531-542. ( 10.1086/423825) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.eBird. 2021. eBird: an online database of bird distribution and abundance. Ithaca, NY: eBird, Cornell Lab of Ornithology. See http://www.ebird.org. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Allen LW, Garrett K, Wilmer MC. 2016. Los Angeles county breeding bird atlas. Los Angeles, CA: Los Angeles Audubon Society. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Miller A. 1941. Speciation in the avian genus Junco. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 44, 173-434. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Walters M, Diamant ES, Wong F, Cen C, Yeh PJ. 2023. Phenotypic plasticity and the anthropause: an urban bird becomes less aggressive. Anim. Behav. 200, 71-80. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2023.02.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Blumstein DT. 2006. Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim. Behav. 71, 389-399. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Blumstein DT, Samia DSM, Stankowich T, Cooper WEJ. 2015. Best practice for the study of escape behavior. In Escaping from predators: an integrative view of escape decisions (eds Cooper WE, Blumstein DT), pp. 407-419. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Morelli F, et al. 2019. Contagious fear: escape behavior increases with flock size in European gregarious birds. Ecol. Evol. 9, 6096-6104. ( 10.1002/ece3.5193) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bressler SA, Diamant ES, Tingley MW, Yeh PJ. 2020. Nests in the cities: adaptive and non-adaptive phenotypic plasticity and convergence in an urban bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20202122. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.2122) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cornell Lab of Ornithology. 2021. NestWatch. See https://nestwatch.org/.
  • 31.Google LLC. 2022 Google COVID-19 Community Mobility Reports. See https://www.google.com/covid19/mobility/ (accessed 3 October 2022).
  • 32.R Core Team. 2020. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Silge J, Chow F, Kuhn M, Wickham H, RStudio. 2022. rsample: general resampling infrastructure.
  • 34.Therneau TM, Atkinson B. 2022. Package: rpart.
  • 35.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1-48. ( 10.18637/jss.v067.i01) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stoffel MA, Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. 2017. rptR: repeatability estimation and variance decomposition by generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 8, 1639-1644. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12797) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Warnes GR, Bolker B, Lumley T, Johnson RC. 2018. Package: gmodels.
  • 38.Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P, Heiberger RM, Schuetzenmeister A, Scheibe S. 2022. Package: multcomp.
  • 39.Ferguson SM, Gilson LN, Bateman PW. 2019. Look at the time: diel variation in the flight initiation distance of a nectarivorous bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 147. ( 10.1007/s00265-019-2757-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Seltmann MW, Öst M, Jaatinen K, Atkinson S, Mashburn K, Hollmén T. 2012. Stress responsiveness, age and body condition interactively affect flight initiation distance in breeding female eiders. Anim. Behav. 84, 889-896. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.012) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Morelli F, et al. 2022. Flight initiation distance and refuge in urban birds. Sci. Total Environ. 842, 156939. ( 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2022.156939) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mikula P, et al. 2022. Urban birds' flight responses were unaffected by the COVID-19 shutdowns. bioRxiv. ( 10.1101/2022.07.15.500232) [DOI]
  • 43.Gordo O, Brotons L, Herrando S, Gargallo G. 2021. Rapid behavioural response of urban birds to COVID-19 lockdown. Proc. R. Soc. B 288, 20202513. ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.2513) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Díaz M, Moller AP. 2023. Lockdown effects on fear: direct and indirect effects mediated by release of urban predators. Sci. Total Environ. 872, 162122. ( 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2023.162122) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Seress G, Sándor K, Vincze E, Pipoly I, Bukor B, Ágh N, Liker A. 2021. Contrasting effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on urban birds’ reproductive success in two cities. Sci. Rep. 11, 17649. ( 10.1038/s41598-021-96858-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Waite TA, Chhangani AK, Campbell LG, Rajpurohit LS, Mohnot SM. 2007. Sanctuary in the city: urban monkeys buffered against catastrophic die-off during ENSO-related drought. Ecohealth 4, 278-286. ( 10.1007/s10393-007-0112-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cook WM, Faeth SH. 2006. Irrigation and land use drive ground arthropod community patterns in an urban desert. Environ. Entomol. 35, 1532-1540. ( 10.1093/ee/35.6.1532) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Özgöc-Çağlar D, Katti M, Reid S, Schleder B, Bushoven J, Jones A, Delcore H. 2011. Interactions between urban water policy, residential irrigation, and plant & bird diversity in the Fresno-Clovis Metro Area. Nat. Preced. ( 10.1038/npre.2011.6228.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jolkkonen J, Gaston KJ, Troscianko J. 2023. Artificial lighting affects the landscape of fear in a widely distributed shorebird. Commun. Biol. 6, 131. ( 10.1038/s42003-023-04486-x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Petrelli AR, Levenhagen MJ, Wardle R, Barber JR, Francis CD. 2017. First to flush: the effects of ambient noise on songbird flight initiation distances and implications for human experiences with nature. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5, 67. ( 10.3389/fevo.2017.00067) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lukas K, Stansell HM, Yeh PJ, Nonacs P. 2023. Urban junco flight initiation distances correlate with approach velocities of anthropogenic sounds. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 35, 134-144. ( 10.1080/03949370.2021.2024263) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Xu G, Xiu T, Li X, Liang X, Jiao L. 2021. Lockdown induced night-time light dynamics during the COVID-19 epidemic in global megacities. Int. J. Appl. Earth Obs. Geoinf. 102, 102421. ( 10.1016/j.jag.2021.102421) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Adam MG, Tran PTM, Balasubramanian R. 2021. Air quality changes in cities during the COVID-19 lockdown: a critical review. Atmos. Res. 264, 105823. ( 10.1016/j.atmosres.2021.105823) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Efe B. 2022. Air quality improvement and its relation to mobility during COVID-19 lockdown in Marmara Region, Turkey. Environ. Monit. Assess. 194, 255. ( 10.1007/s10661-022-09889-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Galiwango R, Bainomugisha E, Kivunike F, Kateete DP, Jjingo D. 2022. Air pollution and mobility patterns in two Ugandan cities during COVID-19 mobility restrictions suggest the validity of air quality data as a measure for human mobility. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 30, 34 856-34 871. ( 10.1007/s11356-022-24605-1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Carrete M, Tella JL. 2010. Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls: a new hypothesis on disturbance-induced habitat selection. Biol. Lett. 6, 167-170. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0739) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Carter AJ, Goldizen AW, Tromp SA. 2010. Agamas exhibit behavioral syndromes: bolder males bask and feed more but may suffer higher predation. Behav. Ecol. 21, 655-661. ( 10.1093/beheco/arq036) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Arroyo B, Mougeot F, Bretagnolle V. 2017. Individual variation in behavioural responsiveness to humans leads to differences in breeding success and long-term population phenotypic changes. Ecol. Lett. 20, 317-325. ( 10.1111/ele.12729) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Chan AAY-H, Giraldo-Perez P, Smith S, Blumstein DT. 2010. Anthropogenic noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hypothesis. Biol. Lett. 6, 458-461. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1081) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Chan AAY-H, Blumstein DT. 2011. Attention, noise, and implications for wildlife conservation and management. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 131, 1-7. ( 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.01.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Diamant ES, MacGregor-Fors I, Blumstein DT, Yeh PJ. 2023. Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings. bioRxiv. ( 10.1101/2023.01.04.522762) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 62.Diamant ES, MacGregor-Fors I, Blumstein DT, Yeh PJ. 2023. Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings. Figshare. Dataset. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.21743810) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 63.Diamant ES, MacGregor-Fors I, Blumstein DT, Yeh PJ. 2023. Electronic Supplementary Material from Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.23899330.v2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Citations

  1. Diamant ES, MacGregor-Fors I, Blumstein DT, Yeh PJ. 2023. Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings. Figshare. Dataset. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.21743810) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  2. Diamant ES, MacGregor-Fors I, Blumstein DT, Yeh PJ. 2023. Electronic Supplementary Material from Urban birds become less fearful following COVID-19 reopenings. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.23899330.v2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Data Availability Statement

Data and code are accessible via Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.21743810 [62]. Supplementary material is available online at [63].


Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES