Abstract
Background
Tree nuts and peanuts (henceforth, nuts) are nutrient-dense foods rich in neuroprotective components; thus, their consumption could benefit cognitive health. However, evidence to date is limited and inconsistent regarding the potential benefits of nuts for cognitive function.
Objective
To prospectively evaluate the association between nut consumption and 2-y changes in cognitive performance in older adults at cognitive decline risk.
Methods
A total of 6,630 participants aged 55 to 75 y (mean age 65.0±4.9 y, 48.4% women) with overweight/obesity and metabolic syndrome completed a validated semi-quantitative food frequency questionnaire and a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests at baseline and a 2-y follow-up. Composite cognitive scores were used to assess global, general, attention, and executive function domains. Nut consumption was categorized as <1, ≥1 to <3, ≥3 to <7, and ≥7 servings/wk (1 serving=30 g). Multivariable-adjusted linear regression models were fitted to assess associations between baseline nut consumption and 2-y cognitive changes.
Results
Nut consumption was positively associated with 2-y changes in general cognitive function (P-trend <0.001). Compared with participants consuming <1 serving/wk of nuts, those categorized as consuming ≥3 to <7 and ≥7 servings/wk showed more favorable changes in general cognitive performance (β z-score [95% CI] = 0.06 [0.00,0.12] and 0.13 [0.06,0.20], respectively). No significant changes were observed in the multivariable-adjusted models for other cognitive domains assessed.
Conclusion
Frequent nut consumption was associated with a smaller decline in general cognitive performance over 2 y in older adults at risk of cognitive decline. Randomized clinical trials to verify our findings are warranted.
Keywords: aging, nuts, unsaturated fatty acids, cognitive decline, cognition, older people
Introduction
Beyond the usual consequences of biological aging, a decline of cognitive function is a common harbinger of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease, which is recognized as a public health priority [1]. By 2050, dementia prevalence is estimated to triple, and currently, there are no effective curative treatments available [1,2]. Therefore, proactive strategies targeting modifiable risk factors to prevent or delay the onset or progression of cognitive decline are imperative [2]. Diet is considered an important modifiable lifestyle factor and plays a pivotal role in controlling other modifiable risk factors shared by cardiovascular and neurodegenerative diseases, such as hypertension, obesity, and diabetes [2].
Tree nuts and peanuts (henceforth, nuts), characteristic components of commonly recommended healthy dietary patterns for sustaining optimal cognitive function [[3], [4], [5], [6]], are nutrient-dense foods with antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties [7]. The myriad of nutrients and biologically active compounds that nuts contain, such as unsaturated fatty acids, high-quality vegetable protein, an array of vitamins and beneficial minerals, dietary fiber, phenolic compounds, and phytosterols, appear to have neuroprotective effects [7,8]. In addition to the established associations of nut consumption with reduced cardiovascular [9,10] and cardiometabolic [11] disease outcomes and its potentially beneficial relationship with body weight and body fat reduction [12], diabetes [13,14], hypertension [15], and depression [16], nuts may also benefit brain health.
Epidemiological evidence for the association between nut consumption and cognitive performance is limited. Most cross-sectional studies have supported a positive relationship between nut consumption and cognitive function [7,17,18]. However, results from prospective studies of the association between nut consumption and cognitive decline or changes in cognitive performance over time have been inconsistent [7,17,18]. Similarly, randomized clinical trials (RCTs) examining the effects of nut intake on cognitive outcomes are few, and results have been inconsistent, showing either beneficial or no effects on cognitive decline [7,17,18].
Overall, evidence to date regarding the beneficial impact of nut consumption on cognitive function and/or cognitive decline remains inconclusive, though more favorable outcomes tend to be observed in populations at a higher risk of cognitive decline and dementia (e.g., older age, less education, obesity, cardiometabolic risk, etc.) [18,19]. Therefore, to establish nut-related dietary strategies to prevent or delay cognitive decline and eventually dementia, further investigation targeting populations at high risk is warranted.
Considering the limitations of the evidence from current cross-sectional studies and challenges associated with conducting RCTs, long-term and large-scale prospective cohort studies are needed to reduce the knowledge gaps previously mentioned [5]. Therefore, this study aimed to prospectively investigate the association between nut consumption and 2-y changes in cognitive performance in older Spanish adults with overweight/obesity harboring the metabolic syndrome, thus being at risk of cognitive decline, in the large cohort of the PREDIMED-Plus (PREvención con DIeta MEDiterránea) trial.
Methods
Study design and participants
The present longitudinal cohort study was conducted within the framework of the PREDIMED-Plus trial, based on the assessment of baseline nut consumption and measures of cognitive function assessed at baseline and 2 y of follow-up. Detailed information about the PREDIMED-Plus study protocol is available at https://www.predimedplus.com/en/, Appendix A, and elsewhere [20,21]. The research ethics committees of all centers approved the study protocol, and participants provided written informed consent. The trial was registered in 2014 at: [www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN89898870]. Eligible participants were community-dwelling adults aged 55 to 75 y, with overweight or obesity (BMI 27 to 40 kg/m2), who at baseline met at least 3 criteria for the metabolic syndrome [22]. For the present study, participants who had not completed the baseline dietary questionnaire had reported energy intakes outside predefined limits (≥800 to ≤ 4000 kcal/d for men, ≥500 to ≤3500 kcal/d for women) [23] or had reported nut consumption of ≥100g/d [24] were excluded.
Exposure: nut consumption
A validated semi-quantitative FFQ [25] was administered by trained dietitians in individual interviews to assess the habitual consumption of 143 food items during the previous year. Nut consumption at baseline was considered the exposure variable in the present study. The FFQ contained 4 questions related to nuts (i.e., almonds, pistachios, walnuts, and other nuts) with 9 possible frequency categories ranging from ‘never to almost never’ to ‘> 6 servings/day’ (serving size = 30 g). Intraclass correlation coefficients for relative reproducibility and validity for nut intake were 0.80 and 0.55, respectively [25]. Information collected from the FFQ was converted to grams per day by multiplying the numerical value of each frequency category by the nut serving size (30 g/serving) and was further recoded into 4 categories based on the distribution of the original nut consumption measure (right-skewed) of the study population, globally recommended quantities of nut intake [26], and prior studies [24,27]: <1 serving/wk, ≥1 to <3 servings/wk, ≥3 to <7 servings/wk, and ≥7 servings/wk.
Outcome: cognitive performance
Cognitive performance was assessed by trained personnel at baseline and at the 2-y follow-up visit. A battery of 8 neuropsychological tests, validated for the Spanish population, including MMSE, the Clock Drawing Test (CDT), the Verbal Fluency Tests (VFTs), the forward and backward version of the Digit Span Test (DST-f and DST-b, respectively) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III), and the Trail Making Test part A (TMT-A) and B (TMT-B), were administered in personal interviews. Appendix A provides a detailed description of the neuropsychological tests assessed.
Each cognitive test at baseline and at 2-y follow-up was standardized for each participant to a z-score using the mean and SD of baseline data, and the difference between the standardized scores at 2 timepoints was calculated to examine changes in cognitive performance over time [28,29]. Composite measures for 3 cognitive domains (attention, executive function, and general cognition) and one global assessment of cognitive function (GCF) were calculated for each participant. Composite cognitive assessments were determined by adding or subtracting each individual test z-score based on whether a higher score indicates higher or lower cognitive performance, respectively, as shown in Appendix B. Following this, these 4 composite scores were further re-standardized to z-scores using the mean and SD of baseline composite score data. The 2-y change in the 4 composite scores was considered the primary outcome of interest in the present study.
Covariate assessment
Sociodemographic, lifestyle, medical history, food consumption, and anthropometric information were collected by trained personnel at baseline. Depressive symptomatology was evaluated by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) with the cut-off point established for depressive status risk as a score ≥14 [30,31]. Appendix A provides a detailed description of the covariate assessment.
Statistical analyses
Baseline characteristics of the study population were presented and compared by nut consumption categories as means ± SD using one-way ANOVA for continuous variables and numbers (percentages) using the chi-square test for categorical variables. The associations between nut consumption (exposure) either as continuous or categorical (with the first group [<1 serving/wk] as the reference category) and 2-y changes in each of the cognitive function measurements (outcome) were analyzed fitting multivariable linear regression models and presented as β-coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (CI). If a given cognitive function assessment was missing, this test was not included in the analysis for that participant. Models are presented and fully adjusted for potential confounders. Interaction, stratified, and sensitivity analyses were further performed as appropriate. Appendix A provides a detailed description of the statistical analyses.
Results
A total of 6,630 participants (mean age 65.0 ± 4.9 y, 48.4% women) were included in the study. For details regarding the number of individuals assessed in each cognitive outcome analysis, see Appendix C. Baseline sociodemographic, anthropometric, and lifestyle factors, medical history, and cognitive performance were similar between the primary and analyzed samples (Appendix G).
Baseline characteristics of the study population overall and by categories of total nut consumption at baseline are presented in Table 1 and Appendix D. At baseline, the mean daily nut consumption ranged from 1.7 ± 1.8 g in the lowest to 43.7 ± 14.5 g in the highest category (overall mean: 14.6 ± 16.2 g/d). Walnuts were the most consumed nut among the considered nut types (overall mean: 7.0 ± 9.4 g/d). Participants in the highest category of nut consumption were more likely to have higher education levels, to partake in more physical activity, and to show better adherence to the Mediterranean diet (MedDiet) with greater consumption of vegetables, fruits, legumes, fish, and olive oil, compared to those in the lowest category. A lower BMI, waist circumference, and a lower percentage of current smokers and participants with depressive symptoms, were also observed in the highest compared with the lowest nut consumption category.
TABLE 1.
Total Nut Consumption |
P2 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total | <1 s/wk1 | ≥1 to <3 s/wk | ≥3 to <7 s/wk | ≥7 s/wk | ||
Frequency, n | 6,630 | 2,432 | 1,796 | 1,306 | 1,096 | |
Total nuts, g/day, median [IQR] | 9 [2, 24] | 2 [0, 4] | 9 [6, 13] | 21 [17, 26] | 39 [32, 51] | <0.001 |
Walnuts | 2 [0, 13] | 0 [0, 0] | 2 [2, 4] | 13 [4, 13] | 24 [13, 30] | <0.001 |
Almonds | 2 [0, 4] | 0 [0, 0] | 2 [0, 4] | 4 [2, 13] | 13 [0, 13] | <0.001 |
Other nuts | 2 [0, 4] | 0 [0, 0] | 2 [0, 4] | 4 [0, 9] | 13 [0, 17] | <0.001 |
Sociodemographic variables | ||||||
Age, y | 65.0 ± 4.9 | 65.0 ± 4.9 | 64.8 ± 5.0 | 64.9 ± 4.9 | 65.4 ± 4.7 | 0.022 |
Women, n (%) | 3,210 (48.4) | 1,203 (49.5) | 873 (48.6) | 618 (47.3) | 516 (47.1) | 0.473 |
Education level, n (%) | ||||||
Primary or less | 3,266 (49.3) | 1,233 (50.7) | 874 (48.7) | 664 (50.8) | 495 (45.2) | 0.006 |
Secondary | 1,913 (28.9) | 717 (29.5) | 517 (28.8) | 352 (27.0) | 327 (29.8) | |
College | 1,451 (21.9) | 482 (19.8) | 405 (22.6) | 290 (22.2) | 274 (25.0) | |
Civil status, n (%) | ||||||
Single, divorced, or separated | 854 (12.9) | 341 (14.0) | 208 (11.6) | 154 (11.8) | 151 (13.8) | 0.109 |
Married | 5,088 (76.7) | 1,833 (75.4) | 1,391 (77.5) | 1,030 (78.9) | 834(76.1) | |
Widower | 688 (10.4) | 258 (10.6) | 197 (11.0) | 122 (9.3) | 111 (10.1) | |
Disease present at recruitment | ||||||
Type 2 diabetes, n (%) | 2,041 (30.8) | 778 (32.0) | 533 (29.7) | 398 (30.5) | 332 (30.3) | 0.411 |
Hypertension, n (%) | 5,570 (84.0) | 2,078 (85.4) | 1,494 (83.2) | 1,091 (83.5) | 907 (82.8) | 0.105 |
Hypercholesterolemia, n (%) | 4,637 (69.9) | 1,661 (68.3) | 1,269 (70.7) | 934 (71.5) | 773 (70.5) | 0.152 |
Medication use, n (%) | ||||||
Insulin or other antidiabetic drugs | 1,768 (26.7) | 659 (27.1) | 476 (26.5) | 350 (26.8) | 283 (25.8) | 0.881 |
Antihypertensive agents | 5,204 (78.5) | 1,947 (80.1) | 1,395 (77.7) | 1,015 (77.7) | 847 (77.3) | 0.129 |
Statins or other hypolipidemic drugs | 3,453 (52.1) | 1,253 (51.5) | 945 (52.6) | 678 (51.9) | 577 (52.7) | 0.881 |
Depressive symptomatology, n (%) | 1,366 (20.6) | 547 (22.5) | 368 (20.5) | 239 (18.3) | 212 (19.3) | 0.014 |
Anthropometric variables | ||||||
BMI, kg/m2 | 32.6 ± 3.4 | 32.9 ± 3.5 | 32.6 ± 3.4 | 32.4 ± 3.5 | 32.0 ± 3.2 | <0.001 |
Waist circumference, cm | ||||||
Women | 104.0 ± 9.2 | 105.0 ± 9.2 | 103.9 ± 9.3 | 103.1 ± 8.9 | 102.7 ± 9.2 | <0.001 |
Men | 110.9 ± 8.8 | 111.8 ± 9.1 | 110.8 ± 8.4 | 110.5 ± 8.7 | 109.9 ± 8.5 | <0.001 |
Lifestyle variables | ||||||
Smoking status, n (%) | ||||||
Current smoker | 821 (12.4) | 349 (14.4) | 203 (11.3) | 143 (11.0) | 126 (11.5) | 0.029 |
Former smoker | 2,865 (43.2) | 1,030 (42.4) | 789 (43.9) | 567 (43.4) | 479 (43.7) | |
Never smoked | 2,944 (44.4) | 1,053 (43.3) | 804 (44.8) | 596 (45.6) | 491 (44.8) | |
Physical activity, METs/min/day | 352.0 ± 329.3 | 326.8 ± 330.2 | 343.9 ± 310.2 | 379.7 ± 339.4 | 388.2 ± 340.2 | <0.001 |
Cognitive performance assessment, raw scores | ||||||
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) | (n = 6,442) 28.2 ± 1.9 | (n = 2,358) 28.1 ± 2.0 | (n = 1,752) 28.3 ± 1.9 | (n = 1,274) 28.3 ± 1.8 | (n = 1,058) 28.2 ± 1.9 | 0.021 |
Clock Drawing Test (CDT) | (n = 6,446) 5.9 ± 1.2 | (n = 2,358) 5.9 ± 1.3 | (n = 1,753) 6.0 ± 1.2 | (n = 1,274) 5.9 ± 1.2 | (n = 1,061) 6.0 ± 1.2 | 0.495 |
Verbal Fluency tasks semantical (VFT-a) | (n = 6,580) 16.0 ± 4.9 | (n = 2,416) 16.0 ± 5.0 | (n = 1,782) 15.9 ± 4.8 | (n = 1,295) 16.1 ± 4.8 | (n = 1,087) 16.2 ± 4.9 | 0.374 |
Verbal Fluency tasks phonological (VFT-p) | (n = 6,580) 12.2 ± 4.5 | (n = 2,416) 12.1 ± 4.6 | (n = 1,782) 12.1 ± 4.6 | (n = 1,295) 12.3 ± 4.4 | (n = 1,087) 12.4 ± 4.4 | 0.198 |
Trail Making Test Part A3 (TMT-A) | (n = 6,565) 52.8 ± 28.6 | (n = 2,414) 53.8 ± 30.0 | (n = 1,779) 53.2 ± 29.3 | (n = 1,294) 52.2 ± 27.4 | (n = 1,078) 50.9 ± 25.2 | 0.038 |
Trail Making Test Part B3 (TMT-B) | (n = 6,547) 130.1 ± 72.4 | (n = 2,403) 131.4 ± 73.6 | (n = 1,774) 131.0 ± 73.2 | (n = 1,293) 128.6 ± 70.1 | (n = 1,077) 127.5 ± 70.9 | 0.389 |
Digit Span test forward (DST-f) | (n = 5,653) 8.8 ± 2.5 | (n = 2,092) 8.8 ± 2.5 | (n = 1,516) 8.7 ± 2.5 | (n = 1,118) 8.9 ± 2.5 | (n = 927) 8.9 ± 2.4 | 0.322 |
Digit Span test backward (DST-b) | (n = 5,650) 5.1 ± 2.2 | (n = 2,090) 5.1 ± 2.2 | (n = 1,516) 5.0 ± 2.2 | (n = 1,118) 5.1 ± 2.3 | (n = 926) 5.2 ± 2.2 | 0.441 |
Data are presented as n (%) and mean ± SD or median [IQR] for categorical and continuous variables, respectively.
Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; METs, metabolic equivalents; s/wk, serving(s) per week.
1 serving=30 g.
P value for differences between categories of total nut consumption was calculated by Pearson’s Chi-square test or one-way ANOVA, as appropriate.
Inverse neuropsychological assessment score, lower scores represent better cognitive performance.
Figure 1 and Appendix E show the associations (β coefficients and 95% CI) between baseline nut consumption and changes in cognitive performance over the 2-y follow-up period. After adjusting for potential confounding factors, nut consumption was positively associated with 2-y changes in cognitive performance, as assessed by the general cognitive function composite score (P-trend<0.001) and the CDT score (P-trend <0.001). In particular, in the multivariable-adjusted models where nut consumption was treated as a continuous variable, a 1-serving/day higher nut consumption was associated with a 0.07 z-score more favorable change in general cognitive function (95% CI: 0.03 to 0.12, p<0.001), and a 0.09 z-score more favorable change in CDT (95% CI: 0.04 to 0.14, P<0.001 (Appendix E). In addition, nut consumption was also significantly associated with a lower cognitive decline for the GCF (P-trend=0.015) in the basic model (Appendix E). This association attenuated after additional adjustment for cardiovascular disease risk factors. Furthermore, results in relation to the MMSE screening test showed that participants who consumed ≥3 to <7 servings per wk of nuts had better cognitive performance evolution after the 2-y follow-up than those who consumed less than 1 serving per wk of nuts (β=0.08 z-score, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.14, P=0.011). This significant finding did not extend to the highest category (≥7 servings per wk) of nut consumption, and no overall trend between MMSE score changes and nut consumption was observed (P-trend=0.078).
The associations of nut consumption with 2-y changes in cognitive function remained similar in all sensitivity analyses (Appendix E). We found no significant interactions between nut consumption and sex, education level, smoking status, randomized intervention group, or the presence of type 2 diabetes, hypertension, and hypercholesterolemia (all p for interaction values >0.05; data not shown). Conversely, there were significant synergistic multiplicative interactions between nut consumption and depressive status in relation to changes in GCF (p=0.041), general cognitive function (p=0.013), attention (p=0.043), MMSE (p=0.013), and DST-f (p=0.004) (Appendix F). In the stratified analysis, we also observed that over the 2-y follow-up, higher nut consumption was significantly associated with less cognitive decline in individuals with depression at baseline but not in those without depression (data not shown).
Discussion
The present study provides novel prospective findings of the associations between nut consumption at baseline and subsequent changes in cognitive function. Our results show that a higher frequency of nut consumption is associated with favorable changes in the general cognitive function composite and CDT score, suggesting a potential dose-response relationship between nut consumption and a delay in cognitive decline over the period of 2 y, regardless of the baseline cognitive status, sociodemographic, cardiometabolic, and lifestyle factors including diet, in older adults with overweight/obesity, metabolic syndrome, and at risk of cognitive decline. These findings were robust in the sensitivity analyses performed. We also found a potential synergistic interaction between nut consumption and depression, suggesting that participants with depressive symptomatology at baseline tended to benefit more from the consumption of nuts than those without depression.
The observed positive association between nut consumption and cognitive performance is in line with findings from most existing cross-sectional studies, in which nut consumption has been associated with better cognitive performance and a lower risk of cognitive impairment [18]. Our findings concur in part with the results of prior prospective studies conducted in different middle-aged and older populations, showing that nut consumption was associated with less cognitive decline [32], better cognitive performance [32,33], and related to a lower risk of cognitive impairment [[34], [35], [36]]. Yet, in some of these prospective studies, the sample size was limited [32]; the temporal relationship could not be determined since no cognitive assessment was conducted at baseline when collecting nut consumption data [34]; and in one study, the associations between nut consumption and the trend in delayed cognitive decline weakened after additional adjustment for cardiovascular disease risk factors [33]. In 2 prospective cohort studies involving 15,467 US female nurses aged 70 y and older [37] and a population-weighted sample of 3,632 older US adults [38], cross-sectional associations between nut consumption and better cognitive performance were consistently observed. Still, unlike what was revealed in our study, none of them found an association between nut consumption and potential cognitive decline postponement over time. The weakened variability in cognitive change that could be attributed to nut consumption in these studies may be due to various factors, such as the advanced age of the participants (mean age 74 y), i.e., many older adults might have already experienced cognitive decline before inclusion in these studies [37,38]; the relative homogeneity and high education level of the population studied [37] since higher former education levels and lifelong higher educational attainment is a strong protective factor against cognitive decline and dementia risk [2]; and the use of telephone-based assessments for cognitive outcomes, which were more prone to measurement bias and less accurate to detect cognitive changes [37,38].
Evidence from clinical trials assessing the effects of nut intake on cognitive function is limited. Two PREDIMED sub-studies reported improvements in cognitive function in those participants randomized to a MedDiet intervention supplemented with 30 g/d mixed nuts after a mean of 6.5 y or 4.1 y, compared with those in the low-fat control diet arm [39,40]. The effects of different types of nut consumption (e.g., almonds [41,42], walnuts [19], and peanuts [43]) on cognitive performance and cognitive decline in middle-aged and older adults were inconsistent, either showing improvements in cognitive function [42,43] or no effect in delaying cognitive decline [19,41], but in no case was worse cognitive status observed compared with control diets. Some possible reasons for this inconsistency might be due to the duration of the interventions, the sample size studied, the background diets, the sociodemographic-economic and health status of the study populations, and most importantly, the overall risk of cognitive impairment/dementia, as persons at a higher risk (such as the PREDIMED-Plus population) are those that can benefit most from an intervention with neuroprotective agents such as certain nut components [18,19,39,40].
Several potential mechanisms underlying the associations between nut intake and cognitive function have been proposed. Oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, and reduced blood flow are considered crucial mechanisms that trigger the formation of arterial plaques, induce cell death and organic dysfunction, leading to cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases [17]. Evidence has accumulated on the beneficial role of nuts in improving antioxidant status, endothelial function, peripheral inflammation [7,18,44], and of nut constituents in reducing amyloid-beta (Aβ) production, aggregation, and Aβ-induced toxicity [8]. Moreover, nut consumption has also been associated with reduced incidence of, and mortality from, different cardiovascular diseases [7,10] and reductions in body weight and adiposity gain [12]. Moreover, clinical trials with nuts have demonstrated beneficial effects on blood pressure [15], blood lipid profile [7], and insulin sensitivity [13]. Given that cardiometabolic disorders and cognitive decline share similar vascular risk factors [2,7,18], nut consumption might benefit cognitive function via modulation of most vascular risks. In the present study, nut consumption was significantly associated with a more favorable change in GCF; however, this association weakened after further adjustment for cardiovascular disease risk factors, suggesting that nut consumption may partially postpone cognitive decline by reducing the cardiovascular disease risk profile. The neurological and cardiometabolic benefits of nuts may be explained by their nutrient-dense profile and high content of bioactive compounds. Nuts are rich in nutrients like vitamin E, folate, carotenoids, and selenium, as well as different phenolic compounds, which are potent antioxidant and anti-inflammatory agents [7,8]. Nuts also contain a high amount of unsaturated fatty acids (particularly, linoleic and α-linolenic acids in walnuts) that have vasculo-protective and anti-inflammatory effects, besides their indispensable roles in maintaining neuronal structure and function, modulating neuronal plasticity and various metabolic processes [8,45,46]. The high-quality vegetable protein and amino acids found in nuts, like L-arginine, can exert neuroprotective effects via anti-atherosclerotic and antioxidative mechanisms, modulating neuroinflammation and improving endothelial function [7,8]. The optimal mineral composition of nuts, being high in calcium, magnesium, and potassium but devoid of sodium, has demonstrated beneficial effects on blood pressure and insulin sensitivity [15,47]. Furthermore, the high content of dietary fiber, along with unsaturated fat and polyphenols, may potentially modulate gut microbiota and, subsequently, cognitive function through the “gut-brain axis” [7,48].
The major strengths of our study include: 1) its prospective study design with a 2-y follow-up period, whereby beneficial associations between nut consumption and cognitive function were observed in a relatively short time frame in a population at high risk; 2) the nut intake categorization in the analysis is based on common nut recommendations, hence facilitating the ability to interpret study findings; 3) the measurement of different cognitive domain changes by using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery with the construction of composite scores; 4) the control of covariables by adjusting the statistical models for several potential relevant confounders, and 5) the robustness of our results after performing several sensitivity and stratified analyses. Our study also has limitations. Firstly, due to the inherent characteristics of observational studies, the possibility of reverse causality and residual confounding from factors not assessed in our analyses (e.g., apoE carrier status) cannot be discounted; therefore, a cause-effect relationship could not be determined. Secondly, nut consumption was assessed once at baseline from a single FFQ, which may be prone to measurement error, misclassification, and recall bias as it relies on individuals’ memory; this may result in an underestimation of the associations. Lastly, the observed results in an older sample of overweight/obese individuals with cardiovascular disease risk factors may not be extended to the general population and should be interpreted with caution; however, the present findings support that the consumption of nuts might benefit cognitive performance in individuals at high risk of cognitive decline.
In conclusion, frequent nut consumption may help to delay cognitive decline in older adults with overweight/obesity, metabolic syndrome, and at risk of cognitive decline, even over a relatively short 2-y period. This finding needs to be reproduced in further epidemiological and clinical studies before dietary recommendations to delay cognitive decline and prevent or delay the onset or progression of cognitive impairment and dementia may be made.
Funding
This work was supported by the official Spanish Institutions for funding scientific biomedical research, CIBER Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBEROBN) and Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII), through the Fondo de Investigación para la Salud (FIS), which is co-funded by the European Regional Development Fund (6 coordinated FIS projects leaded by JS-S and JoV, including the following projects: PI13/00673, PI13/00492, PI13/00272, PI13/01123, PI13/00462, PI13/00233, PI13/02184, PI13/00728, PI13/01090, PI13/01056, PI14/01722, PI14/00636, PI14/00618, PI14/00696, PI14/01206, PI14/01919, PI14/00853, PI14/01374, PI14/00972, PI14/00728, PI14/01471, PI16/00473, PI16/00662, PI16/01873, PI16/01094, PI16/00501, PI16/00533, PI16/00381, PI16/00366, PI16/01522, PI16/01120, PI17/00764, PI17/01183, PI17/00855, PI17/01347, PI17/00525, PI17/01827, PI17/00532, PI17/00215, PI17/01441, PI17/00508, PI17/01732, PI17/00926, PI19/00957, PI19/00386, PI19/00309, PI19/01032, PI19/00576, PI19/00017, PI19/01226, PI19/00781, PI19/01560, PI19/01332, PI20/01802, PI20/00138, PI20/01532, PI20/00456, PI20/00339, PI20/00557, PI20/00886, PI20/01158); the Especial Action Project entitled: Implementación y evaluación de una intervención intensiva sobre la actividad física Cohorte PREDIMED-Plus grant and the Recercaixa (number 2013ACUP00194) grant to JS-S; grants from the European Research Council (Advanced Research Grant 2014–2019; agreement #340918); grants from the Consejería de Salud de la Junta de Andalucía (PI0458/2013, PS0358/2016, PI0137/2018); the PROMETEO/2017/017 and PROMETEO/2021/021 grants from the Consellería de Innovación, Universidades, Ciencia y Sociedad digital de la Generalitat Valenciana; Grant PID2019-108858RB-I00 funded by AEI 10.13039/501100011033 and by “ERDF A way of making Europe”; the SEMERGEN grant; the AICO/2021/347 grants from the Generalitat Valenciana. This research was also partially funded by EU-H2020 Grants (Eat2beNICE/ H2020-SFS-2016-2); and the Horizon 2020 PRIME study (Prevention and Remediation of Insulin Multimorbidity in Europe; grant agreement #847879). JN is supported by a predoctoral grant from Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (FPU 20/00385). SKN is supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). JS-S, the senior author of this paper, was partially supported by ICREA under the ICREA Academia program. None of the funding sources took part in the design, collection, analysis, interpretation of the data, writing the report, or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Author disclosures
JN reported receiving a research grant for an unrelated project from the INC International Nut and Dried Fruit Council through her institution. SKN is a volunteer member of the not-for-profit group Plant-Based Canada and has received a research grant for an unrelated project from the INC International Nut and Dried Fruit Council through her institution. NB has received a research grant for an unrelated project from the INC International Nut and Dried Fruit Council through her institution. ER reports receiving grant support through his institution from the California Walnut Commission (Folsom, CA), in addition to personal funds for project supervision and advice and serving as a non-paid member of its Scientific Advisory Committee; funds for travel and accommodation from the International Nut and Dried Fruit Council; and personal funds from Alexion for serving on the advisory committee. RE reported receiving grants from Fundación Dieta Mediterránea, Spain and Cerveza y Salud, Spain, and personal fees for given lectures from Brewers of Europe, Belgium, Fundación Cerveza y Salud, Spain, Instituto Cervantes, Alburquerque, USA; Instituto Cervantes, Milan, Italy, Lilly Laboratories, Spain, and Wine and Culinary International Forum, Spain, and nonfinancial support to organize a National Congress on Nutrition, and feeding trials with product from Grand Fountain and Uriach Laboratories, Spain. FF-A reports consultation fees from Novo Nordisk and editor-in-Chief honoraria from Wiley. AS-V has received research funding through his institution and support to attend professional meetings from the California Walnut Commission (CWC, Folsom, CA). JS-S reported receiving research support from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia, the European Commission, the USA National Institutes of Health; receiving consulting fees or travel expenses from Eroski Foundation, Instituto Danone, Nestle, and Abbott Laboratories, receiving nonfinancial support from Hojiblanca, Patrimonio Comunal Olivarero, the California Walnut Commission, Almond Board of California, La Morella Nuts, Pistachio Growers and Borges SA; serving on the board of and receiving grant support through his institution from the INC International Nut and Dried Fruit Council and the Eroski Foundation; and personal fees from Instituto Danone; Serving in the Board of Danone Institute International. The rest of the authors (i.e., FJB-G, DC, CO, JAM, AMA-G, JW, JeV, DR, JL-M, FJT, JMS-L, LS-M, NCI, JAT, JMF-G, XP, MD-R, PM-M, JoV, CV, LD, FF-A, MR-C, CMS, OP, AG-R, LMC-G, EG-G, MAZ, AC, RC, SM-D, LT-S, AMG-P, ET, SR, CO-A, RDLT, PJP-O, MG-DLH, CS-O, MM) have declared no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.
Author responsibilities
JS-S, DC, JAM, ÁMA-G, JW, JeV, DR, JL-M, RE, FJT, LS-M, JAT, XP, MD-R, PM-M, JoV, CV, LD, and ER contributed to study concept and design, conducting the study and to data collection and management from the participants of PREDIMED-Plus study. JN, SKN, NB and JS-S contributed to the present study concept and design, and drafted the manuscript. JN and SKN performed the statistical analyses. All authors reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content and approved the final version to be published.
Data Availability
Data described in the manuscript, code book, and analytic code will be made available in a controlled data-sharing collaboration model. Investigators who are interested in this study can contact the PREDIMED Steering Committee by sending a request letter to predimed_scommittee@googlegroups.com. A data-sharing agreement indicating the characteristics of the collaboration and data management will be completed for the proposals that are approved by the Steering Committee.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the PREDIMED-Plus participants for their enthusiastic collaboration, the PREDIMED-Plus personnel for outstanding support, and the personnel of all associated primary care centers for their exceptional effort. CIBEROBN, CIBERESP, and CIBERDEM are initiatives of the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII), Madrid, Spain. The Hojiblanca (Lucena, Spain) and Patrimonio Comunal Olivarero (Madrid, Spain) food companies donated extra-virgin olive oil. The Almond Board of California (Modesto, CA), American Pistachio Growers (Fresno, CA), and Paramount Farms (Wonderful Company, LLC, Los Angeles, CA) donated nuts for the PREDIMED-Plus pilot study. The Cátedra URV-World Forum for Nutrition Research on Tree Nuts and Dried Fruits for its support. The authors also thank the PREDIMED-Plus Biobank Network as a part of the National Biobank Platform of the ISCIII for storing and managing the PREDIMED-Plus biological samples. The Institut de Recerca en Nutrició i Seguretata Alimentaria (INSA-UB). The University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, is recognized as a Maria de Maeztu Unit of Excellence grant CEX2021-001234-M) funded by MICIN/AEI/FEDER, UE).
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajcnut.2023.05.032.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following is the Supplementary data to this article:
References
- 1.World Health Organisation . World Health Organization; Geneva: 2017. Global action plan on the public health response to dementia 2017 - 2025; p. 52.http://www.who.int/mental_health/neurology/dementia/action_plan_2017_2025/en/ [Internet] Available from: [Google Scholar]
- 2.Livingston G., Huntley J., Sommerlad A., Ames D., Ballard C., Banerjee S., et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396:413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Van Den Brink A.C., Brouwer-Brolsma E.M., Berendsen A.A.M., Van De Rest O. The Mediterranean, dietary approaches to stop hypertension (DASH), and Mediterranean-dash intervention for neurodegenerative delay (MIND) diets are associated with less cognitive decline and a lower risk of Alzheimer’s disease—a review. Adv. Nutr. 2019;10:1040–1065. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmz054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rajaram S., Jones J., Lee G.J. Plant-based dietary patterns, plant foods, and age-related cognitive decline. Adv. Nutr. 2019;10:S422–S436. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmz081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dalile B., Kim C., Challinor A., Geurts L., Gibney E.R., Galdos M.V., et al. The EAT–Lancet reference diet and cognitive function across the life course. Lancet Planet Health. 2022;6:e749–e759. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(22)00123-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gauci S., Young L.M., Arnoldy L., Lassemillante A.C., Scholey A., Pipingas A. Dietary patterns in middle age: effects on concurrent neurocognition and risk of age-related cognitive decline. Nutr Rev. 2022;80:1129–1159. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuab047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ros E., Singh A., O’keefe J.H. Nuts: natural pleiotropic nutraceuticals. Nutrients. 2021;13:3269. doi: 10.3390/nu13093269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gorji N., Moeini R., Memariani Z. Almond, hazelnut and walnut, three nuts for neuroprotection in Alzheimer’s disease: a neuropharmacological review of their bioactive constituents. Pharmacol. Res. 2018;129:115–127. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2017.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bitok E., Sabaté J. Nuts and cardiovascular disease. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2018;61:33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2018.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Becerra-Tomás N., Paz-Graniel I., Kendall C., Kahleova H., Rahelić D., Sievenpiper J.L., et al. Nut consumption and incidence of cardiovascular diseases and cardiovascular disease mortality: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Nutr Rev. 2019;77:691–709. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuz042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kim Y., Keogh J., Clifton P.M. Nuts and cardio-metabolic disease: a review of meta-analyses. Nutrients. 2018;10:1935. doi: 10.3390/nu10121935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nishi S.K., Viguiliouk E., Blanco Mejia S., Kendall C.W.C., Bazinet R.P., Hanley A.J., et al. Are fatty nuts a weighty concern? A systematic review and meta-analysis and dose–response meta-regression of prospective cohorts and randomized controlled trials. Obes. Rev. 2021;22 doi: 10.1111/obr.13330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tindall A.M., Johnston E.A., Kris-Etherton P.M., Petersen K.S. The effect of nuts on markers of glycemic control: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019;109:297–314. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Becerra-Tomás N., Paz-Graniel I., Hernández-Alonso P., Jenkins D.J.A., Kendall C.W.C., Sievenpiper J.L., et al. Nut consumption and type 2 diabetes risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021;113:960–971. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mohammadifard N., Salehi-Abargouei A., Salas-Salvadó J., Guasch-Ferré M., Humphries K., Sarrafzadegan N. The effect of tree nut, peanut, and soy nut consumption on blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled clinical trials. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015;101:966–982. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.091595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fernández-Rodríguez R., Jiménez-López E., Garrido-Miguel M., Martínez-Ortega I.A., Martínez-Vizcaíno V., Mesas A.E. Does the evidence support a relationship between higher levels of nut consumption, lower risk of depression, and better mood state in the general population? A systematic review. Nutr. Rev. 2022;80:2076–2088. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuac022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Theodore L.E., Kellow N.J., McNeil E.A., Close E.O., Coad E.G., Cardoso B.R. Nut consumption for cognitive performance: a systematic review. Adv. Nutr. 2021;12:777–792. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmaa153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Nishi S.K., Sala-Vila A., Julvez J., Sabaté J., Ros E. Impact of nut consumption on cognition across the lifespan. Nutrients. 2023;15:1000. doi: 10.3390/nu15041000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sala-Vila A., Valls-Pedret C., Rajaram S., Coll-Padrós N., Cofán M., Serra-Mir M., et al. Effect of a 2-year diet intervention with walnuts on cognitive decline. The walnuts and healthy aging (WAHA) study: a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2020;111:590–600. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Salas-Salvadó J., Díaz-López A., Ruiz-Canela M., Basora J., Fitó M., Corella D., et al. Effect of a lifestyle intervention program with energy-restricted mediterranean diet and exercise on weight loss and cardiovascular risk factors: one-year results of the PREDIMED-Plus trial. Diabetes Care. 2019;42:777–788. doi: 10.2337/dc18-0836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sayón-Orea C., Razquin C., Bulló M., Corella D., Fitó M., Romaguera D., et al. Effect of a nutritional and behavioral intervention on energy-reduced Mediterranean diet adherence among patients with metabolic syndrome: interim analysis of the PREDIMED-Plus randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2019;322:1486–1499. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.14630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Alberti K.G.M.M., Eckel R.H., M Grundy S., Zimmet P.Z., Cleeman J.I., Donato K.A., et al. Harmonizing the metabolic syndrome. Circulation. 2009;120:1640–1645. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.192644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Willett W. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2012. Nutritional Epidemiology. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ibarrola-Jurado N., Bulló M., Guasch-Ferré M., Ros E., Martínez-González M.A., Corella D., et al. Cross-sectional assessment of nut consumption and obesity, metabolic syndrome and other cardiometabolic risk factors: the PREDIMED study. PLoS One. 2013;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fernández-Ballart J.D., Piñol J.L., Zazpe I., Corella D., Carrasco P., Toledo E., et al. Relative validity of a semi-quantitative food-frequency questionnaire in an elderly Mediterranean population of Spain. Br. J. Nutr. 2010;103:1808–1816. doi: 10.1017/S0007114509993837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Neale E.P., Tran G., Brown R.C. Barriers and facilitators to nut consumption: a narrative review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:9127. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17239127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Julibert A., del Mar Bibiloni M., Gallardo-Alfaro L., Abbate M., Martínez-González M.Á., Salas-Salvadó J., et al. Metabolic syndrome features and excess weight were inversely associated with nut consumption after 1-year follow-up in the PREDIMED-Plus study. J. Nutr. 2020;150:3161–3170. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxaa289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Duff K. Evidence-based indicators of neuropsychological change in the individual patient: relevant concepts and methods. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012;27:248–261. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nooyens A.C.J., Baan C.A., Spijkerman A.M.W., Verschuren Monique W.M. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the doetinchem cohort study. Diabetes Care. 2010;33:1964–1969. doi: 10.2337/dc09-2038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sanz J., Luis A., Carmelo P., Resumen V. Adaptación española del Inventario para la depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): 2. Propiedades psicométricas en población general. / The spanish adaptation of Beck’s Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II): 2. Psychometric properties in the general population. Clin. Salud. 2003;14:249–280. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Beck A., Steer R., Brown G., BDI-II . The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1996. Beck depression inventory–II-Second Edition Manual. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rabassa M., Zamora-Ros R., Palau-Rodriguez M., Tulipani S., Miñarro A., Bandinelli S., et al. Habitual nut exposure, assessed by dietary and multiple urinary metabolomic markers, and cognitive decline in older adults: the InCHIANTI study. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2020;64:1–11. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.201900532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nooyens A.C.J., Bueno-De-Mesquita H.B., Van Boxtel M.P.J., Van Gelder B.M., Verhagen H., Verschuren W.M.M. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem cohort study. Br. J. Nutr. 2011;106:752–761. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511001024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jiang Y.W., Sheng L.T., Feng L., Pan A., Koh W.P. Consumption of dietary nuts in midlife and risk of cognitive impairment in late-life: the Singapore Chinese health study. Age Ageing. 2021;50:1215–1221. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afaa267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Li F., Jiang W., Wang J., Zhang T., Gu X., Zhai Y. Beneficial effects of nut consumption on cognitive function among elderly: findings from a 6-year cohort study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:236. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.816443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Li M., Shi Z. A Prospective association of nut consumption with cognitive function in Chinese adults aged 55+ _ China health and nutrition survey. J. Nutr. Health Aging. 2019;23:211–216. doi: 10.1007/s12603-018-1122-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.O’Brien J., Okereke O., Devore E., Rosner B., Breteler M., Grodstein F. Long-term intake of nuts in relation to cognitive function in older women. J. Nutr. Health Aging. 2014;18:496–502. doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0014-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bishop N.J., Zuniga K.E. Investigating walnut consumption and cognitive trajectories in a representative sample of older US adults. Public Health Nutr. 2021;24:1741–1752. doi: 10.1017/S1368980020001287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Martínez-Lapiscina E.H., Clavero P., Toledo E., Estruch R., Salas-Salvadó J., San Julián B., et al. Mediterranean diet improves cognition: the PREDIMED-NAVARRA randomised trial. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2013;84:1318–1325. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Valls-Pedret C., Sala-Vila A., Serra-Mir M., Corella D., De La Torre R., Martínez-González M.Á., et al. Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive decline: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern. Med. 2015;175:1094–1103. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2015.1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Coates A., Morgillo S., Yandell C., Scholey A., Buckley J., Hill A. An almond-enriched diet improves biomarkers of cardiometabolic health and increases alertness without changing cognitive performance in older overweight adults. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2020;79:375. doi: 10.3390/nu12041180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mustra Rakic J., Tanprasertsuk J., Scott T.M., Rasmussen H.M., Mohn E.S., Chen C.Y.O., et al. Effects of daily almond consumption for six months on cognitive measures in healthy middle-aged to older adults: a randomized control trial. Nutr. Neurosci. 2022;25:1466–1476. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2020.1868805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barbour J.A., Howe P.R.C., Buckley J.D., Bryan J., Coates A.M. Cerebrovascular and cognitive benefits of high-oleic peanut consumption in healthy overweight middle-aged adults. Nutr. Neurosci. 2016;20:555–562. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2016.1204744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cofán M., Rajaram S., Sala-Vila A., Valls-Pedret C., Serra-Mir M., Roth I., et al. Effects of 2-year walnut-supplemented diet on inflammatory biomarkers. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2020;76:2282–2284. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2020.07.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Scarmeas N., Anastasiou C.A., Yannakoulia M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:1006–1015. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30338-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sala-Vila A., Fleming J., Kris-Etherton P., Ros E. Impact of α-Linolenic acid, the vegetable ω-3 fatty acid, on cardiovascular disease and cognition. Adv. Nutr. 2022;13:1584–1602. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmac016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Simental-Mendía L.E., Sahebkar A., Rodríguez-Morán M., Guerrero-Romero F. A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials on the effects of magnesium supplementation on insulin sensitivity and glucose control. Pharmacol. Res. 2016;111:272–282. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2016.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.van Soest A.P.M., Hermes G.D.A., Berendsen A.A.M., van de Rest O., Zoetendal E.G., Fuentes S., et al. Associations between pro- and anti-inflammatory gastro-intestinal microbiota, diet, and cognitive functioning in Dutch healthy older adults: The NU-AGE study. Nutrients. 2020;12:3471. doi: 10.3390/nu12113471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data described in the manuscript, code book, and analytic code will be made available in a controlled data-sharing collaboration model. Investigators who are interested in this study can contact the PREDIMED Steering Committee by sending a request letter to predimed_scommittee@googlegroups.com. A data-sharing agreement indicating the characteristics of the collaboration and data management will be completed for the proposals that are approved by the Steering Committee.