Skip to main content
Chemical Senses logoLink to Chemical Senses
. 2023 Sep 20;48:bjad038. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjad038

Liking of salt is associated with depression, anxiety, and stress

Celeste Ferraris 1,, Christopher J Scarlett 2,3, Tamara Bucher 4,5,6, Emma L Beckett 7,8,9
PMCID: PMC10628984  PMID: 37738157

Abstract

Early research has shown variations in salt taste qualities in depression, anxiety, and stress. These studies evaluated changes to salt taste intensity and liking (pleasantness) of salt solutions but not of salty foods. Therefore, an Australian population survey (n = 424) was conducted where participants rated recalled intensity and liking of salt index foods and completed the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-21) to measure these states. Standard least squares regression (post hoc Tukey’s HSD) compared means between groups, and nominal logistic regression assessed differences in distributions between categories. Higher salt liking was found in participants with DASS-21 scores indicative of severe depression (68.3 vs. 60.0, P = 0.005) and severe anxiety (68.4 vs. 60.0, P = 0.001) in comparison to those with normal scores, in all models. Higher salt liking was found in participants with DASS-21 scores indicative of moderate stress (67.7 vs. 60.2, P = 0.009) in the unadjusted model only. Higher salt liking was found in females with DASS-21 scores indicative of anxiety and stress, and in males with indicative depression and anxiety. No relationships between salt taste intensity ratings and the mood states were found. Results indicate that liking salty foods is positively correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress scores. Further research on the relationships between salt liking and intake of salt and salty foods, and the biological mechanisms of these mood states are needed to direct the application of findings toward potential new risk assessment measures, dietary interventions, or therapeutics.

Keywords: DASS-21, foods, mood, sodium, survey, taste

1. Introduction

Depression and anxiety are increasingly common health conditions (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2018; National Mental Health Commission 2018) and leading contributors to the global burden of disease (James et al. 2018). Since the start of the COVID-19 pandemic, there has been an estimated 27.6% increase in depressive disorders and a 25.6% increase in anxiety disorders worldwide (Santomauro et al. 2021). Psychological stress or distress is a contributing factor to depression and anxiety (Hankin et al. 2004) and is fast becoming a significant health concern alone (Mariotti 2015; Devi et al. 2021). Diagnosis of depression, anxiety, or stress can be challenging due to unknown etiologies (Devane et al. 2005; Bystritsky et al. 2013) and their often-comorbid appearance (Groen et al. 2020; Kalin 2020). In addition, treatment effectiveness can be widely variable in depression (Khan et al. 2002; Fournier et al. 2010; Porcelli et al. 2011; Linde et al. 2015; Cipriani et al. 2018) and anxiety (Baldwin et al. 2011; Locher et al. 2017; Slee et al. 2019), and treatment side effects can be significant and lead to medication non-adherence (Gartlehner et al. 2008; Amick et al. 2015; Locher et al. 2017; Slee et al. 2019). Therefore, further elucidation of the complex etiologies and treatment methods are needed.

Diet is a well-known modifier of mental health outcomes. Diets low in whole foods are significantly related to increased risks of anxiety (McMartin et al. 2013; Nguyen et al. 2017; Brookie et al. 2018) and depression (McMartin et al. 2013; Liu et al. 2016; Brookie et al. 2018). These relationships are bi-directional and cyclical in that mental health conditions influence eating habits and dietary choices, which then impact mental health (Gardner et al. 2014; Farhangi et al. 2018). The interest in food and mood relationships has led to many studies evaluating interventions aimed at increasing diet quality to improve mental health (Opie et al. 2015; Ramsden et al. 2015; Firth et al. 2019). However, in practice, improving an individual’s diet quality is challenging and ongoing adherence is low (Kesse-Guyot et al. 2011; Kris‐Etherton et al. 2020). Research on taste qualities has provided knowledge on how we choose the foods we eat (Drewnowski et al. 1999; Beckett et al. 2014; Ervina et al. 2021; Pagliarini et al. 2021) and may be valuable in improving mental health outcomes from dietary interventions.

Variations in taste perception contribute to individual food preferences (Vink et al. 2020; Ervina et al. 2021) and therefore may modulate the intake of foods (Liem and Russell 2019) related to mental health (McMartin et al. 2013; Nguyen et al. 2017; Brookie et al. 2018). Beyond whole foods, the substrates within are also well-studied for their roles in mental health (Muscaritoli 2021). While the sensory experience of taste influences food choice (Vink et al. 2020; Ervina et al. 2021), there may also be interactions between taste and food substrates that have physiological functions or impacts on mental health. For example, diets associated with poor mental health are typically high in discretionary foods which are also high in sodium (Rees et al. 2021). Salt intake has recently been associated with depression and anxiety (Grippo et al. 2006; Morris et al. 2006, 2008, 2010; Goldstein and Leshem, 2014; Özdemir et al. 2014; Sawant et al. 2019; Bojdani et al. 2020), and correcting low serum sodium levels can improve symptoms of depression (Sawant et al. 2019; Bojdani et al. 2020). Variations in aspects of salt taste such as thresholds, intensity, and liking, can impact salt intake (Hayes et al. 2010; Pilic et al. 2020; Veček et al. 2020) and therefore, these taste qualities have been used as markers in mood research (Heath et al. 2006; Al’Absi et al. 2012; Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013; Büttner et al. 2015).

Two studies in otherwise healthy populations found people with high trait anxiety had higher salt taste thresholds (Heath et al. 2006; Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013). One of these studies induced stress in people with trait anxiety and found that salt taste thresholds increased further (Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013). Variations in salt taste intensity have also been studied in stress, where higher stress levels (Itoh et al. 2022) and a rise in the stress biomarker cortisol (Al’Absi et al. 2012) were associated with a reduction in the perceived intensity of salty solutions. As secondary outcomes in a study exploring biomarkers in depression treatment, lower salt taste intensity and higher salt liking (pleasantness) were associated with a longer duration of depression (Büttner et al. 2015). Despite various interdependent relationships between aspects of salt taste, such as an inverted U-shape relationship between salt liking and intensity (Endrizzi et al. 2021), a negative association between salt taste intensity and salt liking (Spetter et al. 2010; Yeung et al. 2016; Chamoun et al. 2019; Yeung 2019; Veček et al. 2020), and a positive association between salt liking and salt taste thresholds (Hayes et al. 2010; Chamoun et al. 2019; Puputti et al. 2019; Pilic et al. 2020), not all aspects of salt taste have been studied in depression, anxiety, or stress.

Biologically, there are common processes between salt taste and mental health that suggest possible mechanisms for the relationships. Neurotransmitters such as serotonin (5-HT) and norepinephrine (NE) are modifiable factors that can alter salt taste (Heath et al. 2006; Roper and Chaudhari 2017; Smith et al. 2021) and mood (Dean and Keshavan 2017; Kaur and Singh 2017). Therefore, there may be salt taste-associated changes to neurotransmitters, impacting mental health. High salt concentrations may modulate mood through stimulation of the trigeminal nerve (TN) (Simon et al. 2008; Bigiani 2020, 2021) which also has an ameliorating effect on symptoms of depression (Schrader et al. 2011; Cook et al. 2013; Paulino Trevizol and Cordeiro 2016; Generoso et al. 2019) and anxiety (Trevizol et al. 2015; Freire et al. 2020). Other studies have demonstrated increased neuronal activity in the ventral pallidum (VP), the center of hedonic liking in the brain (Smith et al. 2009; Root et al. 2015), when salt liking increases under sodium depletion (Tindell et al. 2006) and in depression (Knowland et al. 2017). These common biological factors between salt taste, depression, and anxiety provide further support for additional research to be undertaken on their relationships.

While there is early evidence of changes to salt taste thresholds in anxiety and stress (Heath et al. 2006; Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013), there has been limited exploration of the relationships between these mood states and salt taste intensity and liking. In addition, changes to salt taste in depression have not been explored as a primary aim of research. The existing studies used sodium chloride solutions to assess aspects of salt taste (Heath et al. 2006; Al’Absi et al. 2012; Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013). However, humans consume salt in or with foods, and the relationships between these mood states and ratings of salt taste intensity and liking of salty foods have not been studied. Therefore, this cross-sectional study surveyed participants to determine their indicative level of depression, anxiety, and stress on the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-21), and their recalled perceived intensity and liking of salt index foods for correlation analysis. The outcomes of this research may be valuable in developing new risk assessment measures, dietary interventions, or therapeutics. For example, screening for mood conditions through measuring alterations to taste, or stimulating specific tastes to affect mood.

2. Materials and methods

2.1 Study design and recruitment

This cross-sectional study utilized survey data collected from Australian residents that were over 18 years of age. Participants were recruited through the University of Newcastle and social media channels (Facebook, LinkedIn, and Twitter) with snowball recruitment employed to engage further respondents. QualtricsXM (SAP, USA) software was used to create and administer the survey. The survey with 3 blocks (taste, mood, and demographics) was designed to be mobile and desktop computer friendly. Responses were collected over ~15 weeks (10 May 2021 to 24 August 2021). Consent was required for participation and ethics approval was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Newcastle (Reference No. H2021-0109).

2.2 Recalled salt taste intensity and liking

The first block of survey questions gathered data on recalled perceptions of salt taste intensity and liking for ten index foods: table salt, potato chips (plain), corn chips, bacon, french fries, processed meats, popcorn, hot dogs, nuts (salted), and crackers. These foods were identified as salt index foods in a taste intensity and liking recall study by Cornelis et al. 2017 that corroborated results across different study samples. Participants rated how intense they recalled the saltiness (salt intensity) of the index foods using a labeled magnitude scale (Green et al. 1996; Bartoshuk et al. 2004) and their liking (salt liking) of each food using a labeled affective magnitude scale (Schutz and Cardello 2001). For intensity, ratings of between 0 (barely detectable) and 100 (strongest imaginable) were possible, with 50 as moderate. For liking, ratings of between 0 (most disliked imaginable) and 100 (most liked imaginable) were possible, with 50 as neutral. Singular arbitrary unit ratings (0, 1, 2, 3 to 100) could be chosen with a left or right sliding bar on both the intensity and liking scales. If participants were unfamiliar with a food or did not eat it, they were able to select “not applicable.” The order of appearance of each index food was randomised between respondents. The ratings of salt intensity and liking for each salt index food were summed and the average was calculated to determine a mean score per participant.

2.3 Depression, anxiety, and stress

Depression, anxiety, and stress levels were estimated through completion of the DASS-21, a self-report screening tool for the severity of negative emotions experienced over the previous week (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995b). Reliability, validity, and internal consistency of the DASS-21 have been demonstrated in clinical (Clara et al. 2001; Gloster et al. 2008) and non-clinical adult samples (Henry and Crawford 2005; Norton 2007; Pezirkianidis et al. 2018). In addition, strong concurrent validity against other previously validated measures of depression, anxiety, and stress has been shown (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995a; Henry and Crawford 2005). The 3-factor structure applied in this study that is DASS-D for depression, DASS-A for anxiety, DASS-S for stress, has been validated in adult populations (Norton 2007; Gloster et al. 2008; Pezirkianidis et al. 2018), and across diverse racial and cultural groups (Norton 2007; Pezirkianidis et al. 2018).

Using 21 items, 7 items per mood state, participants rated the severity and frequency of their experienced depression, anxiety, and stress on a 4-point scale from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much or most of the time) (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995b). Following DASS guidelines, scores for each subscale were summed and multiplied by 2 to determine the final score, with subsequent placement of participants in the categories of normal, mild, moderate, severe, and extremely severe for each mood (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995b).

2.4 Demographics and taste acuity

The third block of questions collected demographic data including age, sex, height, weight, education, income, and smoking status. From height and weight data, body mass index (BMI) measures were calculated (Marfell-Jones 2001). Data on reported taste acuity was collected through participant selection from taste sense categories of normal, distorted, hypersensitive, partial loss, complete loss, or other, which allowed for the participant to specify. This enabled exclusion to be made for altered taste function that may be a result of medications (Doty et al. 2008), comorbidities, or other pathology (Bromley 2000).

2.5 Statistical analysis

The analyses of data were undertaken with JMP (Pro V.14.2.0; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA 27513). Age and BMI were collected as continuous variables and then converted to categories for analysis. Age quartiles were created (18–30 years, 31–41 years, 42–52 years, and > 52 years), and BMI scores were categorized according to the World Health Organisation classifications (World Health Organisation 2021). The cohort characteristics and DASS-21 scores were described with categorical variable distributions (number, percentage). Mean scores for recalled salt intensity and liking across the 11 salt index foods were calculated as continuous data and distributions reported accordingly (mean, standard deviation (SD), min/max). The reliability and internal consistency of the DASS-21 and recalled salt taste intensity and liking scales were assessed with Cronbach’s alpha (Henry and Crawford 2005) using the acceptable threshold score of > 0.7 (Bland and Altman 1997; Tavakol and Dennick 2011).

For continuous outcome variables, least squares means were compared between groups using standard least squares regression analyses with Tukey’s post hoc tests. For categorical outcome variables, nominal logistic regression analyses were used to assess the differences in distributions between categories. P trends were reported to 1 significant figure and when intergroup P-values were statistically significant (< 0.05) they were noted by marking which means were significantly different. Adjustments were made for the potential confounders of age, sex, education, income, smoking status, and BMI, and the analyses were stratified by sex.

3. Results

3.1 Participant characteristics

From 715 participants who gave consent and commenced the survey, data were excluded where responses were incomplete (n = 202), respondents lived outside of Australia (n = 14), and if surveys were completed faster than half the median completion time of 509 s (n = 2). Participants reporting altered taste acuity were also excluded from the analyses (n = 64). To allow for robust statistical analyses, 9 participants who identified as “non-binary” or chose “prefer not to say” were excluded because of low numbers. The total sample size was 424.

The average age for the total sample was 41.3 (±SD 14.3) years, for females 40.7 (±SD 13.3) years, and for males 43.4 (±SD 17.9) years. The sample was predominantly female (80.2%) (Table 1). The mean BMI for the total sample was 27.4 kg/m2, for females 27.5 kg/m2, and for males 27.0 kg/m2 placing all groups in the overweight category. Several categories within each variable were collapsed prior to statistical analyses due to low numbers. For household income, “< $20,000,” “$20,000 to $49,999” and “$50,000 to $79,999” were collapsed to “< $80,000”; and “$80.000 to $109,999” and $110,000 to $149,999’ were collapsed to “$80,000 to $149,999.” The largest proportion of participants had a household income of > $AUD 150,000 per year (29.3%) and this did not differ by sex (Table 1). For education, “< Year 12 or equivalent,” “Year 12 or equivalent” and “Technical and Further Education (TAFE) or technical diploma” (vocational training) were collapsed to “≤ TAFE or diploma.” The largest proportion of participants in the total sample and in females were educated at Bachelor’s degree level (35.6% and 38.2% respectively), and in males at Postgraduate level (31.0%) (Table 1). For smoking, “Currently smoking,” “Quit smoking < 12 months ago” and “Quit smoking > 12 months ago” were collapsed to “History of smoking.” Most participants in the total sample had never smoked (66.5%) and this did not differ by sex (Table 1).

Table 1.

Key demographic variable distributions in the total sample, females and males.

Variable Total Females Males
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Age (years)
 18–30 109 (25.7) 85 (25.0) 24 (28.6)
 31–41 106 (25.0) 92 (27.1) 14 (16.7)
 42–52 115 (27.1) 93 (27.4) 22 (26.2)
 > 52 94 (22.2) 70 (20.6) 24 (28.6)
Sex
 Females 340 (80.2)
 Males 84 (19.4)
BMI (kg/m2)
 < 18.5 Underweight 5 (1.2) 4 (1.1) 1 (1.2)
 18.5–24.9 Normal 173 (40.8) 144 (42.4) 29 (34.5)
 25–29.9 Overweight 132 (31.1) 94 (27.7) 38 (45.2)
 ≥ 30 Obese 114 (26.9) 98 (28.8) 16 (19.1)
Household income (AUD/year)
 < $20,000 16 (3.8) 15 (4.4) 1 (1.2)
 $20,000 to $49,999 42 (9.09) 37 (10.9) 5 (6.0)
 $50,000 to $79,999 43 (10.1) 36 (10.6) 7 (8.3)
 $80,000 to $109,999 71 (16.8) 54 (15.9) 17 (20.2)
 $110,000 to $149,999 69 (16.3) 57 (16.8) 12 (14.3)
 > $150,000 124 (29.3) 97 (28.5) 27 (32.1)
 Prefer not to say 59 (13.9) 44 (12.9) 15 (17.9)
Education
 < Year 12 or equivalent 12 (2.8) 11 (3.2) 1 (1.2)
 Year 12 or equivalent 62 (14.6) 42 (12.4) 20 (23.8)
 TAFE or technical diploma 63 (14.9) 47 (13.8) 16 (19.1)
 Bachelor’s degree 151 (35.6) 130 (38.2) 21 (25.0)
 Postgraduate degree 134 (31.6) 108 (31.8) 26 (31.0)
 Prefer not to say 2 (0.1) 2 (5.9) 0 (0)
Smoking
 Currently smoke 23 (5.4) 15 (4.4) 8 (9.5)
 Quit smoking < 12 months ago 9 (2.1) 8 (2.4) 1 (1.2)
 Quit smoking > 12 months ago 105 (24.8) 86 (25.3) 19 (22.6)
 Never smoked 282 (66.5) 227 (66.8) 55 (65.5)
 Prefer not to say 5 (1.2) 4 (1.2) 1 (1.2)

AUD, Australian dollars; TAFE, Technical and Further Education (vocational training). Percentages may not sum to 100% due to rounding.

3.2 Recalled salt intensity and liking distributions

The mean recalled salt intensity index score in the total sample was 54.3 (±SD 17.3), in females was 54.7 (±SD17.6), and in males was 52.6 (±SD 16.2) (Table 2). The mean recalled salt liking index score in the total sample was 61.5 (±SD 16.6), in females 61.1 (±SD 16.8), and in males 63.2 (±SD 15.9) (Table 2). There were no statistically significant differences between females and males in mean salt intensity (P = 0.3) or salt liking scores (P = 0.05). Both measures were normally distributed. Cronbach’s alpha values for the taste scales were 0.90 and 0.82, respectively, demonstrating high internal consistency.

Table 2.

Mean recalled salt intensity and liking index scores in the total sample, females and males.

Total Females Males
Mean ±SD (min–max) Mean ±SD (min–max) Mean ±SD (min–max)
Salt intensity 54.3 ±17.3 (5.2–95.5) 54.7 ±17.6 (5.2–96.5) 52.6 ±16.2 (13.4–85.3)
Salt liking 61.5 ±16.6 (6.3–100.0) 61.1 ±16.8 (13.4–100.0) 63.2 ±15.9 (6.3–98.0)

SD, standard deviation.

3.3 DASS-21 distributions

Reliability and internal consistency values were above the acceptable Cronbach’s alpha threshold with DASS-D α at 0.93, DASS-A α at 0.82, and DASS-S α at 0.93. DASS-21 scores indicative of depression, anxiety, and stress were found in 37.3%, 34.6%, and 35.5% of the cohort respectively (Table 3). There was a statistically significant difference between females and males for DASS-S scores (χ2 = 11.9, P = 0.02) but not for DASS-D (χ2 = 9.4, P = 0.05) or DASS-A (χ2 = 4.3, P = 0.07) scores.

Table 3.

DASS-21 distributions in the total sample, females and males.

DASS-21 category Total Females Males
n (%) n (%) n (%)
DASS-D
 Normal 266 (62.7) 217 (63.8) 49 (58.3)
 Mild 58 (13.7) 49 (14.4) 9 (10.7)
 Moderate 48 (11.3) 39 (11.5) 9 (10.7)
 Severe 24 (5.7) 13 (3.8) 11 (13.1)
 Extremely severe 28 (6.6) 22 (6.5) 6 (7.1)
DASS-A
 Normal 277 (65.3) 215 (63.2) 62 (73.8)
 Mild 28 (6.6) 27 (7.9) 1 (1.2)
 Moderate 62 (14.6) 50 (14.7) 12 (14.3)
 Severe 26 (6.1) 21 (6.2) 5 (6.0)
 Extremely severe 31 (7.3) 27 (7.9) 4 (4.8)
DASS-S
 Normal 273 (64.4) 213 (62.7) 60 (71.4)
 Mild 54 (12.7) 44 (12.9) 10 (11.9)
 Moderate 59 (13.9) 46 (13.5) 13 (15.5)
 Severe 29 (6.8) 28 (8.2) 1 (1.2)
 Extremely severe 9 (2.1) 9 (2.7) 0 (0)

DASS-D, depression; DASS-A, anxiety; DASS-S, stress. Percentages may not sum to 100% due to rounding.

Prior to further statistical analysis, several classifications within each DASS-21 mood grouping were collapsed due to low numbers. For DASS-D, DASS-A, and DASS-S “severe” and “extremely severe” were collapsed into “severe.” For DASS-A, “mild” and “moderate” were collapsed into “mild/moderate.”

3.4 Relationships between recalled salt taste liking, intensity, and potential confounding variables

When age was treated as a continuous variable, recalled salt liking decreased as age increased (P trend = 0.01; Tables 4 and 5). However, there were no statistically significant differences between the age groups (Tables 4 and 5). Salt liking increased as BMI increased (P trend = 0.02) but the intergroup means did not differ significantly (Tables 4 and 5). Salt liking was not associated with sex, income, education, or history of smoking (Tables 4 and 5).

Table 4.

Relationships between recalled salt liking, salt intensity and confounding variables in the total sample.

Variable Salt liking Salt intensity
LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend
Age (years)
 18-30 64.4 a ±1.6 0.01 49.5 b ±1.6 0.008
 31-41 63.9 a ±1.6 56.5 a ±1.7
 42-52 59.1 a ±1.5 55.3 ab ±1.6
 >52 58.6 a ±1.7 56.3 a ±1.8
Sex
 Females 61.1 a ±0.9 0.6 54.7 a ±0.9 0.6
 Males 63.2 a ±1.8 52.6 a ±1.9
BMI (kg/m2)
 < 18.5 49.3 a ±7.4 0.02 41.0 a ±7.7 0.2
 18.5-24.9 59.1 a ±1.3 54.5 a ±1.3
 25-29.9 63.7 a ±1.4 55.9 a ±1.5
 ≥ 30 63.3 a ±1.5 52.8 a ± 1.6
Income (AUD/year)
 < $80,000 61.8 a ±1.4 0.9 54.6 a ±1.4 0.03
 $80,000–149,999 60.8 a ±1.6 57.1 ab ±1.7
 > $150,000 61.4 a ±1.5 54.5 ab ±1.6
 PNS 62.5 a ±2.1 48.8 b ±2.2
Education
 ≤ TAFE/Dip. 62.4 a ±1.4 0.3 51.9 b ±1.5 0.03
 Bach.D 60.7 a ±1.3 53.6 ab ±1.4
 Postgrad.D 61.2 a ±1.4 58.0 a ±1.5
 PNS 77.9 a ±9.6 41.9 ab ±9.9
Smoking
 History 62.4 a ±1.4 0.5 54.6 a ±1.5 0.8
 Never 61.0 a ±1.0 54.3 a ±1.0
 PNS 67.0 a ±6.8 49.3 a ±7.1

LSM, least squares mean; SEM, standard error of the mean; BMI, Body Mass Index; PNS, prefer not to say; TAFE, Technical and Further Education (vocational training); Dip., diploma; Bach.D., Bachelor’s degree; Postgrad.D., Postgraduate degree.

Values in the same row (within an analysis) denoted with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s HSD p < 0.05)

Table 5.

Relationships between recalled salt liking, salt intensity, DASS-21 scores, and confounding variables in the total sample (normal taste acuity)

Variable DASS-D (depression) DASS-A (anxiety) DASS-S (stress)
Normal Mild Mod Severe χ2 (P) Normal Mild/Mod Severe χ2 (P) Normal Mild Mod Severe χ2 (P)
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Age (years)
 18-30 55 (13.0) 19 (4.5) 18 (4.3) 17 (4.0) 15.1
(0.09)
45 (10.6) 35 (8.3) 29 (6.8) 52.1 (<0.0001) 49 (11.6) 20 (4.7) 27 (6.4) 13 (3.1) 45.8 (<0.0001)
 31-41 65 (15.3) 16 (3.8) 13 (3.1) 12 (2.8) 66 (15.6) 27 (6.4) 13 (3.1) 60 (14.1) 16 (3.8) 15 (3.5) 15 (3.5)
 42-52 76 (17.9) 14 (3.3) 12 (2.8) 13 (3.1) 86 (20.3) 19 (4.5) 10 (2.4) 85 (20.1) 13 (3.1) 11 (2.6) 6 (1.4)
 >52 70 (16.5) 9 (2.1) 5 (1.2) 10 (2.4) 80 (18.9) 9 (2.1) 5 (1.2) 79 (18.6) 5 (1.2) 6 (1.4) 4 (0.9)
Sex
 Females 217 (51.2) 49 (11.6) 39 (9.2) 35 (8.3) 5.9
(0.1)
215 (50.7) 77 (18.2) 48 (11.3) 3.5
(0.2)
213 (50.2) 44 (10.4) 46 (10.9) 37 (8.7) 11.4
(0.01)
 Males 49 (11.6) 9 (2.1) 9 (2.1) 17 (4.0) 62 (14.6) 13 (3.1) 9 (2.1) 60 (14.2) 10 (2.4) 13 (3.1) 1 (0.2)
BMI (kg/m2)
 < 18.5 3 (0.7) 1 (0.2) 1 (0.2) 0 (0.0) 15.6
(0.08)
3 (0.7) 1 (0.2) 1 (0.2) 12.7
(0.05)
3 (0.7) 2 (0.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 11.5
(0.2)
 18.5-24.9 109 (25.7) 25 (5.9) 18 (4.3) 21 (5.0) 111 (26.2) 39 (9.2) 23 (5.4) 116 (27.4) 18 (4.3) 21 (5.0) 18 (4.3)
 25-29.9 95 (22.4) 15 (3.5) 8 (1.9) 14 (3.3) 100 (23.6) 21 (5.0) 11 (2.6) 89 (21.0) 19 (4.5) 16 (3.8) 8 (1.9)
 ≥ 30 59 (13.9) 17 (4.0) 21 (5.0) 17 (4.0) 63 (14.9) 29 (6.8) 22 (5.2) 65 (15.3) 15 (3.5) 22 (5.2) 12 (2.8)
Income (AUD/year)
 < $80,000 54 (12.7) 13 (3.1) 13 (3.1) 21 (5.0) 14.8
(0.1)
51 (12.0) 24 (5.7) 26 (6.1) 20.6
(0.002)
54 (12.7) 14 (3.3) 18 (4.3) 15 (3.5) 15.1
(0.09)
 $80,000 to $149,999 94 (22.2) 16 (3.8) 16 (3.8) 14 (3.3) 95 (22.4) 32 (7.6) 13 (3.1) 95 (22.4) 20 (4.7) 12 (2.8) 13 (3.1)
 > $150,000 86 (20.3) 18 (4.3) 11 (2.6) 9 (2.1) 92 (21.7) 22 (5.2) 10 (2.4) 83 (19.6) 14 (3.3) 21 (5.0) 6 (1.4)
 PNS 32 (7.6) 11 (2.6) 8 (1.9) 8 (1.9) 39 (9.2) 12 (2.8) 8 (1.9) 41 (9.7) 6 (1.4) 8 (1.9) 4 (0.9)
Education
 ≤ TAFE/Dip. 70 (16.5) 20 (4.7) 17 (4.0) 30 (7.1) 23.8
(0.005)
75 (17.8) 33 (7.8) 29 (6.8) 20.3
(0.002)
78 (18.4) 19 (4.5) 27 (6.4) 13 (3.1) 11.7
(0.2)
 Bach.D 96 (22.7) 22 (5.2) 20 (4.7) 13 (3.1) 97 (22.9) 38 (9.0) 16 (3.8) 96 (22.9) 19 (4.5) 21 (5.0) 15 (3.5)
 Postgrad.D 98 (23.1) 16 (3.8) 11 (2.6) 9 (2.1) 103 (24.3) 19 (4.5) 12 (2.8) 97 (22.9) 16 (3.8) 11 (2.6) 10 (2.4)
 PNS 2 (0.5) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 2 (0.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (0.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
 Smoking
 History 91 (21.5) 19 (4.5) 8 (1.9) 19 (4.5) 10.2
(0.1)
91 (21.5) 26 (6.1) 20 (4.7) 0.9
(0.9)
88 (20.8) 16 (3.8) 20 (4.7) 13 (3.1) 2.4
(0.9)
 Never 171 (40.3) 39 (9.2) 40 (9.4) 32 (7.6) 183 (43.2) 63 (14.9) 36 (8.5) 182 (42.9) 37 (8.7) 39 (9.2) 24 (5.7)
 PNS 4 (0.9) 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (0.2) 3 (0.7) 1 (0.2) 1 (0.2) 3 (0.7) 1 (0.2) 0 (0.0) 1 (0.2)

Mod, moderate; BMI, Body Mass Index; AUD, Australian dollars; PNS, prefer not to say; TAFE, Technical and Further Education (vocational training); Dip., diploma; Bach.D, Bachelor degree; Postgrad.D, Postgraduate degree.

Values in the same row (within an analysis) denoted with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s HSD p < 0.05).

Recalled salt intensity varied with age (P trend = 0.008; (Tables 4 and 5). Salt intensity increased between the youngest age group (18–30 years) and those aged 31–41 years (14.1%, P = 0.02), and > 52 years (13.7%, P = 0.03) (Tables 4 and 5). Salt intensity was associated with income (P trend = 0.03) however the intergroup means did not differ significantly between those that reported income levels (Tables 4 and 5). Salt intensity was associated with education (P trend = 0.03; Tables 4 and 5). Those educated at postgraduate level recalled the saltiness of the index foods to be 11.8% more intense than those educated at TAFE level or below (P = 0.02; Tables 4 and 5). Salt intensity did not vary with sex, BMI, or history of smoking (Tables 4 and 5).

DASS-D scores indicative of depression were more likely in participants with lower education levels (≤ TAFE or technical diploma, P = 0.005; Tables 4 and 5). DASS-A scores indicative of anxiety were more likely in younger participants (18–30 years; P ≤ 0.0001), those with lower incomes (< $AUD 80,000/year, P = 0.002), and in those with lower education levels (≤ TAFE or technical diploma, P = 0.002) (Tables 4 and 5). DASS-S scores indicative of stress were more likely in younger participants (18–30 years, P ≤ 0.0001) and in females (P = 0.01) (Tables 4 and 5).

3.5 Relationships between DASS-21 and recalled salt taste liking and intensity in the total sample

Those with DASS-21 scores indicative of depression, anxiety, and stress had higher mean salt liking scores (Table 6), however, there were no associations between salt intensity and any of the mood states (Table 7).

Table 6.

Recalled salt liking varies by DASS-21 scores indicative of depression, anxiety, and stress in the total sample (unadjusted and adjusted models).

DASS-21 Recalled salt liking
Unadjusted Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend
DASS-D
Normal 60.0b ±1.0 0.01 60.8b ±1.2 0.03 66.0b ±3.9 0.02 63.2b ±4.3 0.04
Mild 61.5ab ±2.2 61.6ab ±2.3 67.1ab ±4.5 64.3ab ±4.8
Moderate 62.7ab ±2.4 62.5ab ±2.5 68.6ab ±4.6 65.4ab ±4.9
Severe 68.3a ±2.3 68.3a ±2.3 74.0a ±4.5 70.7a ±4.8
DASS-A
Normal 60.0b ±1.0 0.002 61.1b ±1.1 0.01 66.4b ±3.9 0.01 63.4b ±4.3 0.03
Mild/Moderate 62.1ab ±1.8 62.3ab ±1.9 67.8ab ±4.3 64.5ab ±4.7
Severe 68.4a ±2.2 68.4a ±2.3 73.9a ±4.5 70.3a ±4.9
DASS-S
Normal 60.2b ± 1.0 0.02 61.3a ± 1.2 0.08 67.0a ± 3.9 0.07 64.4a ± 4.3 0.2
Mild 60.7ab ± 2.2 60.7a ± 2.3 66.3a ± 4.5 63.2a ± 4.8
Moderate 67.7a ± 2.1 67.4a ± 2.3 73.2a ± 4.5 69.4a ± 4.9
Severe 62.8ab ± 2.7 62.8a ± 2.8 68.4a ± 4.8 65.4a ± 5.1

Model 1: age and sex; Model 2: age, sex, education, income, and smoking; Model 3: age, sex, education, income, smoking, and BMI

LSM, least squares mean; SEM, standard error of the mean; CI, confidence interval.

Values in the same row (within an analysis) denoted with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s HSD P < 0.05).

Table 7.

Recalled salt intensity does not vary by DASS-21 scores indicative of depression, anxiety, and stress in the total sample (unadjusted and adjusted models).

DASS-21 Recalled salt intensity
Unadjusted Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend LSM ±SEM P trend
DASS-D
Normal 53.9a ±1.1 0.6 53.0a ±1.3 0.5 45.6a ±4.1 0.5 43.1a ±4.5 0.4
Mild 56.6a ±2.3 56.5a ±2.4 49.2a ±4.7 47.2a ±5.1
Moderate 52.8a ±2.5 53.1a ±2.6 45.4a ±4.8 44.1a ±5.2
Severe 55.1a ±2.4 55.3a ±2.4 47.5a ±4.6 45.5a ±5.1
DASS-A
Normal 54.5a ±1.0 0.2 53.5a ±1.2 0.3 46.0a ±4.1 0.1 43.5a ±4.5 0.2
Mild/Moderate 56.0a ±1.8 56.3a ±2.0 48.6a ±4.4 46.6a ±4.9
Severe 50.6a ±2.3 51.8a ±2.5 42.9a ±4.6 41.3a ±5.1
DASS-S
Normal 54.9a ±1.1 0.8 53.9a ±1.2 1.0 46.2a ±4.1 1.0 43.7a ±4.5 1.0
Mild 52.8a ±2.4 53.0a ±2.5 44.7a ±4.7 42.6a ±5.0
Moderate 52.9a ±2.3 53.7a ±2.4 45.6a ±4.7 43.7a ±5.2
Severe 54.7a ±2.8 54.3a ±3.0 45.6a ±4.9 43.4a ±5.4

Model 1: age and sex; Model 2: age, sex, education, income, and smoking; Model 3: age, sex, education, income, smoking, and BMI.

LSM, least squares mean; SEM, standard error of the mean; CI, confidence interval.

Values in the same row (within an analysis) denoted with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s HSD P < 0.05).

3.5.1 DASS-D and recalled salt liking.

Higher salt liking was associated with DASS-21 scores indicative of depression in the unadjusted and adjusted models (P trend range = 0.01–0.04; Table 6). Salt liking was 13.8% higher in those with scores indicative of severe depression than in those with normal scores in the unadjusted model (60.0 vs. 68.3, P = 0.005, 95% CI 1.8–14.7; Table 6). Statistical significance was maintained for this relationship after adjusting for age and sex (60.8 vs. 68.3, P = 0.02, 95% CI 1.0–13.9), age, sex, income, education, and smoking (66.0 vs. 74.0, P = 0.01, 95% CI 1.3–14.7), and age, sex, income, education, smoking, and BMI (63.2 vs. 70.7, P = 0.02, 95% CI 0.8–14.1) Table 6).

3.5.2 DASS-A and recalled salt liking.

Higher salt liking was associated with DASS-21 scores indicative of anxiety in the unadjusted and adjusted models (P trend range = 0.002–0.03; Table 6). Salt liking was 14% higher in those with scores indicative of severe anxiety than in those with normal scores (60.0 vs. 68.4, P = 0.001, 95% CI 2.8–14.0; Table 6). Statistical significance was maintained for this relationship after adjusting for age and sex (61.1 vs. 68.4; P = 0.01, 95% CI 1.4–13.1), age, sex, income, education, and smoking (66.4 vs. 73.9; P = 0.01, 95% CI 1.5–13.6), and age, sex, income, education, smoking, and BMI (63.4 vs. 70.3; P = 0.02, 95% CI 0.8–13.0) Table 6).

3.5.3 DASS-S and recalled salt liking.

Higher salt liking was associated with DASS-21 scores indicative of stress in the unadjusted model (P trend = 0.02; Table 6). Salt liking was 12.5% higher in those with scores indicative of moderate stress than in those with normal scores (60.2 vs. 67.7, P = 0.009, 95% CI 1.4–13.5; Table 6). Statistical significance was not maintained in the adjusted models.

3.6 Relationships between DASS-21 and recalled salt taste liking and intensity, by sex

Females with DASS-21 scores indicative of anxiety and stress had higher mean salt liking scores (Supplementary Table S1). In the crude analyses only, males with indicative depression and anxiety had higher mean salt liking scores (Supplementary Table S1). In females and males, there were no associations between salt intensity and any of the mood states (Supplementary Table S2).

3.6.1 DASS-D and recalled salt liking.

The model P trends (unadjusted and adjusted) for the relationships between DASS-21 scores indicative of depression and salt liking were not statistically significant for either females or males (Supplementary Table S1). However, in males salt liking was 19.4% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicative of severe depression than in those with normal scores in the unadjusted model only (59.7 vs. 71.3, P = 0.04, 95% CI 0.2–23.0; Supplementary Table S1).

3.6.2 DASS-A and recalled salt liking.

In females, higher salt liking was associated with DASS-21 scores indicative of anxiety in the unadjusted and adjusted models (P trend range = 0.002–0.0005; Supplementary Table S1) and in males, the association was significant in the unadjusted model only (P trend = 0.04; Supplementary Table S1).

In females, salt liking was 15.9% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicative of severe anxiety than in those with mild/moderate scores in the unadjusted model (60.2 vs. 69.8, P = 0.004, 95% CI 2.5–16.8; Supplementary Table S1). This association was statistically significant in the models adjusting for age (59.4 vs. 68.9, P = 0.005, 95% CI 2.4–16.7; Supplementary Table S1), age, income, education, and smoking (65.4 vs. 74.8, P = 0.007, 95% CI 2.2–16.8; Supplementary Table S1), and age, income, education, smoking, and BMI (60.8 vs. 70.0, P = 0.003, 95% CI 2.6–15.8; Supplementary Table S1). In females, salt liking was 17.1% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicative of severe anxiety than in those with normal scores in the unadjusted model (59.6 vs. 69.8, P = 0.003, 95% CI 4.1–16.4; Supplementary Table S1). This association was statistically significant in the models adjusting for age (59.8 vs. 68.9, P = 0.003, 95% CI 2.6–15.6; Supplementary Table S1), age, income, education, and smoking (65.1 vs. 74.8, P = 0.002, 95% CI 3.0–16.5; Supplementary Table S1), and age, education, income, smoking, and BMI (60.8 vs. 70.0, P = 0.003, 95% CI 2.6–15.8; Supplementary Table S1).

In males, salt liking was 19.9% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicative of mild/moderate anxiety than in those with normal scores in the unadjusted model only (61.3 vs. 73.5, P = 0.03, 95% CI 0.9–23.4; Supplementary Table S1).

3.6.3 DASS-S and recalled salt liking.

In females, higher salt liking was associated with DASS-21 scores indicative of stress in the unadjusted, age adjusted, and age, income, education, and smoking-adjusted models (P trend range = 0.01–0.04; Supplementary Table S1) but not in the fully adjusted model inclusive of BMI. In males, there were no statistically significant relationships.

In females, salt liking was 14.6% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicative of moderate stress than in those with normal scores in the unadjusted model (59.5 vs. 68.2, P = 0.007, 95% CI 1.8–15.7; Supplementary Table S1). This association was statistically significant in the models adjusting for age (59.8 vs. 67.3, P = 0.04, 95% CI 0.2–14.6; Supplementary Table S1), and age, income, education, and smoking (65.9 vs. 74.0, P = 0.03, 95% CI 0.7–15.4; Supplementary Table S1), but not in the fully adjusted model inclusive of BMI.

4. Discussion

This exploratory study aimed to assess relationships between recalled salt taste liking and intensity, and indices of depression, anxiety, and stress. The survey findings demonstrate higher recalled liking of salt index foods when DASS-21 scores indicate severe levels of depression and anxiety, and moderate levels of stress. Females had higher recalled salt liking when DASS-21 scores indicated severe and mild/moderate anxiety, and moderate stress. Males had higher recalled salt liking when DASS-21 scores indicated severe depression and mild/moderate anxiety in the crude analysis only. There were no relationships between recalled intensity ratings of salt index foods and DASS-21 scores for the total sample or by sex. While these novel findings do not demonstrate causation, they indicate that further research is important to determine if salt taste may be related via signaling, genetic or other biological mechanisms, with potential future roles in diagnosis, prevention, or treatment.

Recalled salt liking was 11.8–13.8% higher in participants with DASS-21 scores indicative of severe levels of depression in comparison with those recording normal scores in the crude analysis and adjusted models. A prior study in people with diagnosed major depressive disorder (n = 30) similarly found higher salt liking (pleasantness) correlated with longer periods of depression (Büttner et al. 2015). Conversely, no correlation was found between salt liking (pleasantness) and symptoms of depression in a healthy sample (n = 33) (Scinska et al. 2004). Factors likely contributing to the different outcomes between the current study and Scinska et al. (2004) include the use of a recall method for taste measures versus a taste test of salty solutions, the use of different depression assessment tools (DASS-21 vs. Beck Depression Inventory [BDI]), and the substantially larger sample size (n = 424) of the current study. The DASS-21 is reported to better discriminate between depression and other affective states than the BDI (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995a), and to have predictive power in the diagnosis of depression (Gloster et al. 2008; Bener et al. 2016). The association of recalled salt liking to severe levels of depression (DASS-21) and not moderate or mild levels in this study, suggests those with scores indicative of depression may be clinically depressed. Therefore, the predictive power of the DASS-21 for depression further aligns the results of this study with the positive association between salt liking and depression found in the study executed in a clinical setting using salt solutions (Büttner et al. 2015). Further studies in both clinical and non-clinical populations would be beneficial in elucidating findings and providing direction for settings most suited to their application.

Recalled salt liking was 10.9–14% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicating severe levels of anxiety in comparison with those recording normal scores in the crude analysis and adjusted models. Previously, a negative correlation has been found between salt liking (pleasantness) and anxiety in people with major depressive disorder (Büttner et al. 2015). In the study by Büttner et al. (2015), anxiety was measured with 1 question (Item 10) from the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, an instrument primarily designed to assess symptoms of depression, not anxiety (Carmody et al. 2006). Therefore, the use of DASS-21 in the present study has likely measured scores of anxiety more accurately and reliably. Other research has shown that salt taste thresholds (the concentrations at which saltiness is detected) are significantly higher in anxiety than in healthy samples (Heath et al. 2006; Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013) and that salt liking is positively correlated to salt taste thresholds (Hayes et al. 2010; Chamoun et al. 2019; Puputti et al. 2019; Pilic et al. 2020). These previous studies indicate salt liking may increase in anxiety and the present study supports this theory. However, the prior studies assessed salt taste using salt solutions at varying threshold concentrations including suprathreshold levels therefore the comparison to the outcomes of the current study using recall and salt index foods is hypothetical and requires further exploration.

Recalled salt liking was 12.5% higher in those with DASS-21 scores indicating moderate stress levels however, when adjusting for the confounding variables there was no statistical significance. Complexity in the relationship between salt liking and stress has been demonstrated in a small (n = 38) sample of younger (20.3 years) people with no history of mental health disorders, where the physiological measure of stress, cortisol, was not associated to salt liking but there was a negative correlation between self-reported distress after induced stress (Al’Absi et al. 2012). The reasons for the varied outcomes are unclear. However, as salt thresholds and liking are negatively correlated (Hayes et al. 2010; Pilic et al. 2020), the higher salt liking may be a result of a lowering of salt taste thresholds previously demonstrated in stress (Ileri-Gurel et al. 2013). As the most under-researched relationship, salt liking and stress require more robust exploratory and mechanistic investigations using both salty solutions and tasting of salty foods.

The results of the analyses for recalled salt liking by sex has highlighted some differences to those in the total sample. While an association was found between higher recalled salt liking and depression in the total sample the relationship was only found in males (crude analysis), and not in females. Salt liking was higher in those with severe versus normal anxiety in the total sample, in those with severe versus mild/moderate and normal anxiety in females, and in those with mild/moderate anxiety versus normal in males (crude analysis). The finding of higher salt liking in moderate stress was found in the crude analysis in the total sample and in females in all models with the inclusion of BMI as the exception. These varying outcomes may be due to the predominantly female composition of the study sample however this does not explain the differences completely. Previous studies on salt taste qualities and mood (Scinska et al. 2004; Al’Absi et al. 2012; Büttner et al. 2015) did not analyze data by sex so comparative discussion is not possible. However, both salt taste qualities (Hayes et al. 2010; Calvo and Egan 2015; Martin and Sollars 2017 ) and mood (Eid et al. 2019; Rubinow and Schmidt 2019; Seney and Sebille 2014) are known to vary between the sexes therefore there may be sex-hormone mediated roles influencing the relationships that need to be explored.

Recalled salt taste intensity was not associated with depression, anxiety, or stress in the total sample or by sex. Similar to liking, prior research on these relationships has measured taste intensity through taste tests using salty solutions often at supraphysiological concentrations (Scinska et al. 2004; Al’Absi et al. 2012; Itoh et al. 2022). This may account for the varying outcomes in relation to the present study that used recall taste measures. For example, reduced salt intensity (solutions) has been correlated with the rise in cortisol occurring during acute stress (Al’Absi et al. 2012) and to increasing levels of self-reported stress (Itoh et al. 2022). In contrast, the lack of association between recalled salt taste intensity and depression in the present study was also found in a healthy sample of older adults (63 years) that rated the intensity of salty solutions (Scinska et al. 2004). The robustness of the present result is increased by the better discriminatory and diagnostic power for depression of the DASS-21 versus the BDI (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995a; Gloster et al. 2008; Bener et al. 2016) used by Scinska et al. (2004). Additional reasons for these varied outcomes may be differences in the physiology of acute and chronic stress, and use of different mood assessment measures. Finally, differentiating between intensities of varying salt solutions is more difficult than other tastes (Puputti et al. 2018) and recall of taste intensity is likely more challenging than recalling the hedonic experience of taste liking.

Several physiological factors may explain the findings from this study and highlight possible avenues for application and future research. High salt concentrations modulate salt taste receptor release of 5-HT and NE (Roper and Chaudhari 2017), neurotransmitters altered in depression (Dean and Keshavan 2017), and anxiety (Kaur and Singh 2017). If salt taste alterations are indicative of neurotransmitter imbalance, taste tests may be utilized in progress monitoring or diagnosis. Genetic polymorphisms found in the salt taste receptor TRPV1 are associated with altered salt taste sensitivity and preference (Dias et al. 2013; Chamoun et al. 2018; Pilic and Mavrommatis 2018) and to reduced anxiolytic behaviors and antidepressant effects in mice (Marsch et al. 2007). Whether variations in genes coding for salt taste receptors are associated with depression, anxiety, and stress is yet to be elucidated but may be useful in improving their detection, prevention, or treatment. Low serum sodium levels have been linked to depression (Grippo et al. 2006; Morris et al. 2006, 2008, 2010; Özdemir et al. 2014; Sawant et al. 2019), and correction of hyponatraemia can improve symptoms (Sawant et al. 2019; Bojdani et al. 2020). Therefore, salt liking potentially increases in depression in response to low serum sodium or the reduced physiological impacts of serum sodium. For example, increased salt liking under sodium depletion (Tindell et al. 2006) is reflected by increased neuronal activity in the VP the center of hedonic liking in the brain (Smith et al. 2009; Root et al. 2015). Increased firing of VP neurons is also found in depression and silencing of VP neurons can ameliorate depressive-like behaviors (Knowland et al. 2017). Increased salt intake driven by liking of salt (Hayes et al. 2010; Pilic et al. 2020) may lead to TN nerve stimulation (Simon et al. 2008; Bigiani, 2020, 2021) which ameliorates depression (Schrader et al. 2011; Cook et al. 2013; Paulino Trevizol and Cordeiro 2016; Generoso et al. 2019) and anxiety (Trevizol et al. 2015; Freire et al. 2020). Liking of salty foods can be reduced through exposure to lower sodium diets (Riis et al. 2021). Therefore, it is possible that a dietary intervention reducing liking of salty foods may increase TN nerve stimulation improving anxiety and depression or may reduce VP neuronal firing improving depression.

This study is the first to find associations between recalled liking of salt-index foods and DASS-21. A conservative approach to generalizing these findings is recommended as the sample was predominantly female and highly educated with incomes above average. However, none of the demographic variables influenced the statistically significant relationships in depression and anxiety. The larger numbers of participants experiencing some level of depression, anxiety, and stress (~36%) added power to the data and is somewhat reflective of early evidence of the increased prevalence of mental health issues reported during the COVID-19 pandemic when this study was undertaken (Morgan 2020; Aknin et al. 2022). Surveys are susceptible to sampling, response bias, and non-response errors but also have advantages such as removing interviewer bias and providing anonymity to participants (Fricker and Schonlau 2002; Van Selm and Jankowski 2006). In this case, the survey format allowed for the capture of a large sample size and broader experiences of recalled salt taste measures and mood not previously recorded. Questionnaires are an appropriate method for population studies such as this (Leshem 1998; Keskitalo et al. 2007; Cornelis et al. 2017; Wanich et al. 2018), and have been moderately correlated with outcomes from laboratory studies that measure the more sensory experience of food (Deglaire et al. 2012). The use of salt-index foods rather than supraphysiological salt solutions to obtain recall measures of salt liking and intensity allows for the results to be considered in real-world settings. The use of multiple salty foods to arrive at means for recalled salt liking and intensity reduced the influence that interindividual differences between participants’ taste experiences may exert on outcomes (Cornelis et al. 2017). However, the recall method used in this study to assess the salt taste qualities is highly subjective and the next steps in research should include the tasting of salt index foods in a clinical setting.

The aim of this cross-sectional study was to explore the relationships between aspects of salt taste and mood. The novel findings demonstrate that recalled liking of salt index foods increases when DASS-21 scores indicate severe depression and anxiety, and moderate stress. Further examination of the relationships between salt liking and mental health and the mechanisms that may be involved in the associations would be beneficial to proving/disproving these findings and moving toward their appropriate application in clinical and non-clinical settings. Future research should include tasting of salty foods in clinical settings, data on salt intake, serum sodium levels, genetics, and ethnicity, and have a more even sex distribution. Interventions that measure neurotransmitter levels and nervous system responses common to both salt taste and depression, anxiety, and stress would be valuable to understand the mechanisms involved in the correlations found and better direct any potential new assessment methods or treatments.

Supplementary Material

bjad038_suppl_Supplementary_Tables_S1-S2

Contributor Information

Celeste Ferraris, School of Environmental and Life Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW 2258, Australia.

Christopher J Scarlett, School of Environmental and Life Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW 2258, Australia; Hunter Medical Research Institute, Newcastle, NSW 2305, Australia.

Tamara Bucher, School of Environmental and Life Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW 2258, Australia; Priority Research Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition, The University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW 2308, Australia; Hunter Medical Research Institute, Newcastle, NSW 2305, Australia.

Emma L Beckett, School of Environmental and Life Sciences, The University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW 2258, Australia; Priority Research Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition, The University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW 2308, Australia; Hunter Medical Research Institute, Newcastle, NSW 2305, Australia.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, C.F. and E.L.B.; Data curation, C.F. and E.L.B.; Formal analysis, C.F. and E.L.B.; Funding acquisition, E.L.B.; Investigation, C.F. and E.L.B.; Methodology, C.F. and E.L.B.; Project administration, C.F and E.L.B.; Resources, E.L.B.; Writing—original draft, C.F. and E.L.B.; Writing—review and editing, C.F., C.J.S., T.B., and E.L.B.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Data availability

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

References

  1. Aknin LB, De Neve J-E, Dunn EW, Fancourt DE, Goldberg E, Helliwell JF, Jones SP, Karam E, Layard R, Lyubomirsky S, et al. Mental health during the first year of the Covid-19 pandemic: a review and recommendations for moving forward. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2022:17(4):915–936. 10.1177/17456916211029964 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Al’Absi M, Nakajima M, Hooker S, Wittmers L, Cragin T.. Exposure to acute stress is associated with attenuated sweet taste. Psychophysiology. 2012:49(1):96–103. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01289.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amick HR, Gartlehner G, Gaynes BN, Forneris C, Asher GN, Morgan LC, Coker-Schwimmer E, Boland E, Lux LJ, Gaylord S, et al. Comparative benefits and harms of second generation antidepressants and cognitive behavioral therapies in initial treatment of major depressive disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2015:351:h6019. 10.1136/bmj.h6019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Australian Bureau of Statistics. National Health Survey: First results 2017-18 ABS; 2018. [accessed 2022 August 3]. https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/health/health-conditions-and-risks/national-health-survey-first-results/latest-release.
  5. Baldwin D, Woods R, Lawson R, Taylor D.. Efficacy of drug treatments for generalised anxiety disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2011:342:d1199. 10.1136/bmj.d1199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB, Green BG, Hoffman HJ, Ko CW, Lucchina LA, Marks LE, Snyder DJ, Weiffenbach JM.. Valid across-group comparisons with labeled scales: the gLMS versus magnitude matching. Physiol Behav. 2004:82(1):109–114. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.02.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beckett EL, Martin C, Yates Z, Veysey M, Duesing K, Lucock M.. Bitter taste genetics—the relationship to tasting, liking, consumption and health. Food Funct. 2014:5(12):3040–3054. 10.1039/C4FO00539B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bener A, Alsulaiman R, Lg D, Hr E.. Comparison of reliability and validity of the Breast Cancer Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS- 21) with the Beck Depression Inventory-(BDI-II) and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2016:4(4):196–202. 10.19070/2332-3000-1600035 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bigiani A. Does ENaC work as sodium taste receptor in humans? Nutrients. 2020:12(4):1195. 10.3390/nu12041195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bigiani A. The origin of saltiness: oral detection of NaCl. Curr Opin Physiol. 2021:19:156–161. 10.1016/j.cophys.2020.11.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bland JM, Altman DG.. Statistics notes: Cronbach’s alpha. BMJ. 1997:314(7080):572. 10.1136/bmj.314.7080.572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bojdani E, De Souza F, Rajagopalan AK, Chen A, Cloutier AA, Nold TR, Tahera D.. Hyponatremia and psychiatric diseases. London, UK: IntechOpen. 2020. 10.5772/intechopen.90011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bromley SM. Smell and taste disorders: a primary care approach. Am Fam Physician. 2000:61(2):427–36, 438. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brookie KL, Best GI, Conner TS.. Intake of raw fruits and vegetables is associated with better mental health than intake of processed fruits and vegetables. Front Psychol. 2018:9(487):1–14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00487 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Büttner M, Jezova D, Greene B, Konrad C, Kircher T, Murck H.. Target-based biomarker selection—mineralocorticoid receptor-related biomarkers and treatment outcome in major depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2015:66-67:24–37. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bystritsky A, Khalsa SS, Cameron ME, Schiffman J.. Current diagnosis and treatment of anxiety disorders. P T. 2013:38(1):30–57. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Calvo SSC, Egan JM. The endocrinology of taste receptors. N Rev Endocrin. 2015:11:213-227. 10.1038/nrendo.2015.7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carmody TJ, Rush AJ, Bernstein I, Warden D, Brannan S, Burnham D, Woo A, Trivedi MH.. The Montgomery Äsberg and the Hamilton ratings of depression: a comparison of measures. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006:16(8):601–611. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chamoun E, Liu AAS, Duizer LM, Darlington G, Duncan AM, Haines J, Ma DWL.. Taste sensitivity and taste preference measures are correlated in healthy young adults. Chem Senses. 2019:44(2):129–134. 10.1093/chemse/bjy082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chamoun E, Mutch DM, Allen-Vercoe E, Buchholz AC, Duncan AM, Spriet LL, Haines J, Ma DWL, Guelph Family Health Study. A review of the associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms in taste receptors, eating behaviors, and health. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2018:58(2):194–207. 10.1080/10408398.2016.1152229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cipriani A, Furukawa TA, Salanti G, Chaimani A, Atkinson LZ, Ogawa Y, Leucht S, Ruhe HG, Turner EH, Higgins JPT, et al. Comparative efficacy and acceptability of 21 antidepressant drugs for the acute treatment of adults with major depressive disorder: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet. 2018:391(10128):1357–1366. 10.1016/s0140-6736(17)32802-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Clara IP, Cox BJ, Enns MW.. Confirmatory factor analysis of the depression-anxiety-stress scales in depressed and anxious patients. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2001:23(1):61–67. 10.1023/A:1011095624717 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cook IA, Schrader LM, Degiorgio CM, Miller PR, Maremont ER, Leuchter AF.. Trigeminal nerve stimulation in major depressive disorder: acute outcomes in an open pilot study. Epilepsy Behav. 2013:28(2):221–226. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cornelis MC, Tordoff MG, El-Sohemy A, Van Dam RM.. Recalled taste intensity, liking and habitual intake of commonly consumed foods. Appetite. 2017:109:182–189. 10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dean J, Keshavan M.. The neurobiology of depression: an integrated view. Asian J Psychiatr. 2017:27:101–111. 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.01.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deglaire A, Méjean C, Castetbon K, Kesse-Guyot E, Urbano C, Hercberg S, Schlich P.. Development of a questionnaire to assay recalled liking for salt, sweet and fat. Food Qual Preference. 2012:23(2):110–124. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Devane, CL., Chiao, E., Franklin M, Kruep EJ. Anxiety disorders in the 21st century: status, challenges, opportunities, and comorbidity with depression. Am J Manag Care. 2005:11(12 Suppl):S344–S353. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Devi S, Alexandre YO, Loi JK, Gillis R, Ghazanfari N, Creed SJ, Holz LE, Shackleford D, Mackay LK, Heath WR, et al. Adrenergic regulation of the vasculature impairs leukocyte interstitial migration and suppresses immune responses. Immunity. 2021:54(6):1219–1230.e7e1217. 10.1016/j.immuni.2021.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dias AG, Rousseau D, Duizer L, Cockburn M, Chiu W, Nielsen D, El-Sohemy A.. Genetic variation in putative salt taste receptors and salt taste perception in humans. Chem Senses. 2013:38(2):137–145. 10.1093/chemse/bjs090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Doty RL, Shah M, Bromley SM.. Drug-induced taste disorders. Drug Saf. 2008:31(3):199–215. 10.2165/00002018-200831030-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Drewnowski A, Henderson SA, Levine A, Hann C.. Taste and food preferences as predictors of dietary practices in young women. Public Health Nutr. 1999:2(4):513–519. 10.1017/S1368980099000695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eid RS, Gobinath AR, Galea LAM.. Sex differences in depression: insights from clinical and preclinical studies. Prog Neurobiol. 2019:176:86–102. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Endrizzi I, Cliceri D, Menghi L, Aprea E, Charles M, Monteleone E, Dinella C, Spinelli S, Pagliarni E, Laureati M, et al. Relationship between intensity and liking for chemosensory stimuli in food models: a large scale consumer segmentation. Foods. 2021:11(1):1-23. 10.3390/foods11010005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ervina E, Almli VL, Berget I, Spinelli S, Sick J, Dinnella C.. Does responsiveness to basic tastes influence preadolescents’ food liking? Investigating taste responsiveness segment on bitter-sour-sweet and salty-umami model food samples. Nutrients. 2021:13(8):2721. 10.3390/nu13082721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Farhangi MA, Dehghan P, Jahangiry L.. Mental health problems in relation to eating behavior patterns, nutrient intakes and health related quality of life among Iranian female adolescents. PLoS One. 2018:13(4):e0195669. 10.1371/journal.pone.0195669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Firth J, Marx W, Dash S, Carney R, Teasdale SB, Solmi M, Stubbs B, Schuch FB, Carvalho AF, Jacka F, et al. The effects of dietary improvement on symptoms of depression and anxiety: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med. 2019:81(3):265–280. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fournier JC, Derubeis RJ, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S, Amsterdam JD, Shelton R C, Fawcett J.. Antidepressant drug effects and depression severity. JAMA. 2010:303(1):47–53. 10.1001/jama.2009.1943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Freire RC, Cabrera-Abreu C, Milev R.. Neurostimulation in anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Kim YK, editor. Anxiety disorders: advances in experimental medicine and biology. London: Springer Nature; 2020. p. 331–346. 10.1007/978-981-32-9705-0_18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fricker RD, Schonlau M.. Advantages and disadvantages of internet research surveys: evidence from the literature. Field Methods. 2002:14(4):347–367. 10.1177/152582202237725 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gardner MP, Wansink B, Kim J, Park S-B.. Better moods for better eating?: How mood influences food choice. J Consum Psychol. 2014:24(3):320–335. 10.1016/j.jcps.2014.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gartlehner G, Thieda P, Hansen RA, Gaynes BN, Deveaugh-Geiss A, Krebs EE, Lohr KN.. Comparative risk for harms of second-generation antidepressants. Drug Saf. 2008:31(10):851–865. 10.2165/00002018-200831100-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Generoso MB, Taiar IT, Garrocini LP, Bernardon R, Cordeiro Q, Uchida RR, Shiozawa P.. Effect of a 10-day transcutaneous trigeminal nerve stimulation (TNS) protocol for depression amelioration: a randomized, double blind, and sham-controlled phase II clinical trial. Epilepsy Behav. 2019:95:39–42. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gloster AT, Rhoades HM, Novy D, Klotsche J, Senior A, Kunik M, Wilson N, Stanley MA.. Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 in older primary care patients. J Affect Disord. 2008:110(3):248–259. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.01.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Goldstein P, Leshem M.. Dietary sodium, added salt, and serum sodium associations with growth and depression in the U.S. general population. Appetite. 2014:79:83–90. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Green BG, Dalton P, Cowart B, Shaffer G, Rankin K, Higgins J.. Evaluating the “labeled magnitude scale” for measuring sensations of taste and smell. Chem Senses. 1996:21(3):323–334. 10.1093/chemse/21.3.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Grippo AJ, Moffitt JA, Beltz TG, Johnson AK.. Reduced hedonic behavior and altered cardiovascular function induced by mild sodium depletion in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2006:120(5):1133–1143. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Groen RN, Ryan O, Wigman JTW, Riese H, Penninx BWJH, Giltay EJ, Wichers M, Hartman CA.. Comorbidity between depression and anxiety: assessing the role of bridge mental states in dynamic psychological networks. BMC Med. 2020:18(1):308. 10.1186/s12916-020-01738-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Miller N, Haeffel GJ.. Cognitive vulnerability-stress theories of depression: examining affective specificity in the prediction of depression versus anxiety in three prospective studies. Cognit Ther Res. 2004:28(3):309–345. 10.1023/b:cotr.0000031805.60529.0d. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hayes JE, Sullivan BS, Duffy VB.. Explaining variability in sodium intake through oral sensory phenotype, salt sensation and liking. Physiol Behav. 2010:100(4):369–380. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.03.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Heath TP, Melichar JK, Nutt DJ, Donaldson LF.. Human taste thresholds are modulated by serotonin and noradrenaline. J Neurosci. 2006:26(49):12664–12671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Henry JD, Crawford JR.. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005:44(2):227–239. 10.1348/014466505x29657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ileri-Gurel E, Pehlivanoglu B, Dogan M.. Effect of acute stress on taste perception: in relation with baseline anxiety level and body weight. Chem Senses. 2013:38(1):27–34. 10.1093/chemse/bjs075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Itoh M, Kitagawa A, Ouchi H, Yamaguchi M, Watanabe R, Sone H, Kamiyama S.. Effects of visual and aromatic stimulations on the perception of five fundamental tastes. Biosci Biotechnol Biochem. 2022:86:655–664. 10.1093/bbb/zbac029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, Abay SM, Abbafati C, Abbasi N, Abbastabar H, Abd-Allah F, Abdela J, Abdelalim A, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet. 2018:392(10159):1789–1858. 10.1016/s0140-6736(18)32279-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kalin NH. The critical relationship between anxiety and depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2020:177(5):365–367. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20030305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kaur S, Singh R.. Role of different neurotransmitters in Anxiety: a systemic review. Int J Pharm Sci Res. 2017:8(2):411–421. 10.13040/IJPSR.0975-8232.8(2).411-21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Keskitalo K, Tuorila H, Spector TD, Cherkas LF, Knaapila A, Silventoinen K, Perola M.. Same genetic components underlie different measures of sweet taste preference. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007:86(6):1663–1669. 10.1093/ajcn/86.5.1663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kesse-Guyot E, Amieva H, Castetbon K, Henegar A, Ferry M, Jeandel C, Hercberg S, Galan P; SU.VI.MAX 2 Research Group. Adherence to nutritional recommendations and subsequent cognitive performance: findings from the prospective Supplementation with Antioxidant Vitamins and Minerals 2 (SU.VI.MAX 2) study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011:93(1):200–210. 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29761 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Khan A, Leventhal RM, Khan SR, Brown WA.. Severity of depression and response to antidepressants and placebo: an analysis of the food and drug administration database. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002:22(1):40–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Knowland D, Lilascharoen V, Pacia CP, Shin S, Wang EH-J, Lim BK.. Distinct ventral pallidal neural populations mediate separate symptoms of depression. Cell. 2017:170(2):284–297.e18.e218. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.06.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kris‐Etherton PM, Petersen KS, Velarde G, Barnard ND, Miller M, Ros E, O’Keefe JH, Williams K, Horn LV, Na M, et al. Barriers, opportunities, and challenges in addressing disparities in diet‐related cardiovascular disease in the united states. J Am Heart Assoc. 2020:9(7):1-22. : 10.1161/jaha.119.014433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Leshem M. Salt preference in adolescence is predicted by common prenatal and infantile mineralofluid loss. Physiol Behav. 1998:63(4):699–704. 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00525-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Liem DG, Russell CG.. The influence of taste liking on the consumption of nutrient rich and nutrient poor foods. Front Nutr. 2019:6:174. 10.3389/fnut.2019.00174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Linde K, Kriston L, Rucker G, Jamil S, Schumann I, Meissner K, Sigterman K, Schneider A.. Efficacy and acceptability of pharmacological treatments for depressive disorders in primary care: systematic review and network meta-analysis. Ann Fam Med. 2015:13(1):69–79. 10.1370/afm.1687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Liu X, Yan Y, Li F, Zhang D.. Fruit and vegetable consumption and the risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Nutrition. 2016:32(3):296–302. 10.1016/j.nut.2015.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Locher C, Koechlin H, Zion SR, Werner C, Pine DS, Kirsch I, Kessler RC, Kossowsky J.. Efficacy and safety of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, and placebo for common psychiatric disorders among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017:74(10):1011–1020. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH.. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995a:33(3):335–343. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF.. Manual for the depression anxiety and stress scales. 2nd ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995b. [Google Scholar]
  69. Marfell-Jones M, Norton K, Carter L, Olds T. . International standards for anthropometric assessment. South Australia: International Society for the Advancement of Kinanthropometry; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  70. Mariotti A. The effects of chronic stress on health: new insights into the molecular mechanisms of brain–body communication. Future Sci OA. 2015:1(3):FSO23. 10.4155/fso.15.21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Marsch R, Foeller E, Rammes G, Bunck M, Kossl M, Holsboer F, Zieglgansberger W, Landgraf R, Lutz B, Wotjak CT.. Reduced anxiety, conditioned fear, and hippocampal long-term potentiation in transient receptor potential vanilloid type 1 receptor-deficient mice. J Neurosci. 2007:27(4):832–839. 10.1523/jneurosci.3303-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Martin LJ, Sollars SI.. Contributory role of sex differences in the variations of gustatory functions. J Neurosci Res. 2017:85(1–2):594–603. 10.1002/jnr.23819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. McMartin SE, Jacka FN, Colman I.. The association between fruit and vegetable consumption and mental health disorders: evidence from five waves of a national survey of Canadians. Prev Med. 2013:56(3–4):225–230. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Morgan R. Mental health conditions anxiety and depression increased rapidly during lockdown. 2020. [accessed 2022 Apr 21]. http://www.roymorgan.com/findings/8566-australian-mental-health-conditions-covid-19-pandemic-november-2020-202011090621.
  75. Morris MJ, Na ES, Grippo AJ, Johnson AK.. The effects of deoxycorticosterone-induced sodium appetite on hedonic behaviors in the rat. Behav Neurosci. 2006:120(3):571–579. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.571 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Morris MJ, Na ES, Johnson AK.. Salt craving: the psychobiology of pathogenic sodium intake. Physiol Behav. 2008:94(5):709–721. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Morris MJ, Na ES, Johnson AK.. Mineralocorticoid receptor antagonism prevents hedonic deficits induced by a chronic sodium appetite. Behav Neurosci. 2010:124(2):211–224. 10.1037/a0018910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Muscaritoli M. The impact of nutrients on mental health and well-being: insights from the literature. Front Nutr. 2021:8:656290. 10.3389/fnut.2021.656290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. National Mental Health Commission. Monitoring mental health and suicide prevention reform: National report 2018. NMHC; 2018. [accessed 2022 August 7]. https://nacchocommunique.files.wordpress.com/2018/10/monitoring-mental-health-and-suicide-prevention-reform-national-report-2018.pdf.
  80. Nguyen B, Ding D, Mihrshahi S.. Fruit and vegetable consumption and psychological distress: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses based on a large Australian sample. BMJ Open. 2017:7(3):e014201. 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Norton PJ. Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS-21): psychometric analysis across four racial groups. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2007:20(3):253–265. 10.1080/10615800701309279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Opie RS, O’Neil A, Itsiopoulos C, Jacka FN.. The impact of whole-of-diet interventions on depression and anxiety: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr. 2015:18(11):2074–2093. 10.1017/s1368980014002614 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Özdemir O, Soyoral Y, Işık M, Özdemir P, Balahoroglu R.. Is there any relationship between sodium and depression?. J Mood Disorders. 2014:4(4):163. [Google Scholar]
  84. Pagliarini E, Proserpio C, Spinelli S, Lavelli V, Laureati M, Arena E, Di Monaco R, Menghi L, Gallina Toschi T, Braghieri A, et al. The role of sour and bitter perception in liking, familiarity and choice for phenol-rich plant-based foods. Food Qual Prefer. 2021:93:104250. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104250 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Paulino Trevizol A, Cordeiro Q.. Trigeminal nerve stimulation for major depressive disorder: an updated systematic review. Arch Neurosci. 2016:4(1):e39263. 10.5812/archneurosci.39263 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Pezirkianidis C, Karakasidou E, Lakioti A, Stalikas A, Galanakis M.. Psychometric properties of the Depression, Anxiety, Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) in a Greek sample. ­Psychology. 2018:09(15):2933–2950 10.4236/psych.2018.915170 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pilic L, Lubasinski NJ, Berk M, Ward D, Graham CA-M, Da Silva Anastacio V, King A, Mavrommatis Y.. The associations between genetics, salt taste perception and salt intake in young adults. Food Qual Prefer. 2020:84:103954. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103954 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Pilic L, Mavrommatis Y.. Genetic predisposition to salt-sensitive normotension and its effects on salt taste perception and intake. Br J Nutr. 2018:120(7):721–731. 10.1017/s0007114518002027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Porcelli SMD, Drago AMD, Fabbri C, Gibiino SMD, Calati RPP, Serretti AMDP.. Pharmacogenetics of antidepressant response. J Psychiatr Neurosci. 2011:36(2):87–113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Puputti S, Aisala H, Hoppu U, Sandell M.. Multidimensional measurement of individual differences in taste perception. Food Qual Prefer. 2018:65:10–17. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.12.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Puputti S, Aisala H, Hoppu U, Sandell M.. Factors explaining individual differences in taste sensitivity and taste modality recognition among Finnish adults. J Sens Stud. 2019:34(4):e12506-n/a. 10.1111/joss.12506 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  92. Ramsden CE, Faurot KR, Zamora D, Palsson OS, Macintosh BA, Gaylord S, Taha AY, Rapoport SI, Hibbeln JR, Davis JM, et al. Targeted alterations in dietary n-3 and n-6 fatty acids improve life functioning and reduce psychological distress among patients with chronic headache. Pain. 2015:156(4):587–596. 10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460348.84965.47 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rees J, Radavelli Bagatini S, Lo J, Hodgson JM, Christophersen CT, Daly RM, Magliano DJ, Shaw JE, Sim M, Bondonno CP, et al. Association between fruit and vegetable intakes and mental health in the Australian diabetes obesity and lifestyle cohort. Nutrients. 2021:13(5):1447. 10.3390/nu13051447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Riis NL, Bjoernsbo KS, Toft U, Trolle E, Hyldig G, Hartley IE, Keast R, Lassen AD.. Impact of salt reduction interventions on salt taste sensitivity and liking, a cluster randomized controlled trial. Food Qual Prefer. 2021:87:104059. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Root DH, Melendez RI, Zaborszky L, Napier TC.. The ventral pallidum: Subregion-specific functional anatomy and roles in motivated behaviors. Prog Neurobiol. 2015:130:29–70. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Roper SD, Chaudhari N.. Taste buds: cells, signals and synapses. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017:18(8):485–497. 10.1038/nrn.2017.68 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Rubino DR, Schmidt PJ.. Sex differences and the neurobiology of affective disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019:1(44):111–128. 10.1038/s41386-018-0148-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Santomauro DF, Mantilla Herrera AM, Shadid J, Zheng P, Ashbaugh C, Pigott DM, Abbafati C, Adolph C, Amlag JO, Aravkin AY, et al. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet. 2021:398(10312):1700–1712. 10.1016/s0140-6736(21)02143-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Sawant N, Parkar S, Rupani K, Bansal H, Singh S.. Hyponatremia misdiagnosed as depression. Ann Indian Psychiatry. 2019:3(2):168–170. 10.4103/aip.aip_31_19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Schrader LM, Cook IA, Miller PR, Maremont ER, DeGiorgio CM.. Trigeminal nerve stimulation in major depressive disorder: first proof of concept in an open pilot trial. Epilepsy Behav. 2011:22(3):475–478. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.06.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Schutz HG, Cardello AV. A labeled affective magnitude (LAM) scale for assessing food liking/disliking. J Sens Stud. 2001:16(2):117–159. 10.1111/j.1745-459X.2001.tb00293.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Scinska A, Sienkiewicz-Jarosz H, Kuran W, Ryglewicz D, Rogowski A, Wrobel E, Korkosz A, Kukwa A, Kostowski W, Bienkowski P.. Depressive symptoms and taste reactivity in humans. Physiol Behav. 2004:82(5):899–904. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Seney ML, Sibille E.. Sex differences in mood disorders: perspectives from humans and rodent models. Biol Sex Differ. 2014:5(17):1-10. 10.1186/s13293-014-0017-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Simon SA, De Araujo IE, Stapleton JR, Nicolelis MA L.. Multisensory processing of gustatory stimuli. Chemosens Percept. 2008:1(2):95–102. 10.1007/s12078-008-9014-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Slee A, Nazareth I, Bondaronek P, Liu Y, Cheng Z, Freemantle N.. Pharmacological treatments for generalised anxiety disorder: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet. 2019:393(10173):768–777. 10.1016/s0140-6736(18)31793-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Smith KS, Tindell AJ, Aldridge JW, Berridge KC.. Ventral pallidum roles in reward and motivation. Behav Brain Res. 2009:196(2):155–167. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Smith SA, Trotter PD, McGlone FP, Walker SC.. Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on human taste perception. Chem Senses. 2021:46:bjaa078. 10.1093/chemse/bjaa078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Spetter MS, Smeets PAM, De Graaf C, Viergever MA.. Representation of sweet and salty taste intensity in the brain. Chem Senses. 2010:35(9):831–840. 10.1093/chemse/bjq093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Tavakol M, Dennick R.. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int J Med Educ. 2011:2:53–55. 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Tindell AJ, Smith KS, Peciña S, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW.. Ventral pallidum firing codes hedonic reward: when a bad taste turns good. J Neurophysiol. 2006:96(5):2399–2409. 10.1152/jn.00576.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Trevizol AP, Shiozawa P, Sato IA, Calfat ELDB, Alberto RL, Cook IA, Medeiros HH, Cordeiro Q.. Trigeminal nerve stimulation (TNS) for generalized anxiety disorder: a case study. Brain Stimul. 2015:8(3):659–660. 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Van Selm M, Jankowski NW.. Conducting online surveys. Qual Quant. 2006:40(3):435–456. 10.1007/s11135-005-8081-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  113. Veček NN, Mucalo L, Dragun R, Miličević T, Pribisalić A, Patarčić I, Hayward C, Polašek O, Kolčić I.. The association between salt taste perception, mediterranean diet and metabolic syndrome: a cross-sectional study. Nutrients. 2020:12(4):1164. 10.3390/nu12041164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Vink JM, Van Hooijdonk KJM, Willemsen G, Feskens EJM, Boomsma DI.. Causes of variation in food preference in the Netherlands. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2020:23(4):195–203. 10.1017/thg.2020.66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wanich U, Sayompark D, Riddell L, Cicerale S, Liem D, Mohebbi M, Macfarlane S, Keast R.. Assessing food liking: comparison of food liking questionnaires and direct food tasting in two cultures. Nutrients. 2018:10(12):1957. 10.3390/nu10121957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. World Health Organisation. Body mass index—BMI; 2021. [accessed 2022 July 10]. https://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/disease-prevention/nutrition/a-healthy-lifestyle/body-mass-index-bmi.
  117. Yeung AWK. Neuroanatomical correlates of perceiving the intensity and pleasantness of intense saltiness in healthy subjects. Curr Sci. 2019:116:69–74. 10.18520/cs/v116/i1/69-74 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Yeung AWK, Tanabe HC, Suen JLK, Goto TK.. Taste intensity modulates effective connectivity from the insular cortex to the thalamus in humans. Neuroimage. 2016:135:214–222. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

bjad038_suppl_Supplementary_Tables_S1-S2

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.


Articles from Chemical Senses are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES