Abstract
Background:
Both cognitive and non-cognitive (e.g., traits like curiosity) factors are critical for social and emotional functioning and independently predict educational attainment. These factors are heritable and genetically correlated with a range of health-relevant traits and behaviors in adulthood (e.g., risk-taking, psychopathology). However, whether these associations are present during adolescence, and to what extent these relationships diverge, could have implications for adolescent health and well-being.
Methods:
Using data from 5,517 youth of European ancestry from the ongoing Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM Study, we examined associations between polygenic scores (PGS) for cognitive and non-cognitive factors and outcomes related to cognition, socioeconomic status, risk tolerance and decision-making, substance initiation, psychopathology, and brain structure.
Results:
Cognitive and non-cognitive PGSs were both positively associated with cognitive performance and family income, and negatively associated with ADHD and severity of psychotic-like experiences. The cognitive PGS was also associated with greater risk-taking, delayed discounting, and anorexia, as well as lower likelihood of nicotine initiation. The cognitive PGS was further associated with cognition scores and anorexia in within-sibling analyses, suggesting these results do not solely reflect the effects of assortative mating or passive gene-environment correlations. The cognitive PGS showed significantly stronger associations with cortical volumes than the non-cognitive PGS and was associated with right hemisphere caudal anterior cingulate and pars-orbitalis in within-sibling analyses, while the non-cognitive PGS showed stronger associations with white matter fractional anisotropy and a significant within-sibling association for right superior corticostriate-frontal cortex.
Conclusions:
Our findings suggest that PGSs for cognitive and non-cognitive factors show similar associations with cognition and socioeconomic status as well as other psychosocial outcomes, but distinct associations with regional neural phenotypes in this adolescent sample.
Keywords: Cognitive performance, polygenic scores, educational attainment, academic achievement, middle childhood, neuroimaging
INTRODUCTION
As educational attainment (EduA) is among the strongest predictors of positive outcomes across the lifespan (e.g., income, health, well-being; Gutacker et al., 2023; Raghupathi & Raghupathi, 2020; Zajacova & Lawrence, 2018), it is important to understand what contributes to educational attainment. Despite often being thought of as a purely “environmental” factor, EduA itself is moderately heritable (h2=0.41–0.47; Heath et al., 1985; Silventoinen et al., 2020), and the genetic contributions to EduA can be broken down into component traits. Evidence that EduA is strongly impacted by cognitive ability and a set of broadly defined “non-cognitive skills” (e.g., emotion regulation and personality traits such as grit and curiosity; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003; Duckworth et al., 2007, 2019; Kovas et al., 2015; Malanchini et al., 2019; Noftle & Robins, 2007) has inspired recent approaches that have deconstructed the genetic architecture of EduA into cognitive and non-cognitive components that have shared and unique associations with EduA-related phenotypes (e.g., risk-taking and psychopathology; Malanchini et al., 2023; Tucker-Drob et al., 2016; Tucker-Drob & Harden, 2012). The extent to which these differential associations extend to childhood, before education has been completed, remains poorly understood (Malanchini et al., 2023). In the present study, we examined whether cognitive and non-cognitive polygenic score (PGS) associations mirror those found in primarily adult sample studies and use within-sibling analyses to assess whether results show evidence of confounding (e.g., by population stratification, assortative mating, or passive gene-environment correlations).
Deconstructing the Genetic Architecture of Educational Attainment into Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Components
Demange et al., 2021 demonstrated that EduA can be genetically parsed into cognitive and non-cognitive factors. To study genetic influences on “non-cognitive skills,” Demange et al. 2021 performed a novel “GWAS-by-subtraction” by residualizing the genetic effects of EduA (N=1,131,881; Lee et al., 2018) on cognitive performance in a Cholesky decomposition using genomic structural equation modeling (Grotzinger et al., 2019), leaving a residual “non-cognitive” genetic factor (N=510,715) and “cognitive” genetic factor (N=257,841; Lee et al., 2018) (see Figure 1 for overview). In this way, the authors generated a new genome-wide association study (GWAS) of “non-cognitive skills” that represents genetic influences on EduA that are not shared with cognitive performance (by design, the “non-cognitive” factor is orthogonal to the “cognitive” factor). Their study found 157 independent loci associated with the non-cognitive factor and observed that the non-cognitive factor showed distinct associations with other relevant phenotypes, including positive genetic correlations with risk tolerance and some psychiatric disorders (e.g., bipolar disorder and schizophrenia), and was positively genetically correlated with personality traits including conscientiousness, extraversion, and agreeableness while the cognitive factor showed negative or null correlations with these same phenotypes (Demange et al., 2021).
While the GWASs mentioned above consisted largely of adult participants, adolescence is a critical stage of both cognitive and non-cognitive development. Prior studies have shown that early differences in cognitive and non-cognitive factors (e.g., personality; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003; Noftle & Robins, 2007) may contribute to EduA, employment outcomes, and overall success later in life (Duckworth et al., 2019; Moffitt et al., 2011). A recent preprint reported that the association between a non-cognitive PGS and academic achievement nearly doubled between the ages of 7 and 16 (Malanchini et al., 2023), suggesting that genetic factors related to non-cognitive facets of EduA may become particularly influential during adolescence, a critical developmental period for factors related to EduA. Importantly, examinations of genetic associations between cognitive and non-cognitive factors and brain imaging have mostly been conducted in adult samples (Demange et al., 2021). Little is known of the brain mechanisms related to these factors in adolescence, a critical time period for neural plasticity. In sum, despite adolescence being a critical time for future academic achievement, little is known about the cognitive and non-cognitive influences on EduA and their contributions to relevant traits, behaviors, and brain structure in adolescence.
The Current Study
In the current study, we estimated the associations between PGS for cognitive and non-cognitive factors of EduA and outcome measures among children of European ancestry enrolled in the ongoing Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (Volkow et al., 2018; analytic n up to 5,517). We focused on behavioral phenotypes implicated in studies on adults (i.e., cognition, family socioeconomic status, risk tolerance & decision-making, substance initiation, psychopathology; Lee et al., 2018) and also examined associations with brain structure. Finally, as prior research has shown that genetic confounds (e.g., assortative mating; Horwitz et al., 2023) can inflate GWAS test statistics and polygenic score associations (Okbay et al., 2022), we performed post hoc within-sibling analyses to assess whether any significant associations may be independent of assortative mating, passive gene-environment correlation, or other sociodemographic confounders.
METHODS
Participants
The ongoing Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development SM (ABCD) Study® is a longitudinal study following 11,879 children (ages 8.9–11 at baseline; born between 2005–2009) recruited from 22 research sites across the United States to study the development of complex behavior and biology from middle childhood to late adolescence/young adulthood in the context of experience and genetic background (Volkow et al., 2018). It includes a family-based component in which twin (n=2108), triplet (n=30), non-twin siblings (n=1,589), and singletons (n=8,148) were recruited. Caregivers provided written informed consent and their children provided verbal assent. For the present study, we used data from the baseline (2016–2018; ages: 9–11) as well as 2-year (FU2; 2018–2021; ages 11–13) and 3-year (FU3; 2021–2022; ages 9–14) ) follow-up sessions. Analyses were only conducted in individuals with genetic ancestry most similar to those of European genetic ancestry reference populations (see Genetic Data section below), due to the lack of relevant well-powered discovery GWAS in other ancestries and the low predictive utility of PGS when applied across ancestries (Martin et al., 2019). After excluding individuals with missing outcome or covariate data, described below, analytic Ns ranged from 5,168–5,517 (baseline), 3352–5,006 (FU2), and 2,968–4745 (FU3), with the exception of substance use data as described below.
Measures
Cognitive performance, risk tolerance/decision making, substance use initiation, psychopathology, and neuroimaging data were drawn from the baseline, FU2, and FU3 assessments from the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive (NDA; https://nda.nih.gov/); data release 4.0 and 5.0). Socioeconomic status and genomic data (release 3.0) were derived from the baseline session.
Cognition.
Crystallized and fluid intelligence as well as their total composite were estimated from the NIH ToolBox assessment (Luciana et al., 2018).
Socioeconomic status.
Caregiver-reported combined past-12-month family income (Luciana et al., 2018) and neighborhood deprivation index, a composite of neighborhood socioeconomics, were drawn from baseline data (Fan et al., 2021).
Risk tolerance and decision-making.
Risk tolerance was measured at baseline using the single item, “I enjoy taking risks” from the sensation seeking scale of the UPPS-P (Watts et al., 2020). Delayed discounting was measured at baseline using the single item cash choice task where youth decide whether they would “rather have $75 in three days or $115 in 3 months” (Luciana et al., 2018; Wulfert et al., 2002). This single item measure was used, as opposed to behavioral data acquired (Kohler et al., 2022) due to quality control procedures of these data resulting in the exclusion of large amounts of baseline data.
Substanc use initation.
Lifetime alcohol, nicotine, and cannabis initiation was assessed from the annual substance use interview and mid-year substance phone interviews (Miller et al., 2023; Volkow et al., 2018) from baseline to FU3. Individuals endorsing substance use only in the context of religious ceremonies were excluded. Substance naive participants endorsed no substance initiation (e,g., substance naive individuals being compared to those who initiatiate cannabis use had not used alcohol, tobacco or other substances). Substance initiation analytic Ns ranged from 2,968 to 4,745 (see Supplemental Table 1–2 for additional information).
Psychopathology.
We assessed seven mental health related measures. Given that schizophrenia typically presents later in adolescence, we included a summary score of the severity of youth psychotic-like experiences (Karcher & Barch, 2021) from FU2. We also included a caregiver-reported diagnosis screener for autism from the baseline assessment (Barch et al., 2018). In a similar vein, as most psychopathologies assessed tend to onset in mid-adolescence, when possible we used baseline to FU2 to generate lifetime KSADS-5 diagnoses (Kaufman et al., 1997) to capture this critical time period for all assessed mental health diagnoses. Lifetime KSADS-5 diagnoses were created for: obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), anorexia, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder (MDD), and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The analytical N for these measures ranged from 3,352 to 3,737 and all definitions and items used to create the lifetime mental health diagnoses measures can be found in Supplemental Table 1.
Neuroimaging.
Indices of gray matter structure (i.e., cortical thickness, cortical surface area, and subcortical and cortical volumes) and white matter tracts (i.e., fractional anisotropy [FA], mean diffusivity [MD] were derived using the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., 2006) and Atlas Tract (Basser et al., 1994), respectively. No task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were examined due to test–retest reliability concerns (Elliott et al., 2020). Acquisition and preprocessing methodology (Casey et al., 2018; Hagler et al., 2019) as well as additional information can be found in the Supplemental Table 3 and Supplemental Methods.
Polygenic Scores
Quality control was performed on the genomic data (ABCD data release 3.0) using the Rapid Imputation and COmputational PIpeLIne for Genome-Wide Association Studies (Lam et al., 2020). Briefly, after performing QC and using a combination of parent-reported demographic information and principal components analysis to identify a subset of n=5,556 individuals of European ancestry, the genetic data were imputed to the TOPMed imputation reference panel (Taliun et al., 2021; Supplemental Methods). Only individuals of European ancestry were analyzed as the Demange et al. discovery GWAS only included individuals of European ancestry, and there is poor predictive utility across ancestries which may lead to erroneous conclusions (e.g., false negatives; Martin et al., 2019). Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with imputation Rsq>0.8 and minor allele frequency>0.01 were retained for PGS analyses.
We used PRS-CS (Ge et al., 2019) to calculate polygenic scores in the European ancestry subset of the ABCD Study sample, using effect sizes from the Demange et al. GWAS of “cognitive skills” (GWAS catalog accession GCST90011875; effective n=257,700; Demange et al., 2021) and “non-cognitive skills” (GWAS catalog accession GCST90011874; effective n=510,795; Demange et al., 2021) and the 1000 Genomes Phase 3 European reference panel (1000 Genomes Project Consortium et al., 2015). We used the ‘auto’ function of PRS-CS, allowing the software to learn the global shrinkage parameter from the data (see Supplemental Methods for details).
Statistical Analyses
Analyses were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework and conducted using mixed effects models implemented using lmer (for continuous outcomes) and glmer (for dichotomous outcomes) from the lme4 package (Austin, 2010; Bates et al., 2022) in R (v4.3; R Core Team). Age, sex, and the first 10 genetic principal components were included as fixed effect covariates with family ID and recruitment site as random intercepts to account for data dependence. For imaging models, recruitment site was replaced by MRI serial number. Imaging models also included MRI manufacturer, global brain metrics representing the mean for each modality, and mean motion for DTI as fixed effects. We used false discovery rate (FDR; Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) to account for multiple testing (pfdr<0.05); FDR was applied separately to the non-imaging phenotypes and each respective imaging modality.
We deviated from our pre-registered analyses in two ways: First, we tested whether the regression coefficients for the cognitive and non-cognitive PGS significantly differed from each other in each model (pdiff; Supplemental Methods). Second, we conducted post hoc within-sibling analyses to assess whether any significant associations arising from primary analyses (aside from familial SES due to sibling similarity) may plausibly represent direct genetic effects. Significant within-sibling effects indicate that these associations are unconfounded by population stratification, assortative mating, passive gene-environment correlations, and other potential population-level confounds (though it should be noted that active and evocative rGE will still influence within-sibling variation in PGS effects; Brumpton et al., 2020; Howe et al., 2022; Young et al., 2018). For these analyses, we included both the family mean PGS and a sibling’s deviation from their family mean PGS as predictors in a mixed-effect model, as has been done previously (Selzam et al., 2019) (see Supplemental Methods for analysis details).
RESULTS
Demographic descriptive statistics for the baseline analytic sample (max N=5,517) are available in Table 1. The cognitive and non-cognitive polygenic scores (PGS) were negatively correlated with one another (r=−0.12, p<2e-16).
Table 1:
Variable | Mean (SD)/n (%) |
---|---|
| |
baseline | |
Sex (male) | 2612 (47.0%) |
Age (years) | 9.93(0.63) |
Household income | |
<$35,000 | 375(7.1%) |
$35,000–$49,000 | 283(5.1%) |
$50,000–$74,999 | 717(13.5%) |
$75,000–$99,999 | 896(16.9%) |
$100,000–$199,999 | 2178(41.1%) |
$200,000 | 849(16.0%) |
Highest caregiver education | |
Less than high school | 25 (0.45%) |
High school degree or equivalent | 188(3.4%) |
Some college, associate degree | 1046(18.8%) |
College degree | 1753(31.6%) |
Master’s degree | 1723(31.0%) |
Doctorate/professional degree | 829(14.8%) |
Parental marital status | |
Married or co-habiting | 4721(85.1%) |
Widowed | 41(0.7%) |
Divorced/separated | 620(11.2%) |
Never married | 164(3.0%) |
Behavioral and sociodemographic outcomes
Cognition (Figure 2A; Supplemental Table 4).
The cognitive PGS was positively associated with all 3 cognition measures (i.e., fluid, crystallized, total score); Bs>0.136, pfdrs<3.51E-24). The non-cognitive PGS was associated with crystallized intelligence and total scores (Bs>0.05, pfdrs<7.64E-04), but not fluid intelligence (B=0.013, pfdr=0.715). For all 3 measures, the cognition scores were more strongly associated with the cognitive PGS relative to non-cognitive PGS (pdiff<0.05).
SES (Figure 2B; Supplemental Table 4).
The cognitive PGS was associated with greater family SES (family income: B=0.035, pfdr=3.36E-05; neighborhood deprivation index: B=−0.014, pfdr=0.028). In contrast, the non-cognitive PGS was significantly associated only with total past-12-month combined family income (B =0.04, pfdr=3.21E-07), but not the neighborhood deprivation index (B=−0.004, pfdr=0.812). Associations with income and neighborhood deprivation did not significantly differ between the cognitive and non-cognitive PGS.
Risk & decision making (Figure 2C; Supplemental Table 4).
The cognitive PGS was associated with increased odds of delay-discounting (OR=1.076, pfdr=0.028) and was positively associated with self-reported risk-taking (B=0.031, pfdr=0.049). There were no significant associations between either of the risk and decision-making outcomes and the non-cognitive PGS (pfdr>0.05).
Substance use (Figure 2D; Supplemental Table 4).
The cognitive PGS was associated with decreased odds of nicotine initiation (OR=0.818, pfdr=0.029). There were no significant associations between any of the substance initiation measures and the non-cognitive PGS (pfdr>0.05). Substance use associations with the cognitive and non-cognitive PGS did not significantly differ (pdiff>0.05).
Psychopathology (Figure 2E; Supplemental Table 4).
Both the cognitive and non-cognitive PGS were associated with decreased odds of ADHD diagnosis (cog PGS: OR=0.908, pfdr=0.028; non-cog PGS: OR=0.889, pfdr=0.008) and lower severity of psychotic-like experiences (PLE) (Bs<−0.0316, pfdr<0.050). The cognitive PGS was also significantly associated with increased odds of an anorexia diagnosis (OR=1.113, pfdr=0.044). The cognitive and non-cognitive PGS did not significantly differ from one another in their associations with any of the assessed mental health outcomes (pdiffs>0.05).
Within Sibling Analyses (Figure 3; Supplemental Table 5).
Post-hoc analyses revealed that within-family variation in the cognitive PGS was associated with all three cognitive outcomes and anorexia diagnosis after accounting for between-family variation (ps<0.317). No associations with the non-cognitive PGS showed significant within-family effects (ps>0.317)
Brain Structure
Global Gray Matter Indices (Figure 4A; Supplemental Table 6).
Both cognitive and non-cognitive PGS were positively associated with all (n=9) global volumes of interest including whole brain, whole brain cortical volume, total left and right hemisphere cortical volume, left and right cerebral-white-matter, subcortical, supratentorial, and intracranial volume; (Bs> 0.030, pfdrs<0.0021). All associations were significantly larger for the cognitive PGS relative to the non-cognitive PGS (all pdiff<0.05).
Global White Matter Tracts (Figure 4A; Supplemental Table 6).
The non-cognitive PGS was positively associated with the global measure of mean diffusivity (B=0.029, pfdr=0.023). The cognitive PGS was not associated with any global atlas tract fiber measures, but the associations for the non-cognitive PGS were not significantly stronger than that observed for the cognitive PGS.
Volume (Figure 4B; Supplemental Table 7).
The cognitive PGS was positively associated with the following 13 regional brain volumes: (7 lateral and 3 bilateral: 1–2) bilateral inferior temporal gyri, 3–4) bilateral precentral gyri, 5–6) bilateral superior temporal gyri, 7) right (R) banks of superior temporal sulcus, 8) R caudal anterior cingulate, 9) R inferior parietal gyrus, 10) R middle temporal gyrus, 11) R pars orbitalis, 12) R temporal pole, 13) left (L) rostral anterior cingulate (B=0.032–0.0383, pfdr=0.005–0.035). The following regional associations were significantly greater for the cognitive PGS relative to the non-cognitive PGS: rh-pars orbitalis, lh-superiortemporal, rh-middle temporal, rh-inferior parietal, and bilateral inferior temporal volumes. The non-cognitive PGS was not signficantly associated with any regional cortical volume.
DTI-FA (Figure 4C; Supplemental Table 7).
The cognitive and non-cognitive PGS were associated with 1 bilateral white matter tract and 10 white matter tracts (1 unilateral, 1, interhemispheric, and 4 bilateral), respectively. The cognitive PGS was positively associated with the bilateral corticospinal/pyramidal tract (B=0.035–0.041, pfdr=0.001–0.005). Tracts associated with the non-cognitive PGS included: 1–2) bilateral corticospinal/pyramidal, 3–4) bilateral superior corticostriatal-frontal cortex, 5–6) bilateral superior corticostriatal-parietal cortex, 7–8) bilateral superior corticostriatal, bilateral corpus callosum, and bilateral forceps minor (Bs=−0.029–0.038, pfdrs=0.001–0.046). Both the forceps minor and the left superior corticostriatal-frontal cortex showed associations with the non-cognitive PGS that were of significantly greater magnitude (pdiffs<0.05) than with the cognitive PGS.
DTI- MD (Figure 4D; Supplemental Table 7).
The cognitive PGS was negatively associated with the bilateral corticospinal/pyramidal tracts (Bs=−0.035 - −0.027, pfdr=0.003–0.026). The non-cognitive PGS was not significantly associated with any tract MD, but its association with the bilateral corticospinal/pyramidal tracts did not significantly differ from that observed for the cognitive PGS.
No significant regions in any neuroimaging modality showed evidence for laterality; i.e., estimates within one hemisphere resided within the 95% confidence interval of the other hemisphere.
Within Sibling Analyses (Figure 5; Supplemental Table 8).
Post-hoc analyses revealed that within-family variation in the cognitive PGS contributed to associations with two regional volume measures (right hemisphere caudal anterior cingulate and pars orbitalis) after accounting for significant between-family variation (ps<0.048). The non-cognitive PGS showed significant within-family effects for the FA measure right superior corticostriate-frontal cortex (p<0.034).
DISCUSSION
Our study of phenotypic correlates of genetic propensity for cognitive and non-cognitive factors during middle childhood/early adolescence (ns=2,968–5,517) revealed four broad findings: First, cognitive and non-cognitive polygenic liability both showed associations with cognition scores, socioeconomic status, and psychopathology. Notably, the associations between cognition scores and the cognitive PGS were of significantly greater magnitude than those of the non-cognitive PGS, but there were no other significant differences between the PGSs. Second, as revealed by our within-family analyses, associations between the cognitive PGS and some outcomes (e.g., cognition, anorexia) were consistent with direct genetic effects and/or evocative/active rGE. Third, the cognitive PGS was more strongly associated with greater regional cortical volumes than the non-cognitive PGS, while the non-cognitive PGS generally showed greater magnitude of associations with white matter tract fractional anisotropy. Fourth, within-family analyses revealed significant within-family associations between the cognitive PGS and right hemisphere caudal anterior cingulate and pars orbitalis, while the non-cognitive PGS had significant within-family associations with the FA tract right superior corticostriate-frontal cortex. Overall, these findings converge with other recent data to suggest that the phenotypic correlates of genetic liability to cognitive and non-cognitive factors underlying educational attainment are present during childhood, prior to the completion of most education, and largely mirror patterns in adults.
Polygenic Scores for Cognitive and Non-cognitive Factors Related to Educational Attainment Have Shared and Unique Correlates
The cognitive PGS was associated with higher fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and a total combined score. The largest association was with crystallized intelligence, mirroring a prior ABCD study where researchers found that an intelligence PGS was most predictive of the crystallized intelligence score (Loughnan et al., 2023). The cognitive PGS was also associated with lower neighborhood deprivation and higher family income. These findings reflect prior research showing that cognitive PGS are associated with increased family income as well as better neighborhood environment (Judd et al., 2020), and that cognitive development in children is linked to both family income and their grandfather’s occupational status (Najman et al., 2004). Interestingly, we found that the cognitive PGS was associated with delayed discounting and greater risk tolerance, as well as decreased odds for initiating nicotine use; this latter finding is congruent with previous research showing that greater educational attainment may correlate with decreased likelihood of substance initiation (Fothergill et al., 2008; Morin et al., 2012). The cognitive PGS was also associated with decreased odds of an ADHD diagnosis and lower psychotic-like experience (PLE) severity scores, but greater risk for anorexia. This latter finding is generally in line with work showing a positive association between cognitive performance and educational attainment and anorexia (Demange et al., 2023; THE BRAINSTORM CONSORTIUM et al., 2018; Watson et al., 2019), while the correlation with lower severity of PLEs is highly consistent with other work showing PLEs to be related to decreased school performance (Davies et al., 2018; Villa et al., 2023) and general cognitive deficits (e.g., executive functioning; Sheffield et al., 2018).
While the non-cognitive PGS was also associated with higher crystallized and total cognition scores, associations with the non-cognitive PGS were significantly smaller than with the cognitive PGS (pdiffs 2.2e-16 – 1.51E-12). Similar to findings with the cognitive PGS, the non-cognitive PGS was associated with higher family income, lower risk of ADHD, and lower PLE severity scores. The association of both PGSs with lower likelihood of an ADHD diagnosis is very consistent with previous studies (de Zeeuw et al., 2014). While some measures were only significantly associated with the cognitive PGS (e.g., risk-taking, nicotine initiation, anorexia), only the cognition scores (crystallized, fluid, and total) showed effect sizes which statistically differed (pdiff < 0.05) for the cognitive PGS vs. non-cognitive PGS.
In contrast to the phenotypic outcomes described above, neuroimaging measures showed a greater number of associations which significantly differed for the cognitive and non-cognitive PGSs. Globally, all volume measures (N = 9; Figure 4) were significantly associated with both the cognitive and non-cognitive PGSs, but all associations were significantly stronger for the cognitive PGS. In contrast, there were no significant differences between the effect sizes of the two PGSs across the assessed global diffusion tensor imaging modalities. While mean diffusivity was only significantly associated with the non-cognitive PGS, this effect size did not significantly differ from that of the cognitive PGS. The cognitive PGS was associated primarily with regional cortical volumes (N = 13; Figure 4), while the non-cognitive PGS was associated only with regional white matter modalities (N =10; Figure 4). Of note, across the majority of assessed non-global metrics, there were significant differences in the strength of association between the two PGS. For example, bilateral inferior temporal volume, a region implicated in studies of academic achievement, showed a significantly stronger association with the cognitive PGS (Mackey et al., 2015). Conversely, the tract forceps minor, which has previously been implicated in executive functioning and achievement (Loe et al., 2019), showed a significantly stronger (negative) association with the non-cognitive PGS. Overall, these findings suggest that cognitive skills that contribute to educational attainment may relate more strongly to cortical volumes, while non-cognitive factors of educational success may be better reflected in the micro structure of the white matter tracts connecting between brain regions.
Evidence for Direct and Indirect Genetic Effects on Phenotypes
Using within-sibling analyses to contrast within- vs. between-family estimates for phenotypic correlates, we sought to determine whether associations with the polygenic scores were consistent with possible confounding by passive rGE or other mechanisms (e.g., assortative mating). The cognitive PGS showed significant within-sibling associations with all three cognition measures (fluid, crystallized, and total score; Supplemental Table 5, Figure 3), suggesting that associations between the cognitive PGS and these cognitive measures reflect direct genetic effects and/or evocative/active rGE, rather than passive rGE or other confounding factors. Interestingly, the cognitive PGS showed a significant within-sibling association with anorexia, suggesting direct genetic effects or evocative/active rGE influence this relationship as well. The cognitive PGS also had significant within-sibling associations with the right hemisphere caudal anterior cingulate and pars orbitalis measures, both of which have been associated with the salience brain network and cognitive salience (Snyder et al., 2021). No other within-sibling associations were significant, consistent with effects of passive rGE or other confounding factors that vary between families (de Zeeuw et al., 2014; Mitchell et al., 2022).
The non-cognitive PGS showed a significant within-sibling association with FA tract right superior corticostriate-frontal cortex, which is associated with many relevant pathways, including learning and reward sensitivity (Shipp, 2017). The null within-sibling findings for all other phenotypes for the non-cognitive PGS suggest that associations between the non-cognitive PGS and cognition scores and other outcomes may be due to passive rGE (e.g., parents with greater genetic predisposition for motivation or curiosity might enroll their children in additional courses or training) or confounding factors that vary between families, such as assortative mating. Further, these null findings do not seem to be due solely to the lower sample size of these models (N=1,702), as the non-cognitive PGS was associated with crystallized and total neurocognition scores when we randomly resampled 1,702 individuals 5,000 times and averaged across regression models in these smaller samples, and these “total” effect sizes were similar to the full effects presented in Figures 2 and 4 (see “Total” effects in Supplemental Tables 5–6). Our findings for the non-cognitive PGS contrast with a recent preprint from Malanchini et al., 2023 who found evidence that both passive and evocative/active rGE mechanisms appear to contribute to the association between non-cognitive genetic factors and academic achievement in a sample of adolescents (Malanchini et al., 2023). Our conflicting results may be partly explained by a combination of social, educational, and cultural differences between the participants of the ABCD study (American) and the Malanchini study (England and Wales), which may influence related non-cognitive factors of educational attainment (Breinholt & Jaeger, 2020; Mendez & Zamarro, 2018).
Findings in Middle Childhood and Early Adolescence Largely Mirror Findings in Adult Samples
The majority of the current findings in this early adolescent sample mirror findings in Demange et al., 2021 as well as other majority adult samples (Mitchell et al., 2022). Similar to Demange et al., the cognitive PGS was associated with higher fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and a total combined score, while the non-cognitive PGS was only associated with higher crystallized and total scores. Additionally, our findings that both the cognitive and non-cognitive PGSs were associated with measures related to socioeconomic status (Carter et al., 2019) are congruent with findings from Demange et al., and others (Carter et al., 2019). Further, our results for the assessed neuroimaging phenotypes largely mirror those of the Demange et al. study, as well as prior research on academic achievement which has found that larger global brain volumes were positively associated with polygenic scores for educational attainment in a sample of young adults (Demange et al., 2021; Mitchell et al., 2020). Given the congruent findings between previous studies performed in samples of adults (de Zeeuw et al., 2014; Demange et al., 2021) it may be that neural mechanisms associated with academic achievement are developed as early as middle childhood and may be temporally stable until later in life (Lövdén et al., 2020).
However, there were several domains where findings between adolescent and adult samples partly diverge. Notably, Demange et al., 2021 found that their cognitive factor was genetically correlated with lower risk tolerance, whereas we observed a positive association between the cognitive PGS and risk-taking in this sample. These opposite findings may potentially be explained by age differences across the samples: for example, in adolescents, cognitive abilities have previously been linked to greater risk-taking (Mischel et al., 1989). Another potential reason for this divergence is that adolescence is a neurodevelopmental period characterized by delayed cognitive control development (Shulman et al., 2016). As such, individual differences in cognitive control will be more tightly linked to risk-taking in adolescence than in adulthood. Additionally, Demange et al., found negative genetic correlations between the non-cognitive factor and health-risk behaviors, whereas the current study found no significant associations between the non-cognitive PGS and risk tolerance, decision-making, or substance initiation. However, our null findings may be due to a combination of low endorsement of risky behaviors in our younger adolescent sample, as well as limitations of these measures in ABCD (i.e., the ABCD study has fewer assessments of health-risk behaviors). In the domain of psychopathology, we found that both the cognitive and non-cognitive PGSs were negatively associated with PLE severity scores, diverging from Demange et al.’s observed positive relationship between the non-cognitive factor and schizophrenia risk, as well as other studies that have linked greater creativity (Rajagopal et al., 2023) and other aspects of academic success to greater risk for schizophrenia (Karlsson, 2004). The negative associations we observed between the PLE severity scores and both PGSs may suggest that cognitive and non-cognitive factors capture aspects of psychosocial functioning that are protective against development of prodromal psychosis in this adolescent sample (Junghoon et al., 2023).
Strengths and Limitations
Strengths of this study include a relatively large sample size, assessments taken during a developmentally important time period (adolescence), deep phenotyping across multiple psychosocial and neuroimaging measures, and the incorporation of within-family analyses to control for potential confounding factors. However, this manuscript is not without limitations. First, this is a cross-sectional study in a European ancestry subsample of individuals whose caregivers volunteered to participate in the research study (Schoeler et al., 2023). This limits study generalizability, which may be especially important for better understanding potential relationships between facets of academic achievement and factors related to health and well being. Second, the outcome measures studied were constrained by what was available in the ABCD Study, and therefore do not include all potential measures of interest (e.g., personality traits that are considered to be important to non-cognitive aspects of educational attainment and success; Humphries & Kosse, 2017; Malanchini et al., 2023; Tucker-Drob et al., 2016; von Stumm et al., 2011). Relatedly, we were unable to assess all of the same imaging modalities that prior papers have reported on, including mode of anisotropy. Third, it is still unclear what exactly is represented by the “non-cognitive” factor GWAS used to create polygenic scores for the current study, and whether this non-cognitive factor would differ if the input GWASs were derived from samples of different ages (e.g., childhood, adolescence, older age). The authors of the original study (Demange et al., 2021) posit that preferences of risk and decision-making, personality traits, and socially desirable behaviors contribute to the non-cognitive factor of educational attainment captured by their study, as evidenced by patterns of genetic correlations with relevant phenotypes. However, as the authors note, the original GWAS of cognitive performance (Lee et al., 2018) may not have captured all relevant aspects of cognitive ability across the lifespan, and thus the separation of “cognitive” vs. “non-cognitive” may be incomplete. Future studies should focus on explicating the nature of the non-cognitive factors and traits that contribute to academic achievement.
Conclusion
Overall, the results of this study provide evidence that as early as adolescence, polygenic scores for cognitive and non-cognitive facets of educational attainment share both overlapping and unique associations with psychosocial outcomes and neuroimaging measures. We speculate that the majority of these PGS associations are stable across adolescence and adulthood; however, further studies are needed before such a conclusion can be made. As the participants of the ABCD Study continue to age, this will be an invaluable sample in which to characterize the degree to which genetic and environmental effects on academic achievement, psychopathology, health behaviors, and neural phenotypes change across development into adulthood.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
We are thankful to families who have participated in the ABCD Study as well as study staff and investigators.
Funding.
This study was funded by R01DA054750 (RB, AA). ECJ was supported by K01DA051759. AJG was supported by NSF DGE-213989. NRK was supported by K23MH12179201. ASH was supported by K01AA030083. DAAB was supported by K99AA030808. APM was supported by T32DA015035 SEP was supported by F31AA029934. DAB was supported by R21AA027827 Data for this study were provided by the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, which was funded by the National Institutes of Health (grants U01DA041022, U01DA041025, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041093, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147) and additional federal partners (https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html).
Footnotes
COI. The authors report no conflicts of interest or competing interests.
Ethics Approval. Working with ABCD NDA data was approved by the Washington University in St. Louis Institutional Review Board: IRB ID#201708123.
Code availability. https://github.com/WashU-BG/ABCD_cog_non_cog_2023
Data availability.
All ABCD data used in this study are available through the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive (NDA), which may be accessed here (https://nda.nih.gov).
References
- 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, Auton A., Brooks L. D., Durbin R. M., Garrison E. P., Kang H. M., Korbel J. O., Marchini J. L., McCarthy S., McVean G. A., & Abecasis G. R. (2015). A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature, 526(7571), 68–74. 10.1038/nature15393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin P. C. (2010). Estimating Multilevel Logistic Regression Models When the Number of Clusters is Low: A Comparison of Different Statistical Software Procedures. The International Journal of Biostatistics, 6(1), 16. 10.2202/1557-4679.1195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D. M., Albaugh M. D., Avenevoli S., Chang L., Clark D. B., Glantz M. D., Hudziak J. J., Jernigan T. L., Tapert S. F., Yurgelun-Todd D., Alia-Klein N., Potter A. S., Paulus M. P., Prouty D., Zucker R. A., & Sher K. J. (2018). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 55–66. 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basser P. J., Mattiello J., & Lebihan D. (1994). Estimation of the Effective Self-Diffusion Tensor from the NMR Spin Echo. Journal of Magnetic Resonance, Series B, 103(3), 247–254. 10.1006/jmrb.1994.1037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D., Maechler M., Bolker [aut B., cre, Walker S., Christensen R. H. B., Singmann H., Dai B., Scheipl F., Grothendieck G., Green P., Fox J., Bauer A., & simulate.formula) P. N. K.. (2022). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using “Eigen” and S4 (1.1–31) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
- Benjamini Y., & Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Breinholt A., & Jaeger M. M. (2020). How does cultural capital affect educational performance: Signals or skills? The British Journal of Sociology, 71(1), 28–46. 10.1111/1468-4446.12711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brumpton B., Sanderson E., Heilbron K., Hartwig F. P., Harrison S., Vie G. Å., Cho Y., Howe L. D., Hughes A., Boomsma D. I., Havdahl A., Hopper J., Neale M., Nivard M. G., Pedersen N. L., Reynolds C. A., Tucker-Drob E. M., Grotzinger A., Howe L., … Davies N. M. (2020). Avoiding dynastic, assortative mating, and population stratification biases in Mendelian randomization through within-family analyses. Nature Communications, 11(1), Article 1. 10.1038/s41467-020-17117-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter J. L., Richards M., Hotopf M., & Hatch S. L. (2019). The roles of non-cognitive and cognitive skills in the life course development of adult health inequalities. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 232, 190–198. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.04.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey B. J., Cannonier T., Conley M. I., Cohen A. O., Barch D. M., Heitzeg M. M., Soules M. E., Teslovich T., Dellarco D. V., Garavan H., Orr C. A., Wager T. D., Banich M. T., Speer N. K., Sutherland M. T., Riedel M. C., Dick A. S., Bjork J. M., Thomas K. M., … ABCD Imaging Acquisition Workgroup. (2018). The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 43–54. 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamorro-Premuzic T., & Furnham A. (2003). Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(4), 319–338. 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00578-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davies J., Sullivan S., & Zammit S. (2018). Adverse life outcomes associated with adolescent psychotic experiences and depressive symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 53(5), 497–507. 10.1007/s00127-018-1496-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Zeeuw E. L., van Beijsterveldt C. E. M., Glasner T. J., Bartels M., Ehli E. A., Davies G. E., Hudziak J. J., Consortium S. S. G. A., Rietveld C. A., Groen-Blokhuis M. M., Hottenga J. J., de Geus E. J. C., & Boomsma D. I. (2014). Polygenic scores associated with educational attainment in adults predict educational achievement and ADHD symptoms in children. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 165(6), 510–520. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demange P. A., Boomsma D. I., van Bergen E., & Nivard M. G. (2023). Evaluating the causal relationship between educational attainment and mental health. medRxiv: The Preprint Server for Health Sciences, 2023.01.26.23285029. 10.1101/2023.01.26.23285029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Demange P. A., Malanchini M., Mallard T. T., Biroli P., Cox S. R., Grotzinger A. D., Tucker-Drob E. M., Abdellaoui A., Arseneault L., van Bergen E., Boomsma D. I., Caspi A., Corcoran D. L., Domingue B. W., Harris K. M., Ip H. F., Mitchell C., Moffitt T. E., Poulton R., … Nivard M. G. (2021). Investigating the genetic architecture of noncognitive skills using GWAS-by-subtraction. Nature Genetics, 53(1), Article 1. 10.1038/s41588-020-00754-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan R. S., Ségonne F., Fischl B., Quinn B. T., Dickerson B. C., Blacker D., Buckner R. L., Dale A. M., Maguire R. P., Hyman B. T., Albert M. S., & Killiany R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968–980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duckworth A. L., Peterson C., Matthews M. D., & Kelly D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1087–1101. 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duckworth A. L., Quirk A., Gallop R., Hoyle R. H., Kelly D. R., & Matthews M. D. (2019). Cognitive and noncognitive predictors of success. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(47), 23499–23504. 10.1073/pnas.1910510116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott M. L., Knodt A. R., Ireland D., Morris M. L., Poulton R., Ramrakha S., Sison M. L., Moffitt T. E., Caspi A., & Hariri A. R. (2020). What Is the Test-Retest Reliability of Common Task-Functional MRI Measures? New Empirical Evidence and a Meta-Analysis. Psychological Science, 31(7), 792–806. 10.1177/0956797620916786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan C. C., Marshall A., Smolker H., Gonzalez M. R., Tapert S. F., Barch D. M., Sowell E., Dowling G. J., Cardenas-Iniguez C., Ross J., Thompson W. K., & Herting M. M. (2021). Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study Linked External Data (LED): Protocol and practices for geocoding and assignment of environmental data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 52, 101030. 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fothergill K. E., Ensminger M. E., Green K. M., Crum R. M., Robertson J., & Juon H.-S. (2008). The Impact of Early School Behavior and Educational Achievement on Adult Drug Use Disorders: A Prospective Study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 92(1–3), 191–199. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge T., Chen C.-Y., Ni Y., Feng Y.-C. A., & Smoller J. W. (2019). Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nature Communications, 10(1), Article 1. 10.1038/s41467-019-09718-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grotzinger A. D., Rhemtulla M., de Vlaming R., Ritchie S. J., Mallard T. T., Hill W. D., Ip H. F., Marioni R. E., McIntosh A. M., Deary I. J., Koellinger P. D., Harden K. P., Nivard M. G., & Tucker-Drob E. M. (2019). Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), Article 5. 10.1038/s41562-019-0566-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutacker N., Kinge J. M., & Olsen J. A. (2023). Inequality in quality-adjusted life expectancy by educational attainment in Norway: An observational study. BMC Public Health, 23(1), 805. 10.1186/s12889-023-15663-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagler D. J., Hatton S., Cornejo M. D., Makowski C., Fair D. A., Dick A. S., Sutherland M. T., Casey B. J., Barch D. M., Harms M. P., Watts R., Bjork J. M., Garavan H. P., Hilmer L., Pung C. J., Sicat C. S., Kuperman J., Bartsch H., Xue F., … Dale A. M. (2019). Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. NeuroImage, 202, 116091. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath A. C., Berg K., Eaves L. J., Solaas M. H., Corey L. A., Sundet J., Magnus P., & Nance W. E. (1985). Education policy and the heritability of educational attainment. Nature, 314(6013), Article 6013. 10.1038/314734a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz T. B., Balbona J. V., Paulich K. N., & Keller M. C. (2023). Evidence of correlations between human partners based on systematic reviews and meta-analyses of 22 traits and UK Biobank analysis of 133 traits. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(9), Article 9. 10.1038/s41562-023-01672-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howe L. J., Nivard M. G., Morris T. T., Hansen A. F., Rasheed H., Cho Y., Chittoor G., Ahlskog R., Lind P. A., Palviainen T., van der Zee M. D., Cheesman R., Mangino M., Wang Y., Li S., Klaric L., Ratliff S. M., Bielak L. F., Nygaard M., … Davies N. M. (2022). Within-sibship genome-wide association analyses decrease bias in estimates of direct genetic effects. Nature Genetics, 54(5), Article 5. 10.1038/s41588-022-01062-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphries J. E., & Kosse F. (2017). On the interpretation of non-cognitive skills – What is being measured and why it matters. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 136, 174–185. 10.1016/j.jebo.2017.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Judd N., Sauce B., Wiedenhoeft J., Tromp J., Chaarani B., Schliep A., van Noort B., Penttilä J., Grimmer Y., Insensee C., Becker A., Banaschewski T., Bokde A. L. W., Quinlan E. B., Desrivières S., Flor H., Grigis A., Gowland P., Heinz A., … Klingberg T. (2020). Cognitive and brain development is independently influenced by socioeconomic status and polygenic scores for educational attainment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(22), 12411–12418. 10.1073/pnas.2001228117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Junghoon P., Eunji L., Gyeongcheol C., Heungsun H., Bogyeom K., Gakyung K., Yoonie J. Y., & Jiook C. (2023). Gene-Environment Pathways to Cognitive Development and Psychotic-Like Experiences in Children. eLife, 12. 10.7554/eLife.88117 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Karcher N. R., & Barch D. M. (2021). The ABCD study: Understanding the development of risk for mental and physical health outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 131–142. 10.1038/s41386-020-0736-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson J. L. (2004). Psychosis and academic performance. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 184, 327–329. 10.1192/bjp.184.4.327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J., Birmaher B., Brent D., Rao U., Flynn C., Moreci P., Williamson D., & Ryan N. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980–988. 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler R. J., Lichenstein S. D., & Yip S. W. (2022). Hyperbolic discounting rates and risk for problematic alcohol use in youth enrolled in the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study. Addiction Biology, 27(2), e13160. 10.1111/adb.13160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovas Y., Garon-Carrier G., Boivin M., Petrill S. A., Plomin R., Malykh S. B., Spinath F., Murayama K., Ando J., Bogdanova O. Y., Brendgen M., Dionne G., Forget-Dubois N., Galajinsky E. V., Gottschling J., Guay F., Lemelin J.-P., Logan J. A. R., Yamagata S., … Vitaro F. (2015). Why children differ in motivation to learn: Insights from over 13,000 twins from 6 countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 51–63. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lam M., Awasthi S., Watson H. J., Goldstein J., Panagiotaropoulou G., Trubetskoy V., Karlsson R., Frei O., Fan C.-C., De Witte W., Mota N. R., Mullins N., Brügger K., Lee S. H., Wray N. R., Skarabis N., Huang H., Neale B., Daly M. J., … Ripke S. (2020). RICOPILI: Rapid Imputation for COnsortias PIpeLIne. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England), 36(3), 930–933. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btz633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J. J., Wedow R., Okbay A., Kong E., Maghzian O., Zacher M., Nguyen-Viet T. A., Bowers P., Sidorenko J., Karlsson Linnér R., Fontana M. A., Kundu T., Lee C., Li H., Li R., Royer R., Timshel P. N., Walters R. K., Willoughby E. A., … Cesarini D. (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50(8), Article 8. 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loe I. M., Adams J. N., & Feldman H. M. (2019). Executive Function in Relation to White Matter in Preterm and Full Term Children. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 6, 418. 10.3389/fped.2018.00418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loughnan R. J., Palmer C. E., Thompson W. K., Dale A. M., Jernigan T. L., & Chieh Fan C. (2023). Intelligence Polygenic Score Is More Predictive of Crystallized Measures: Evidence From the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Psychological Science, 34(6), 714–725. 10.1177/09567976231160702 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lövdén M., Fratiglioni L., Glymour M. M., Lindenberger U., & Tucker-Drob E. M. (2020). Education and Cognitive Functioning Across the Life Span. Psychological Science in the Public Interest: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 21(1), 6–41. 10.1177/1529100620920576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciana M., Bjork J. M., Nagel B. J., Barch D. M., Gonzalez R., Nixon S. J., & Banich M. T. (2018). Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 67–79. 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey A. P., Finn A. S., Leonard J. A., Jacoby Senghor D. S., West M. R., Gabrieli C. F. O., & Gabrieli J. D. E. (2015). Neuroanatomical Correlates of the Income Achievement Gap. Psychological Science, 26(6), 925–933. 10.1177/0956797615572233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malanchini M., Allegrini A. G., Nivard M. G., Biroli P., Rimfeld K., Cheesman R., Stumm S. von, Demange P. A., Bergen E. van, Grotzinger A. D., Raffington L., Fuente J. D. la, Pingault J.-B., Harden K. P., Tucker-Drob E. M., & Plomin R. (2023). Genetic contributions of noncognitive skills to academic development (p. 2023.04.03.535380). bioRxiv. 10.1101/2023.04.03.535380 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Malanchini M., Engelhardt L. E., Grotzinger A. D., Harden K. P., & Tucker-Drob E. M. (2019). “Same but different”: Associations between multiple aspects of self-regulation, cognition, and academic abilities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(6), 1164–1188. 10.1037/pspp0000224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin A. R., Kanai M., Kamatani Y., Okada Y., Neale B. M., & Daly M. J. (2019). Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nature Genetics, 51(4), Article 4. 10.1038/s41588-019-0379-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendez I., & Zamarro G. (2018). The intergenerational transmission of noncognitive skills and their effect on education and employment outcomes. Journal of Population Economics, 31(2), 521–560. [Google Scholar]
- Miller A. P., Baranger D. A. A., Paul S. E., Hatoum A. S., Rogers C., Bogdan R., & Agrawal A. (2023). Characteristics Associated With Cannabis Use Initiation by Late Childhood and Early Adolescence in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. JAMA Pediatrics. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2023.1801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mischel W., Shoda Y., & Rodriguez M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science (New York, N.Y.), 244(4907), 933–938. 10.1126/science.2658056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell B. L., Cuéllar-Partida G., Grasby K. L., Campos A. I., Strike L. T., Hwang L.-D., Okbay A., Thompson P. M., Medland S. E., Martin N. G., Wright M. J., & Rentería M. E. (2020). Educational attainment polygenic scores are associated with cortical total surface area and regions important for language and memory. NeuroImage, 212, 116691. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116691 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell B. L., Hansell N. K., McAloney K., Martin N. G., Wright M. J., Renteria M. E., & Grasby K. L. (2022). Polygenic influences associated with adolescent cognitive skills. Intelligence, 94, 101680. 10.1016/j.intell.2022.101680 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt T. E., Arseneault L., Belsky D., Dickson N., Hancox R. J., Harrington H., Houts R., Poulton R., Roberts B. W., Ross S., Sears M. R., Thomson W. M., & Caspi A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698. 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morin A. J. S., Rodriguez D., Fallu J.-S., Maïano C., & Janosz M. (2012). Academic achievement and smoking initiation in adolescence: A general growth mixture analysis. Addiction, 107(4), 819–828. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03725.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Najman J. M., Aird R., Bor W., O’Callaghan M., Williams G. M., & Shuttlewood G. J. (2004). The generational transmission of socioeconomic inequalities in child cognitive development and emotional health. Social Science & Medicine, 58(6), 1147–1158. 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00286-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noftle E. E., & Robins R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 116–130. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okbay A., Wu Y., Wang N., Jayashankar H., Bennett M., Nehzati S. M., Sidorenko J., Kweon H., Goldman G., Gjorgjieva T., Jiang Y., Hicks B., Tian C., Hinds D. A., Ahlskog R., Magnusson P. K. E., Oskarsson S., Hayward C., Campbell A., … Young A. I. (2022). Polygenic prediction of educational attainment within and between families from genome-wide association analyses in 3 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 54(4), Article 4. 10.1038/s41588-022-01016-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raghupathi V., & Raghupathi W. (2020). The influence of education on health: An empirical assessment of OECD countries for the period 1995–2015. Archives of Public Health, 78(1), 20. 10.1186/s13690-020-00402-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajagopal V. M., Ganna A., Coleman J. R. I., Allegrini A., Voloudakis G., Grove J., Als T. D., Horsdal H. T., Petersen L., Appadurai V., Schork A., Buil A., Bulik C. M., Bybjerg-Grauholm J., Bækvad-Hansen M., Hougaard D. M., Mors O., Nordentoft M., Werge T., … Demontis D. (2023). Genome-wide association study of school grades identifies genetic overlap between language ability, psychopathology and creativity. Scientific Reports, 13(1), Article 1. 10.1038/s41598-022-26845-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeler T., Speed D., Porcu E., Pirastu N., Pingault J.-B., & Kutalik Z. (2023). Participation bias in the UK Biobank distorts genetic associations and downstream analyses. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(7), Article 7. 10.1038/s41562-023-01579-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selzam S., Ritchie S. J., Pingault J.-B., Reynolds C. A., O’Reilly P. F., & Plomin R. (2019). Comparing Within- and Between-Family Polygenic Score Prediction. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 105(2), 351–363. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2019.06.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheffield J. M., Karcher N. R., & Barch D. M. (2018). Cognitive Deficits in Psychotic Disorders: A Lifespan Perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 28(4), 509–533. 10.1007/s11065-018-9388-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shipp S. (2017). The functional logic of corticostriatal connections. Brain Structure and Function, 222(2), 669–706. 10.1007/s00429-016-1250-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman E. P., Smith A. R., Silva K., Icenogle G., Duell N., Chein J., & Steinberg L. (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103–117. 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silventoinen K., Jelenkovic A., Sund R., Latvala A., Honda C., Inui F., Tomizawa R., Watanabe M., Sakai N., Rebato E., Busjahn A., Tyler J., Hopper J. L., Ordoñana J. R., Sánchez-Romera J. F., Colodro-Conde L., Calais-Ferreira L., Oliveira V. C., Ferreira P. H., … Kaprio J. (2020). Genetic and environmental variation in educational attainment: An individual-based analysis of 28 twin cohorts. Scientific Reports, 10(1), Article 1. 10.1038/s41598-020-69526-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder W., Uddin L. Q., & Nomi J. S. (2021). Dynamic functional connectivity profile of the salience network across the life span. Human Brain Mapping, 42(14), 4740–4749. 10.1002/hbm.25581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taliun D., Harris D. N., Kessler M. D., Carlson J., Szpiech Z. A., Torres R., Taliun S. A. G., Corvelo A., Gogarten S. M., Kang H. M., Pitsillides A. N., LeFaive J., Lee S., Tian X., Browning B. L., Das S., Emde A.-K., Clarke W. E., Loesch D. P., … Abecasis G. R. (2021). Sequencing of 53,831 diverse genomes from the NHLBI TOPMed Program. Nature, 590(7845), Article 7845. 10.1038/s41586-021-03205-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- THE BRAINSTORM CONSORTIUM, Anttila V., Bulik-Sullivan B., Finucane H. K., Walters R. K., Bras J., Duncan L., Escott-Price V., Falcone G. J., Gormley P., Malik R., Patsopoulos N. A., Ripke S., Wei Z., Yu D., Lee P. H., Turley P., Grenier-Boley B., Chouraki V., … Neale B. M. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science, 360(6395), eaap8757. 10.1126/science.aap8757 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker-Drob E. M., Briley D. A., Engelhardt L. E., Mann F. D., & Harden K. P. (2016). Genetically-mediated associations between measures of childhood character and academic achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), 790–815. 10.1037/pspp0000098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker-Drob E. M., & Harden K. P. (2012). Learning Motivation Mediates Gene-by-Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Mathematics Achievement in Early Childhood. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 37–45. 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa F. M., Crippa A., Rosi E., Nobile M., Brambilla P., & Delvecchio G. (2023). ADHD and eating disorders in childhood and adolescence: An updated minireview. Journal of Affective Disorders, 321, 265–271. 10.1016/j.jad.2022.10.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N. D., Koob G. F., Croyle R. T., Bianchi D. W., Gordon J. A., Koroshetz W. J., Pérez-Stable E. J., Riley W. T., Bloch M. H., Conway K., Deeds B. G., Dowling G. J., Grant S., Howlett K. D., Matochik J. A., Morgan G. D., Murray M. M., Noronha A., Spong C. Y., … Weiss S. R. B. (2018). The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 4–7. 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Stumm S., Hell B., & Chamorro-Premuzic T. (2011). The Hungry Mind: Intellectual Curiosity Is the Third Pillar of Academic Performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 6(6), 574–588. 10.1177/1745691611421204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson H. J., Yilmaz Z., Thornton L. M., Hübel C., Coleman J. R. I., Gaspar H. A., Bryois J., Hinney A., Leppä V. M., Mattheisen M., Medland S. E., Ripke S., Yao S., Giusti-Rodríguez P., Hanscombe K. B., Purves K. L., Adan R. A. H., Alfredsson L., Ando T., … Bulik C. M. (2019). Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nature Genetics, 51(8), Article 8. 10.1038/s41588-019-0439-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts A. L., Smith G. T., Barch D. M., & Sher K. J. (2020). Factor structure, measurement and structural invariance, and external validity of an abbreviated youth version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Psychological Assessment, 32(4), 336–347. 10.1037/pas0000791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wulfert E., Block J. A., Santa Ana E., Rodriguez M. L., & Colsman M. (2002). Delay of Gratification: Impulsive Choices and Problem Behaviors in Early and Late Adolescence. Journal of Personality, 70(4), 533–552. 10.1111/1467-6494.05013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young A. I., Frigge M. L., Gudbjartsson D. F., Thorleifsson G., Bjornsdottir G., Sulem P., Masson G., Thorsteinsdottir U., Stefansson K., & Kong A. (2018). Relatedness disequilibrium regression estimates heritability without environmental bias. Nature Genetics, 50(9), Article 9. 10.1038/s41588-018-0178-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zajacova A., & Lawrence E. M. (2018). The relationship between education and health: Reducing disparities through a contextual approach. Annual Review of Public Health, 39, 273–289. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044628 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All ABCD data used in this study are available through the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive (NDA), which may be accessed here (https://nda.nih.gov).