Abstract
Background
To examine the relationship between social camouflage and mental health in Japanese autistic adults and make an international comparison with a sample from the UK.
Methods
This study analysed secondary data of participants with a self-reported diagnosis of autism from Japan (N = 210; 123 men and 87 women) and the UK (N = 305; 181 women, 104, men, and 18 nonbinary). The relationships between the quadratic term of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire and mental health scales, including depression and anxiety, were assessed.
Results
The UK sample showed linear relationships, whereas the Japanese sample showed significant nonlinear relationships. The quadratic terms of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire slightly explained generalised anxiety (β = .168, p = .007), depression (β = .121, p = .045), and well-being (β = − .127, p = .028). However, they did not explain the association between social anxiety and the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire.
Limitations
Participants had self-reported diagnoses, and while the autism-spectrum quotient provides a cut-off value for screening, it does not enable confirming diagnoses. Mean scores of the Japanese version of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire were lower as compared to the original CAT-Q, which implies that the social camouflage strategy types used by autistic people in Japan and the UK could differ. The cross-sectional design limits causal inferences.
Conclusion
In the UK, more social camouflage was associated with poorer mental health scores, whereas too little or too much social camouflage was associated with a low mental health score in Japan. The Japanese population is seemingly less aware of and educated on autistic characteristics and considers ‘average’ behaviour a good thing. This could influence Japanese autistic people’s social camouflage use, differing from that of autistic people in the UK. The differences in the relationship between social camouflage and mental health between Japan and the UK could be associated with national-level divergence regarding the culture of autism.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13229-023-00579-w.
Keywords: Autistic adults, Social camouflage, Cross-cultural study, Mental health, Japan, UK
Background
Autistic people tend to show differences in social communication and interaction; they tend to express a focus on specific interests and are sensitive to sensory stimulation [1]. Although autism can be identified in infancy, it can also be diagnosed across people’s lifespan, in turn helping them identify their specific strengths and needs. Importantly, many autistic people are not diagnosed until adulthood [2, 3]. This study uses the terms autistic people and autism, not autism spectrum disorder, to avoid ableist language [4].
As autistic individuals grow into adults, mental health issues account for a more significant percentage of the symptoms that they experience than the core symptoms of autism [5–7]. Researchers have reported that 79% of autistic adults have mental health problems, such as depression, anxiety, social anxiety, and obsessive–compulsive symptoms [8, 9]. In addition to these psychiatric disorders, almost all medical conditions were significantly more common in autistic adults, including immune disorders, gastrointestinal disorders, sleep disorders, seizures, obesity, dyslipidaemia, hypertension, and diabetes [10]. Although autism is not a mental health problem, autistic people could experience functional impairments resulting from autistic traits and the aforementioned mental health conditions, which together could lead to an increased risk of withdrawal from employment, suicide, self-harm, and low quality of life [11–14]. Therefore, identifying the link between autism and mental health is crucial to promoting mental health prevention in this population.
Although data on the mental health of autistic people in Japan are limited, autistic children who receive support in early childhood (age 10 years or younger) have better mental health outcomes in adulthood than those who do not receive support [15]. A 20 year follow-up study of individuals diagnosed with autism in early childhood found that approximately 40% of them had a good quality of life later on in life [16]. These pieces of evidence indicate that mental health maintenance in autistic individuals is associated with support from an early age.
Social camouflage refers to the efforts by autistic people to consciously or unconsciously modify their behaviour to fit in with non-autistic people [2]. It also refers to strategies that they use, either consciously or unconsciously, to conceal their autistic characteristics and compensate for the social difficulties associated with autism [7]. As many as 70% of autistic individuals are aware that they engage in social camouflage and ‘have to’ do so [17, 18]. Social camouflage is described as having the three elements of ‘compensation’ (i.e. strategies to overcome social difficulties associated with autistic traits), ‘masking’ (i.e. strategies to hide one’s autistic traits), and ‘assimilation’ (i.e. strategies to blend in with others in social situations [19]). Many autistic individuals engage in social camouflage and attempt to use social interaction to obtain job opportunities and other benefits. The aforementioned ‘need’ of autistic individuals to engage in social camouflage forces them to continuously pretend that they are non-autistic. This is associated with significant manifestations of mental health deterioration, such as depression, generalised anxiety, social anxiety [20, 21], suicide attempts, and burnout because of exhaustion and fatigue [22].
Although one of the reasons autistic individuals are motivated to engage in social camouflage is a desire to fit in with society, some believe they engage in such behaviours as a response to social stigma. Stigma can be found in various forms in society, and one’s perception of the social stigma of autism is a significant sociopsychological factor contributing to mental health deterioration in autistic adults [23]. Autistic teenagers feel ashamed if they perceive that they are bothering others, which reduces their willingness to achieve reasonable adaptation [22]. Social stigma is more prevalent in Asian than in Western countries. For example, some researchers have reported greater knowledge, familiarity, and acceptance of autism in the UK than in Malaysia [24]; some have also reported that South Koreans showed more stigma toward autism than Americans [25]. Furthermore, while 38% of the Chinese participants of a study endorsed stigma toward autism, only 14% of the American participants did so [26], and American students showed less stigma toward autism than Lebanese students [27]. In Japan, Japanese students showed more stigma toward autism than American students [28]. Markus and Kitayama [29] refer to Japanese and other East Asian cultures as ‘cultures of interdependence’. In these cultures, the primary challenge faced by individuals is to conform without standing out and pay more attention to others than oneself. Thus, the ‘uniqueness’ of autistic people can be perceived negatively, and it can threaten relationships and interpersonal harmony within the community. Japan has an interdependent culture, and researchers have indeed described that being ‘unique’ in Japan is often seen as a ‘threat’ [28]. These delineations led us to consider the possibility that the culture of interdependence could force Japanese autistic individuals to over-adapt or camouflage to non-autistic groups.
As aforementioned, one of the key motivations for autistic people to engage in social camouflage is their desire to fit in with other people, make a good impression, make friends, and get to know others better [2, 30, 31]. Another motivation that autistic adults identified for engaging in social camouflage was to avoid adverse reactions from other people for looking autistic [2, 30, 31]. These motivations are similar to non-autistic individuals’ for using impression-management strategies. However, autistic people might require more effort to use these strategies, use them in more situations, and use them for a longer period than non-autistic individuals [19]. Another study showed that the perception of stigma toward autism promotes social camouflage among autistic individuals [32]. In 2022, academicians investigated the relationship between the perception of stigma toward autism and social camouflage in Japan [33]. Their results were similar to those of another research [32] finding that autistic individuals with a higher social stigma perception engaged more in social camouflage. Therefore, autistic people’s perceptions of social stigma toward autism contribute to their greater engagement in social camouflage in both the UK and Japan.
If autistic people use social camouflage as a strategy for dealing with the stigma toward autism, it could be reasonable to expect that a reduction in social stigma leads to a lower need to engage in social camouflage. Cage et al. [34] also noted that feeling accepted by others as an autistic person could be a protective factor against depression [34]. However, researchers investigating social camouflage and mental health have mostly referred to the outcomes of this association of autistic people based only on Western culture. Reducing social stigma against autism reduces the social camouflage of autistic individuals, and the amount of social camouflage that autistic individuals engage in could vary by country because the social stigma of autism varies by culture [27, 28, 35]. Given that the social stigma experienced by autistic people varies from culture to culture, the difference between social camouflage and mental health problems is also expected to differ from culture to culture. Still, to date, there is no identification.
In Japan, as noted above, few studies have examined the association between autistic adults and mental health. Among the few examples, the studies by Kamio et al. [15] and Iwasa [16] examined the quality of life of Japanese autistic adults but not their mental health. Moreover, to our knowledge, the influence of social camouflage on mental health deterioration among autistic people in Asian countries, such as Japan, has yet to be explored. The situation of Japanese autistic adults, whose psychosocial quality of life tends to be lower than that of the general Japanese adult population [15], is similar to that of autistic adults in Western Europe. However, past studies conducted with Japanese autistic adults have focused mostly on the link between poorer mental health and backward-looking factors (e.g. IQ, the timing of diagnosis, and maternal support during childhood [15, 16]), and none thus far has looked at the association between mental health and social camouflage in Japanese autistic individuals. Since different cultural backgrounds can influence the relationship of these variables and the aforementioned studies corroborate this possibility, there could be differences between Eastern and Western societies for the association of mental health and social camouflage among autistic individuals. Therefore, we considered it worthwhile to make an international comparison of social camouflage and its influence on various psychosocial factors of autistic people.
Study aims
This study is a replication and re-analysis of the study by Hull et al. [20], which investigated autistic adults in the UK; it aims to clarify the characteristics of Japanese autistic people regarding social camouflage and mental health, as well as compare them with those of autistic people in the UK. In Hull et al. [20], the scholars observed a linear correlation between social camouflage and mental health issues (i.e. depression, social anxiety, and general anxiety) in autistic people. However, we hypothesised that too much or too little engagement in social camouflage would negatively affect the mental health of autistic people in Japan. Given this cultural context and the tendency toward negative reactions to the ‘differences’ of autism, we hypothesised that the relationship between social camouflage and mental health is quadratic. Hull et al. [20] did not consider the possibility that either too much or too little engagement in social camouflage could negatively affect mental health. Accordingly, while they did consider a monotonically increasing quadratic relationship, we not only used the methodology present in Hull et al.’s [20] investigation but also probed into whether the following posit is true: the longer the distance between the score and the average, the poorer the mental health. That is, we checked for the existence of a quadratic curve for the analysed association. The current research also analysed the relationship between sex and mental health in Japanese and British autistic adults.
Methods
Participants
This study conducted secondary analyses of the data from a past study conducted to standardise the Japanese version of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q; Hongo et al. [36]). To compare data from Japanese and UK samples, we used the dataset of Hull et al. [20] study, with a sample of adults with a self-reported diagnosis of autism in the UK (N = 305, aged 18–75 years), regarding scores for the CAT-Q and other mental health questionnaires. We requested the authors for access to their dataset and approval for its use in research, which they approved. In Hongo et al.’s [36] study, which was the main subject of our investigation, participants were 210 Japanese autistic men and women. Although Hongo et al. [36] excluded data from six participants who consistently gave the same response for scales (i.e. straight liners) that included reversed items, we used data from all participants following the procedures in Hull et al. [20] because responses from straight liners cannot necessarily be regarded as invalid responses [37]. The results of the analyses while excluding straight liners are shown in Additional file 1.
The descriptive statistics of participants in Japan are presented in Table 1. The questionnaire in Hull et al. [20] study allowed participants to respond that they were of nonbinary gender, whereas the data of Hongo et al. [36] study only contained two options for sex: men and women. Of the participants in Hull et al. [20] study, 104 (34.1%) were men, 181 (59.3%) were women, and 18 (5.9%) were nonbinary. Their age ranged from 18 to 75 years (M = 41.90 years, standard deviation [SD] = 13.60), and the mean age at diagnosis was 34.89 years (SD = 14.44). Of the participants in Hongo et al. [36] study, 123 (58.6%) were men, and 87 (41.4%) were women. Their age ranged from 20 to 64 years (M = 37.53 years, standard deviation [SD] = 10.33), and the mean age at diagnosis was 28.70 years (SD = 12.16). Furthermore, 114 (55.9%) were employed full- or part-time, 21 (10.3%) were students, 11 (5.39%) were full-time homemakers, and 58 (28.4%) were unemployed or unable to work.
Table 1.
Total sample(N = 210) | Female subsample (n = 87) | Male subsample (n = 123) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | n | Mean | SD | n | Mean | SD | n | |
Age (Years) | 37.61 | 10.31 | 210 | 38.86 | 10.31 | 87 | 35.84 | 10.05 | 123 |
Diagnosis | |||||||||
Autism spectrum disorder (%) | 131 | (62%) | 73 | (59%) | 58 | (67%) | |||
Developmental disability (%) | 55 | (26%) | 34 | (28%) | 21 | (24%) | |||
Pervasive developmental disorders (%) | 74 | (35%) | 41 | (33%) | 33 | (38%) | |||
Autistic disorder (%) | 38 | (18%) | 27 | (22%) | 11 | (13%) | |||
Asperger disorder (%) | 73 | (35%) | 41 | (33%) | 32 | (37%) | |||
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (%) | 58 | (28%) | 34 | (28%) | 24 | (28%) | |||
Schizophrenia (%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | |||
Dementia (%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | |||
Intellectual disability (%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | 0 | (0%) | |||
Autistic traits (BAPQ) | 4.00 | .70 | 205 | 3.93 | .67 | 85 | 4.10 | .73 | 120 |
Social camouflage (CAT-Q) | 105.29 | 21.94 | 209 | 101.20 | 20.48 | 86 | 111.14 | 22.61 | 123 |
Social anxiety (LSAS) | 72.77 | 36.87 | 200 | 69.89 | 35.19 | 83 | 76.82 | 38.76 | 117 |
Generalised anxiety (GAD-7) | 9.03 | 6.17 | 208 | 7.87 | 5.66 | 85 | 10.71 | 6.49 | 123 |
Depression (PHQ-9) | 11.93 | 7.26 | 209 | 10.40 | 6.91 | 87 | 14.08 | 7.19 | 122 |
Well-being (WEMWBS) | 36.25 | 10.22 | 209 | 37.28 | 9.77 | 87 | 34.80 | 10.65 | 123 |
BAPQ Broad autism phenotype questionnaire; CAT-Q Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire; GAD-7 Generalized anxiety disorder assessment-7; LSAS Liebowitz social anxiety scale; PHQ-9 Patient health questionnaire-9; WEMWBS Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale
The inclusion criteria were being at or above the legal age to provide informed consent on own behalf in Japan (20 years of age) and being officially diagnosed with autism: ‘autism spectrum disorder’, ‘autism’, ‘Asperger’s syndrome/disorder’, or ‘pervasive developmental disorder’, and not otherwise specified. Those who reported being self-diagnosed or diagnosed with only ‘autistic traits’ were excluded from this study.
Hongo et al. [36] recruited participants and patients attending medical institutions between February 2020 and April 2021 through online survey panels. As an honorarium, participants received 1000 yen (approximately USD 7). The Research Ethics Committee of Chiba University Research Ethics Committee (reference M10053) approved this study, and written and oral informed consent was obtained from all participants before the study onset.
Materials and procedures
Since this study was a secondary analysis of the data of Hongo et al. [36], readers are referred to this cited study for more details on participants’ recruitment and inclusion criteria, among other methodological topics.
The Japanese version of the camouflaging autistic traits questionnaire
The questionnaire included the Japanese version of the CAT-Q (CAT-Q-J [36]). The CAT-Q comprises 25 items across three subscales [19], as follows: compensation (nine items), masking (eight items), and assimilation (eight items). Each item is rated on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). A higher total score for the CAT-Q scale indicated more severe social camouflaging. In total, 402 participants (200 autistic and 202 non-autistic) completed the CAT-Q. The CAT-Q has good sensitivity and specificity [19], and its internal consistency in Hongo et al. [36] was good (total sample, Cronbach’s α = 0.88). Although the CAT-Q has three subscales, to ensure compatibility with the methods used by Hull et al. [19], this study used only the total score.
The Japanese version of the liebowitz social anxiety scale
The questionnaire contained the Japanese version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS-J), which is based on the LSAS [38]. This self-report scale comprises 48 items that measure social anxiety and two subscales with 24 items each: social fear and social avoidance. Each scale was assessed using a four-point Likert scale, as follows: social fear was rated from 0 (no feeling at all) to 3 (powerful feeling), and social avoidance was rated from 0 (never) to 3 (two-thirds or 100% of avoidance probability). Thus, the total score ranged from 0 to 144. The cut-off value for social anxiety was 44 points (sensitivity, 93.3%; specificity, 90.0%; [39]). The LSAS-J has high reliability and validity [38], and its internal consistency in Hongo et al. [36] was excellent (total sample, Cronbach’s α = 0.98).
The Japanese version of the Warwick–Edinburgh mental well-being scale
The questionnaire included the Japanese version of the Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-being scale (WEMWBS-J), which is based on the WEMWBS [40]. This 14-item self-report questionnaire measures general well-being over the last two weeks, with higher scores suggesting more positive mental well-being [41]. An example item is ‘I’ve been feeling relaxed’, and items are rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (none of the time) to 5 (all the time). The WEMWBS has demonstrated acceptable validity and reliability [41]. The internal consistency of the WEMWBS in Hongo et al. [36] was excellent (total sample, Cronbach’s α = 0.91).
The Japanese version of the patient health questionnaire-9
The questionnaire included the Japanese version of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), which is a simplified assessment tool for measuring major depressive disorder [42]. Responses were scored using a four-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day), and total scores ranged from 0 to 27 points. The internal consistency of the PHQ-9 in Hongo et al. [36] was excellent (total sample, Cronbach’s α = 0.90).
The Japanese version of the generalized anxiety disorder-7
The questionnaire contained the Japanese version of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7), which is a simplified version of the GAD [42]. Responses were scored using a four-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day), and total scores ranged from 0 to 21 points. The internal consistency of the GAD-7 in this study (total sample) was excellent (Cronbach’s α = 0.91).
Statistical analysis
Because data were missing for some measures of some participants, multiple imputations were performed to reduce the potential for bias by maximising the usable proportion of the sample. Multiple imputations were performed using the missing values package in SPSS (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA), with estimates from five imputations pooled to produce imputed data. The missing values were supplemented for missing scale (not item) scores, as was done in Hull et al. [20]. Bivariate correlations between all variables were calculated for the total sample (Additional file 1: Table S1). Bivariate correlations between all variables for the sample excluding straight liners are shown in (Additional file 1: Table S2).
Hongo et al. [36] conducted a replication study in Japan of the research by Hull et al. [20], allowing for data comparisons to be made. Specifically, we used hierarchical multiple regression analyses with a quadratic term for social camouflage (CAT-Q2) and an interaction term (camouflage*sex) to predict the mental health variables. Hull et al. [20] did not perform centring (subtracting the mean from individual scores) for the scores for CAT-Q in their study. However, we performed a centring procedure on the Japanese and UK CAT-Q data. Standardisation was performed for each scale because standardisation also serves as a centring procedure. All variables were standardised before being entered into the multiple regression models to show the standardised partial regression coefficients. The significance level was set at 5%.
Results
Is there a quadratic relationship between social camouflage and mental health problems, such as generalised anxiety, depression, and social anxiety that is related to social camouflage in autistic individuals?
The UK data did not show nonlinear relationships, whereas the Japanese data showed significant nonlinear relationships. The results are presented in Table 2, where the quadratic terms of the CAT-Q-J slightly explained generalised anxiety (GAD-7; β = 0.168, p = 0.007), depression (PHQ-9; β = 0.121, p = 0.045), and well-being (WEMWBS-J; β = − 0.127, p = 0.028). However, the quadratic terms did not explain the association between social anxiety (LSAS-J) and the CAT-Q-J. The results of these analyses, excluding straight liners, are presented in Additional file 1: Table S3. Scatter plots (before multiple assignments between the CAT-Q-J and other variables) are shown in Fig. 1 (Scatter plots excluding straight liners are shown in Additional file 1: Fig. S1).
Table 2.
Japan (N = 210) | UK (N = 305) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | β | p | 95% CI | β | p | 95% CI | |||
Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | ||||||
Model 1 | |||||||||
(Generalised anxiety) | |||||||||
Step 1 | |||||||||
Age | 0.053 | 0.403 | − 0.071 | 0.177 | − 0.129 | 0.059 | − 0.245 | − 0.013 | |
BAPQ | 0.423 | < 0.001 | 0.299 | 0.547 | 0.399 | 0.054 | 0.292 | 0.506 | |
Step 2 | |||||||||
Age | 0.066 | 0.321 | − 0.064 | 0.195 | − 0.089 | 0.060 | − 0.210 | 0.032 | |
BAPQ | 0.410 | < 0.001 | 0.281 | 0.539 | 0.347 | 0.056 | 0.237 | 0.457 | |
CAT-Q | 0.050 | 0.475 | − 0.087 | 0.186 | 0.219 | 0.061 | 0.098 | 0.340 | |
Step 3 | |||||||||
Age | 0.072 | 0.267 | − 0.055 | 0.200 | − 0.089 | 0.060 | − 0.210 | 0.032 | |
BAPQ | 0.411 | < 0.001 | 0.284 | 0.539 | 0.346 | 0.056 | 0.236 | 0.457 | |
CAT-Q | 0.068 | 0.325 | − 0.067 | 0.203 | 0.217 | 0.064 | 0.090 | 0.345 | |
CAT-Q2 | 0.168 | 0.007 | 0.045 | 0.290 | − 0.005 | 0.056 | − 0.115 | 0.104 | |
Model 2 | |||||||||
(Depression) | |||||||||
Step 1 | |||||||||
Age | 0.074 | 0.229 | − 0.046 | 0.193 | − 0.151 | 0.058 | − .0266 | − 0.036 | |
BAPQ | 0.488 | < 0.001 | 0.368 | 0.608 | 0.370 | 0.054 | 0.265 | 0.476 | |
Step2 | |||||||||
Age | 0.092 | 0.145 | − 0.032 | 0.216 | − 0.129 | 0.061 | − 0.250 | −0.008 | |
BAPQ | 0.469 | < 0.001 | 0.345 | 0.593 | 0.342 | 0.055 | 0.233 | 0.450 | |
CAT-Q | 0.073 | 0.265 | − 0.055 | 0.201 | 0.122 | 0.062 | − 0.003 | 0.246 | |
Step 3 | |||||||||
Age | 0.097 | 0.122 | − 0.026 | 0.220 | − 0.129 | 0.061 | − .0250 | − 0.008 | |
BAPQ | 0.470 | < 0.001 | 0.347 | 0.593 | 0.341 | 0.056 | 0.232 | 0.450 | |
CAT-Q | 0.086 | 0.187 | − 0.042 | 0.214 | 0.121 | 0.066 | − 0.011 | 0.252 | |
CAT-Q2 | 0.121 | 0.045 | 0.002 | 0.240 | − 0.004 | 0.058 | − 0.118 | 0.110 | |
Model 3 | |||||||||
(Social anxiety) | |||||||||
Step 1 | |||||||||
Age | 0.062 | 0.289 | − 0.053 | 0.177 | − 0.116 | 0.048 | − .210 | − 0.022 | |
BAPQ | 0.592 | < 0.001 | 0.481 | 0.704 | 0.600 | 0.047 | 0.508 | 0.692 | |
Step 2 | |||||||||
Age | 0.075 | 0.228 | − 0.047 | 0.196 | − 0.076 | 0.047 | − 0.168 | 0.017 | |
BAPQ | 0.580 | < 0.001 | 0.463 | 0.697 | 0.547 | 0.047 | 0.454 | 0.640 | |
CAT-Q | 0.048 | 0.437 | − 0.074 | 0.171 | 0.224 | 0.048 | 0.130 | 0.319 | |
Step 3 | |||||||||
Age | 0.077 | 0.211 | − 0.044 | 0.199 | − .075 | 0.047 | − 0.168 | 0.018 | |
BAPQ | 0.580 | < 0.001 | 0.463 | 0.698 | 0.545 | 0.047 | 0.452 | 0.638 | |
CAT-Q | 0.056 | 0.372 | − 0.067 | 0.179 | 0.219 | 0.052 | 0.117 | 0.322 | |
CAT-Q2 | 0.070 | 0.226 | − 0.043 | 0.183 | − 0.017 | 0.054 | − 0.124 | 0.089 | |
Model 4 | |||||||||
(Well-being) | |||||||||
Step 1 | |||||||||
Age | − 0.088 | 0.137 | − 0.204 | 0.028 | |||||
BAPQ | − 0.528 | < 0.001 | − 0.644 | − 0.412 | |||||
Step 2 | |||||||||
Age | − 0.046 | 0.443 | − 0.165 | 0.072 | |||||
BAPQ | − 0.570 | < 0.001 | − 0.690 | − 0.451 | |||||
CAT-Q | 0.163 | 0.010 | 0.039 | 0.286 | |||||
Step 3 | |||||||||
Age | − 0.052 | 0.389 | − 0.169 | 0.066 | |||||
BAPQ | − 0.571 | <0 .0001 | − 0.690 | − 0.453 | |||||
CAT-Q | 0.149 | 0.018 | 0.026 | 0.272 | |||||
CAT-Q2 | − 0.127 | 0.028 | − 0.241 | − 0.014 |
BAPQ Broad autism phenotype questionnaire; CAT-Q Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire; β Standardised beta; CI Confidence interval. In the UK sample, well-being was not measured. The quadratic term of standardised social camouflage scores (CAT-Q2) was added in Step 3
Is the relationship between mental health and social camouflage restrained by sex?
To examine the influence of sex on the relationship between social camouflage and mental health, hierarchical linear regression analyses were run with the binary variable of sex (men and women) as a baseline predictor (alongside age, autistic traits, and social camouflage) in the first stage. Then, an interaction term (camouflage*sex) was added to the models in the second stage. The interactions between sex and social camouflage were not significant in any model (Table 3). The results of these analyses excluding straight liners are presented in Additional file 1: Table S4.
Table 3.
Variable | β | p | 95% CI | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | ||||
Model 1a | |||||
(Generalised anxiety) | |||||
Step 1 | |||||
Age | 0.083 | 0.201 | − 0.044 | 0.211 | |
Sex | 0.190 | 0.003 | 0.064 | 0.316 | |
BAPQ | 0.396 | < .0001 | 0.269 | 0.523 | |
CAT-Q | 0.015 | 0.823 | − 0.120 | 0.151 | |
Step 2 | |||||
Age | 0.083 | 0.202 | − 0.045 | 0.211 | |
Sex | 0.190 | 0.003 | 0.064 | 0.316 | |
BAPQ | 0.396 | < 0.001 | 0.269 | 0.524 | |
CAT-Q | 0.016 | 0.821 | − 0.120 | 0.152 | |
CAT-Q*Sex | − 0.002 | 0.970 | − 0.127 | 0.122 | |
Model 2a | |||||
(Depression) | |||||
Step 1 | |||||
Age | 0.110 | 0.077 | − 0.012 | 0.232 | |
Sex | 0.194 | 0.002 | 0.073 | 0.314 | |
BAPQ | 0.455 | < 0.001 | 0.333 | 0.577 | |
CAT-Q | 0.038 | 0.559 | − 0.090 | 0.166 | |
Step 2 | |||||
Age | 0.110 | 0.077 | − 0.012 | 0.232 | |
Sex | 0.195 | 0.002 | 0.074 | 0.316 | |
BAPQ | 0.453 | < 0.001 | 0.331 | 0.576 | |
CAT-Q | 0.041 | 0.534 | − 0.088 | 0.169 | |
CAT-Q*Sex | − 0.024 | 0.696 | − 0.142 | 0.095 | |
Model 3a | |||||
(Social anxiety) | |||||
Step 1 | |||||
Age | 0.078 | 0.209 | − 0.044 | 0.199 | |
Sex | 0.033 | 0.593 | − 0.090 | 0.157 | |
BAPQ | 0.577 | < 0.001 | 0.460 | 0.695 | |
CAT-Q | 0.043 | 0.502 | − 0.082 | 0.167 | |
Step 2 | |||||
Age | 0.078 | 0.207 | − 0.043 | 0.200 | |
Sex | 0.037 | 0.558 | − 0.087 | 0.160 | |
BAPQ | 0.574 | < 0.001 | 0.456 | 0.692 | |
CAT-Q | 0.049 | 0.442 | − .0076 | 0.174 | |
CAT-Q*Sex | − 0 .053 | 0.354 | − 0.165 | 0.059 | |
Model 4a | |||||
(Well-being) | |||||
Step 1 | |||||
Age | − 0 .055 | 0.360 | − 0.174 | 0.063 | |
Sex | − 0.097 | 0.107 | − 0.214 | 0.021 | |
BAPQ | − 0.563 | < 0.001 | − 0.682 | − .0444 | |
CAT-Q | 0.180 | 0.005 | 0.055 | 0.305 | |
Step 2 | |||||
Age | − 0.055 | 0.364 | − 0.173 | 0.063 | |
Sex | − 0 .092 | 0.125 | − 0.209 | 0.026 | |
BAPQ | − 0.569 | < 0.001 | − 0.688 | − 0.450 | |
CAT-Q | 0.190 | 0.003 | 0.065 | 0.315 | |
CAT-Q*Sex | − .0080 | 0.178 | − 0.196 | 0.036 |
BAPQ Broad autism phenotype questionnaire; CAT-Q Camouflaging autistic traits questionnaire; β Standardised beta; CI Confidence interval
Discussion
This study aimed to clarify the characteristics of Japanese autistic people regarding social camouflage and mental health and compare them with those of autistic people in the UK. It showed that too much or too little engagement in social camouflage would negatively affect the mental health of autistic people in Japan, as we hypothesised. In the hierarchical multiple regression analysis, the Japanese dataset showed nonlinear results (i.e. too little or too much social camouflage was related to more severe mental problems), whereas the UK dataset showed linear results. In the UK, more social camouflage was associated with poorer mental health, while too little or too much social camouflage was associated with poorer mental health in Japan.
The average engagement in social camouflage among Japanese autistic people was lower than that among autistic people in the UK.
In the Autism Knowledge Quiz [43], which conducted a randomised controlled trial to quantify autism awareness, Japanese autistic adolescents had a lower self-understanding of the autism spectrum disorder than British autistic adolescents. In addition, while the cut-off for the autism-spectrum quotient [44] is 33 points in the UK, the cut-off is 26 points in Japan [45]. This means that autistic people in Japan have few opportunities to receive education about autistic traits and have difficulty understanding what autistic traits are, causing them to feel that they do not fit in with society. For example, instead of the term social camouflage, a similar concept exists in Japan, i.e. autistic people over-adapt. This is considered to be synonymous with social camouflage in terms of the content. Relatedly, in an international comparative study conducted in 2023, autistic individuals in Japan were found to be overadjusted in eight countries (Australia, Belgium, Canada, Japan, New Zealand, South Africa, the United Kingdom, and the United States), and Japanese autistic individuals had both the lowest social acceptance and the lowest internal acceptance scores [46]. This indicates a high external pressure in Japan to be like normal people [46]. Japan is an East Asian country with a relatively collectivistic and homogeneous culture and high cultural density [28, 47], and Japanese society has a particularly high stigma against autistic individuals because they sometimes deviate from social norms. For example, compared to the UK and India, a stronger emphasis is placed on social conformity in Japanese society [48].
Japanese people tend to be intolerant of ‘people who look different’, such as autistic people. However, it is also rare for Japanese people to point this out to Japanese autistic people because people in the general population tend to be nonverbal toward autistic people. Therefore, autistic people in the country do not have many opportunities to become aware of their autistic traits. Consequently, strategies for adaptation could manifest as over-adaptation in Japanese autistic adults because they could end up preferring to ‘follow one’s orders’ rather than engage in social camouflage, which is an impression-management strategy that requires some understanding of autistic traits.
Another reason for the results of a reduced engagement in social camouflage among Japanese autistic adults could be related to the fact that regarding social orientation, people in Asia are less independent and more interdependent than people in Western countries [29]. As mentioned in the Introduction section, the term interdependence refers to the emphasis on remaining harmonious with a homogeneous group, and many Asian cultures have a culture of interdependence [29]. Western cultures generally do not assume overt ties between individuals and do not recognise the value of interdependence as much as Asian cultures. High levels of interdependence lead to high levels of cohesion between groups, thus also leading to the exclusion of groups with attributes that deviate from shared moral boundaries [49]. Based on these descriptions, we hypothesised that living in a culture characterised by interdependence could be related to moderate levels of engagement in social camouflage, having a positive influence on mental health among autistic individuals. Moderate levels of social camouflage were associated with moderate levels of mental health, specifically in Japanese autistic adults. This is consistent with the reports that both autistic and non-autistic Japanese people engage less in social camouflage than their British counterparts [36].
The relationship between social stigma and social camouflage
In both Japan and the UK, perceived social stigma was one of the factors associated with social camouflage; however, a study showed that the prevalence of social stigma among non-autistic people was greater in Asian than in Western countries [28]. At first glance, these results for perceived social stigma and actual social stigma could appear contradictory. Nonetheless, our results showed that the perception of social stigma of autistic people in Japan is lower than that of autistic people in the UK. Therefore, there could be a dissociation between the actual existence and the perception of social stigma in Japan. While the average age of autism diagnosis in 40 countries is 60.48 months (range: 30.90–234.57 months [50]), the average age of diagnosis in Japan is approximately 10 years (120 months [15]), which shows a delay in the diagnosis in Japan. This suggests that Japanese society has little awareness or understanding of autism. Thus, the lower social camouflage of Japanese autistic people than that of autistic people in the UK could be partly because Japanese autistic people are unaware of their autistic characteristics owing to the lack of educational opportunities for autism.
The influence of sex on the relationship between mental health problems and social camouflage
The relationship between social camouflage and mental health problems was not linear in Japan, different from that in the UK. One of the reasons could be the predominance of collectivism in Japan, making it so that most social behaviours of Japanese people are largely determined by goals, attitudes, and values that are shared with a group of people [51]. Non-autistic Japanese people also seem to have fewer opportunities to receive education on, and have less understanding of, autistic traits at both the social and individual levels [28]. This could mean that they feel more secure by behaving in a more ‘discreet’ manner, as this could avoid social exposure, rather than behaving more in line with their specific traits. However, such repressive attempts to assimilate into the group may be related to poor mental health, depending on the extent of such efforts. Alongside these influences, gender role expectations in the Japanese culture could also be related to how autistic adults engage in social camouflage—as well as the association of the latter with mental health.
Our research indicates that like Hull et al. [20] study, the link between social camouflage and mental health (such as generalised anxiety, depression, and social anxiety) remains consistent across sexes among autistic adults. However, some scholars have found that autistic women tend to engage in social camouflage more so than autistic men [52]. There is some evidence suggesting that social camouflage could be influenced by sex and the time of diagnosis [53], although the reasons for these variations are not yet fully understood. Some believe that gender role expectations play a role in encouraging social camouflage. For instance, traditional gender roles in Japan (such as women being expected to be quiet, do household chores, and take care of children, while men are expected to earn money and work hard) put different social pressures on each gender [54]. Although these expectations are divergent, they could still motivate both sexes to engage in social camouflage, which could explain why there was no interaction effect for both sexes among Japanese autistic adults. However, the link between sex and social camouflage requires further research, as there are no previous studies in Japan.
Furthermore, Hull et al. [20] argued that research examining sex in the context of social camouflage and mental health needs to specifically address the experiences of people with nonbinary gender identity. In the Japanese survey, there was no item for people to express their gender as being nonbinary, whereas the survey conducted in the UK allowed people to express their gender, and a small number of people responded that they were nonbinary. Nonetheless, the sample size for these nonbinary respondents was not sufficient to enable statistical comparisons to be made. Correlation analysis of mental health problems and social camouflage in the nonbinary subsample was also not significant. Notwithstanding, significant associations could be identified in larger properly sized samples and should be conducted in Japan in the future.
Limitations
This study has several limitations. First, the autistic participants had self-reported diagnoses, and while the autism-spectrum quotient provides a cut-off value for screening, it does not enable confirming diagnoses. Second, the mean score for the CAT-Q-J was lower than that for the original CAT-Q [55]. This implies that the social camouflage strategy types used by autistic people in Japan and the UK could differ, albeit this study does not examine this topic. This could be further explored in future studies. Finally, although this study conducted a hierarchical multiple regression analysis, it was cross-sectional and did not enable examining causal relationships. In the future, researchers could conduct a study using longitudinal data with the variables of age and time to clarify the causal relationship between social camouflage and mental health among autistic Japanese individuals.
Conclusion
In Japanese autistic adults, the association between social camouflage and mental health problems was non-linear, with moderate social camouflage being positively associated with mental health. However, excessive social camouflage was found to worsen mental health. This can be attributed to multiple factors, including strong social stigma, poor understanding of autism, low social acceptance, and a cultural background that values averageness.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Sayaka Yoshimura and Hiroyuki Ohtani for their assistance in data collection as well as everyone who participated in the study.
Abbreviations
- SD
Standard deviation
- CAT-Q
Camouflaging autistic traits questionnaire
- CAT-Q-J
Japanese version of the CAT-Q
- LSAS-J
Liebowitz social anxiety scale
- WEMWBS-J
Warwick–Edinburgh mental well-being scale
- PHQ-9
Patient health questionnaire-9
- GAD-7
Generalized anxiety disorder-7
Author contributions
FO Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing-original draft preparation. LH Supervision, Data curation. WM Supervision. TT Visualisation, Data curation. SG Visualisation. MS Visualisation and Writing. MT Investigation. KT Writing—Reviewing and Editing. ES Writing—Reviewing and Editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This study was funded by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (No. 22H01089). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Availability of data and materials
The anonymised data set is available upon reasonable request from Fumiyo Oshima (c21ujsw35117c@faculty.gs.chiba-u.jp). The anonymised dataset of the sample from Japan is available in Additional file 2: Supplementary Data 1 (Data S1).
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Chiba University Research Ethics Committee (reference M10053), and all participants provided consent to participate online before they took part in the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association; 2013; p. DSM-5.
- 2.Hull L, Petrides KV, Allison C, Smith P, Baron-Cohen S, Lai MC, et al. “Putting on my best normal”: social camouflaging in adults with autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:2519–2534. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.O'Nions E, Petersen I, Buckman JEJ, Charlton R, Cooper C, Corbett A, et al. Autism in England: assessing underdiagnosis in a population-based cohort study of prospectively collected primary care data. Lancet Reg Health - Eur. 2023;29:100626. doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2023.100626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bottema-Beutel K, Kapp SK, Lester JN, Sasson NJ, Hand BN. Avoiding ableist language: suggestions for autism researchers. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3:18–29. doi: 10.1089/aut.2020.0014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Camm-Crosbie L, Bradley L, Shaw R, Baron-Cohen S, Cassidy S. ‘People like me don’t get support’: autistic adults’ experiences of support and treatment for mental health difficulties, self-injury and suicidality. Autism. 2019;23:1431–1441. doi: 10.1177/1362361318816053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cresswell L, Cage E. ‘Who am I?’: an exploratory study of the relationships between identity, acculturation and mental health in autistic adolescents. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:2901–2912. doi: 10.1007/S10803-019-04016-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Han E, Scior K, Avramides K, Crane L. A systematic review on autistic people’s experiences of stigma and coping strategies. Autism Res. 2022;15:12–26. doi: 10.1002/AUR.2652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lever AG, Geurts HM. Psychiatric co-occurring symptoms and disorders in young, middle-aged, and older adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:1916–1930. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2722-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Spain D, Zıvralı Yarar E, Happé F. Social anxiety in adults with autism: a qualitative study. Int J Qual Stud Health Well Being. 2020;15:1803669. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2020.1803669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Croen LA, Zerbo O, Qian Y, Massolo ML, Rich S, Sidney S, et al. The health status of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism. 2015;19:814–823. doi: 10.1177/1362361315577517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cassidy S, Bradley L, Shaw R, Baron-Cohen S. Risk markers for suicidality in autistic adults. Mol Autism. 2018 doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0226-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Knüppel A, Telléus GK, Jakobsen H, Lauritsen MB. Characteristics of young adults with autism spectrum disorder performing different daytime activities. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:542–555. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3730-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Russell AJ, Murphy CM, Wilson E, Gillan N, Brown C, Robertson DM, et al. The mental health of individuals referred for assessment of autism spectrum disorder in adulthood: a clinic report. Autism. 2016;20:623–627. doi: 10.1177/1362361315604271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Saddichha S, Al-Desouki M, Lamia A, Linden IA, Krausz M. Online interventions for depression and anxiety – a systematic review. Health Psychol Behav Med. 2014;2:841–881. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2014.945934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kamio Y, Inada N, Koyama T. A nationwide survey on quality of life and associated factors of adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2013;17:15–26. doi: 10.1177/1362361312436848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Iwasa M, Shimizu Y, Sasayama D, Imai M, Ohzono H, Ueda M, et al. Twenty-year longitudinal birth cohort study of individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder before seven years of age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63:1563–1573. doi: 10.1111/JCPP.13614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hull L, Mandy W. Protective effect or missed diagnosis? Females with autism spectrum disorder. Future Neurol. 2017;12:159–169. doi: 10.2217/fnl-2017-0006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mandy W. Social camouflaging in autism: Is it time to lose the mask? Autism. 2019;23:1879–1881. doi: 10.1177/1362361319878559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hull L, Mandy W, Lai MC, Baron-Cohen S, Allison C, Smith P, et al. Development and validation of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q) J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:819–833. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3792-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hull L, Levy L, Lai MC, Petrides KV, Baron-Cohen S, Allison C, et al. Is social camouflaging associated with anxiety and depression in autistic adults? Mol Autism. 2021;12:13. doi: 10.1186/s13229-021-00421-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Ruigrok AN, Bullmore ET, Suckling J, et al. Neural self-representation in autistic women and association with ‘compensatory camouflaging’. Autism. 2019;23:1210–1223. doi: 10.1177/1362361318807159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mogensen L, Mason J. The meaning of a label for teenagers negotiating identity: experiences with autism spectrum disorder. In: Brady G, Lowe P, Lauritzen SO, editors. Children, health and well-being: policy debates and lived experience. Amsterdam: Wiley; 2015. pp. 255–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cage E, Troxell-Whitman Z. Understanding the reasons, contexts and costs of camouflaging for autistic adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:1899–1911. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Vries M, Cader S, Colleer L, Batteux E, Yasdiman MB, Tan YJ, et al. University students’ notion of autism spectrum conditions: a cross-cultural study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:1281–1294. doi: 10.1007/S10803-019-04343-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Murray K, Jassi A, Mataix-Cols D, Barrow F, Krebs G. Outcomes of cognitive behaviour therapy for obsessive–compulsive disorder in young people with and without autism spectrum disorders: a case controlled study. Psychiatry Res. 2015;228:8–13. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2015.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yu L, Stronach S, Harrison AJ. Public knowledge and stigma of autism spectrum disorder: comparing China with the United States. Autism. 2020;24:1531–1545. doi: 10.1177/1362361319900839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Obeid R, Daou N, Denigris D, Shane-Simpson C, Brooks PJ, Gillespie-Lynch K. A cross-cultural comparison of knowledge and stigma associated with autism spectrum disorder among college students in Lebanon and the United States. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:3520–3536. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2499-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Someki F, Torii M, Brooks PJ, Koeda T, Gillespie-Lynch K. Stigma associated with autism among college students in Japan and the United States: an online training study. Res Dev Disabil. 2018;76:88–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev. 1991;98:224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cage E, Troxell-Whitman Z. Understanding the relationships between autistic identity, disclosure, and camouflaging. Autism Adulthood. 2020;2:334–338. doi: 10.1089/AUT.2020.0016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Livingston LA, Colvert E, Social Relationships Study Team, Bolton P, Happé F Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: Exploring compensation in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;60:102–110. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Perry E, Mandy W, Hull L, Cage E. Understanding camouflaging as a response to autism-related stigma: a social identity theory approach. J Autism Dev Disord. 2022;52:800–810. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04987-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tamura M, Cage E, Perry E, Hongo M, Takahashi T, Seto M, et al. Understanding camouflaging, stigma, and mental health for autistic people in Japan. 2023, 10.21203/rs.3.rs-2614748/v2
- 34.Cage E, di Monaco J, Newell V. Experiences of autism acceptance and mental health in autistic adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48:473–484. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3342-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gillespie-Lynch K, Daou N, Sanchez-Ruiz MJ, Kapp SK, Obeid R, Brooks PJ, et al. Factors underlying cross-cultural differences in stigma toward autism among college students in Lebanon and the United States. Autism. 2019;23:1993–2006. doi: 10.1177/1362361318823550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hongo M, Oshima F, Guan S, Takahashi T, Nitta Y, Seto M, et al. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Camouflaging autistic Traits Questionnaire. Psychiatry, ArXiv. 2022. 10.31234/OSF.IO/ZDC6G. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 37.Huang JL, Curran PG, Keeney J, Poposki EM, DeShon RP. Detecting and deterring insufficient effort responding to surveys. J Bus Psychol. 2012;27(1):99–114. doi: 10.1007/s10869-011-9231-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Liebowitz MR. Social phobia. Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry. 1987;22:141–173. doi: 10.1159/000414022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Asakura S, Inoue S, Sasaki F, Sasaki Y, Kitagawa N, Inoue T, et al. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. Seishin Igaku. 2002;44(10):1077–1084. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, et al. The Warwick–Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5:63. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Suganuma S, Hirano M, Nakano M, Shimoyama H. Development of the Japanese version of Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS): the hedonic/Eudaimonic State index. Jpn J Clin Psychol. 2016;16:471–5. http://search.jamas.or.jp/link/ui/2016355081.
- 42.Muramatsu K, Miyaoka H, Kamijima K, Muramatsu Y, Tanaka Y, Hosaka M, et al. Performance of the Japanese version of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (J-PHQ-9) for depression in primary care. Gen Hospital Psychiatr. 2018;52:64–69. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2018.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gordon K, Murin M, Baykaner O, Roughan L, Livermore-Hardy V, Skuse D, et al. A randomised controlled trial of PEGASUS, a psychoeducational programme for young people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:468–476. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kurita H, Koyama T, Osada H. Autism-Spectrum quotient-Japanese version and its short forms for screening normally intelligent persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;59:490–496. doi: 10.1111/J.1440-1819.2005.01403.X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Keating CT, Hickman L, Geelhand P, Takahashi T, Leung J, Schuster B, et al. Global perspectives on autism acceptance, camouflaging behaviours and mental health in autism spectrum disorder: a registered report protocol. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0261774. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0261774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Varnum MEW, Grossmann I, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010;19:9–13. doi: 10.1177/0963721409359301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Carruthers S, Kinnaird E, Rudra A, Smith P, Allison C, Auyeung B, et al. A cross-cultural study of autistic traits across India Japan and the UK. Mol Autism. 2018 doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0235-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Opotow S. Moral exclusion and injustice: an introduction. J Soc Issues. 1990;46:1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00268.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.van’t Hof M, Tisseur C, van Berckelear-Onnes I, van Nieuwenhuyzen A, Daniels AM, Deen M. Age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis from 2012 to 2019. Autism. 2021;25:862–873. doi: 10.1177/1362361320971107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Triandis H. Collectivism v. individualism: A reconceptualisation of a basic concept in cross-cultural social psychology. In: Verma, GK, Bagley, C, (Eds). Cross-Cultural Studies of Personality, Attitudes and Cognition. London: Palgrave MacMillan; 1988, pp. 60–95. 10.1007/978-1-349-08120-2_3.
- 52.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Auyeung B, Szatmari P, et al. Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism. Autism. 2017;21:690–702. doi: 10.1177/1362361316671012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.McQuaid GA, Lee NR, Wallace GL. Camouflaging in autism spectrum disorder: examining the roles of sex, gender identity, and diagnostic timing. Autism. 2022;26:552–559. doi: 10.1177/13623613211042131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lee KS, Tufiş PA, Alwin DF. Separate spheres or increasing equality? Changing gender beliefs in Postwar Japan. J Marriage Fam. 2010;72:184–201. doi: 10.1111/J.1741-3737.2009.00691.X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hongo M, Oshima F, Guan S, Takahashi T, Nitta Y, Seto M, et al. Reliability and Validity of the Japanese Version of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire.” PsyArXiv, 202210.31234/osf.io/zdc6g. [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The anonymised data set is available upon reasonable request from Fumiyo Oshima (c21ujsw35117c@faculty.gs.chiba-u.jp). The anonymised dataset of the sample from Japan is available in Additional file 2: Supplementary Data 1 (Data S1).