Abstract
Purpose:
Damage to the adult primary visual cortex (V1) causes vision loss in the contralateral hemifield, initiating a process of trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration (TRD). Here, we examined retinal correlates of TRD using a new metric to account for global changes in inner retinal thickness, and asked if perceptual training in the intact or blind field impacts its progression.
Methods:
We performed a meta-analysis of optical coherence tomography (OCT) data in 48 participants with unilateral V1 stroke and homonymous visual defects, who completed clinical trial NCT03350919. After measuring the thickness of the macular ganglion cell and inner plexiform layers (GCL-IPL), and the peripapillary retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL), we computed individual laterality indices (LI) at baseline and after ~6 months of daily motion discrimination training in the intact- or blind-field. Increasingly positive LI denoted greater layer thinning in retinal regions affected versus unaffected by the cortical damage.
Results:
Pre-training, the affected GCL-IPL and RNFL were thinner than their unaffected counterparts, generating LI values positively correlated with time since stroke. Participants trained in their intact-field exhibited increased LIGCL-IPL. Those trained in their blind-field had no significant change in LIGCL-IPL. LIRNFL did not change in either group.
Conclusions:
Relative shrinkage of the affected versus unaffected macular GCL-IPL can be reliably measured at an individual level and increases with time post-V1 stroke. Relative thinning progressed during intact-field training, but appeared to be halted by training within the blind field, suggesting a potentially neuroprotective effect of this simple behavioral intervention.
Introduction
Cortical blindness (CB) following unilateral damage to the primary visual cortex (V1) or its immediate afferents presents as a homonymous, contra-lesional visual field defect. Although partial recovery can occur spontaneously in the first few months after damage1–4, there are no widely-accepted, validated treatments for the resulting visual defect5. Standard of care remains “no intervention”, although occasionally, patients are prescribed compensatory (e.g. saccadic) training or substitution (e.g. prism lenses) therapies6–9. Research also continues to show that visual perceptual training can partially restore vision in CB, measurable by both clinical perimetry and psychophysical tests of visual performance10–21.
The importance of developing some form of restorative therapy for CB is further highlighted by burgeoning evidence that once patients reach the chronic stage of >6 months post-stroke, visual field defects do not remain completely stable, as was initially thought22. Instead there appears to be progressive worsening of the perimetrically-defined blind field (BF) without intervention11,19,22,23. The most plausible explanation for such deterioration of the BF over time is TRD, which involves the progressive shrinkage and even die-back of neurons in the early visual pathways24–31. In humans, structural MRI analyses have shown that the optic tract ipsilateral to occipital cortex damage is often reduced in size25,29,30,32–35, as are the thicknesses of the ganglion cell and nerve fiber layers in corresponding regions of the retina in each eye24,28–32,34,36–45.
Retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) are responsible for pre-processing and ferrying visual information to the rest of the visual system. As such, their loss or dysfunction could significantly threaten the potential to recover visual functions in participants with V1 damage. Specifically, retinal neurons in the therapeutically-targetable retino-geniculate-striate pathways susceptible to trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration (TRD) after occipital stroke. Isolating specific consequences of TRD in retinal regions known to synapse with V1-lesion-projecting neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus is crucial to better understand the relationship between TRD and visual retraining. Approaches to re-train the visual deficit have been shown to confer perimetrically-computed improvements to CB visual fields16,46. However, most literature has focused on benefits to visual perception resulting from visual retraining, with limited knowledge of the effects of training on anatomical substrates of vision16,46. If visual training strengthens existing circuitry or recruits neuronal neighbors, similar to rehabilitation for motor stroke47–49, this could potentially impact retinal cells that provide input to residual visual pathways. As such, the present study asked two questions: 1) what is the extent and time-course of relative thinning in affected versus unaffected inner retinal layers in humans with unilateral occipital strokes, and 2) does the stimulation afforded by visual training impact the progression of inner retinal thinning in such stroke patients? To answer these questions, we performed a meta-analysis of optical coherence tomography (OCT) data collected as part of a recently-completed, multicenter, randomized, double-masked, clinical trial titled the “Hemianopia Intervention Study” (HIS; ClinicalTrials.gov identifier, NCT03350919). The HIS clinical trial design and results have been published in detail50, but in brief, the trial involved 2 pre-training clinic visits to establish eligibility and measure baseline parameters, a 6-months at-home phase during which training was administered to either the intact field (IF) or BF, and 1 post-training clinic visit to evaluate the effect of training. The primary outcome measure for the HIS clinical trial was change in the 24–2 Humphrey perimetric mean deviation (PMD) from baseline, with significant improvements reported for people trained in their BF, and not those trained in their IF.50 However, the trial also performed OCT imaging and collected measurements of GCL-IPL and RNFL thicknesses in the affected and unaffected retina of each eye in each participant at each time-point. This rich data set provided us a unique opportunity to both measure the extent of TRD in this patient cohort, and analyze the impact of two different of visual training interventions on TRD progression.
Methods
Participants
The HIS trial (NCT03350919) recruited 48 CB participants (see Table 1 for demographics) at 3 US academic medical centers: 20 at the University of Rochester’s Flaum Eye Institute, 18 at the University of Pennsylvania’s Scheie Eye Institute and 8 at the University of Miami’s Bascom Palmer Eye Institute. All procedures were approved by the Western Institutional Review Board (WIRB#1181904), adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and all participants gave written, informed consent.
Table 1. Participant demographics.
Subject code | Sex | Age range (years) | Time since stroke (months) | Affected visual hemifield | Training Group |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
CB1 | m | 46–50 | 32 | R | Intact |
CB2 | f | 46–50 | 13 | R | Intact |
CB3 | m | 70–75 | 20 | L | Intact |
CB4 | m | 60–65 | 61 | L | Intact |
CB5 | m | 60–65 | 43 | L | Intact |
CB6 | m | 55–60 | 105 | R | Intact |
CB7 | f | 70–75 | 338 | L | Intact |
CB8 | m | 70–75 | 10 | L | Intact |
CB9 | m | 66–70 | 6 | R | Intact |
CB10 | m | 70–75 | 4 | L | Intact |
CB11 | m | 70–75 | 58 | L | Intact |
CB12 | f | 66–70 | 16 | R | Intact |
CB13 | f | 50–55 | 24 | L | Intact |
CB14 | m | 60–65 | 130 | L | Intact |
CB15 | m | 56–60 | 13 | L | Intact |
CB16 | m | 46–50 | 63 | L | Intact |
CB17 | m | 60–65 | 8 | L | Intact |
CB18 | f | 46–50 | 15 | R | Intact |
CB19 | m | 60–65 | 20 | R | Intact |
CB20 | m | 40–45 | 5 | R | Intact |
CB21 | m | 66–70 | 3 | R | Intact |
CB22 | m | 56–60 | 4 | R | Intact |
CB23 | m | 70–75 | 6 | L | Intact |
CB24 | m | 66–70 | 60 | L | Blind |
CB25 | f | 50–55 | 38 | R | Blind |
CB26 | f | 50–55 | 18 | L | Blind |
CB27 | m | 40–45 | 15 | R | Blind |
CB28 | m | 50–55 | 373 | R | Blind |
CB29 | m | 60–65 | 5 | R | Blind |
CB30 | m | 70–75 | 47 | R | Blind |
CB31 | m | 56–60 | 8 | L | Blind |
CB32 | m | 60–65 | 47 | L | Blind |
CB33 | m | 40–45 | 11 | R | Blind |
CB34 | m | 56–60 | 4 | L | Blind |
CB35 | f | 40–45 | 10 | R | Blind |
CB36 | m | 66–70 | 105 | L | Blind |
CB37 | m | 66–70 | 36 | R | Blind |
CB38 | m | 30–35 | 5 | R | Blind |
CB39 | f | 46–50 | 5 | L | Blind |
CB40 | f | 60–65 | 3 | L | Blind |
CB41 | m | 46–50 | 14 | R | Blind |
CB42 | m | 66–70 | 3 | L | Blind |
CB43 | m | 70–75 | 6 | L | Blind |
Participants were between 21 and 75 years of age, with an MRI-confirmed occipital lesion resulting in unilateral homonymous hemianopia. Additionally, their lesions had to have occurred after the age of 18 and a minimum of 90 days prior to screening. Participants were also required to reliably fixate with both eyes during psychophysical testing and clinical Humphrey perimetry, with fixation losses, false-negative, and false-positive rates during perimetry of <20%. Participants were excluded from the study if they presented with any ocular or neurologic disease that would interfere with training. Concurrent use of any other form of visual therapy, or of medications that would affect training were additional exclusion criteria.
HIS clinical trial design and training intervention
As mentioned earlier, the HIS clinical trial50 involved 2 pre-training clinic visits, a 6-month at-home training phase and 1 post-training clinic visit. While the primary outcome measure was change in the 24–2 Humphrey PMD from baseline to 6 months post-training, OCT data were also collected at each study visit, followed by computerized psychophysical testing focused on instructing participants to correctly perform the training task. Once enrolled, participants were randomized to 2 training arms: intact field (IF) or blind field (BF) training - in a 1:1 ratio, using a permuted block design stratified by site. Participants randomized to these training groups did not differ in age (BF-trained: 56±12 years, range 32–72 years; IF-trained: 61±9 years, range 45–74 years; unpaired t-test p= 0.0990, CI95=−11.83 to 1.057) or time since stroke (BF-trained: 41±82 months, range 3–373 months; IF-trained: 43±72 months, range 3–338 months; unpaired t-test p=0.9096, CI95=−50.37 to 44.98).
The training intervention was a 2-alternative, forced-choice (2AFC), direction discrimination task using random dot stimuli presented either inside the BF or at a corresponding location in the IF (Table 1, Supplementary Materials, Supplementary Fig. S1). During the home training segment, two participants withdrew and their data are not included herein. The two cohorts trained for a comparable number of days (unpaired t-test p=0.3598, CI95=−43.82 to 16.27; BF-trained: 101±46 days; IF-trained: 115±51 days).
During pre-training, in-clinic assessment, participants received instructions and underwent baseline testing with this task within their intact and blind hemifields, with fixation enforced binocularly using an Eyelink Duo Mobile eye-tracker (SR Research, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada). Training locations were selected at sites where performance first dropped to chance (50% correct) after a 1° lateral shift along the x-axis from the intact into the BF. Using the location where performance first drops to chance as a starting point affords proximity to intact circuitry, enhancing the possibility that training may recruit perilesional V151, and/or induce plasticity and re-integration of residual, damaged circuitry. IF-training locations were selected to be mirror-symmetric to those chosen for training in the BF. CB participants were then sent home to train and were asked to perform 300 trials of the 2AFC task once daily for a minimum of 5 days per week, at their assigned, training location. Participants trained at a single location at a time, either in the IF or BF. Once performance improved sufficiently (at least 10 sessions at a threshold <25° with a standard deviation of less than 5°), the location was moved 1° laterally away from the vertical meridian. Participant performance as a result of these interventions has been published50 and will not be repeated here in detail.
Humphrey visual field testing and analysis
Each participant’s visual deficit was quantified through Humphrey visual field (HVF) perimetry, which was performed twice in both eyes during each study visit. The University of Rochester and the University of Pennsylvania used a Humphrey Field Analyzer II-i, and the University of Miami used a Humphrey Field Analyzer 3 (Zeiss Humphrey Systems, Atlanta, GA), with all sites using a 24–2 testing pattern. A white, size III stimulus was presented on a background with a luminance of 11.3 cd/m2 and thresholds were calculated with the Swedish Interactive Threshold Algorithm (SITA-standard). Participants’ visual acuity was corrected to 20/20 for testing, and fixation was controlled using the gaze/blind spot automatic settings. The first test was excluded in both eyes to account for potential learning effects. If the second field set was not deemed reliable or could not be completed, the first set was used instead. Participants who did not have complete, reliable pre- and post-training visual fields were excluded from the present HVF analyses(n=5); an additional two participants failed to complete training and were also removed from our analysis. Two metrics were derived from HVF tests: the perimetric mean deviation (MD) and the average luminance detection sensitivity across the entire blind hemifield of vision. The MD is calculated by the perimeter using an internal, weighted variance from age-defined normal population values, to estimate the amount of vision lost across the measured visual field. In the present study, sensitivity thresholds from the blind hemifield (STBF) were additionally averaged in each eye to capture deficit-specific changes. We then took the monocular MD and STBF and averaged them to generate a binocular (OU) version of each metric, for pre- and post-training comparisons, in order to compare with binocularly-computed OCT laterality indices.
Optical coherence tomography (OCT) procedures and analysis
Retinal OCT was performed using Cirrus HD machines (Carl Zeiss Meditech) at each study site before and after training. A 512 × 128 Mac Cube scan was used to examine the ganglion cell and inner plexiform layers (GCL-IPL) around the fovea, and 200 × 200 optic nerve cube scans were used to examine the retinal nerve fiber layer (RNFL). Scans were excluded if they failed to meet signal strength ≥7 in each eye.
OCT analyses performed as part of the HIS clinical trial50 differed from those performed here in several ways. First, they involved group-level comparisons of GCL-IPL and RNFL thickness changes pre- to post-training across affected and unaffected retinal regions, separated by blind field sector and for each eye independently. Here, for the GCL-IPL, we combined the 2 nasal sectors together, and the two temporal sectors together. Furthermore, after reviewing OCT raw data, we excluded 3 HIS participants due to retinal folding or epiretinal membrane/RNFL detachment severe enough to impact layer thickness measurements. In remaining participants, we then computed a laterality index (LI) to account for individual, baseline thickness variances using the following formula:
LI was computed for the GCL-IPL using the 2 nasal and 2 temporal sector values of both eyes, excluding superior and inferior sectors that overlapped the vertical meridian (Fig. 1A). The nasal and temporal macular segments of each eye corresponding to the blind or intact hemifield were then averaged together according to each participant-specific deficit. For example, a right-sided visual deficit (left-sided occipital lesion) is represented in the nasal sectors of the right eye and the temporal sectors of the left eye (see example in Fig. 1A).
Computing a laterality for the RNFL regions impacted by the deficit attempted to account for the crossed and uncrossed fibers in corresponding peripapillary sections52 (Fig. 2A). Superior and inferior peripapillary regions comprised of uncrossed fibers, and nasal peripapillary regions comprised of crossed fibers represent intact or blind hemifields. For example, the same right-sided visual deficit area described above was represented by the superior and inferior RNFL sectors of the left eye as well as the nasal RNFL sector of the right eye (Fig. 2A).
Statistical Analyses
Paired t-tests were used to assess within-subject differences. For independent sample comparisons, unpaired t-tests were used when contrasting 2 groups. If standard deviations were not the same in each group, Welch’s correction was used. Linear regressions were used to model the relationship between explanatory variables and dependent outcomes, with r values and 95% confidence intervals (CI95) for rho provided, and significance estimated using a t-test.
Results
Baseline retinal layer thicknesses – effects of time since stroke
Prior to intervention, GCL-IPL thicknesses corresponding to the blind or intact hemifields were significantly different from each other, with the affected hemiretina’s GCL-IPL being thinner than the unaffected hemiretina’s (Fig.1B). We then computed LI to factor out possible global retinal phenomena (e.g., aging-related, metabolic, etc.) in order to better isolate lesion-specific degeneration in retinal regions corresponding to perimetrically-defined visual deficits. The LIGCL-IPL was positive, averaging 0.056 ± 0.06, with a range of −0.068 to 0.29. An LI of 0 would indicate no relative thinning of the lesion-projecting compared to the non-lesion projecting part of the retina, while positive LI values denote thinning in retinal areas representing the blind hemifield relative to those representing the intact hemifield. Importantly, the LIGCL-IPL was positively correlated with time since stroke (Fig. 1C), with greater thinning of the affected hemiretina GCL-IPL in participants imaged beyond 12 months post-stroke compared to those imaged prior to this timepoint (Fig. S2A).
A similar pattern of results was obtained for the peripapillary RNFL, which was thinner for segments carrying RGC axons representing the visual field defect compared to those carrying predominantly intact field fibers (Fig.2B). As a result, LIRNFL averaged 0.019±0.04, ranging from −0.10 to 0.11. Moreover, just like LIGCL-IPL, LIRNFL was positively correlated with time since stroke (Fig. 2C), with relative thinning most pronounced beyond 12 months post-lesion (Fig. S2B). Overall, these data show clear GCL-IPL and RNFL thinning in regions of the retina carrying either RGC somata, dendrites and/or axons representing blind regions of the visual field. They also show greater thinning at later than earlier timepoints, especially >12 months after occipital stroke.
Effect of visual training on GC complex thickness
We next asked whether visual training altered signs of TRD at the level of the retina. Here, we analyzed CB patients who completed 6 months of visual training as part of the HIS clinical trial50. As previously reported, global direction discrimination training in the perimetrically-defined BF of CB patients elicits improvements not only on the trained task, but also on binocular (OU) Humphrey perimetry11,50. Consistent with this observation, participants trained in their BF exhibited a systematic improvement in OU MD (Fig.3A). To ascertain if the change in MD was driven by the blind hemifield (versus improved ability to perform Humphrey perimetry across the entire test area), we also computed OU STBF change for the blind hemifield. OU STBF improved significantly following BF training (Fig. 3B), contrasting with a lack of significant changes - for both OU MD and STBF - in the IF-trained cohort (Fig. 3D, E). Notably, a strong correlation exists between MD and STBF in both cohorts, pre- and post-training (Fig. 3C, F).
Having established a subtle but differential effect of training on perimetry between the two cohorts, we then asked if - and to what degree - the two types of intervention impacted retinal thinning. For LIGCL-IPL, there was a significant overall increase pre- to post-training across all participants (Fig. 4A). However, no detectable changes occurred pre- to post-training overall in LIRNFL (Fig. 4B). Separating the two interventions, LIGCL-IPL was significantly larger post-training in those who trained in their intact field (Fig.5A), but not in BF-trained participants (Fig.5B). Furthermore, in the IF training group, raw GCL-IPL thicknesses were significantly lower in the post-training affected hemiretina (Fig. 5C). Post-training affected GCL-IPL thicknesses also significantly changed from pre-training in the BF training group (Fig. 5D). Additionally, a significant difference in magnitude of change in the affected relative to unaffected hemiretinas was present in IF trained participants and, critically, no such difference was found in BF trained participants (Fig. 5E). However, we fail to reject the null hypothesis that the pre-post differences of the affected hemiretinas differ by training type (Fig. 5E).
When assessing the impact of training on the RNFL, no significant change in LIRNFL was found in either group pre- to post-training (Fig. 6A, B). Similarly, no significant pre-post training differences were observed in either training cohort for raw RNFL thickness (Fig. 6C, D). Additionally, when assessing pre-post change, no significant differences were seen between or within training groups (Fig. 6E).
Consistent with these findings, changes in STBF and LIGCL-IPL were directly [and inversely] correlated in those trained in their blind hemifield (Fig. 7A), but not in those trained in their IF (Fig. 7B). No significant correlations were observed between changes in STBF and LIRNFL in either training cohort (Fig. 7C, D).
Discussion
The present study asked – for the first time – whether visual stimulation provided by perceptual training alters the progression of retinal ganglion cell layer complex thinning after stroke damage to the occipital cortex in adult humans. First, we confirmed prior reports of relative thinning in the affected versus unaffected retinas’ GCL-IPL and RNFL after unilateral V1 damage28,29,31,32,34,36,39,41,53 using non-invasive OCT imaging. Second, the spread of post-stroke times at participant enrollment allowed us to define a time-course for this thinning. Finally, we now provide evidence that a simple behavioral intervention slows or blocks the progression of relative GCL-IPL thinning whereas comparable stimulation of the intact hemifield of vision fails to do so.
Occipital damage causes variable, progressive shrinkage of the GC complex
Our observations showed that the largest, positive deviations from 0 in LIGCL-IPL and LIRNFL occurred beyond 12 months post-stroke. While some deviation in LIGCL-IPL (but not LIRNFL) was also observed in our earliest participants, there was considerable inter-individual variability which precluded a significance analysis in the present cohort. Large deviations of LI values from 0 were previously observed for optic tract volumes using structural magnetic resonance imaging, starting from ~6 months post stroke, albeit also with large inter-individual variability35. This time-course differential makes some sense if one considers that the optic tract contains the distal portions of RGC axons, right before they synapse in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. These distal axons, being closer to the V1 lesion site, might exhibit earlier signs of target loss and degeneration than the cell bodies and dendritic arbors of the parent cells in the retina, but once again, a larger sample size earlier post-stroke would be needed to make this determination from a statistically-valid standpoint.
As stated earlier, a positive LI reflects a relative thinning of the lesion-projecting vs. intact-hemisphere-projecting portions of the macular GCL-IPL. This relative thinning could be attributable to shrinking of the RGC soma, cell death, and/or changes in cell branching; similarly, relative thinning in the RNFL could result from RGC axonal loss or shrinkage or both24,28,29,32,34,37,40,53–55. Though past studies show that RGCs are ultimately lost over time after occipital damage28,29,31,55,56, there is also evidence that RGCs change size based on metabolic activity or the beginning stages of apoptosis57,58. As such, slowing or even reversing retinal thinning may be possible if intervention occurs prior to significant cell death.
Importantly, both the GCL-IPL and RNFL were previously reported to thin with increasing age in humans59, a fact confirmed in the present data set, and is likely related to cell loss and/or shrinkage (Fig. S3). However, it is important to note that by computing and tracking changes in LI rather than raw layer thicknesses, we were able to dissociate the impact of the occipital stroke and subsequent training interventions, from this natural trend.
Visual training blocks the progression of relative GC complex thinning
In spite of initial retinal ganglion cell complex thinning at baseline, participants who trained in their BF for 6 months showed improvements in binocular performance metrics derived from Humphrey perimetry and seemed to avoid the increase in LIGCL-IPL that occurred in participants randomized to train in their IF. While GCL-IPL thickness decreased in both groups, the change in GCL-IPL thickness of the affected relative to the unaffected hemiretina was only significant in the IF-trained participants. Coupled with a failure to reject the null hypothesis in pre- to post-training differences of LIGCL-IPL in the BF-trained group, this suggests a subtle but significant effect of training location on GCL-IPL thinning within a given patient, which is lost when comparing effects across individuals. The inherent variability in OCT layer thickness in small cohorts makes it difficult to compare groups directly. This is further complicated by variability introduced due to time-dependent TRD. In the future, increasing the sample size to increase sensitivity is crucial to better understanding the anatomical underpinnings of visual retraining. This is a difficult endeavor with two critical limitations 1) CB participants with lesions limited to the occipital cortex are rare and challenging to recruit and 2) once recruited, CB participants require time-intensive testing and evaluation. Alleviating these limitations would require expansion of collaborating facilities and personnel, as well as relaxing inclusion and exclusion recruitment criteria, leading to a more heterogenous patient population. However, despite current limitations these within-group comparisons provide novel insights into training-dependent changes within the early visual pathway.
These surprising observations suggest first that OCT imaging and our derived LI metric is a sensitive biomarker for assessing the impact of training in post-stroke CB patients. Just as importantly, it also suggests that an intervention which locally stimulates RGCs in a retinal area deprived of several key central targets may benefit the structural integrity of these residual cells. In turn, this may increase the likelihood that these neurons are retained long-term in the residual visual circuitry, perhaps providing the neural substrates of training-induced recovery of visual functions seen deeper into the visual deficit51. Conversely, training within the intact field locally stimulates circuits that are not directly affected by V1 damage-mediated TRD26,29,43. Though V1 areas of both hemispheres representing visual information along the vertical meridian are connected via callosal axonal projections60, notable due to the training locations of these participants, these interhemispheric connections do not appear to provide enough benefit to anterior portion of the visual pathway to be observable at the level of the retina.
So, what could underlie the stabilization of LIGCL-IPL in BF-trained participants? As mentioned earlier, damaged RGCs undergo changes in their dendritic arbors in the IPL61,62 and in supporting cells, such as muller glia57,58,63, which span the entire thickness of the retina. Training in the BF could increase the energy demands of stimulated RGCs, and by consequence, of surrounding supporting cells, in turn causing structural changes manifested as a cell-size increase and/or shrinkage prevention36,57. Changes in surviving RGCs are of course likely occurring in tandem with RGC loss due to retrograde degeneration – a phenomenon on which visual training’s effects are unknown.
An important question emerging from the present results is whether the stabilization of the LIGCL-IPL persists after BF training stops. If this phenomenon relies on increased retinal activity due to training, it is possible that physiological mechanisms of TRD will eventually overcome the benefits gained once training ceases. However, it is also possible that if participants incorporate their regained visual abilities into everyday usage, they could maintain them and sustain their associated circuits.
Finally, we saw no significant changes in LIRNFL or RNFL thickness in either training cohort, although several factors likely limited our ability to detect such changes with OCT, including the anatomical complexities of the RNFL in different peripapillary zones, the very small volume of the RNFL overall, our relatively small sample size and inter-subject variability. Future studies using larger sample sizes, more detailed analyses and better imaging resolution will be required to rigorously elucidate the impact of training on the RNFL.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the present work investigated the impact of a visual training intervention administered either inside the blind or intact field of occipital stroke patients on the progression of TRD at the level of the inner retina. We found that relative thinning in the GCL-IPL and RNFL mirrored a distinct time-course post-stroke previously reported in the literature. Training for ~6 months with a motion discrimination task inside the blind hemifield appeared to block the progression of relative thinning in the ganglion cell complex. In contrast, this relative thinning proceeded unabated when training was administered to the intact field of vision. Our results provide the first evidence of a greater structural benefit in the retina for a behavioral intervention that stimulates circuitry impacted by V1 damage, over one that stimulates the intact circuitry.
Supplementary Material
Non-standard Abbreviations and Acronyms:
- CB
Cortical Blindness
- OCT
Optical Coherence Tomography
- LI
Laterality Index
- dLGN
dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus
- GCL-IPL
Ganglion Cell Layer-Inner Plexiform Layer
- RGC
Retinal Ganglion Cell
- HIS
Hemianopia Intervention Study
- HVF
Humphrey Visual Field
- OU
Oculus Uterque (both eyes)
- OS
Oculus Sinister (left eye)
- OD
Oculus Dextrus (right eye)
- SEM
Standard Error Mean
- PMD
Perimetric Mean Deviation
- STBF
Mean Sensitivity of Blind Field
- TRD
Trans-synaptic Retrograde Degeneration
Funding Statement
This work was supported by NIH funding (R01 EY027314 to KRH, as well as T32 EY007125 and P30 EY001319 to the Center for Visual Science), and by an Unrestricted Grant from the Research to Prevent Blindness (RPB) Foundation to the Flaum Eye Institute. The HIS clinical trial was funded by a Center of Emerging and Innovative Science for Empire State Development (project no. 1730C004), a Center of Excellence (project no. 1689bC2) and EnVision Solutions LLC. The sponsors and funding organizations had no role in the design or conduct of this research.
Footnotes
Commercial Relationships Disclosures: KRH is an inventor on US Patent No. 7,549,743. The remaining authors have no competing interests.
Disclosures: Berkeley K. Fahrenthold, None; Matthew R. Cavanaugh, None; Madhura Tamhankar, None; Byron L. Lam, None; Steven E. Feldon, None; Brent A. Johnson, None; Krystel R. Huxlin, inventor on US Patent No. 7,549,743 (P)
References
- 1.Gray CS, French JM, Bates D, Cartlidge NE, Venables GS, James OF. Recovery of visual fields in acute stroke: homonymous hemianopia associated with adverse prognosis. Age Ageing. 1989;18:419–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lin S, George BZ, Wilson-Holt NJ. Perimetric demonstration of spontaneous visual field recovery following occipital lobe haemorrhage. BMJ Case Rep. 2013;2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Zhang X, Kedar S, Lynn MJ, Newman NJ, Biousse V. Homonymous Hemianopia in Stroke. J. Neuroophthalmol. 2006;26:180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Zhang X, Kedar S, Lynn MJ, Newman NJ, Biousse V. Natural history of homonymous hemianopia. Neurology. 2006;66:901–905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Perez C, Chokron S. Rehabilitation of homonymous hemianopia: insight into blindsight. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2014;8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Peli E. Field expansion for homonymous hemianopia by optically induced peripheral exotropia. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2000;77:453–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sahraie A, Smania N, Zihl J. Use of NeuroEyeCoach™ to Improve Eye Movement Efficacy in Patients with Homonymous Visual Field Loss. BioMed Res. Int. 2016;2016:5186461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, Gerstman LJ, Lieberman A, Lakin P, Hodges G, Ezrachi O. Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1977;58:479–486. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pollock A, Hazelton C, Rowe FJ, Jonuscheit S, Kernohan A, Angilley J, Henderson CA, Langhorne P, Campbell P. Interventions for visual field defects in people with stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019;5:CD008388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bergsma DP, Elshout JA, van den Berg AV. Segregation of Spontaneous and Training Induced Recovery from Visual Field Defects in Subacute Stroke Patients. Front. Neurol. 2017;8:681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cavanaugh MR, Huxlin KR. Visual discrimination training improves Humphrey perimetry in chronic cortically induced blindness. Neurology. 2017;88:1856–1864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cavanaugh MR, Zhang R, Melnick MD, Das A, Roberts M, Tadin D, Carrasco M, Huxlin KR. Visual recovery in cortical blindness is limited by high internal noise. J. Vis. 2015;15:9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chokron S, Perez C, Obadia M, Gaudry I, Laloum L, Gout O. From blindsight to sight: cognitive rehabilitation of visual field defects. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2008;26:305–320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Das A, Tadin D, Huxlin KR. Beyond blindsight: properties of visual relearning in cortically blind fields. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:11652–11664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Huxlin KR, Martin T, Kelly K, Riley M, Friedman DI, Burgin WS, Hayhoe M. Perceptual relearning of complex visual motion after V1 damage in humans. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:3981–3991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Melnick MD, Tadin D, Huxlin KR. Relearning to See in Cortical Blindness. Neuroscientist. 2016;22:199–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Raninen A, Vanni S, Hyvärinen L, Näsänen R. Temporal sensitivity in a hemianopic visual field can be improved by long-term training using flicker stimulation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2007;78:66–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sahraie A, Macleod M-J, Trevethan CT, Robson SE, Olson JA, Callaghan P, Yip B. Improved detection following Neuro-Eye Therapy in patients with post-geniculate brain damage. Exp. Brain Res. 2010;206:25–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Saionz EL, Tadin D, Melnick MD, Huxlin KR. Functional preservation and enhanced capacity for visual restoration in subacute occipital stroke. Brain. 2020;143:1857–1872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Trevethan CT, Urquhart J, Ward R, Gentleman D, Sahraie A. Evidence for perceptual learning with repeated stimulation after partial and total cortical blindness. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2012;8:29–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Vaina LM, Soloviev S, Calabro FJ, Buonanno F, Passingham R, Cowey A. Reorganization of retinotopic maps after occipital lobe infarction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;26:1266–1282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wang V, Saionz E, Cavanaugh M, Huxlin K. Natural progression of perimetric visual field defects after V1 stroke. J. Vis. 2017;17:51–52. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Saionz EL, Cavanaugh MR, Johnson BA, Harrington D, Aguirre GK, Huxlin KR. The natural history of homonymous hemianopia revisited [Internet]. 2022. [cited 2023 Feb 16];2022.10.06.22280668. Available from: 10.1101/2022.10.06.22280668v1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beatty R, Sadun A, Smith L, Vonsattel J, Richardson E. Direct demonstration of transsynaptic degeneration in the human visual system: a comparison of retrograde and anterograde changes. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1982;45:143–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bridge H, Jindahra P, Barbur J, Plant GT. Imaging Reveals Optic Tract Degeneration in Hemianopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52:382–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cowey A, Stoerig P, Perry VH. Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells after damage to striate cortex in macaque monkeys: selective loss of P beta cells. Neuroscience. 1989;29:65–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cowey A, Alexander I, Stoerig P. Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells and optic tract in hemianopic monkeys and humans. Brain. 2011;134:2149–2157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Herro A, Lam B. Retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells in homonymous hemianopsia. Clin. Ophthalmol. 2015;1057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jindahra P, Petrie A, Plant GT. Retrograde trans-synaptic retinal ganglion cell loss identified by optical coherence tomography. Brain. 2009;132:628–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jindahra P, Petrie A, Plant GT. The time course of retrograde trans-synaptic degeneration following occipital lobe damage in humans. Brain. 2012;135:534–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Park H-YL, Park YG, Cho A-H, Park CK. Transneuronal Retrograde Degeneration of the Retinal Ganglion Cells in Patients with Cerebral Infarction. Ophthalmology. 2013;120:1292–1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Meier PG, Maeder P, Kardon RH, Borruat F-X. Homonymous Ganglion Cell Layer Thinning After Isolated Occipital Lesion: Macular OCT Demonstrates Transsynaptic Retrograde Retinal Degeneration. J. Neuroophthalmol. 2014;1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Millington RS, Yasuda CL, Jindahra P, Jenkinson M, Barbur JL, Kennard C, Cendes F, Plant GT, Bridge H. Quantifying the pattern of optic tract degeneration in human hemianopia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2014;85:379–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Yamashita T, Miki A, Goto K, Araki S, Takizawa G, Ieki Y, Kiryu J, Tabuchi A, Iguchi Y, Kimura K, et al. Retinal Ganglion Cell Atrophy in Homonymous Hemianopia due to Acquired Occipital Lesions Observed Using Cirrus High-Definition-OCT. J. Ophthalmol. 2016;2016:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fahrenthold BK, Cavanaugh MR, Jang S, Murphy AJ, Ajina S, Bridge H, Huxlin KR. Optic Tract Shrinkage Limits Visual Restoration After Occipital Stroke. Stroke. 2021;52:3642–3650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Schneider CL, Prentiss EK, Busza A, Matmati K, Matmati N, Williams ZR, Sahin B, Mahon BZ. Survival of retinal ganglion cells after damage to the occipital lobe in humans is activity dependent. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2019;286:9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Goto K, Miki A, Yamashita T, Araki S, Takizawa G, Nakagawa M, Ieki Y, Kiryu J. Sectoral analysis of the retinal nerve fiber layer thinning and its association with visual field loss in homonymous hemianopia caused by post-geniculate lesions using spectral-domain optical coherence tomography. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 2016;254:745–756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Keller J, Sánchez-Dalmau BF, Villoslada P. Lesions in the Posterior Visual Pathway Promote Trans-Synaptic Degeneration of Retinal Ganglion Cells. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mehta JS, Plant GT. Optical Coherence Tomography (OCT) Findings in Congenital/Long-Standing Homonymous Hemianopia. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 2005;140:727–729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mitchell JR, Oliveira C, Tsiouris AJ, Dinkin MJ. Corresponding Ganglion Cell Atrophy in Patients With Postgeniculate Homonymous Visual Field Loss: J. Neuroophthalmol. 2015;35:353–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Porrello G, Falsini B. Retinal ganglion cell dysfunction in humans following post-geniculate lesions: specific spatio–temporal losses revealed by pattern ERG. Vision Res. 1999;39:1739–1748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Shin H-Y, Park H-YL, Choi J-A, Park CK. Macular ganglion cell–inner plexiform layer thinning in patients with visual field defect that respects the vertical meridian. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 2014;252:1501–1507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Simmen CF, Fierz FC, Michels L, Aldusary N, Landau K, Piccirelli M, Traber GL. Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Volume Determined on MRI Correlates With Corresponding Ganglion Cell Layer Loss in Acquired Human Postgeniculate Lesions. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2022;63:18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Yamashita T, Miki A, Goto K, Araki S, Takizawa G, Ieki Y, Kiryu J, Tabuchi A, Iguchi Y, Kimura K, et al. Evaluation of Significance Maps and the Analysis of the Longitudinal Time Course of the Macular Ganglion Cell Complex Thicknesses in Acquired Occipital Homonymous Hemianopia Using Spectral-domain Optical Coherence Tomography. Neuro-Ophthalmol. 44:236–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zangerl B, Whatham A, Kim J, Choi A, Assaad NN, Hennessy MP, Kalloniatis M. Reconciling visual field defects and retinal nerve fibre layer asymmetric patterns in retrograde degeneration: an extended case series: Retinal nerve fibre layer defects in retrograde degeneration. Clin. Exp. Optom. 2017;100:214–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Saionz EL, Busza A, Huxlin KR. Chapter 25 - Rehabilitation of visual perception in cortical blindness [Internet]. In: Quartarone A, Ghilardi MF, Boller F, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2022. [cited 2023 Nov 15]. p. 357–373.Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128194102000308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Brown CE, Aminoltejari K, Erb H, Winship IR, Murphy TH. In Vivo Voltage-Sensitive Dye Imaging in Adult Mice Reveals That Somatosensory Maps Lost to Stroke Are Replaced over Weeks by New Structural and Functional Circuits with Prolonged Modes of Activation within Both the Peri-Infarct Zone and Distant Sites. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:1719–1734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Brown CE, Li P, Boyd JD, Delaney KR, Murphy TH. Extensive Turnover of Dendritic Spines and Vascular Remodeling in Cortical Tissues Recovering from Stroke. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:4101–4109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Harrison TC, Silasi G, Boyd JD, Murphy TH. Displacement of Sensory Maps and Disorganization of Motor Cortex After Targeted Stroke in Mice. Stroke. 2013;44:2300–2306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cavanaugh MR, Blanchard LM, McDermott M, Lam BL, Tamhankar M, Feldon SE. Efficacy of Visual Retraining in the Hemianopic Field after Stroke: Results of a Randomized Clinical Trial. Ophthalmology. 2021;128:1091–1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Barbot A, Das A, Melnick MD, Cavanaugh MR, Merriam EP, Heeger DJ, Huxlin KR. Spared perilesional V1 activity underlies training-induced recovery of luminance detection sensitivity in cortically-blind patients. Nat. Commun. 2021;12:6102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Jansonius NM, Schiefer J, Nevalainen J, Paetzold J, Schiefer U. A mathematical model for describing the retinal nerve fiber bundle trajectories in the human eye: Average course, variability, and influence of refraction, optic disc size and optic disc position. Exp. Eye Res. 2012;105:70–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cowey A, Alexander I, Stoerig P. Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells and optic tract in hemianopic monkeys and humans. Brain. 2011;134:2149–2157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cowey A. Atrophy of Retinal Ganglion Cells after Removal of Striate Cortex in a Rhesus Monkey. Perception. 1974;3:257–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Johnson H, Cowey A. Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells following restricted lesions of striate cortex in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 2000;132:269–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Buren JMV. Trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration in the visual system of primates. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1963;26:402–409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Davis BM, Guo L, Ravindran N, Shamsher E, Baekelandt V, Mitchell H, Bharath AA, De Groef L, Cordeiro MF. Dynamic changes in cell size and corresponding cell fate after optic nerve injury. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:21683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Miettinen TP, Björklund M. Mitochondrial Function and Cell Size: An Allometric Relationship. Trends Cell Biol. 2017;27:393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zhang X, Francis BA, Dastiridou A, Chopra V, Tan O, Varma R, Greenfield DS, Schuman JS, Huang D. Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Analyses of Age Effects on Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer and Ganglion Cell Complex Thickness by Fourier-Domain OCT. Transl. Vis. Sci. Technol. 2016;5:1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Saenz M, Fine I. Topographic Organization of V1 Projections through the Corpus Callosum in Humans. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1224–1229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.El-Danaf RN, Huberman AD. Characteristic Patterns of Dendritic Remodeling in Early-Stage Glaucoma: Evidence from Genetically Identified Retinal Ganglion Cell Types. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:2329–2343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Amato R, Catalani E, Dal Monte M, Cammalleri M, Cervia D, Casini G. Morpho-functional analysis of the early changes induced in retinal ganglion cells by the onset of diabetic retinopathy: The effects of a neuroprotective strategy. Pharmacol. Res. 2022;185:106516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Vecino E, Rodriguez FD, Ruzafa N, Pereiro X, Sharma SC. Glia–neuron interactions in the mammalian retina. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 2016;51:1–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.