Skip to main content
Psychoradiology logoLink to Psychoradiology
. 2023 Oct 10;3:kkad021. doi: 10.1093/psyrad/kkad021

A mini-review on how the COVID-19 pandemic affected intertemporal choice

Xinwen Zhang 1,#, Ziyun Wu 2,#, Qinghua He 3,4,
PMCID: PMC10917382  PMID: 38666127

Abstract

The coronavirus disease (COVID-19) has extremely harmful effects on individual lifestyles, and at present, people must make financial or survival decisions under the profound changes frequently. Although it has been reported that COVID-19 changed decision-making patterns, the underlying mechanisms remained unclear. This mini-review focuses on the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on intertemporal choice, and potential psychological, biological, and social factors that mediate this relationship. A search of the Web of Science electronic database yielded 23 studies. The results showed that under the COVID-19 pandemic, people tended to choose immediate and smaller rewards, and became less patient. In particular, people with negative emotions, in a worse condition of physical health, or who did not comply with their government restriction rules tended to become more "short-sighted" in behavioral terms. Future studies should examine more longitudinal and cross-cultural research to give a broad view about the decision-making change under the COVID-19 pandemic.

Keywords: COVID-19, intertemporal choice, biological factors, psychological factors, social factors

Introduction

In the face of the ongoing public health emergency posed by the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) (Torales et al., 2020; Velavan and Meyer, 2020), governments and individuals alike have had to make difficult decisions, such as whether to adopt stay-at-home restrictions to prevent future virus outbreaks, even though such methods reduce people's contact with each other (Pfefferbaum and North, 2020; Pokhrel and Chhetri, 2021).

Intertemporal choice, a concept from cognitive psychology, can be used to explain the decisions people make in situations such as that of the COVID-19 pandemic as 2020. Intertemporal choice involves evaluating the results of different decisions at several points in time, and the individual must weigh the cost and benefits of a later larger reward against sooner smaller rewards (Chabris et al., 2010). When people prefer immediate rewards over larger rewards in the future, this is known as delay discounting (Keidel et al., 2021).

The degree of delay discounting can vary between individuals and can be affected by experimental procedures or participants' emotions (Lempert and Phelps, 2016; Zhang and Ke, 2019). The COVID-19 pandemic has been linked to a decrease in public psychological well-being, as numerous emotional responses, such as stress or anxiety, have been observed (Coroiu et al., 2020). This has resulted in people becoming less capable of making rational decisions in the pandemic crisis. Research has indicated that during the pandemic, there was an increase in health-related decisions that had negative consequences, such as a rise in substance use and engaging in risky sexual activities. However, some studies have also demonstrated that during this time, people have made fewer decisions with immediate rewards but long-term negative outcomes. The lockdown strategy is a major factor in the disagreement about people's involvement in "short-sighted" behavior; individuals with substance addiction were isolated from their related substance supply, thus decreasing their high-risk behavior, and some of them turned to other high-risk behaviors such as excessive social media use or other impulsive decisions (Dubey et al., 2020). The lockdown strategy in the USA was successful in reducing after-school crime, however, serious battery, intimate partner violence, and homicide saw an increase (Boman and Gallupe, 2020). Nivette et al. (2021) found that the COVID-19 pandemic decreased some types of crime due to a decrease in opportunity, while the motivation of offenders remained unchanged (Boman and Mowen, 2021). Additionally, cultural factors can be considered to explain this discrepancy further. Studies have shown that in cultures that avoid uncertainty, individuals may display higher risk-taking tendencies to reduce the ambiguity caused by external factors (Pantano et al., 2021). As the effect of uncertainty differs from culture to culture, the same level of increasing uncertainty can lead to different patterns of behavior.

Research has found that the COVID-19 pandemic caused an increase in impulsive behavior, such as interpersonal violence, impulsive buying, and addiction. Worry about violence has been higher than before due to the pandemic and its related lessening of efforts (Kravitz-Wirtz et al., 2021). Panic buying has been a major influence on the US population's purchase behavior as a result of the risk of complete lockdown (Ahmed et al., 2020). Tobacco, electronic cigarette, and alcohol addictions have all increased, as well as behavioral addictions such as internet addiction (Dubey et al., 2020). Social media use has become more addictive, as individuals seek to cope with anxiety and uncontrollable feelings (Pollard et al., 2020). Furthermore, recreational screen time has also increased among adolescents in three Australian states (Gardner et al., 2022). This could be due to the compensatory effects of the internet, which was seen as an escape from reality and a means of coping with stress in the pandemic crisis (Haberlin and Atkin, 2022). Twenge and Campbell (2018) discovered a strong link between recreational screen time and low self-control, indicating that people tended to devote more time to leisure activities than what is considered "healthy": using the internet for educational or compulsive purposes (Babic et al., 2017). In addition, a study conducted in five countries showed an upward trend in the consumption of electronic cigarettes during the lockdown period, with nicotine users reporting the excessive use of such products to relieve stress and anxiety related to the pandemic (Yach, 2020). These maladaptive behaviors imply that the COVID-19 pandemic caused distress in individuals, resulting in impulsive thinking and behavior.

We are interested in exploring how the COVID-19 pandemic affected decision-making patterns. We are looking for the factors that have caused this change, which may not only be a shift in the trend, but could also be an increase in the same trend that was present before the pandemic. For example, obesity has always been linked to higher delay discounting (Miranda-Olivos et al., 2021). During lockdowns, people with obesity did less outdoor activity and had a more sedentary lifestyle, leading to an increase in weight for >50% of those affected (Sideli et al., 2021). This increase in weight could lead to even higher delay discounting, meaning that the pandemic has not changed the association, but has instead intensified it. Therefore, we are focusing on the increased effects that have been caused by the pandemic.

This review seeks to analyze the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on intertemporal choice, as well as the psychological factors that may be linked to such changes. The aim is to understand the effects and perceptions of COVID-19 on decision-making, and to pinpoint the factors that influence this variation.

Methods

A literature search was performed using the Web of Science electronic database. The retrieval method used was as follows: TS=(“delay discounting” OR “intertemporal choice” OR “monetary choice”) AND TI=(COVID-19 OR coronavirus), with the publication date ranging from 1 January 2020 to 31 August 2023. After initially finding 32 studies, nine were excluded as they were conference abstracts (n = 1), editorial material (n = 1), dissertations (n = 1), not empirical studies (n = 1), were focused on the discounting of compliance (n = 3) or test-reliability (n = 1), or used without an intertemporal choice task (n = 1). Ultimately, 23 studies were included (Table 1).

Table 1:

Studies included in this mini review.

References Country Study design Sample Measurement of ITC Measurement of the impact of COVID-19 Mediators and/or moderators Summary of findings
Agrawal et al. (2023) USA Cross-Sectional Experiments 12 906 adults Money Earlier or Later Task Stress and pandemic mitigation behaviors The log-transformed discounting rate was found to have a small but significant positive correlation with perceptions of health and financial stress. Moreover, in financial decision-making, social distancing was observed to be significantly linked to discount rates.
Brown et al. (2023) UK Cross-sectional experiments 240 smokers 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire COVID-19-related/traditional health warnings Smokers who demonstrated decreased delay discounting exhibited higher levels of subjective arousal when presented with COVID-19-related health warnings.
Byrne et al. (2021) USA Cross-sectional online questionnaire 404 adults Delay discounting task Beliefs regarding mask-wearing effectiveness Higher temporal discounting was associated with less suitable mask-wearing habits and social distancing.
Calluso et al. (2021) Italy Cross-sectional online questionnaire 353 adults 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Adherence to containment measures Perception of contagion risk The contagion risk index and individual discount rates had an inverse relationship, which was moderated by the individual's perception of contagion risk.
Cannito et al. (2021) Italy Cross-sectional experiments 100 adults 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Time estimation Evaluating surgical masks led to a stronger preference for immediate commodities over money.
Craft et al. (2022) USA Cross-sectional online questionnaire 267 adults Delay discounting task Perceived stress The discount rate was found to have a significant influence on the level of stress perceived in relation to COVID-19.
Crandall et al. (2022) USA Longitudinal online questionnaire A stratified cohort of families (N = 76 dyads) Delay discounting task Pandemic food insecurity During the pandemic, children were observed to demonstrate a greater tendency to discount delayed rewards in response to an increase in psychological stress.
DeAngelis et al. (2022) 96 countries Cross-sectional online questionnaire 3686 adults Monetary 5-trial delay discounting task Stress, stockpiling, physical distancing Stress had a positive impact on delay discounting, which was positively associated with stockpiling and negatively associated with physical distance.
Felton et al. (2022) USA Cross-sectional online questionnaire 72 adults 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Coronavirus impact scale Exposure to COVID-19 Social vulnerability and delay discounting were positively correlated.
Fiorenzato and Cona (2022) Italy Cross-sectional online questionnaire 586 adults Monetary 5-trial delay discounting task COVID-19 pandemic-related information Depression, anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty An increased rate of delay discounting was mainly linked to the interaction between older age and higher levels of financial insecurity, such as fear of job loss.
Halilova et al. (2022) 13 countries Cross-sectional online questionnaire 4452 adults Intertemporal choice procedure COVID-19 vaccination status Unvaccinated individuals showed a greater tendency to discount delayed rewards.
Hall et al. (2022) Canada Cross-sectional online questionnaire 1958 adults 5-trial delay discounting task SARS-CoV-2 infection status Young and middle-aged adults with a positive SARS-CoV-2 infection history and moderate to severe COVID-19 symptoms were more likely to exhibit amplified delay discounting than those without.
Hall et al. (2023) USA Cross-sectional/longitudinal experiments/online questionnaire 2122 adults Delay discounting task COVID-19 history, neurocognitive function, psychiatric symptoms Age, sex The rate of delay discounting was higher among those with a history of a symptomatic COVID-19 infection, with age and sex acting as moderators.
Hudson et al. (2023) Canada Cross-sectional online questionnaire 2002 adults 5-item delay discounting task Future orientation, executive function, mitigation behaviors, vaccination status Lower levels of delay discounting were linked to more frequent mask-wearing and full vaccination status.
Krawiec et al. (2022) Poland Cross-sectional online questionnaire 515 university students Monetary 5-trial delay discounting task Attitudes toward disinfection, distancing, and masks No link was established between public health policy and delay discounting.
Lloyd et al. (2021) UK Cross-sectional online questionnaire 442 adults 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire COVID-19 risk behaviors A steeper inclination to delay discounting was associated with poorer observance of social distancing measures.
SHEN et al. (2023) 18 countries Cross-sectional online questionnaire 26 355 adults Delay discounting tasks Self-rated survival achievement Culture The degree of change in intertemporal choice for different currencies and the degree of change in intertemporal choice at different stages of Chinese and Singaporeans could be used to jointly predict their self-rated survival achievement.
Sonmez (2021) Turkey Cross-sectional online questionnaire 167 adults Hypothetical choices COVID-19 related/classical mortality salience manipulation Contemplating mortality led to a greater inclination to discount future rewards.
Strickland et al. (2022) USA Longitudinal online questionnaire 333 adults Monetary Choice Questionnaire Vaccination intent, vaccination status, probability discounting, health variables A greater inclination to discount future rewards and costs was associated with a lower likelihood of receiving vaccinations.
Wang et al. (2022) China Cross-sectional online questionnaire 491 adults 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Emotion when thinking they may be infected by COVID-19 Likelihood estimates of being infected The group that experienced anticipatory emotions exhibited a lower rate of delay discounting.
Wismans et al. (2021) Belgium, France, Ireland, Italy, the Netherlands, Sweden, Portugal Cross-sectional online questionnaire 6759 students Trial adjusting delay discounting task Compliance behavior and impulsivity Self-reported impulsivity had a negative correlation with compliance behavior, whereas discounting rate had a weak but positive relationship with compliance behavior.
Wu (2022) China Cross-sectional online questionnaire 363 college students Choice titration procedure with 19 choice trials Recall the uncertainty feeling related to COVID-19 Future orientation The future orientation of an individual was a mediator between the feelings of uncertainty and the tendency to discount delayed rewards.
Xiao et al. (2022) China Longitudinal online questionnaire 683 college students 27-item Monetary Choice Questionnaire Brain functional connectivity unique to stress changes under COVID-19 Males exhibited a greater discount than females, and the functional connectome was able to forecast this difference.

COVID-19 pandemic and intertemporal choice

During the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals’ decision-making had been affected by psychological, biological, and social factors (Fig. 1). Studies have revealed that individuals experienced negative emotions such as uncertainty, fear, sadness, disgust, and anger when facing the COVID-19 situation (Wang et al., 2022; Wu et al., 2022). Those with higher levels of uncertainty chose immediate rewards (Wu et al., 2022). From a psychological perspective, it has been found that social vulnerability is positively correlated with delay discounting (Felton et al., 2022), and a weak but positive association between self-reported impulsivity and compliance behavior (Wismans et al., 2021) has been identified. Moreover, they demonstrated that brain connectivity could predict their stress levels, which may lead to choices of smaller rewards sooner. Additionally, Calluso et al. (2021) found a negative correlation between compliance with containment measures and intertemporal discounting. As such, the following sections will further explore these factors in detail.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

A brief outline of the key elements that have an influence on intertemporal decisions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social, biological, and psychological factors are all included.

Psychological factors

There were three studies demonstrating that when individuals were exposed to uncertain conditions, they tended to opt for immediate rewards (Wu et al., 2022; Li et al., 2015). This was likely due to the psychological state of not knowing (Kuang, 2018), or uncertainty distress, that accounts for the attempt to understand unclear conditions (Freeston et al., 2020). During the COVID-19 pandemic, people were still uncertain about the level of protection provided by vaccines, when restrictions would be lifted, and when employees could return to work (Koffman et al., 2020; Szczygielski et al., 2022). This resulted in that individual being more likely to choose smaller, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards.

Previous research has indicated a positive correlation between perceived stress and delay discounting in both adolescents and adults (Craft et al., 2022). Moreover, the discounting rate has been identified as a significant predictor of perceived stress (Craft et al., 2022). The COVID-19 pandemic has further highlighted this association, with studies suggesting an increase in stress levels leading to greater delay discounting (DeAngelis et al., 2022; Agrawal et al., 2023). Notably, children were found to display higher levels of delay discounting due to elevated stress during the pandemic (Crandall et al., 2022).

In the face of the rapidly spreading COVID-19 pandemic, individuals have experienced heightened levels of fear, disgust, anger, and sadness. Fear and disgust are innate emotions that can be adaptive in terms of increasing individual survival, but can also be maladaptive when individuals overreact to the threat (Luo et al., 2021; Miłkowska et al., 2021). Milkowska et al. (2021) found that Polish women reported higher levels of disgust in response to sources of infection during the pandemic in 2020 compared to 2017. Similarly, Fitzpatrick et al. (2020) reported that participants rated an average 7 out of 10 in terms of fear related to the COVID-19 pandemic in the USA. Schimmenti et al. (2020) proposed an integrated model of fear experience during the pandemic, which included fear of the body, fear of significant others, fear of not knowing, and fear of inaction, According to Fiorenzato et al. (2022), a more pronounced rate of delay discounting was linked to a heightened fear of job loss. Studies have indicated that fear, disgust, anger, and sadness are also the most common emotional responses to traumatic events (Trnka and Lorencova, 2020). The outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic, coupled with a lack of knowledge and misunderstanding of government actions, has caused public anger (Malakoutikhah et al., 2021). Smith et al. (2021) found that 56% of participants in the UK expressed anger due to the pandemic. Another study showed that 40% of Brazilian respondents experienced sadness or depression during the pandemic (Barros et al., 2020). This preference toward smaller, immediate rewards during experiences of negative emotions could be explained by emotion regulation strategies, which suggest that people use immediate rewards to compensate for negative emotions (Fichman et al., 1999).

A potential direction of future study involves conducting longitudinal studies to better understand how emotions have changed over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic and how these emotions affect intertemporal choice. Additionally, studies could investigate the underlying mechanisms that cause shifts in intertemporal decision. It has been suggested that people are inherently more sensitive to losses than gains to protect themselves and avoid harm (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Therefore, future studies could explore whether individuals experiencing negative emotions during the pandemic are particularly sensitive to losses.

Biological factors

Two studies have measured biological and physical changes related to the COVID-19 pandemic, and the results have demonstrated that those with worse health conditions exhibited more impulsive behaviors (Xiao et al., 2022). Findings suggest that unvaccinated individuals display higher rates of delay discounting than vaccinated individuals (Halilova et al., 2022). Furthermore, a negative correlation has been observed between delay discounting and the likelihood of vaccination (Strickland et al., 2022). Age and sex are identified as moderators of the relationship between symptomatically experienced COVID-19 infection and delay discounting rate, with older adult females displaying the lowest discounting rate (Hall et al., 2023). Additionally, Hall et al. (2022) found that infection history of COVID-19 and symptom severity are positively associated with the delay discounting rate among young and middle-aged adults. Finally, Brown et al. (2023) observed that graphic health warnings on cigarette packets, particularly those related to COVID-19, elicited greater subjective arousal ratings in smokers with lower delay discounting, whereas those with higher delay discounting showed no significant effect.

A study of 2000 school children aged 2 to 19 years in the USA showed that BMI increased during the COVID-19 pandemic compared to previous years (Knapp et al., 2022), thus making obesity a highlighted concern of the COVID-19 pandemic (Soeroto et al., 2020). Xiao et al. (2022) measured individual differences in the brain functional connectivity network prior to the pandemic, and the results showed that consensus functional connectivity network strength declined in relation to the increase in delay discounting during the pandemic. As the frontal parietal network is unique to stress, it was found to be an essential factor influencing the stress brought by the pandemic. Therefore, those with limited strength in their frontal parietal network were more likely to feel stress during the pandemic and assumed self-control to defend against the stress, which elicited them to make more immediate choices (Xiao et al., 2022).

Further studies are necessary to investigate the biological factors associated with intertemporal choice. For example, cortisol is often referred to as the primary stress hormone, which is an evolutionarily response to “fight or flight.” During the COVID-19 pandemic, people were likely to experience heightened levels of stress, which may have led to an increase in cortisol levels. Future studies should measure cortisol levels while using methods to elicit individuals' emotions about the COVID-19 pandemic to gain a better understanding of the physical changes people experienced as well as the associated brain structural and functional changes during crisis events, and whether these changes affected their decision-making abilities.

Social factors

Containment measures such as wearing masks, implementing lockdowns, and encouraging compliance have been shown to influence intertemporal choice during the COVID-19 pandemic. The World Health Organization (WHO) has proposed two strategies to combat the virus: reducing its circulation and treating it as soon as possible to reduce deaths. To this end, many countries put restrictions in place, such as lockdowns, to contain the virus and, as a result, alter people's lifestyles (Coroiu et al., 2020). Studies have suggested that inadequate mask-wearing behavior and social distancing are associated with a greater degree of temporal discounting (Byrne et al., 2021; DeAngelis et al., 2022). Experiments have demonstrated that when evaluating surgical masks, people showed a stronger preference for immediate commodities compared to value for money (Cannito et al., 2021). Additionally, research has indicated that in financial decision-making, the degree of social distancing, but not mask use, was statistically significantly correlated with discount rates (Agrawal et al., 2023). Studies have indicated that individuals who adhered to containment regulations more strictly were more likely to prefer immediate rewards in the intertemporal choice task (Calluso et al., 2021; Lloyd et al., 2021). This could be attributed to the desire for freedom that could only be enjoyed in the present (Calluso et al., 2021). However, other research has not found a connection between public health policy and delay discounting (Krawiec et al., 2022). To further explore the underlying mechanism, Studies reported a negative relationship between compliance with containment measures and intertemporal discounting (Coroiu et al., 2020; Calluso et al., 2021). Additionally, studies have revealed that delay discounting is inversely associated with mask-wearing behavior and complete vaccination status (Hudson et al., 2023). Furthermore, increased death-related thoughts have been linked to a tendency to choose present-oriented rewards (Sonmez, 2021). It is possible to conduct further research into cross-culture differences in response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Such research could compare the various policies adopted by different countries and the attitudes of individuals toward those policies. Simultaneously, it could provide insight into how decision-making has changed in different countries due to the pandemic. Furthermore, it could take into account the impact of national economic conditions and individual socioeconomic status on intertemporal choices. Finally, such research could help governments to implement more effective interventions, encouraging people to think long-term and learn from the pandemic.

Conclusion

This study investigated that intertemporal choice patterns changed during the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as its underlying mechanism. The results showed that under the COVID-19 pandemic, people tended to choose immediate and smaller rewards, and became less patient. Our findings explored that negative emotions, being in worse physical and biological healthy condition, and incompliance with the social restriction rules made people become behaviorally "short-sighted." This review focused on the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on intertemporal decision-making, yet gave little attention to other pandemics. To gain a better understanding of the effects and mechanisms of pandemics, and to find out the related factors that could lead to better management and coping strategies, future studies should compare COVID-19 to other past pandemics. Furthermore, longitudinal, and cross-cultural research should also be included to provide a comprehensive view of the changes in decision-making during the pandemic .).

Acknowledgement

This work was supported by research grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31972906), Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (SWU2209235), the Innovation Research 2035 Pilot Plan of Southwest University (SWUPilotPlan006), and the Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (CNLZD2102).

Contributor Information

Xinwen Zhang, Faculty of Psychology, MOE Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Southwest University, CN400715,Chongqing, China.

Ziyun Wu, Faculty of Psychology, MOE Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Southwest University, CN400715,Chongqing, China.

Qinghua He, Faculty of Psychology, MOE Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Southwest University, CN400715,Chongqing, China; Southwest University Branch, Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment toward Basic Education Quality, CN400715, Chongqing, China.

Author contributions

Xinwen Zhang (Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing), Ziyun Wu (Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing), and Qinghua He (Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing)

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

References

  1. Agrawal M, Peterson JC, Cohen JDet al. (2023) Stress, intertemporal choice, and mitigation behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Exp Psychol Gen. 152:2695–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahmed RR, Streimikiene D, Rolle JAet al. (2020) The COVID-19 pandemic and the antecedants for the impulse buying behavior of US citizens. J Compet. 12:5–27. [Google Scholar]
  3. Babic MJ, Smith JJ, Morgan PJet al. (2017) Longitudinal associations between changes in screen-time and mental health outcomes in adolescents. Ment Health Phys Act. 12:124–31. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barros MBDA, Lima MG, Malta DCet al. (2020) Report on sadness/depression, nervousness/anxiety and sleep problems in the Brazilian adult population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Epidemiologia e Serviços De Saúde. 29:e2020427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boman IV,JH, Mowen TJ. (2021) Global crime trends during COVID-19. Nat Hum Behaviour. 5:821–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boman JH, Gallupe O (2020) Has COVID-19 changed crime? Crime rates in the United States during the pandemic. Am J Criminal Justice. 45:537–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown CR, Faulkner P (2023) Smokers’ affective responses to COVID-19-related health warnings on cigarette packets: the influence of delay discounting. Nicotine Tob Res. 25:221–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Byrne KA, Six SG, Anaraky RGet al. (2021) Risk-taking unmasked: using risky choice and temporal discounting to explain COVID-19 preventative behaviors. PLoS ONE. 16:e0251073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Calluso C, Devetag MG, Donato C (2021) “I feel therefore I decide”: effect of negative emotions on temporal discounting and probability discounting. Brain Sci. 11:1407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cannito L, Anzani S, Bortolotti Aet al. (2021) Temporal discounting of money and face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic: the role of hoarding level. Front Psychol. 12:642102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chabris CF, Laibson DI, Schuldt JP (2010) Intertemporal choice. In Behavioural and Experimental Economics. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 168–77. [Google Scholar]
  12. Coroiu A, Moran C, Campbell Tet al. (2020) Barriers and facilitators of adherence to social distancing recommendations during COVID-19 among a large international sample of adults. PLoS One. 15:e0239795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Craft WH, Tegge AN, Bickel WK (2022) Chronic pain and COVID-19: the association of delay discounting with perceived stress and pain severity. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 30:692–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crandall AK, Madhudi N, Osborne Bet al. (2022) The effect of food insecurity and stress on delay discounting across families: a COVID-19 natural experiment. BMC Public Health. 22:1576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. DeAngelis BN, Ben Salah A, Al'Absi M. (2022) Stress and COVID-19 related behaviours: the mediating role of delay discounting. Stress Health. 38:140–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dubey MJ, Ghosh R, Chatterjee Set al. (2020) COVID-19 and addiction. Diabetes Metab Syndr: Clin Res Rev. 14:817–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Felton JW, Rabinowitz JA, Strickland JCet al. (2022) Social vulnerability, COVID-19 impact, and decision making among adults in a low-resource community. Behav Processes. 200:104668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fichman L, Koestner R, Zuroff DC, Gordon L (1999) Depressive styles and the regulation of negative affect: a daily experience study. Cogn Ther Res. 23:483–95. [Google Scholar]
  19. Fiorenzato E, Cona G (2022) One-year into COVID-19 pandemic: decision-making and mental-health outcomes and their risk factors. J Affect Disord. 309:418–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fitzpatrick KM, Harris C, Drawve G (2020) Fear of COVID-19 and the mental health consequences in America. Psychol Trauma: Theory Res Pract Policy. 12:S17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Freeston M, Tiplady A, Mawn Let al. (2020) Towards a model of uncertainty distress in the context of coronavirus (COVID-19). Cogn Behav Ther. 13:e31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gardner LA, Debenham J, Newton NCet al. (2022) Lifestyle risk behaviours among adolescents: a two-year longitudinal study of the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic. BMJ Open. 12:e060309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Haberlin KA, Atkin DJ (2022) Mobile gaming and Internet addiction: when is playing no longer just fun and games?. Comput Hum Behav. 126:106989. [Google Scholar]
  24. Halilova JG, Fynes-Clinton S, Green Let al. (2022) Short-sighted decision-making by those not vaccinated against COVID-19. Sci Rep. 12:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hall PA, Ayaz H, Meng Get al. (2023) Neurocognitive and psychiatric symptoms following infection with COVID-19: evidence from laboratory and population studies. Brain Behav Immun Health. 28:100595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hall PA, Meng G, Hudson Aet al. (2022) Cognitive function following SARS-CoV-2 infection in a population-representative Canadian sample. Brain Behav Immun Health. 21:100454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hudson A, Hall PA, Hitchman SCet al. (2023) Cognitive predictors of COVID-19 mitigation behaviors in vaccinated and unvaccinated general population members. Vaccine. 41:4019–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1979) Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 47:263–92. [Google Scholar]
  29. Keidel K, Rramani Q, Weber Bet al. (2021) Individual differences in intertemporal choice. Front Psychol. 12:991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Knapp EA, Dong Y, Dunlop ALet al. (2022) Changes in BMI during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pediatrics. 150:e2022056552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Koffman J, Gross J, Etkind SNet al. (2020) Uncertainty and COVID-19: how are we to respond?. J R Soc Med. 113:211–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kravitz-Wirtz N, Aubel A, Schleimer Jet al. (2021) Public concern about violence, firearms, and the COVID-19 pandemic in California. JAMA Network Open. 4:e2033484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Krawiec JM, Mizak S, Tagliabue Met al. (2022) Delay discounting of money and health outcomes, and adherence to policy guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Public Health. 10:953743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kuang K (2018) Reconceptualizing uncertainty in illness: commonalities, variations, and the multidimensional nature of uncertainty. Ann Int Commun Assoc. 42:181–206. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lempert KM, Phelps EA (2016) The malleability of intertemporal choice. Trends Cogn Sci. 20:64–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Li J, Huang RH, Zeng XQ (2015) The effects of tolerance of uncertainty on intertemporal choices and its context-dependency. J Psychol Sci. 38:680–5. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lloyd A, McKay R, Hartman TKet al. (2021) Delay discounting and under-valuing of recent information predict poorer adherence to social distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Rep. 11:19237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Luo F, Ghanei Gheshlagh R, Dalvand Set al. (2021) Systematic review and meta-analysis of fear of COVID-19. Front Psychol. 12:1311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Malakoutikhah A, Zakeri MA, Derakhtanjani ASet al. (2021) Anxiety, anger, and mindfulness as predictors of general health in the general population during COVID-19 outbreak: a survey in southeast Iran. J Community Psychol. 50:916–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miłkowska K, Galbarczyk A, Mijas Met al. (2021) Disgust sensitivity among women during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front Psychol. 12:622634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miranda-Olivos R, Steward T, Martínez-Zalacaín Iet al. (2021) The neural correlates of delay discounting in obesity and binge eating disorder. J Behav Addict. 10:498–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nivette AE, Zahnow R, Aguilar Ret al. (2021) A global analysis of the impact of COVID-19 stay-at-home restrictions on crime. Nat Hum Behav. 5:868–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pantano E, Priporas CV, Devereux Let al. (2021) Tweets to escape: intercultural differences in consumer expectations and risk behavior during the COVID-19 lockdown in three European countries. J Bus Res. 130:59–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pfefferbaum B, North CS (2020) Mental health and the COVID-19 pandemic. N Engl J Med. 383:510–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pokhrel S, Chhetri R (2021) A literature review on impact of COVID-19 pandemic on teaching and learning. High Educ Future. 8:133–41. [Google Scholar]
  46. Pollard MS, Tucker JS, Green HD (2020) Changes in adult alcohol use and consequences during the COVID-19 pandemic in the US. JAMA Network Open. 3:e2022942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schimmenti A, Billieux J, Starcevic V (2020) The four horsemen of fear: an integrated model of understanding fear experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 17:41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. SHEN SC, Khishignyam BAZARVAANI, DING Yet al. (2023) Changes in the intertemporal choice of people in or close to Chinese culture can predict their self-rated surviving achievement in the fight against COVID-19: a cross-national study in 18 Asian, African, European, American, and Oceanian countries. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 55:435. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sideli L, Lo Coco G, Bonfanti RCet al. (2021) Effects of COVID-19 lockdown on eating disorders and obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 29:826–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Smith LE, Duffy B, Moxham-Hall Vet al. (2021) Anger and confrontation during the COVID-19 pandemic: a national cross-sectional survey in the UK. J R Soc Med. 114:77–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Soeroto AY, Soetedjo NN, Purwiga Aet al. (2020) Effect of increased BMI and obesity on the outcome of COVID-19 adult patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Diabetes Metab Syndr Clin Res Rev. 14:1897–904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sonmez F (2021) The effect of COVID-19-induced mortality salience on delay discounting: a replication. J Econ Sci Assoc. 7:159–66. [Google Scholar]
  53. Strickland JC, Reed DD, Dayton Let al. (2022) Behavioral economic methods predict future COVID-19 vaccination. Transl Behav Med. 12:1004–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Szczygielski JJ, Charteris A, Bwanya PRet al. (2022) The impact and role of COVID-19 uncertainty: a global industry analysis. Int Rev Financial Anal. 80:101837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Torales J, O'Higgins M, Castaldelli-Maia JMet al. (2020) The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 66:317–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Trnka R, Lorencova R (2020) Fear, anger, and media-induced trauma during the outbreak of COVID-19 in the Czech Republic. Psychol Trauma: Theory Res Pract Policy. 12:546–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Twenge JM, Campbell WK (2018) Associations between screen time and lower psychological well-being among children and adolescents: evidence from a population-based study. Prev Med Rep. 12:271–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Velavan TP, Meyer CG (2020) The COVID-19 epidemic. Trop Med Int Health. 25:278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wang X, Wang P, Lu Jet al. (2022) Episodic future thinking and anticipatory emotions: effects on delay discounting and preventive behaviors during COVID-19. Appl Psychol: Health Well-Being. 14:842–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wismans A, Letina S, Wennberg Ket al. (2021) The role of impulsivity and delay discounting in student compliance with COVID-19 protective measures. Pers Individ Differ. 179:110925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wu X, Li J, Li Y (2022) The impact of uncertainty induced by the COVID-19 pandemic on intertemporal choice. J Exp Soc Psychol. 103:104397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Xiao Z, Chen Z, Chen Wet al. (2022) Maladaptive changes in delay discounting in males during the COVID-19 pandemic: the predictive role of functional connectome. Cereb Cortex. 32:4605–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Yach D (2020) Tobacco use patterns in five countries during the COVID-19 lockdown. Nicotine Tob Res. 22:1671–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zhang H, Ke Y (2019) The influence of emotion on inter-temporal choice: based on evaluation tendency framework theory. Can Soc Sci. 15:53–61. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Psychoradiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES