Abstract
Filial imprinting, a crucial ethological paradigm, provides insights into the neurobiology of early learning and its long-term impact on behaviour. To date, only invasive techniques, such as autoradiography or lesion, have been employed to understand this behaviour. The primary limitation of these methods lies in their constrained access to the entire brain, impeding the exploration of brain networks crucial at various stages of this paradigm. Recently, advances in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in the avian brain have opened new windows to explore bird’s brain function at the network level. Here, we developed a ground-breaking non-invasive functional MRI technique for awake, newly hatched chicks that record whole-brain BOLD signal changes throughout imprinting experiments. While the initial phases of memory acquisition imprinting behaviour have been unravelled, the long-term storage and retrieval components of imprinting memories are still unknown. Our findings identified potential long-term storage of imprinting memories across a neural network, including the hippocampal formation, the medial striatum, the arcopallium, and the prefrontal-like nidopallium caudolaterale. This platform opens up new avenues for exploring the broader landscape of learning and memory processes in neonatal vertebrates, contributing to a more comprehensive understanding of the intricate interplay between behaviour and brain networks.
Keywords: fMRI, imprinting, brain, learning, memory acquisition, retrieval
Introduction
Filial imprinting is a learning process by which the young of some organisms can learn about a conspicuous object, usually the mother or siblings, by simply being exposed to it for a short period of time soon after birth1. It owes its great popularity to the work of Nobel-prize-winning ethologist Konrad Lorenz2, but it was originally described by Douglas Spalding3 in the offspring of some nidifugous (precocial) bird species, such as chicks or ducklings (see 4). Visual imprinting has been mostly studied, though acoustic or olfactory imprinting can be observed as well, the latter being prominent in mammals5.
Although in principle visual imprinting can occur with any kind of object, research has shown that the process is actually assisted by a set of biological predispositions which guides an animal’s attention towards those object features that are more likely to be observed in social partners - e.g., preferences in domestic chicks include simple features such red colour (which is prominently observed in the head region of conspecifics), or self-propelled motion (which is typical of living things), as well as more complex assembly of features such as face-like stimuli or biological motion in point-light displays (review in 4,6). Brain research has shown that biological predispositions are associated with the activation of areas of the so-called Social Behavior Network, and in particular of the lateral septum for motion stimuli and of the nucleus taenia (homologous of the mammalian medial amygdala7) for face-like stimuli (review in6).
Interest in filial imprinting quickly spanned from behavioural biology to psychological development and psychopathology, inspiring, for instance, John Bowlby’s theory of attachment, which postulates a crucial role of the mother-child bond for subsequent psychological development and, complementarily, the psychiatric outcomes associated with early mother deprivation (recent reviews in 8,9).
In the 70’s filial imprinting served as a model for the neurobiological investigation of memory. Gabriel Horn and colleagues (review in 10) identified an associative brain region involved in the formation of an imprinting memory, the intermediate medial mesopallium (IMM according to the new avian brain nomenclature; previously referred to as IMHV, intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale11,12). IMM proved to be crucial during the acquisition phase of the visual imprinting memory. More precisely, it was found that exposure to the imprinting object was associated with changes in the left but not in the right IMM13,14. Subsequent studies with auditory imprinting revealed that the imprinting-related area extended ventrally into a medialmost nidopallial area, the nidopallium medial pars medialis (NMm)15,16. Here we will use the label medial nidopallium/mesopallium (MNM) to jointly label the mesopallial and nidopallial entity of the imprinting area.
Experiments involving sequential lesions, first to one side of IMM and subsequently to the other17,18, suggested that the store in the left IMM is only temporary, and the right IMM is implicated in transferring information from the left IMM to another, unknown brain region dubbed S’, and that this transfer appears to be complete within 6 h after the end of exposure19. Thus, to cite Gabriel Horn’s words “We are still some ways from being able to visualize, through the microscope or by using brain imaging techniques, the neural trace of (imprinting) memory”10. To overcome the technical limitations, recent advancements in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) turned it into a cornerstone neuroscientific technique. This powerful, non-invasive procedure serves as an indirect measure of neuronal activity throughout the entire brain, offering a comprehensive perspective at the network level. It appears particularly well-suited to finally find the so-called S’, being it a region or a neural network. To this end, we here developed an awake fMRI platform to explore the imprinting network and the long-term store of imprinting memories in newly-hatched chicks.
We exposed (imprinted) chicks on either a preferred (red) or a non-preferred (blue) colour. After exposure, awake chicks were tested with a sequence alternating the two colours in the scanner. We could demonstrate that chicks imprinted on red colour showed activity in pallial and subpallial brain regions involved with storage and memory retrieval, such as the medial striatum, the arcopallium, the hippocampus, and the nidopallium caudolaterale (a presumed avian equivalent of mammalian prefrontal cortex). Surprisingly, chicks imprinted on blue showed little or no activity in the same regions. However, exploratively we could show that blue-imprinted chicks might have started a process of secondary imprinting as a result of the exposure to the preferred red colour inside the scanner. The results indicated an early activation of mesopallium, as well as a precocial involvement of the Social Behavior Network during the first exposure to a predisposed feature, such as the colour red. We thus, first, established a reliable platform to investigate the long-term imprinting memory. Second, our results might shed light on the so-called S’, the neural basis of the long-term imprinting memory storage which was unknown up to now.
Results
The present study aimed to better understand the neural networks underlying the different learning stages of filial imprinting: memory acquisition, long-term memory storage, and retrieval. To tackle these ambitious questions, we decided to establish a fully non-invasive awake fMRI protocol for newborn chicks. Using the awake fMRI platform, we were able to capture dynamic neural processes in real-time of imprinting memory at the whole brain level, allowing us to observe and analyse the intricate interplay of brain regions involved in filial imprinting memory without interfering with the natural state of the subjects.
Establishment of a fully non-invasive and awake fMRI for the chicks
To enable whole-brain fMRI acquisition in awake chicks, we developed a fully non-invasive set-up to minimise head and body movements (Figure 1B). Before fMRI scans, chicks were imprinted for two days on either a preferred red or a non-preferred blue light ball20. Before scanning, chicks were habituated to the scanner noise using a playback of the magnet noise (Figure 1A). On the third day, after wrapping the animal in a paper tissue to avoid any body-part movements (such as wings and legs), blocks of plasteline were used to comfortably fixate the head, minimising movements and scanner’s noise by covering their ears (Figure 1B).
To record the spontaneous resting-state (to evaluate the stability and reliability of the head fixation system) and task-based BOLD signals, a single-shot multi-slice rapid acquisition with relaxation enhancement (RARE) sequence was adopted from Behroozi et al 21–23. Voxel-wise signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and temporal SNR (tSNR) were calculated over the resting-state (rs-fMRI) and task-based fMRI (tb-fMRI) scans respectively. The tSNR of the RARE sequence in each voxel was calculated after applying motion correction and high-pass temporal filtering (cut-off at 120s) to remove any linear drift. Temporal SNR in the entire telencephalon ranged from 50 to 100 (Figure S1A, B) for both tb- and rs-fMRI scans. Furthermore, the result indicated highly correlated SNR and tSNR for both rs- and tb-fMRI scans (Figure S1C, D).
In order to verify that adequate fixation was achieved during fMRI scans, we used the realignment parameters and the results of the frame-wise displacement (FD) to evaluate the amount of head motion (Figure S2). Overall, the custom-made restrainer yielded a low level of head movements. There were only 2.02 % (218 volumes) and 1,08% (19 volumes) of fMRI volumes with FD higher than 0.2 mm (~40% of voxel size) over all subjects in the task-based and resting-state experiments, respectively (Figure S2A). The median of frame-wise displacement was ~0.03 mm for both tb-fMRI and rs-fMRI experiments. However, most head movements occurred in the y-direction (Figure 2B, C). The respective violin plot information for translations in the y-direction is as follow: tb-fMRI: max/min = 0.22/−0.31 and median ~ 0; and rs-fMRI: max/min = 0.28/−0.30 and median ~ 0. The higher motion parameters in the y-direction were most likely due to the design of the head restrainer, which allowed movements in the dorsoventral direction to avoid blocking the throat.
Distinct BOLD response to identify the acquisition and long-term storage of imprinting memory
We recorded the whole brain BOLD signals from 17 head-restrained awake chicks already imprinted to either a preferred colour, red (n = 9), or a non-preferred colour, blue (n = 8). During fMRI scanning, animals were presented with both colours (Figure 1C), which depending on the previous imprinting training could represent either the imprinted or the control colour. The two colours were presented in a block design manner and a pseudo-random order (48 trials, 24 per condition, see Methods). For the preferred colour group, the imprinting colour (Imp) was red and the control (Cont) was blue, while for the non-preferred colour group the imprinting colour was blue and the control red. To identify the long-term storage of imprinting memory, we first used the contrast of Imp > Cont by combining both groups in a conventional generalised linear model (GLM) based statistical analysis. The first-level results at the single-subject level were then entered into a second-level analysis (random-effect modelling, Z = 2.3 and p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE)) to illustrate the activation clusters at different networks of chick prosencephalon.
Before fMRI scans, chicks were exposed to the imprinting stimulus for 2 days, during which they learned the feature of the imprinting object and stored them as a long-term memory1. Therefore, we expected to find activation in regions involved in memory retrieval. Surprisingly, RedImp + BlueImp > BlueCont + RedCont contrast showed no significantly activated cluster in the chick brain. The activation patterns for both contrasts, RedImp + BlueImp > baseline and BlueCont + RedCont > baseline, were highly similar (Figure 2A). To get to the bottom of this interference, we examined the interaction between the group factor and the Red vs. Blue contrast by analysing the following contrasts: RedImp vs. BlueImp, BlueCont vs. RedCont, RedImp vs. RedCont, and BlueImp vs. BlueCont. As illustrated in Figures 2B and 2C, robust BOLD activation patterns were found within the telencephalon for the contrasts: RedImp > BlueImp and BlueCont < RedCont contrasts. In addition, the RedImp vs. RedCont, and BlueImp vs. BlueCont contrasts demonstrated no significant differences between the different conditions, same colour serving as the imprinting or control stimulus.
To comprehensively investigate the underlying mechanisms behind this discrepancy, we conducted a meticulously designed behavioral experiment aimed at controlling the influence of color on the chick’s preferred choice. As represented in Figure 3, we found no significant difference in the colour preference between the two groups (two-tailed independent sample t-test: t(22)=1.601, p=0.124, d=0.654; mean ± se Red group: 0.718 ± 0.068; Blue group: 0.558 ± 0.072). A significant preference for red was detected in both groups together (two-tailed independent sample t-test: t(23)=2.683, p=0.013, d=0.548; 0.638 ± 0.051). These results confirmed the presence of no significant differences between the Red and the Blue imprinted groups with regard to the preference for the red stimulus. These results might support the idea that Blue imprinted chicks exposed to the preferred colour red immediately started a process of secondary imprinting toward it inside the scanner.
To this end, we decided to analyse both groups independently to determine the brain activity pattern during the acquisition and the recall phase of a long-term memory of imprinting. While Imp > Cont contrast in the red group showed robust activation clusters in many telencephalic as well as diencephalic regions, in the blue group showed no significant activation clusters.
As shown in Figures 2, 3, and S3, this is due to chicks’ preference for red over blue (as demonstrated through the behavioral experiment, ), therefore we used the Cont > Imp contrast (red > blue colour) during the last 20 minutes of scanning (first 16 trial did not include in analysis to control the exposer of the animal into the control colour), to investigate the memory formation phase of a secondary imprinting process24 elicited by the presence of the preferred colour red. As illustrated in Figure 4, the voxel-based group analysis showed robust BOLD responses in different visual prosencephalic regions: the nucleus geniculatus lateralis pars dorsalis (GLd, which receives direct input from the retina25), the right intermediate hyperpallium apicale (IHA, which primarily receives visual thalamic input26), the right hyperpallium intercalatum (HI) and right hyperpallium densocellulare (HD), and bilaterally the hyperpallium apicale (HA, together with HD associative hubs of the thalamofugal pathway26,27) of the thalamofugal pathway, bilaterally the nucleus rotundus (Rot, which is the primary thalamic input region of the tectofugal pathway). Also, parts of the auditory system were activated: bilaterally the ventromedial part of the Field-L complex and the right nucleus ovoidalis (OV), a thalamic auditory nucleus receiving direct input from the avian homologue of the inferior colliculus (torus semicircularis28) that projects to Field-L. We detected significant activation clusters in the associative pallial regions nidopallium medial pars medialis (NMm) and bilaterally in the caudal intermediate medial mesopallium (IMM). Within the two interconnected Social Behavior Network and Mesolimbic Reward System, we detected a significant BOLD increase rightward in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), the nucleus accumbens (Ac) and the medial striatum (MSt), bilaterally in the septum and leftward in the posterior pallial amygdala (PoA) and the ventromedial part of hippocampus (Hp-VM).
As illustrated in Figures 4 and S4, the voxel-based group analysis during the imprinting memory retrieval phase in the red group showed robust BOLD responses in different visual prosencephalic regions: the right GLd, bilaterally in IHA, HI, HD and HA (Figure 4). We found also a significant BOLD rightward increase in part of the auditory system, OV.
Furthermore, we detected a significant increase in the BOLD signal in the associative right MNM (IMM + NMm) and nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL) and in left portions of the caudal mesopallium dorsale (MD) and nidopallium caudocentrale (NCC; all interconnected regions31–33).
Within the two interconnected Social Behavior Network and Mesolimbic Reward System, we detected significant bilateral activation in the ventromedial part of the hippocampus (Hp-VM), while rightward activation clusters in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), in the nucleus accumbens (Ac), in the medial striatum (MSt), in the medial and dorsal arcopallium (respectively AM and AD), in the posterior pallial amygdala (PoA) and in the preoptic, anterior and ventromedial areas of the hypothalamus (respectively POA, AH, and VMH).
Discussion
Imprinting, a well-known form of early learning, has been widely used in the 70’s as a model to study the neurobiology of memory formation (reviews in10,34). Evidence for a crucial role played by the intermediate medial mesopallium (IMM) and NMm (jointly labelled as MNM) during the acquisition of imprinting memory was obtained. Further studies showed that the store in the IMM is only temporary, and that a transfer of information to another, unknown brain region, dubbed S’35, occurs after approximately 6 hours. These studies were conducted with either autoradiographic or lesion techniques and were unable to discover the full imprinting network36. To overcome the limitations of traditional methods, fMRI represents a significant leap forward in our ability to investigate and comprehend brain activities. By providing an indirect measurement, BOLD, of the whole brain in various circumstances, this cutting-edge technology offers scientists with a powerful tool for unravelling complex brain networks and sheds light on their roles in diverse cognitive processes. The enhanced capabilities of fMRI increase our capacity to investigate the dynamic interplay between different brain areas, allowing us to get a deeper understanding of the neural processes that underpin cognition and behaviour.
Here we established a new non-invasive fMRI protocol to study awake brain activity in newly hatched domestic chicks in order to discover the neural pathways of imprinting and the identity of S’. After two days of imprinting training, with either a preferred (red) or a non-preferred (blue) colour, chicks were exposed to a sequence of the two stimulus colours inside the scanner. Data collected could be informative for a network of brain regions involved in the acquisition of secondary imprinting memory (blue stimulus), and, in parallel, a network involved in the long-term storage and retrieval of imprinting memory (red stimulus).
Visual information reaches the pallium both via the tecto- and the thalamofugal visual pathways. We observed a partial involvement of the nucleus rotundus (Rot), the thalamic link of the tectofugal pathway during acquisition (Figure 5A). A rotundal involvement had already been reported in imprinted chicks37 and together with the present results, it could suggests a minor tectofugal role during the early stages of imprinting learning. In contrast, the thalamofugal visual system seems to play a crucial role in processing imprinting information (as also reviewed in38). This pathway consists of the retinorecipient GLd31 that projects to the interstitial nucleus of the hyperpallium apicale (IHA) of the visual Wulst, from where secondary projections reach the three pseudo-layers of the Wulst hyperpallium densocellulare (HD), hyperpallium intercalatum (HI), and hyperpallium apicale (HA)39. We discovered both during memory formation and retrieval (Figure 5B) significant activity patterns of all these thalamofugal components. Indeed, HD of dark-reared chicks exhibits topographically organised responses for red and blue objects40. After imprinting on either one of the two colours, such organisation changes along the rostro-caudal axis showing imprinting-related plasticity already in the Wulst.
Previous studies showed that Wulst lesions lead to anterograde amnesia of visual imprinting memory40. This possibly results from the loss of visual projections from HD to IMM41,42, the associative medial pallial area that is crucial for the acquisition of imprinting memory10. IMM projects back to HA, establishing a loop31. IMM, the ventrally located NMm and the nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL) have been shown to be involved during visual as well as auditory filial imprinting15,43. Here we report a significant brain activation in IMM, NMm, and NCL during memory retrieval and, to a much lesser extent, in IMM and NMm during memory formation. Indeed, NMm and NCL undergo long-lasting synaptic changes after multimodal (visuo-auditory) imprinting training10,43,44. Imprinting training also impacts cell proliferation in NMm and NCL, but not in IMM45. Thus, these three areas play important but differential roles in multimodal filial learning and the subsequent formation of long-term memory. Note that in the present study chicks were also exposed to the noise produced by the scanner. Thus, NMm and NCL on the one and the auditory n. ovoidalis (OV) – Field-L pathway on the other side, could conceivably constitute the neural basis for the acoustic component of acquiring (blue group) or retrieving imprinting memory (red group).
However, the interconnected higher associative regions, NMm and NCL do not only play a role for long-term memory-related mechanisms22,44,46,47. NMm is also involved in sensorimotor learning and sequential behaviour48, while NCL, largely accepted as a prefrontal-like field33, is involved in working memory49,50, executive control51 and in merging multi-sensory information in long-term memory engrams52. This evidence together with the present findings further supports the involvement of these regions in the long-term storage and flexible retrieval of a multimodal imprinting memory trace.
The motor output component of NMm and NCL is established by their projections to arcopallium and medial striatum (MSt)46,53–56. Possibly, the initially pallially processed imprinting trace is thereby transferred into a striatum-dependent response strategy. As a result, striatal S-R associations are formed and once acquired, drive animal’s imprinting behaviour56. This also has been shown for passive avoidance learning. Here, the mnemonic nature of MSt (previously lobus paraolfactorius12) goes hand in hand with that of IMM34,57,58, with increased density of synapses and dendritic spines being detectable some days after training in MSt, but not in IMM58–60. Additionally, after imprinting training, glutamate receptor binding affinity increases both in MSt and arcopallium61–63, while, pre-imprinting arcopallial lesions impair memory acquisition64.
We found enhanced brain activity in the most medial part of MSt both during acquisition and retrieval of imprinting memory, while for the dorsal and medial portions of arcopallium this was only observed for retrieval. These portions of MSt and arcopallium are enriched in the limbic system-associated membrane protein (LAMP)65. We also found a strong mesolimbic involvement in imprinting memory in the two interconnected Social Behavior Network and Mesolimbic Reward System66–68. Here septum was involved only during memory formation. Arcopallium, preoptic area, anterior and ventromedial hypothalamus (POA, AH, VMH) were involved only during memory retrieval. In contrast, Hp, MSt, bed n. of the stria terminalis (BNST), n. accumbens (Ac) and posterior pallial amygdala (PoA) were involved during both memory formation and retrieval. While involvement of these systems in social predispositions associated with imprinting had already been observed6,69,70, this is the first evidence for their involvement during imprinting. Such involvement could represent the motivational component linked to the association. Indeed, in the context of filial imprinting, emotional-motivational engagement must be particularly pronounced at different stages of the learning process. The septum seems to be preferentially involved during the first stages of imprinting and probably driving the chick’s attention toward salient predisposed moving stimuli. Previous studies also revealed septal involvement during the first exposure to a red object moving with abrupt changes of speed or an alive conspecific69,70. Although BNST, Ac, MSt, and PoA seem to participate in both imprinting memory formation and retrieval, we found greater activity in the red group. Such enhanced activity may suggest a stronger emotional-motivational component after memory consolidation of the imprinting engram.
The HD of the Wulst has bidirectional connections with PoA and Hp71,72. We found a hippocampal (Hp) involvement both during imprinting memory formation and retrieval. The hippocampal formation is known for its role in memory in birds and mammals73. However, c-fos immunoreactivity in chicks revealed also a social role of Hp. The dorso- and ventromedial portions are involved in individual recognition in chicks74. The same portions here were found to be involved in imprinting memory, strengthening a regional specialisation of hippocampus dedicated to social memory functions. Indeed, Hp projects ipsi- and contralaterally to IMM59 and is involved in filial imprinting61. We found a left Hp involvement during filial imprinting memory formation (blue group), and a bilateral one during memory retrieval (red group).
Interestingly, the brain activity pattern was predominantly right lateralised. Among the exceptions was a left Hp involvement during imprinting memory formation (blue group), and a bilateral Hp involvement during memory retrieval (red group). Lateralisation is a common feature in the avian brain, especially at different stages of memory formation75–77. Right lateralisation during memory formation has been reported for passive avoidance learning78. Instead, for imprinting learning, time-shifts have been observed in the lateralisation pattern of IMM. The left IMM is involved at first in learning the features of the imprinting object, while the right IMM dominates during memory consolidation and the subsequent establishment of the long-term storage S’18,79. A similar pattern of lateralisation has been proposed in the hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry model (HERA) in humans, where the left hemisphere plays a dominant role during memory encoding and the right during retrieval77. Such evidence together leads to the hypothesis of a dual memory system for imprinting, in which different processes - acquisition and consolidation - take place in different hemispheres, with prominent right lateralisation for consolidation processes18. Indeed, during memory consolidation, a glutamate injection into the right IMM disrupts imprinting memory, but it does not when injected into the left hemisphere80. Our results may add a novel view on the idea of the dual memory system: while the visual thalamofugal nucleus GLd was bilaterally activated during acquisition, only the right side was active during retrieval. It is conceivable that right hemispheric memory consolidation increased top-down projections onto right sided sensory thalamic nuclei in order to focus attention on learned object properties81. This then could activate and synchronize right hemispheric pallial areas according to attentional allocation, thereby inducing a right hemispheric superiority in imprinting memory retrieval82.
Our findings provide a novel completely non-invasive paradigm for studying neural mechanisms at birth in newly hatched chicks. Additionally, our data suggests a prosencephalic neural network that, among others, involves the Social Behavior Network, the Mesolimbic Reward System, and the medial meso-/nidopallium for long-term storage and retrieval of filial imprinting memory. As to be expected, the networks that could be involved in memory formation and retrieval partially overlapped. However, network activity was more pronounced and further involved arcopallium and NCL in the retrieval condition. Thus, consolidation of imprinting memory seems to result in a strengthening and expansion of the neural system that holds the engram in distributed manner. Within this perspective, the long-searched site for imprinting memory dubbed as S’ by Gabriel Horn83 is possibly this whole network within which the “prefrontal” NCL could be a central hub.
Material and Methods
Subjects
All procedures here presented followed all the applicable European Union and Italian laws, and guidelines for animals’ care and use and were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento OPBA and by the Italian Health Ministry (permit number 738/2019). Fifty females were used in the present study. Twenty-six for the MRI procedure: red group (n = 9) imprinted to red colour, blue group (n = 8) imprinted to blue colour, and resting-state group (n= 9). Twenty-four for the behavioural experiment: red group (n = 12) imprinted to the red colour, blue group (n = 12) imprinted to the blue colour. Each chick underwent the experimental procedure only once.
A local commercial hatchery (Azienda Agricola Crescenti, Brescia, Italy) provided fertilised eggs of the Aviagen Ross 308 strain (Gallus gallus domesticus). Eggs were incubated and hatched in the laboratory under controlled temperature (37.7°C) and humidity (60%) in darkness using FIEM MG140/200 Rural LCD EVO incubators. Soon after hatching, chicks were sexed by feather dimorphism, with a black cap on the head in order to prevent any visual stimulation. Twenty-six females were used in the present study. Females were used because they are known to exhibit stronger filial attachment with the imprinting object (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara, 1992; Vallortigara et al., 1990). Each chick underwent the experimental procedure only once. At the end of the experimental procedure, on post-hatching day 3, chicks were caged in groups with water and food ad libitum, at constant temperature (32.3°C) and with a 12:12 day-night light cycle until they were donated to local farmers.
Imprinting
On the day of hatching, chicks were caged individually at a constant temperature of 32.3°C with water and food. In each cage (28×40×32 cm) the imprinting stimulus, a hollow plastic ball (diameter 3.5 cm), was suspended in the middle (7 cm from the floor, Figure 1 A). Two optical fibres (diameter of 2mm) inserted in the ball were flickering at 5 Hz. Chicks prefer to imprint on a flickering than on a stationary light (James, 1959). For one group of chicks, the ball was flickering with red light (N = 9, dominant wavelength = 642 nm, intensity = 16.45 cd/m2,), for the other group with blue light (N = 8, dominant wavelength = 465 nm, intensity = 16.45 cd/m2). Being the only light provided in the environment, the established setup by Behroozi et al. (Behroozi et al., 2020) and a custom-written MATLAB code were used to automatically switch on and off the light, following a day-night cycle 12:12. During the daytime, to habituate the subjects to the noise of the scanner, a recording of the sound was provided twice per day, for a total amount of 5 hours per day, by two loudspeakers (Logitech) placed outside the cages.
Acquisition and Pre-processing of fMRI data
All MRI experiments were recorded using a horizontal-bore small animal MRI scanner (7.0 T Bruker BioSpin, Ettlingen, Germany) equipped with a BGA-9 gradient set (380 mT/m, max. linear slew rate 3,420 T/m/s). A 72 mm transmit birdcage resonator was used for radio-frequency transmission. To reduce the motion artifacts resulting from body parts’ movements, a single-loop 20 mm surface coil was placed around the chicks’ head for signal reception.
Localiser.
At the beginning of each scanning session, a set of scout images (coronal, horizontal, and sagittal scans) were recorded as localisers to identify the position and orientation of the chick’s brain inside the MRI machine. The scout images were acquired using a multi-slice rapid acquisition (RARE) sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 3000 ms, effective echo time (TEeff) = 41.2 ms, RARE factor = 32, N_average = 2, acquisition matrix = 128 × 128, the field of view (FoV) = 20 × 20 mm, spatial resolution = 0.156 × 0.156 mm2, slice thickness = 1 mm, number of slices = 8, slice orientation = coronal/horizontal/sagittal, with a total scan time of 18 s. This information has been used to position 9 coronal slices in a way (~40° regarding coronal direction) to cover the entire telencephalon to record the fMRI time series.
fMRI (task).
The blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) time series were recorded using a single-shot multi-slice RARE sequence adopted from Behroozi et al. (Behroozi et al., 2020, 2018) with the following parameters: TR/TEeff = 4000/51.04 ms, RARE factor = 42, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, FoV = 30 × 30 mm2, 9 coronal slices no gap between slices, slice thickness = 1 mm, slice order = interleaved. Since the eyes’ size is comparable to brain’s one, two saturation slices were manually positioned on the eyes to saturate the possible eye movement artifacts, which can corrupt the BOLD signal. A total of 540 volumes were recorded for each animal.
fMRI (Rest).
Whole-brain resting-state fMRI data (200 volumes) of nine chicks were recorded using a single-shot RARE sequence with the same parameter as the task fMRI sequence.
Structural MRI.
High-resolution anatomical images were acquired using a RARE sequence with following parameters: TR/TEeff = 6000/42.04 ms, RARE factor = 16, N_Average = 4, acquisition matrix = 160 × 160, FoV = 30 × 30 mm2, 39 coronal slices with no gap between slices, slice thickness = 0.33 mm, total scan time = 4 min.
Experimental Task.
Inside the fMRI machine, chicks were presented with two different stimulus types, imprinted (red/blue) and control colour (blue/red) with the same wavelength and intensity as the training phase. The light stimuli were generated using the established setup by Behroozi et al. (Behroozi et al., 2020). Stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random order in an ON/OFF block design experiment (maximum two trials in a row were of the same colour). The duration of ON blocks was 16 s. ON blocks were interleaved with a rest period of 24 s (OFF blocks, inter-trial interval (ITI)). In total, 48 trials were recorded during an fMRI session from each animal (24 trials per stimulus).
Apparatus.
A critical issue during awake fMRI scanning of animals is motion artifacts. Therefore, immobilisation of the animal’s head is essential to acquire an accurate fMRI time series. To this end, awake chicks were immobilised in a nonmagnetic custom-made restrainer, composed of a beak holder, blocks of plasticine around the head to immobilise it in a comfortable way, and a round RF coil on top of the head (Figure 1B). Before the head fixation, the animal’s body was wrapped in paper tissue to prevent the other body parts’ movement (such as wings and feet) to avoid any possible motion artifacts. The animal’s body inside the paper tissue was tapped to the main body of the restrainer using a piece of medical tape.
fMRI data processing
All BOLD time series were pre-processed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL, version 6.0.4, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki), the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI, version 20.0.09 https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/), and Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs, http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/) software. We performed the following pre-processing steps for each run: (i) converting dicom files to nifti format (using dcm2niix function); (ii) upscaling the voxel size by a factor of 10 (using AFNI’s 3drefit); (iii) discarding the first 5 volumes to ensure longitudinal magnetization reached steady state; (iv) motion correction using MCFLIRT (which aligns each volume to the middle volume of each run); (v) slice time correction to account for the long whole-brain acquisition time (4000 ms); interleaved acquisitions); (vi) despiking using 3dDespike algorithm in AFNI; (vii) removing non-brain tissue (using BET and manual cleaning); (viii) spatial smoothing with FWHM = 8 mm (using FSL’s SUSAN, after upscaling voxel size by factor of 10); (ix) global intensity normalization with grand mean = 10000 across scanning sessions for group analysis; (x) high-pass temporal filtering to remove slow drifts (cut-off at 100s); (xi) anatomical brain extraction (using BET function and cleaned manually); (xii) registration of the functional data to the high-resolution structural images using affine linear registration (FLIRT function, six degrees of freedom). For spatial normalization, a population-based template was constructed using antsMultivariateTemplateConstruction.sh script (ANTs). FMRIB’s Nonlinear Image Registration Tool (FNRIT) (Andersson et al., 2007) was used to spatially normalize the single subject anatomical images to the population-based template as a standard space. The head motion of animals was quantified using framewise displacement (FD) (Power et al., 2014). Three animals’ data were excluded due to the excessive head motion (over 20% of volumes were contaminated with FD > 0.2mm). For the remaining animals, the detected motion outliers were modeled as confound regressors during the general linear model (GLM) analysis to reduce the impact of head motion.
General linear model (GLM) analysis
Whole-brain statistical analysis was performed using the FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) to assess stimulus-evoked activation patterns. Single-subject GLM analysis was carried out to convolve the established double-gamma avian hemodynamic response function in pigeon brain by Behroozi et al. (Behroozi et al., 2020) (the closest brain in the structural organization to the chick brain) to the explanatory variables (on/off stimulation). In the first GLM, we incorporated the complete fMRI timeseries using the following two explanatory variables (EVs) and their temporal derivatives: (i) imprinting trials (indicated by red/blue, 24 trials) and (ii) control trials (indicated by blue/red, 24 trials). In the second GLM, we employed three EVs and their temporal derivatives: (i) imprinting trials (last 16 trials); (ii) control trials (last 16 trials); (iii) junk trials (the first 16 trials comprising 8 imprinting and 8 control trials, were used as habituation period to the real magnet environment). In addition, six estimated head motion parameters (three translations and three rotations) and outlier volumes detected based on the FD analysis were modelled as confound EVs to remove the residual motion artifacts. To perform group inference, subject-level parameter estimates were taken into the second-level analysis using the mixed-effect model (FLAME1+2) to produce group-level estimates of each condition. FLAME 1+2 cluster-based approach has been used to threshold the group-level statistical maps for contrasts of interest with a cluster defining voxel threshold of p < 0.001 (Z > 3.1) for red group and p < 0.01 (Z > 2.3) for blue group and entire timeseries analysis and Family Error Wise (FEW) cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05.
Visualization
To visualize the results, we took advantage of the high-resolution anatomical image acquired for another study. Briefly, five post-mortem chick brains were scanned using a fast-low angle shot (FLASH) sequence with following parameters: TR/TE = 50/4 ms, N_average = 6, acquisition matrix = 400 × 400 × 500, voxels size = 0.05× 0.05 × 0.05 mm3, total scan time = 19 h 48 min. The population-based template was co-registered nonlinearly (using FNIRT) to the high-resolution anatomical image of the chick brain. The contrasts of interest, eventually, were non-linearly warped to the high-resolution anatomical image. MANGO software (http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/mango.html, version 4.1) was used for 3D visualization of the activation patterns. Surf Ice software (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/surfice/, version v1.0.20201102 64bit x86–64 Windows) was used for surface rendering the chick brains with overlays to illustrate activated networks during imprinting acquisition and retrieval memory.
Behavioural experiment
Similar to the imprinting procedure employed for the MRI experiment, chicks were individually caged on the day of hatching with the imprinting object until day 3. The Red imprinted group was exposed for two days to the red flickering light (N =12), while the Blue imprinted group to the blue flickering light (N = 12).
On day 3 all chicks were individually exposed to the pseudo random sequence of red and blue colours that was employed for the stimulation inside the scanner (for details see section Acquisition and Pre-processing of fMRI data - Experimental task). Each chick saw 24 times its imprinting colour (red/blue) and 24 times the control colour (red/blue).
After the exposure, each chick was tested individually inside a running wheel to evaluate its colour preference. The test in the running wheel lasted a total of 10 minutes. Each colour was presented for 5 minutes. The sequence of colour presentation was counterbalanced between subjects.
The dependent variable measured was the distance (cm) covered by each subject toward the red and the blue. To estimate chicks’ colour preference, we calculated an index using the formula:
This index could range between 0 (absolute preference for the blue) and 1 (absolute preference for the red), whereas 0.5 represented the absence of preference between the two colours.
To estimate differences between the two imprinting groups we employed a two-tailed independent samples t-test. To estimate colour preference, we employed one-sample two-tailed t-test against chance (0.5).
To test the presence of differences in the colour preference between the two groups, we employed a two-tailed independent samples t-test. To verify the presence of a significant preference for either blue or red, we employed two-tailed one-sample t-test against chance (0.5).
Acknowledgments
Funding
This work was supported by the MIUR-DAAD Joint Mobility Program (project number 33538), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) through grant SFB 874 (A1, B5) project number 122679504, SFB 1280 (A01, A08, and F02) project number 316803389. OG was in addition funded by DFG through Gu 227/16-1 and the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (ERC-2020-ADG, grant agreement No. [101021354, AVIAN MIND]). GV acknowledges grants from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) Grant ERC-2011-ADG_20110406, Project no: 461 295517, PREMESOR), by Fondazione Caritro Grant Bio-marker DSA [40102839], and PRIN 2015. AG acknowledges funding by the European Research Council (ERC, DISCONN; no. 802371), the Brain and Behavior Foundation (NARSAD Independent Investigator Grant #25861), the NIH (1R21MH116473-01A1) and the Telethon foundation (GGP19177).
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Data and material availability
fMRI data for the chick imprinting and resting-state fMRI are available at (https://data.mendeley.com/preview/w6cwvmbxwr,Will be publicly available after acceptance). FSL software (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/, version 6.0.4) and MATLAB (2020b, MathWorks, USA) were used to process fMRI and behavioural data, respectively. Related processing codes can be found at https://github.com/mehdibehroozi/Imprinting-fMRI. All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials.
A preprint version of the present manuscript is present on bioRxiv (Behroozi, Lorenzi et al., 2022).
References
- 1.McCabe B.J. (2013). Imprinting. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 4, 375–390. 10.1002/wcs.1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lorenz K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie 1935 83:2 83, 137–213. 10.1007/BF01905355. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Spalding D.A. (1954). Instinct: With original observations on young animals. The British Journal of Animal Behaviour 2, 2–11. 10.1016/S0950-5601(54)80075-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vallortigara G. (2021). Born Knowing: Imprinting and the Origins of Knowledge (The MIT Press; ) 10.7551/mitpress/14091.001.0001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Müller-Schwarze D., and Müller-Schwarze C. (1971). Olfactory imprinting in a precocial mammal. Nature 229, 55–56. 10.1038/229055a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lorenzi E., and Vallortigara G. (2021). Evolutionary and neural bases of the sense of animacy. In The Cambridge handbook of animal cognition, Kaufman A. B, Call J, and Kaufman J. C, eds. (Cambridge University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- 7.Yamamoto K., Sun Z., Hong B.W., and Reiner A. (2005). Subpallial amygdala and nucleus taeniae in birds resemble extended amygdala and medial amygdala in mammals in their expression of markers of regional identity. Brain Res Bull 66, 341–347. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lemche E. (2020). Research evidence from studies on filial imprinting, attachment, and early life stress: a new route for scientific integration. Acta Ethol 23, 127–133. 10.1007/s10211-020-00346-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vicedo M. (2013). The Nature and Nurture of Love (University of Chicago Press; ) 10.7208/chicago/9780226020693.001.0001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Horn G. (2004). Pathways of the past: The imprint of memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 5, 108–120. 10.1038/nrn1324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jarvis E., Güntürkün O., Bruce L., Csillag A., Karten H., Kuenzel W., Medina L., Paxinos G., Perkel D.J., Shimizu T., et al. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 151–159. 10.1038/nrn1606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Reiner A., Perkel D.J., Bruce L.L., Butler A.B., Csillag A., Kuenzel W., Medina L., Paxinos G., Shimizu T., Striedter G., et al. (2004). Revised Nomenclature for Avian Telencephalon and Some Related Brainstem Nuclei. Journal of Comparative Neurology 473, 377–414. 10.1002/cne.20118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bradley P., Horn G., and Bateson P. (1981). Imprinting - An electron microscopic study of chick hyperstriatum ventrale. Exp Brain Res 41, 115–120. 10.1007/BF00236600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Horn G., Bradley P., and McCabe B.J. (1985). Changes in the structure of synapses associated with learning. Journal of Neuroscience 5, 3161–3168. 10.1523/jneurosci.05-12-03161.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bredenkötter M., and Braun K. (1997). Changes of neuronal responsiveness in the mediorostral neo striatum/hyperstriatum after auditory filial imprinting in the domestic chick. Neuroscience 76, 355–365. 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00381-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bredenkötter M., and Braun K. (2000). Development of Neuronal Responsiveness in the Mediorostral Neostriatum/Hyperstriatum Ventrale during Auditory Filial Imprinting in Domestic Chicks. Neurobiol Learn Mem 73, 114–126. 10.1006/nlme.1999.3923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Horn G., McCabe B.J., and Cipolla-Neto J. (1983). Imprinting in the domestic chick: The role of each side of the hyperstriatum ventrale in acquisition and retention. Exp Brain Res 53, 91–98. 10.1007/BF00239401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cipolla-Neto J., Horn G., and McCabe B.J. (1982). Hemispheric asymmetry and imprinting: The effect of sequential lesions to the hyperstriatum ventrale. Exp Brain Res 48, 22–27. 10.1007/BF00239569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davey J., McCabe B., and Horn G. (1987). Mechanisms of information storage after imprinting in the domestic chick. Behavioural Brain Research 26, 209–210. 10.1016/0166-4328(87)90180-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Salzen E.A., Lily R.E., and McKeown J.R. (1971). Colour preference and imprinting in domestic chicks. Anim Behav 19, 542–547. 10.1016/S0003-3472(71)80109-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Behroozi M., Ströckens F., Helluy X., Stacho M., and Güntürkün O. (2017). Functional Connectivity Pattern of the Internal Hippocampal Network in Awake Pigeons: A Resting-State fMRI Study. Brain Behav Evol 90, 62–72. 10.1159/000475591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Behroozi M., Helluy X., Ströckens F., Gao M., Pusch R., Tabrik S., Tegenthoff M., Otto T., Axmacher N., Kumsta R., et al. (2020). Event-related functional MRI of awake behaving pigeons at 7T. Nat Commun 11, 1–12. 10.1038/s41467-020-18437-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ungurean G., Behroozi M., Böger L., Helluy X., Libourel P.A., Güntürkün O., and Rattenborg N.C. (2023). Wide-spread brain activation and reduced CSF flow during avian REM sleep. Nature Communications 2023 14:1 14, 1–12. 10.1038/s41467-023-38669-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Boakes R., and Panter D. (1985). Secondary imprinting in the domestic chick blocked by previous exposure to a live hen. Anim Behav 33, 353–365. 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80059-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Güntürkün O., and Karten H.J. (1991). An immunocytochemical analysis of the lateral geniculate complex in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 314, 721–749. 10.1002/cne.903140407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Csillag A., and Montagnese C.M. (2005). Thalamotelencephalic organization in birds. In Brain Research Bulletin (Brain Res Bull), pp. 303–310. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shanahan M., Bingman V.P., Shimizu T., Wild M., and Güntürkün O. (2013). Large-scale network organisation in the avian forebrain: A connectivity matrix and theoretical analysis. Front Comput Neurosci 7. 10.3389/fncom.2013.00089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Karten H.J. (1967). The organization of the ascending auditory pathway in the pigeon (Columba livia) I. Diencephalic projections of the inferior colliculus (nucleus mesencephali lateralis, pars dorsalis). Brain Res 6, 409–427. 10.1016/0006-8993(67)90055-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kuenzel W.J., and Masson M. (1988). A stereotaxic atlas of the brain of the chick (Gallus domesticus) (Johns Hopkins University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- 30.Herold C., Bingman V.P., Ströckens F., Letzner S., Sauvage M., Palomero-Gallagher N., Zilles K., and Güntürkün O. (2014). Distribution of neurotransmitter receptors and zinc in the pigeon (Columba livia) hippocampal formation: A basis for further comparison with the mammalian hippocampus. J Comp Neurol 522, 2553–2575. 10.1002/CNE.23549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Aoki N., Yamaguchi S., Kitajima T., Takehara A., Katagiri-Nakagawa S., Matsui R., Watanabe D., Matsushima T., and Homma K.J. (2015). Critical role of the neural pathway from the intermediate medial mesopallium to the intermediate hyperpallium apicale in filial imprinting of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Neuroscience 308, 115–124. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Atoji Y., and Wild J.M. (2009). Afferent and efferent projections of the central caudal nidopallium in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 517, 350–370. 10.1002/cne.22146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.von Eugen K., Tabrik S., Güntürkün O., and Ströckens F. (2020). A comparative analysis of the dopaminergic innervation of the executive caudal nidopallium in pigeon, chicken, zebra finch, and carrion crow. Journal of Comparative Neurology, cne.24878. 10.1002/cne.24878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rose S.P.R. (2000). God’s organism? The chick as a model system for memory studies. Learning and Memory 7, 1–17. 10.1101/lm.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Honey R.C., Horn G., Bateson P., and Walpole M. (1995). Functionally Distinct Memories for Imprinting Stimuli: Behavioral and Neural Dissociations. Behavioral Neuroscience 109, 689–698. 10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Horn G., McCabe B.J., and Bateson P.P.G. (1979). An autoradiographic study of the chick brain after imprinting. Brain Res 168, 361–373. 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90176-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Harvey R.J., McCabe B.J., Solomonia R.O., Horn G., and Darlison M.G. (1998). Expression of the GABAA receptor γ4-subunit gene: Anatomical distribution of the corresponding mRNA in the domestic chick forebrain and the effect of imprinting training. European Journal of Neuroscience 10, 3024–3028. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1998.00354.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nakamori T., Maekawa F., Sato K., Tanaka K., and Ohki-Hamazaki H. (2013). Neural basis of imprinting behavior in chicks. Dev Growth Differ 55, 198–206. 10.1111/dgd.12028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Stacho M., Herold C., Rook N., Wagner H., Axer M., Amunts K., and Güntürkün O. (2020). A cortex-like canonical circuit in the avian forebrain. Science (1979) 369. 10.1126/science.abc5534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Maekawa F., Komine O., Sato K., Kanamatsu T., Uchimura M., Tanaka K., and Ohki-Hamazaki H. (2006). Imprinting modulates processing of visual information in the visual wulst of chicks. BMC Neuroscience 2006 7:1 7, 1–13. 10.1186/1471-2202-7-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Shimizu T., Cox K., and Karten H.J. (1995). Intratelencephalic projections of the visual wulst in pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 359, 551–572. 10.1002/cne.903590404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nakamori T., Sato K., Atoji Y., Kanamatsu T., Tanaka K., and Ohki-Hamazaki H. (2010). Demonstration of a neural circuit critical for imprinting behavior in chicks. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 4467–4480. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3532-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bock J., Schnabel R., and Braun K. (1997). Role of the dorso - caudal neostriatum in filial imprinting of the domestic chick: A pharmacological and autoradiographical approach focused on the involvement of NMDA-receptors. European Journal of Neuroscience 9, 1262–1272. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Metzger M., Jiang S., and Braun K. (1998). Organization of the dorsocaudal neostriatal complex: A retrograde and anterograde tracing study in the domestic chick with special emphasis on pathways relevant to imprinting. Journal of Comparative Neurology 395, 380–404. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Komissarova N. V., and Anokhin K. V. (2008). Effects of an imprinting procedure on cell proliferation in the chick brain. Neurosci Behav Physiol 38, 289–296. 10.1007/s11055-008-0041-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Atoji Y., and Wild J.M. (2012). Afferent and efferent projections of the mesopallium in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 520, 717–741. 10.1002/cne.22763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rook N., Tuff J.M., Isparta S., Masseck O.A., Herlitze S., Güntürkün O., and Pusch R. (2021). AAV1 is the optimal viral vector for optogenetic experiments in pigeons (Columba livia). Commun Biol 4, 1–16. 10.1038/s42003-020-01595-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Helduser S., and Güntürkün O. (2012). Neural substrates for serial reaction time tasks in pigeons. Behavioural Brain Research 230, 132–143. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Diekamp B., Kalt T., and Güntürkün O. (2002). Working memory neurons in pigeons. J Neurosci 22, RC210–RC210. 10.1523/jneurosci.22-04-j0002.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Veit L., Hartmann K., and Nieder A. (2014). Neuronal correlates of visual working memory in the corvid endbrain. Journal of Neuroscience 34, 7778–7786. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0612-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rose J., and Colombo M. (2005). Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain. PLoS Biol 3, 1139–1146. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Moll F.W., and Nieder A. (2015). Cross-modal associative mnemonic signals in crow endbrain neurons. Current Biology 25, 2196–2201. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kröner S., and Güntürkün O. (1999). Afferent and efferent connections of the caudolateral neostriatum in the pigeon (Columba uvia): A retro- and anterograde pathway tracing study. Journal of Comparative Neurology 407, 228–260. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schnabel R., Metzger M., Jiang S., Hemmings H.C., Greengard P., and Braun K. (1997). Localization of dopamine D1 receptors and dopaminoceptive neurons in the chick forebrain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 388, 146–168. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Metzger M., Jiang S., and Braun K. (2002). A quantitative immuno-electron microscopic study of dopamine terminals in forebrain regions of the domestic chick involved in filial imprinting. Neuroscience 111, 611–623. 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00611-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Csillag A. (1999). Striato-telencephalic and striato-tegmental circuits: Relevance to learning in domestic chicks. Behavioural Brain Research 98, 227–236. 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00088-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Gibbs M.E. (2008). Memory systems in the chick: Regional and temporal control by noradrenaline. Brain Res Bull 76, 170–182. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sojka M., Davies H.A., Harrison E., and Stewart M.G. (1995). Long-term increases in synaptic density in chick CNS after passive avoidance training are blocked by an inhibitor of protein synthesis. Brain Res 684, 209–214. 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00403-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bradley P., Davies D.C., and Horn G. (1985). Connections of the hyperstriatum ventrale of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus). Journal of anatomy (Print) 140, 577–589. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Stewart M.G., and Rusakov D.A. (1995). Morphological changes associated with stages of memory formation in the chick following passive avoidance training. Behavioural Brain Research 66, 21–28. 10.1016/0166-4328(94)00119-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.McCabe B.J., and Horn G. (1994). Learning-related changes in Fos-like immunoreactivity in the chick forebrain after imprinting. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91, 11417–11421. 10.1073/pnas.91.24.11417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Johnston A.N., Rogers L.J., and Johnston G.A.R. (1993). Glutamate and imprinting memory: the role of glutamate receptors in the encoding of imprinting memory. Behavioural Brain Research 54, 137–143. 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90072-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Izawa E.I., Yanagihara S., Atsumi T., and Matsushima T. (2001). The role of basal ganglia in reinforcement learning and imprinting in domestic chicks. Neuroreport 12, 1743–1747. 10.1097/00001756-200106130-00045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Lowndes M., Davies D.C., and Johnson M.H. (1994). Archistriatal Lesions Impair the Acquisition of Filial Preferences During Imprinting in the Domestic Chick. European Journal of Neuroscience 6, 1143–1148. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Yamamoto K., and Reiner A. (2005). Distribution of the limbic system-associated membrane protein (LAMP) in pigeon forebrain and midbrain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 486, 221–242. 10.1002/cne.20562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Newman S.W. (1999). The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior. A node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann N Y Acad Sci 877, 242–257. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.O’Connell L.A., and Hofmann H.A. (2011). The Vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: A comparative synthesis. Journal of Comparative Neurology 519, 3599–3639. 10.1002/cne.22735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Goodson J.L., and Kingsbury M.A. (2013). What’s in a name? Considerations of homologies and nomenclature for vertebrate social behavior networks. Horm Behav 64, 103–112. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lorenzi E., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., and Vallortigara G. (2017). Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (Gallus gallus). Neuroscience 354, 54–68. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., and Vallortigara G. (2017). First exposure to an alive conspecific activates septal and amygdaloid nuclei in visually-naïve domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Behavioural Brain Research 317, 71–81. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Atoji Y., Sarkar S., and Wild J.M. (2018). Differential projections of the densocellular and intermediate parts of the hyperpallium in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 526, 146–165. 10.1002/cne.24328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Atoji Y., Saito S., and Wild J.M. (2006). Fiber connections of the compact division of the posterior pallial amygdala and lateral part of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology 499, 161–182. 10.1002/cne.21042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Colombo M., and Broadbent N. (2000). Is the avian hippocampus a functional homologue of the mammalian hippocampus? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24, 465–484. 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00016-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Corrales Parada C.D., Morandi-Raikova A., Rosa-Salva O., and Mayer U. (2021). Neural basis of unfamiliar conspecific recognition in domestic chicks (Gallus Gallus domesticus). Behavioural Brain Research 397, 112927. 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Moorman S., and Nicol A.U. (2015). Memory-related brain lateralisation in birds and humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 50, 86–102. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Rogers L.J., Vallortigara G., and Andrew R.J. (2013). Divided brains: The biology and behaviour of brain asymmetries (Cambridge University Press; ) 10.1017/CBO9780511793899. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Tulving E., Kapur S., Craik F.I.M., Moscovitch M., and Houle S. (1994). Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron emission tomography findings. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91, 2016–2020. 10.1073/pnas.91.6.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Lössner B., and Rose S.P.R. (1983). Passive Avoidance Training Increases Fucokinase Activity in Right Forebrain Base of Day‐Old Chicks. J Neurochem 41, 1357–1363. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1983.tb00833.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.McCabe B.J. (1991). Hemispheric asymmetry of learning-induced changes. In Neural and Behavioural Plasticity (Oxford University Press; ), pp. 262–276. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198521846.003.0010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Johnston A.N.B., and Rogers L.J. (1998). Right hemisphere involvement in imprinting memory revealed by glutamate treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 60, 863–871. 10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00073-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Kastner S., and Ungerleider L.G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23, 315–341. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Saalmann Y.B., Pinsk M.A., Wang L., Li X., and Kastner S. (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science (1979) 337, 753–756. 10.1126/science.1223082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Horn G. (1985). Memory, imprinting, and the brain : an inquiry into mechanisms. 10, 315. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
fMRI data for the chick imprinting and resting-state fMRI are available at (https://data.mendeley.com/preview/w6cwvmbxwr,Will be publicly available after acceptance). FSL software (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/, version 6.0.4) and MATLAB (2020b, MathWorks, USA) were used to process fMRI and behavioural data, respectively. Related processing codes can be found at https://github.com/mehdibehroozi/Imprinting-fMRI. All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials.
A preprint version of the present manuscript is present on bioRxiv (Behroozi, Lorenzi et al., 2022).