Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2024 May 27;18:1382380. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1382380

What is the cognitive footprint of insular glioma?

Noah M Nichols 1, Bahie Ezzat 1,2, Allison C Waters 3, Fedor Panov 1, Raymund L Yong 1, Isabelle M Germano 1,*
PMCID: PMC11163043  PMID: 38859993

Abstract

Cognitive impairment has a profound deleterious impact on long-term outcomes of glioma surgery. The human insula, a deep cortical structure covered by the operculum, plays a role in a wide range of cognitive functions including interceptive thoughts and salience processing. Both low-grade (LGG) and high-grade gliomas (HGG) involve the insula, representing up to 25% of LGG and 10% of HGG. Surgical series from the past 30 years support the role of primary cytoreductive surgery for insular glioma patients; however, reported cognitive outcomes are often limited to speech and language function. The breath of recent neuroscience literature demonstrates that the insula plays a broader role in cognition including interoceptive thoughts and salience processing. This article summarizes the vast functional role of the healthy human insula highlighting how this knowledge can be leveraged to improve the care of patients with insular gliomas.

Keywords: insular glioma, cognition, functional outcomes, connectomics, brain tumor surgery

Highlights

  • The human insula plays a role in a wide range of cognitive functions including interceptive thoughts and salience processing.

  • Insular gliomas represent up to 25% of low-grade gliomas and 10% of high-grade gliomas.

  • Many large case series over the past 30 years have demonstrated acceptable rates of morbidity and mortality after insular glioma surgery.

  • Language outcomes following insular tumor surgery have been widely reported; however, other aspects of higher cognition are often not discussed.

  • The breadth of recent neuroscience studies examining the functional role of the human insula have not been fully translated to patients with intrinsic tumors in this region.

Introduction

Insular gliomas, which comprise roughly 25% of low-grade gliomas (LGG) and 10% of high-grade gliomas (HGG), pose a unique challenge to neurosurgeons given their complex anatomy (Duffau and Capelle, 2004; Renfrow et al., 2023). Cognitive impairment has a profound detrimental impact on long-term outcomes of glioma surgery including quality of life (QoL) and return to work status (Liu et al., 2009; Sanai et al., 2011; Hameed et al., 2018; Noll et al., 2018; Li et al., 2020). Evidence over the past 30 years suggests that insular glioma surgery can be safely performed with an acceptable morbidity profile (Sanai et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011; Li et al., 2020). However, while most surgical series report perioperative speech and language outcomes, the breadth of recent neuroscience studies examining the functional role of the human insula have not been fully translated to patients with intrinsic tumors in this region. Specifically, there is a paucity of data describing how the vast cognitive functions of the insula are impacted by gliomas and their treatment (Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of literature describing functional outcomes in insular glioma patients.

First author Year Cognitive domain Objective Design Methods/Task Number of patients Low-grade glioma (LGG) % Main findings
Duffau 2009 Speech/Language Describe functional outcomes of awake intraoperative language mapping in patients with dominant hemisphere insular glioma Retrospective case series Intraoperative counting and picture naming tasks 24 100 Preoperative dysphasia observed in 29%. 5 patients had language positive sites in the insula. All patients recovered to pre-operative language function status, and 6 patients had improvement in pre-operative language dysfunction
Wu 2011 Comprehensive Characterize pre-and-post operative cognitive function in patients with insular gliomas Retrospective enrollment of cases and controls Comprehensive neurocognitive task-based assessment performed pre-and post-op 33 55 Patients with insular tumors had significantly worse preoperative performance on naming tests
Chen 2016 Social Describe the impact of insular gliomas on cognitive and affective empathic abilities Retrospective enrollment of cases and controls Neuropsychological battery of questionnaires 46 39 Lower alexithymia scale scores as well as lower scores on cumulative AE and CE scales in insular glioma patients compared to controls
Zarino 2021 Speech/Language Characterize language impairment pre-and post-surgery in insular glioma patients Retrospective analysis of prospectively collected data Comprehensive language assessment battery 35 26 Language performance worsened in the acute postoperative period. 44% of patients with dominant hemisphere insular glioma and preoperative language deficit had improved language function after surgery but still pathologic
Gomez-Andres 2022 Attention (Salience) Explore the functional role of the aIC for self- monitoring in a patient undergoing awake craniotomy for tumor resection Prospective enrollment of glioma patients Intraoperative Stroop Task with DCS 1 100 Of the 4 total aIC stimulated sites, only 1 (25%) was associated functional disruption of self-monitoring. The was site was located at the posterior limit of the aIC

aIC, Anterior insular cortex; DCS, Direct Cortical Stimulation.

Clinically relevant cognitive impairment is caused by insular glioma as well as surgical resection. The higher-level cognition in patients with LGG and HGG involving the insula was first characterized in 2011. Patients with insular gliomas demonstrated poorer preoperative performance on visual confrontational naming tasks compared to matched controls (Baker et al., 2018). There was also a tendency for decline in the domains of learning, memory, executive function, and motor function (Wu et al., 2011). This study first suggested that cognitive deficits often go undetected outside of the research setting.

In this review article, we aim to sketch the cognitive footprint of insular glioma in a way that provides recommendations for neuropsychological testing and management of patients affected by this disease.

The function of the insula

Despite tremendous interest from the neuroscience community, the insula remains one of the least understood cerebral regions. In 1955, Penfield and Faulk provided the first clues to insular function when they published findings from direct cortical stimulation (DCS) of the insula after temporal lobectomy in epilepsy patients. Patients reported a spectrum of sensations in contralateral body parts including warmth, numbness, shock, and tightness. Most stimulation sites were in the inferior and posterior insula as the superior region was poorly exposed after temporal lobectomy (Penfield and Faulk, 1955).

In more recent years we have come to realize that the complex anatomical features of the insula are matched by an equally as complex functional role (Table 2). The insula has functionally specific spatial organization. A 2010 large meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging experiments revealed at least three distinct functional regions (Kurth et al., 2010). The dorsal anterior insula (dAI) has connections to the frontal lobe, anterior cingulate, and parietal areas, and is most associated with cognition. The ventral anterior insula (vAI) with connections to limbic areas appears to play a role in socio-emotional processing. The posterior insula (PI) is most associated with sensorimotor processing and there is also a central region which is implicated in olfactory and gustatory function (Figure 1) (Kurth et al., 2010). The emergence of the Human Connectome Project (HCP) and connectomics have led to even further parcellations, with currently 13 insular subdivisions (Glasser et al., 2016; Baker et al., 2018; Morell et al., 2022). While the technical aspects of MRI acquisition vary depending (i.e., oncologic purposes compared to stereotactic electrode implantation or studying healthy populations), standard sequences, including diffusion weighted sequences, performed on 1.5 or 3.0 Telsa machines are adequate (Naidich et al., 2004; Uddin et al., 2014; Nomi et al., 2016; Uddin et al., 2017; Blustajn et al., 2019).

Table 2.

Functions of the human insula.

Category Function Laterality Location
Cognition Salience Right Anterior
Speech Left Anterior
Sensorimotor Processing Interoception Right Anterior
Pain Bilateral Dorsal Posterior
Auditory Bilateral Anterior and Posterior
Vestibular Bilateral Posterior
Chemosensory Bilateral Middle
Socio-emotional Processing Emotion Bilateral Anterior
Empathy Bilateral Anterior

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(A) Surgical anatomy of the insulo-sylvian region relative to an insular glioma. (B) Healthy human insula with associated microvasculature and functional topography.

Speech and language: the unclear but important role of the dominant insula

The role of the insula in speech and language remains controversial. The insula was first implicated in speech function in 1996 when a distinct deficit in articulatory planning was observed in all patients with strokes involving the dominant anterior insula and completely absent in stroke patients with this region spared (Dronkers, 1996). However, by 2019 there was convincing evidence that the insula’s role in speech function was far different. By recording bihemspheric cortical activity with stereo-electroencephalographic (sEEG) electrodes in patients undergoing seizure location, only sparse signaling from frontal operculum and not the insula was recorded immediately before speech output. Instead, activity in the bilateral posterior insula was highest after speech articulation (Woolnough et al., 2019).

The rapid expansion of studies on the neurobiology and mechanisms underlying human speech and language have contributed to a contemporary dual-stream model. The dorsal stream, which includes the superior temporal gyrus, is associated with language processing, spectro-temporal and phonological analysis. The ventral stream, which involves the anterior and middle temporal gyri, facilitates speech recognition and lexical recall (Chang et al., 2015). Recent studies are that the anterior insula plays a role in higher-order cognitive aspects of speech and language processing involving both dorsal and ventral streams (Oh et al., 2014). For example, Cesare et al. demonstrated increased activation on fMRI in the central insula in response to “vitality effects,” which are social cues, like tone or body language, which influence perception of speech (di Cesare et al., 2018).

The specific impact of insular gliomas on language function is not clear, however, several studies have reported mild dysphasia as a presenting symptom in 6–9% of patients with dominant hemisphere insular glioma (Duffau et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2011; Skrap et al., 2012). In a series of 24 patients with dominant hemisphere insular LGG a pre-operative language impairment was noted in of 29% of patients (Chang et al., 2015). Furthermore, 21% of patients had at least one site on the dominant insular cortex where speech arrest was induced with DCS (Chang et al., 2015). The ability to maintain normal language function despite tumor infiltration of the dominant insula may be an example of plasticity.

A more recent study with 35 insular glioma patients who underwent a robust perioperative language assessment showed worse speech performance on all language tasks in patients with dominant hemisphere gliomas {need to add ref.# here]. Patients with fronto-temporo-insular and pure insular gliomas were most likely to present with pre-operative pathologic scores. Language performance worsened in the acute postoperative period. Interestingly, 44% of patients with dominant hemisphere glioma did not recover from aphasic symptoms 3 months after surgery and exhibited a persistent language impairment, albeit improved compared to preoperative and immediate postoperative performance. Moreover, patients with pure dominant hemisphere insular glioma had pathologic scores in the Token Test (TT), Object Naming (ON), Verb Naming (VN), Phonemic Fluency (PF), and Semantic Fluency (SF).

Interoception: awareness of the internal self affects outward behavior

How do you know that your heart is racing before you give a presentation to a large audience? How do you know that you are full when your stomach becomes distended after a meal? How do these two scenarios affect our behavior? Interoception is the summation of internal stimuli which allows us to answer the following question – How do you feel? This is accomplished by ascending viscerosensory inputs that arrive to the insula via the thalamus where they are processed (Craig, 2002; Mazzola et al., 2009; Critchley and Harrison, 2013; Tayah et al., 2013). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated insular activation in response to non-painful tactile as well as painful tactile stimulation (zu Eulenburg et al., 2013). By analyzing changes in whole-brain cerebral blood flow (CBF), a strong correlation between increased CBF in the insula and pain scores were noted (Baier et al., 2014). Additionally, the authors overlaid cluster data and coordinates from previously published studies which suggests somatotopic organization within the insula as well (Segerdahl et al., 2015). These findings support a central role of the insula in pain perception which has been further demonstrated in other studies (Craig et al., 2000; Baier et al., 2014; Jensen et al., 2016). Currently, interoceptive processing is thought to progress in a posterior to anterior direction in which interoceptive stimuli are received by the posterior insula and integrated into perceptual maps in the anterior insula (Craig, 2009; Oh et al., 2014).

Interoceptive processing affects our outward behavior. Emotions are among the core aspects of human awareness and James-Lange first proposed that emotions are first activated by bodily changes (Northoff, 2012). Several functional imaging studies have demonstrated activation of the anterior insula in association with negative emotional experiences, positive emotional experiences, disgust, and even sexual pictures (Pugnaghi et al., 2011; Boucher et al., 2015; Papagno et al., 2016). Similarly, the ability to perceive the emotions and empathy of others also appears to activate the insula. Empathy is a neurocognitive behavioral construct that requires sharing of an emotion which is matched by an appropriate/reciprocal inference of feelings, motivations, and subsequent behavior. Affective empathy (AE) is the ability to experience an appropriate empathetic response to another’s emotional state while cognitive empathy (CE) is the capacity to predict and understand another’s mental state using cognitive processes. Specifically, the left insular cortex is associated with affective as well as cognitive forms of empathy, while the right insular cortex only demonstrated an association with the affective form (Fan et al., 2011).

Interoception and emotional dysfunction have not been adequately characterized in patients with insular glioma. Perhaps the most rigorous examination of this topic was done by Chen et al. in which the authors investigated the impact of insular glioma on AE and CE. They reported lower alexithymia scale scores as well as lower scores on cumulative AE and CE scales in insular glioma patients compared to controls (Chen et al., 2016). There were no differences in outcomes based on tumor laterality. While novel and informative, this study did not address how tumor volume, location, and degree of insular involvement impact outcomes.

Salience processing – which stimuli deserve our attention?

We are constantly bombarded with multiple internal and external stimuli and yet not all occupy our attention to the same degree. Instead, we are able to discern which stimuli are “salient” or important. Salience processing is not always a conscious mental task as it also incorporates visceral and autonomic stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007). Our ability to have focused goal-directed behavior relies on intact salience processing. Connectomics has demonstrated that salience processing is not found in a single cerebral region but rather diffusely localized to several hubs, or networks, which demonstrate coordinated activity during neurocognitive tasks. The insula, specifically the dAI, has been intricately associated with Salience Network (SN) (Uddin, 2015). The SN also includes hubs in the frontal operculum, dorsal prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex (Schimmelpfennig et al., 2023). The frontal aslant tract (FAT) provides nearly half of the connections between SN nodes while the remaining connections are supplied by association U-fibers (Figure 2) (Papagno et al., 2016; la Corte et al., 2021; Briggs et al., 2022).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Non-Traditional Eloquent Networks (NTENs) and associated white matter tracts. The central executive network (CEN) consists of hubs in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). The arcuate fasciculus (AF), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), and frontal aslant tract (FAT) are the major associate bundles of the CEN. The default mode network (DMN) consists of nodes in anterior and posterior cingulate, lateral parietal cortex, precuneus, ventral medial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), and anterolateral middle temporal cortex. The cingulum bundle and U fibers provide the majority of white matter connections between DMN hubs. The salience network (SN) consists of hubs in the anterior insula, frontal operculum, dorsal prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex. The FAT is the major white matter tract of the SN.

Salience processing is enacted by a regulatory effect on other key networks, the Default Mode Network (DMN) and the Central Executive Network (CEN). The DMN demonstrates decreased activity during attention demanding tasks and increased activity during introspective thought and socially cognitive tasks (Frith and Frith, 1999; Raichle et al., 2001; Kelley et al., 2002). Reciprocally, the CEN demonstrates increased activity during attention-demanding tasks. There is emerging evidence which suggests that our ability to execute goal-directed, cognitively demanding tasks involves coordinated regulation of the DMN and CEN. In this “Triple Network Model” the SN is able to influence behavior by detecting salient stimuli, upregulating activity in the CEN, and downregulating activity of the DMN (Sridharan et al., 2008; Menon and Uddin, 2010). SN, DMN, and CEN have been coined non-traditional eloquent networks (NTENs) because of their newly recognized importance compared to well-described peri-sylvian language pathways and motor pathways. Insular activation has been demonstrated with several salience-related tasks such as the Stroop task, stop-signal task, and Simon task (Sharp et al., 2010; Ham et al., 2013; Gomez-Andres et al., 2022).

Several studies have examined NTEN integrity in glioma patients; however, the overarching theme is that while these networks are often affected by gliomas, the correlation with cognitive outcomes has largely gone unexplored. A recent study reported on 85 patients with insular gliomas and compared topologic changes in gray matter and fractional anisotropy in SN hubs between insular glioma patients and healthy controls (Hu et al., 2023). They demonstrated that insular glioma patients had decreases in FA and gray matter in the insula, basal ganglia, and ACC compared to healthy controls, but neurocognitive outcomes were not reported. A cloud-based machine learning platform to evaluate large-scale networks in brain tumor patients was reported by two investigators (Uddin et al., 2014; Hu et al., 2023). NTENs were affected in 93% of patients in one study, however this study did not include insular glioma patients (Uddin et al., 2014). The other study only enrolled insular glioma patients and the SN was affected in 60% of that cohort, but, again, these findings were not translated to cognitive outcomes (Wu et al., 2023).

Insular glioma surgery today

Insular gliomas were historically considered inoperable lesions given the surrounding complex anatomy. The insula is located at the depths of the sylvian fissure and is covered by the opercula of the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes which are eloquent on the dominant side. It is anatomically segmented by three anteriorly projecting short gyri (posterior, middle, and anterior) and two posteriorly projecting long gyri (anterior and posterior) (Tanriover et al., 2004). The middle cerebral artery, with its perforator branches from the M2 segments, along with lenticulostriate arteries, provides the majority of the blood supply to the insula. These vascular structures often pose the most significant risks in insular glioma surgery and the lenticulostriate arteries in particular limit the medial extent of resection. Additionally, the insula is surrounded by eloquent white matter tracts: arcuate fasciculus (AF), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), uncinate fasciculus (UF), inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), and the corticospinal tract (CST) (Dziedzic et al., 2022).

Yaşargil et al. (1992) was the first to describe the transsylvian approach to insular tumors which he categorized based on location. The Berger-Sanai Classification has become the most common classification and can be used to predict extent of resection (EOR) and aid surgical planning. In this location-based classification, the insula is divided into zones (I anterior-superior; II posterior-superior; III posterior-inferior; and IV anterior-inferior) using a line along the sylvian fissure and bisected by a perpendicular line through the foramen of Monroe (Wu et al., 2011). Applying modern surgical techniques has extended the range of surgical options, leading to targeted resections with acceptable morbidity profiles, such as permanent speech deficits in 0–5% of patients and motor deficits in 2–10%.

There has been a shift in operative approach over the past 20 years. The transcortical method through non-functional “windows” is a common technique given the increased risk of retraction-induced ischemic damage from the transsylvian approach (Duffau et al., 2006; Menon and Uddin, 2010; Wu et al., 2011; Przybylowski et al., 2019; Pitskhelauri et al., 2021). The existing literature suggests that the morbidity profile for modern insular glioma surgery is acceptable (Table 3). While new or worsening language and motor deficits immediately after surgery are common, these symptoms are often transient with the majority of patients having resolution several months after surgery (Kurth et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2011; Ham et al., 2013; Glasser et al., 2016). Permanent speech and motor deficits are observed in 0–5% and 2–10%, respectively. It is important to note, however, that a new language deficit particularly speech, is associated with worse overall survival (OS). Speech deficits are commonly expressive in nature; however, with such limited data describing the effect of insular resection on NTENs, additional studies are needed. While preserving speech and motor function has been a central tenant of neurosurgical oncology, with respect to insular glioma, these neurologic functions merely scratch the surface of the functional capacity of the human insula. In fact, there is evidence that the human insula plays vital role in cognition and behavior, but the clinical impact of intrinsic neoplasms in this critical cerebral region remains poorly understood.

Table 3.

Selected surgical series for insular glioma patients.

First author Year Number of patients N (%) Low-grade glioma N (%) Most common type a Dominant hemisphere N (%) Percent transcortical Percent awake craniotomy Percentage of transient speech/motor deficits Percentage of permanent speech/motor deficits
Yaşargil 1992 177 100 (56) Type 5A 94 (53.1) 0 NS NS NS 5 b
Duffau 2000 12 12 (100) d Type 5A 2 (16.6) 100 25 8 50 0 8
Lang 2000 22 11 (50) NS 13 (59) 36 23 27 18 0 9
Duffau 2006 42 42 (100) d Type 5A 12 (28.6) 100 29 23 50 0 7
Duffau 2009 51 51 (100) d Type 5A 14 (27.5) 94 31 19 37 0 3.9
Simon 2009 94 36 (38) Type 5A 38 (40.4) 75 e 0 NS NS 13 13
Sanai 2010 104 70 (60) Zone I 55 (55.8) 100 57 4.5 7.7 0 1.9
Skrap 2011 66 53 (80) c Type 5A 44 (66) NS 65 11 11 2 2
Hervey-Jumper 2016 114 62 (54) Zone I 60 (52.7) 100 45 16 19 0.8 2.6
Hameed 2019 255 201 (78) Zone I 145 (56.8) 100 NS 5.5 3.5 4.7 8.2
Przybylowski 2020 100 32 (32) Zone III 60 (60) 48 12 5.0 13.0 0.0 10
Li 2020 253 149 (58.9) Zone I 119 (47.0) 100 0 9 11 1 2
Pitskhelauri 2021 79 53 (67) Giant 49 (62) 0 3 16 10 1 5
a

Reported as Berger-Sanai or Yaşargil classification.

b

Combined rate of permanent speech and motor deficits.

c

Study only included non-enhancing lesions.

d

Selected only low-grade glioma.

e

Surgical technique included lobectomies for exposure.

NS, Not Specified.

Future directions

The steady flow of new studies describing the function of the insula has outpaced our ability to translate this knowledge towards management of insular glioma patients. Despite the complex functional role of the insula, many patients with insular glioma present without a noticeable clinical deficit. It is possible, however, that if they were examined by neuro-psychological testing, the presence of cognitive deficits could be identified. Addressing this problem is challenging because of obstacles to widespread and routine cognitive testing for glioma patients. Additionally, patients might have poor insight to their degree of cognitive impairment (Tucha et al., 2000). Functional plasticity is a phenomenon that likely explains these findings (Sanai et al., 2008; Satoer et al., 2016). An example is absence of apparent language impairment in dominant hemisphere of insular glioma patients despite tumor invasion of the operculum. Plasticity however often does not fully preserve function (Berzero et al., 2021).

The potential for radiographic biomarkers of cognitive function in brain tumor patients is emerging. Tumor volume was associated with effect on preoperative cognitive function in patients with LGG, including insula, as quantified by standard MRI sequences and voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) The study consisted of 8 patients with dominant hemisphere insular glioma, and they demonstrated significantly poor performance in object naming. Additionally, VLSM revealed that for both left and right hemispheric glioma groups, 36% of patients with working memory (verbal and visual) impairment had insular involvement (Guarracino et al., 2022).

In a similar study using resting state fMRI, ipsilateral and contralateral connectivity in treatment-naive patients with insular LGG were examined and demonstrated the functional plasticity specifically exhibited by the dominant insula. The authors report significantly increased functional connectivity in 33 edges originating from the dominant insular lobe in patients with non-dominant insular gliomas. This suggests that when glioma cells infiltrate the non-dominant insula, the dominant hemisphere compensates by strengthening original functional connections. In contrast, when LGG originates in the dominant insular lobe, the non-dominant insular lobe must compensate by increasing connectivity to nodes in bilateral hemispheres (Fang et al., 2021). Identifying the biologic factors that alter network dynamics will help to define the cognitive impact of insular glioma.

Conclusion

Understanding of cognitive outcomes in insular glioma patients has evolved yet remains incomplete. The insula’s function as a hub for multimodal sensory processing and emotional regulation further complicates the assessment of these deficits. Modern surgical techniques have decreased the rate of permanent speech and motor deficits. Despite these advances, a significant gap persists in understanding the long-term cognitive and subclinical impacts on patients, especially as survival rates improve. Promising avenues for future research include the use of advanced neuroimaging to characterize cognition radiographically and studies aimed at elucidating the biological factors that alter network dynamics. As the focus shifts toward supramaximal safe resections, further exploration is warranted in three specific areas: 1) the complex functions of the insula to better predict cognitive outcomes, 2) the impact of surgical techniques on cognitive functions, and 3) the development of reliable methods for longitudinal cognitive assessment. Such comprehensive research will allow us to have a meaningful impact on the quality of life for insular glioma patients by understanding with greater clarity the cognitive burden of their disease and risk–benefit profile of surgical intervention.

Author contributions

NN: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BE: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FP: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RY: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. IG: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding Statement

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

  1. Baier B., zu Eulenburg P., Geber C., Rohde F., Rolke R., Maihöfner C., et al. (2014). Insula and sensory insular cortex and somatosensory control in patients with insular stroke. Eur. J. Pain 18, 1385–1393. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.501.x, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker C. M., Burks J. D., Briggs R. G., Conner A. K., Glenn C. A., Robbins J. M., et al. (2018). A Connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum-chapter 5: the insula and Opercular cortex. Oper. Neurosurg. 15, S175–S244. doi: 10.1093/ons/opy259, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berzero G., di Stefano A. L., Ronchi S., Bielle F., Villa C., Guillerm E., et al. (2021). IDH-Wildtype lower-grade diffuse gliomas: the importance of histological grade and molecular assessment for prognostic stratification. Neuro-Oncology 23, 955–966. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/noaa258, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blustajn J., Krystal S., Taussig D., Ferrand-Sorbets S., Dorfmüller G., Fohlen M. (2019). Optimizing the detection of subtle insular lesions on MRI when insular epilepsy is suspected. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 40, 1581–1585. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A6143, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boucher O., Rouleau I., Lassonde M., Lepore F., Bouthillier A., Nguyen D. K. (2015). Social information processing following resection of the insular cortex. Neuropsychologia 71, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.008, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Briggs R. G., Young I. M., Dadario N. B., Fonseka R. D., Hormovas J., Allan P., et al. (2022). Parcellation-based Tractographic Modeling of the salience network through meta-analysis. Brain Behav. 12:e2646. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2646, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chang E. F., Raygor K. P., Berger M. S. (2015). Contemporary model of language organization: an overview for neurosurgeons. J. Neurosurg. 122, 250–261. doi: 10.3171/2014.10.JNS132647, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen P., Wang G., Ma R., Jing F., Zhang Y., Wang Y., et al. (2016). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities in patients with insular glioma. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 962–975. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0445-0, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Craig A. D. (2002). How do You feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Craig A. D. (2009). How do You feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Craig A. D., Chen K., Bandy D., Reiman E. M. (2000). Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 184–190. doi: 10.1038/72131, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Critchley H. D., Harrison N. A. (2013). Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron 77, 624–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. di Cesare G., Marchi M., Errante A., Fasano F., Rizzolatti G. (2018). Mirroring the social aspects of speech and actions: the role of the insula. Cereb. Cortex New York, N.Y. 1991 28, 1348–1357. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx051, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dronkers N. F. (1996). A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384, 159–161. doi: 10.1038/384159a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Duffau H., Capelle L. (2004). Preferential brain locations of low-grade gliomas. Cancer 100, 2622–2626. doi: 10.1002/cncr.20297, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Duffau H., Moritz-Gasser S., Gatignol P. (2009). Functional outcome after language mapping for insular World Health Organization grade II gliomas in the dominant hemisphere: experience with 24 patients. Neurosurg. Focus. 27:E7. doi: 10.3171/2009.5.FOCUS0938, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Duffau H., Taillandier L., Gatignol P., Capelle L. (2006). The insular lobe and brain plasticity: lessons from tumor surgery. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 108, 543–548. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2005.09.004, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dziedzic T. A., Bala A., Marchel A. (2022). Anatomical aspects of the insula, opercula and Peri-insular white matter for a transcortical approach to insular glioma resection. Neurosurg. Rev. 45, 793–806. doi: 10.1007/s10143-021-01602-5, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fan Y., Duncan N. W., de Greck M., Northoff G. (2011). Is there a Core neural network in empathy? An FMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 903–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fang S., Zhou C., Wang Y., Jiang T. (2021). Contralesional functional network reorganization of the insular cortex in diffuse low-grade glioma patients. Sci. Rep. 11:623. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79845-3, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Frith C. D., Frith U. (1999). Interacting minds--a biological basis. Science (New York, N.Y.) 286, 1692–1695. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1692, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Glasser M. F., Coalson T. S., Robinson E. C., Hacker C. D., Harwell J., Yacoub E., et al. (2016). A multi-modal Parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gomez-Andres A., Cunillera T., Rico I., Naval-Baudin P., Camins A., Fernandez-Coello A., et al. (2022). The role of the anterior insular cortex in self-monitoring: a novel study protocol with electrical stimulation mapping and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cortex 157, 231–244. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.09.008, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Guarracino I., Pauletto G., Ius T., Palese F., Skrap M., Tomasino B. (2022). Presurgical cognitive status in patients with low-grade glioma and epilepsy: testing the effects of seizures, Antiseizure medications, and tumor localization. Brain Behav. 12:e2560. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2560, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ham T., Leff A., de Boissezon X., Joffe A., Sharp D. J. (2013). Cognitive control and the salience network: an investigation of error processing and effective connectivity. J. Neurosci. 33, 7091–7098. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4692-12.2013, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hameed N. U., Qiu T., Zhuang D., Lu J., Yu Z., Wu S., et al. (2018). Transcortical insular glioma resection: clinical outcome and predictors. J. Neurosurg. 131, 706–716. doi: 10.3171/2018.4.JNS18424, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hu G., Wu Z., Cao B., Shi Q., Zhang Z., Fan X., et al. (2023). Structural alterations of the salience network in patients with insular glioma. Brain Behav. 13:e2969. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2969, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jensen K. B., Regenbogen C., Ohse M. C., Frasnelli J., Freiherr J., Lundström J. N. (2016). Brain activations during pain: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of patients with pain and healthy controls. Pain 157, 1279–1286. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kelley W. M., Macrae C. N., Wyland C. L., Caglar S., Inati S., Heatherton T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related FMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 785–794. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kurth F., Eickhoff S. B., Schleicher A., Hoemke L., Zilles K., Amunts K. (2010). Cytoarchitecture and probabilistic maps of the human posterior insular cortex. Cereb. Cortex New York, N.Y.: 1991 20, 1448–1461. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp208, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kurth F., Zilles K., Fox P. T., Laird A. R., Eickhoff S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 519–534. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. la Corte E., Eldahaby D., Greco E., Aquino D., Bertolini G., Levi V., et al. (2021). The frontal aslant tract: a systematic review for neurosurgical applications. Front. Neurol. 12:641586. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.641586, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Li Z., Li G., Liu Z., Pan Y., Hou Z., Wu L., et al. (2020). Transcortical approach for insular gliomas: a series of 253 patients. J. Neuro-Oncol. 147, 59–66. doi: 10.1007/s11060-020-03390-2, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Liu R., Solheim K., Polley M.-Y., Lamborn K. R., Page M., Fedoroff A., et al. (2009). Quality of life in low-grade glioma patients receiving Temozolomide. Neuro-Oncology 11, 59–68. doi: 10.1215/15228517-2008-063, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mazzola L., Isnard J., Peyron R., Guénot M., Mauguière F. (2009). Somatotopic Organization of Pain Responses to direct electrical stimulation of the human insular cortex. Pain 146, 99–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.07.014, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Menon V., Uddin L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Morell A. A., Eichberg D. G., Shah A. H., Luther E., Lu V. M., Kader M., et al. (2022). Using machine learning to evaluate large-scale brain networks in patients with brain Tumors: traditional and non-traditional eloquent areas. Neuro-Oncol. Adv. 4:vdac142. doi: 10.1093/noajnl/vdac142, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Naidich T. P., Kang E., Fatterpekar G. M., Delman B. N., Gultekin S. H., Wolfe D., et al. (2004). The insula: anatomic study and MR imaging display at 1.5 T. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 25, 222–232, PMID: [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Noll K. R., Bradshaw M. E., Weinberg J. S., Wefel J. S. (2018). Neurocognitive functioning is associated with functional Independence in newly diagnosed patients with temporal lobe glioma. Neuro-Oncol. Pract. 5, 184–193. doi: 10.1093/nop/npx028, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nomi J. S., Farrant K., Damaraju E., Rachakonda S., Calhoun V. D., Uddin L. Q. (2016). Dynamic functional network connectivity reveals unique and overlapping profiles of insula subdivisions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1770–1787. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23135, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Northoff G. (2012). From emotions to consciousness – a neuro-phenomenal and neuro-relational approach. Front. Psychol. 3:303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00303, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Oh A., Duerden E. G., Pang E. W. (2014). The role of the insula in speech and language processing. Brain Lang. 135, 96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.06.003, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Papagno C., Pisoni A., Mattavelli G., Casarotti A., Comi A., Fumagalli F., et al. (2016). Specific disgust processing in the left insula: new evidence from direct electrical stimulation. Neuropsychologia 84, 29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.036, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Penfield W., Faulk M. E., Jr. (1955). The insula; further observations on its function. Brain J. Neurol. 78, 445–470. doi: 10.1093/brain/78.4.445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pitskhelauri D., Bykanov A., Konovalov A., Danilov G., Buklina S., Sanikidze A., et al. (2021). Transsylvian insular glioma surgery: new classification system, clinical outcome in a consecutive series of 79 cases. Oper. Neurosurg. 20, 541–548. doi: 10.1093/ons/opab051, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Przybylowski C. J., Baranoski J. F., So V. M., Wilson J., Sanai N. (2019). Surgical morbidity of Transsylvian versus transcortical approaches to insular gliomas. J. Neurosurg. 132, 1731–1738. doi: 10.3171/2018.12.JNS183075, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pugnaghi M., Meletti S., Castana L., Francione S., Nobili L., Mai R., et al. (2011). Features of somatosensory manifestations induced by intracranial electrical stimulations of the human insula. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2049–2058. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.03.013, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Raichle M. E., MacLeod A. M., Snyder A. Z., Powers W. J., Gusnard D. A., Shulman G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Renfrow J. J., Julian B.-Q., Brown D. A., Tatter S. B., Laxton A. W., Lesser G. J., et al. (2023). A review on the surgical management of insular gliomas. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 50, 1–9. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2021.248, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sanai N., Mirzadeh Z., Berger M. S. (2008). Functional outcome after language mapping for glioma resection. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 18–27. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa067819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sanai N., Polley M.-Y., McDermott M. W., Parsa A. T., Berger M. S. (2011). An extent of resection threshold for newly diagnosed glioblastomas. J. Neurosurg. 115, 3–8. doi: 10.3171/2011.2.jns10998, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Satoer D., Visch-Brink E., Dirven C., Vincent A. (2016). Glioma surgery in eloquent areas: can we preserve cognition? Acta Neurochir. 158, 35–50. doi: 10.1007/s00701-015-2601-7, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schimmelpfennig J., Topczewski J., Zajkowski W., Jankowiak-Siuda K. (2023). The role of the salience network in cognitive and affective deficits. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1133367. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1133367, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Segerdahl A. R., Mezue M., Okell T. W., Farrar J. T., Tracey I. (2015). The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 499–500. doi: 10.1038/nn.3969, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sharp D. J., Bonnelle V., de Boissezon X., Beckmann C. F., James S. G., Patel M. C., et al. (2010). Distinct frontal Systems for Response Inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 6106–6111. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000175107, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Skrap M., Mondani M., Tomasino B., Weis L., Budai R., Pauletto G., et al. (2012). Surgery of insular nonenhancing gliomas: volumetric analysis of Tumoral resection, clinical outcome, and survival in a consecutive series of 66 cases. Neurosurgery 70, 1081–1094. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e31823f5be5, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sridharan D., Levitin D. J., Menon V. (2008). A critical role for the right Fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tanriover N., Rhoton A. L., Kawashima M., Ulm A. J., Yasuda A. (2004). Microsurgical anatomy of the insula and the Sylvian fissure. J. Neurosurg. 100, 891–922. doi: 10.3171/jns.2004.100.5.0891, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tayah T., Savard M., Desbiens R., Nguyen D. K. (2013). Ictal bradycardia and asystole in an adult with a focal left insular lesion. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 115, 1885–1887. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2013.04.011, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Tucha O., Smely C., Preier M., Lange K. W. (2000). Cognitive deficits before treatment among patients with brain Tumors. Neurosurgery 47, 324–334. doi: 10.1097/00006123-200008000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Uddin L. Q. (2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Uddin L. Q., Kinnison J., Pessoa L., Anderson M. L. (2014). Beyond the tripartite cognition-emotion-interoception model of the human insular cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 16–27. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00462, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Uddin L. Q., Nomi J. S., Hébert-Seropian B., Ghaziri J., Boucher O. (2017). Structure and function of the human insula. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 34, 300–306. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Woolnough O., Forseth K. J., Rollo P. S., Tandon N. (2019). Uncovering the functional anatomy of the human insula during speech. eLife 8:e53086. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53086, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wu Z., Hu G., Cao B., Liu X., Zhang Z., Dadario N. B., et al. (2023). Non-traditional cognitive brain network involvement in Insulo-Sylvian gliomas: a case series study and clinical experience using Quicktome. Chin. Neurosurg. J. 9:16. doi: 10.1186/s41016-023-00325-4, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wu A. S., Witgert M. E., Lang F. F., Xiao L., Bekele B. N., Meyers C. A., et al. (2011). Neurocognitive function before and after surgery for insular gliomas. J. Neurosurg. 115, 1115–1125. doi: 10.3171/2011.8.JNS11488, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yaşargil M. G., von Ammon K., Cavazos E., Doczi T., Reeves J. D., Roth P. (1992). Tumours of the limbic and Paralimbic systems. Acta Neurochir. 118, 40–52. doi: 10.1007/BF01400725, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. zu Eulenburg P., Baumgärtner U., Treede R. D., Dieterich M. (2013). Interoceptive and multimodal functions of the Operculo-insular cortex: tactile, nociceptive and vestibular representations. NeuroImage 83, 75–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.057, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES