Abstract
Reading is one of the most complex skills that we utilize daily, and it involves the early development and interaction of various lower-level subskills, including phonological processing and oral language. These subskills recruit brain structures, which begin to develop long before the skill manifests and exhibit rapid development during infancy. However, how longitudinal trajectories of early brain development in these structures support long-term acquisition of literacy subskills and subsequent reading is unclear. Children underwent structural and diffusion MRI scanning at multiple timepoints between infancy and second grade and were tested for literacy subskills in preschool and decoding and word reading in early elementary school. We developed and implemented a reproducible pipeline to generate longitudinal trajectories of early brain development to examine associations between these trajectories and literacy (sub)skills. Furthermore, we examined whether familial risk of reading difficulty and children’s home literacy environments, two common literacy-related covariates, influenced those trajectories. Results showed that individual differences in curve features (e.g., intercepts and slopes) for longitudinal trajectories of volumetric, surface-based, and white matter organization measures were linked directly to phonological processing and indirectly to first-grade decoding and word reading skills via phonological processing. Altogether, these findings suggest that the brain bases of phonological processing, previously identified as the strongest behavioral predictor of reading and decoding skills, may already begin to develop by birth but undergo further refinement between infancy and preschool. The present study underscores the importance of considering academic skill acquisition from the very beginning of life.
Classifications: Biological Sciences/Neuroscience
Keywords: brain, development, infant, literacy, longitudinal, MRI
1. Introduction
“…the best way to determine how a child learns is to follow them closely while they are learning.” (1, 2).
Reading acquisition is a multifactorial, developmental process that begins long before the skill manifests. Behaviorally, the acquisition of reading necessitates the acquisition and complex interplay of lower-level literacy subskills. In turn, these subskills represent waypoint products of brain development that began in utero. Therefore, understanding how reading skill emerges requires examining its behavioral subskills and the developmental trajectories of the brain areas subserving it starting from the very beginnings of life.
Literacy development represents a model process through which to examine academic skill acquisition, because literacy develops hierarchically, with lower-level “subskills” interacting and driving the emergence of higher-level academic skills (e.g., 3). For instance, phonological processing, which refers to the ability to detect, understand, and manipulate speech sounds (4–7), is the most consistent predictor of subsequent decoding and word reading (4, 8–10). Meanwhile, various studies have shown that both word reading and oral language skills, which encompass abilities supporting listening comprehension (11); e.g., vocabulary and syntactic knowledge), are important subskills for reading comprehension (12). However, these subskills themselves have protracted development and the brain structures that support them begin developing long before they manifest, with the most rapid development transpiring perinatally (13, 14).
Several cross-sectional and prospective studies have been conducted linking performance of these key literacy subskills to brain architecture. For instance, cross-sectional studies in preschoolers and kindergarteners have shown that phonological processing is associated with brain structure and function in left temporoparietal, occipitotemporal, and inferior frontal regions and tracts (15–20). This set of regions has also been linked with preschool and kindergarten oral language skills in some studies (21–23), but other studies have not shown associations (17, 24). Looking to early development, infant brain function and white matter organization have been shown to relate prospectively to (pre)school-age literacy outcomes (25–29), including phonological processing and oral language skills (30–32). Taken together, these studies suggest that certain brain areas may be important for academic skill performance at a particular time during development or as ‘neural scaffolding’ that supports subsequent language development (33). However, recent work in school-age children using large-scale, multi-site cross-sectional datasets showed little evidence for stable associations between individual differences in white matter organization and reading performance. Rather, using a separate longitudinal dataset, investigators found associations between slopes of white matter growth and reading gains, suggesting a dynamic interplay between brain development and learning to read (34). Moreover, the brain itself is a dynamic system that undergoes its most rapid development during infancy and early childhood (13, 14), making cross-sectional and prospective designs suboptimal to capture individual differences in early skill acquisition (2) As such, individual longitudinal trajectories, which capture heterogenous rates of brain and skill development between infancy and second grade, are needed to examine the acquisition of (sub)skills important for literacy.
Therefore, the overall goal of the current study is to examine the relationship between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development and acquisition of reading-related subskills, which we undertook in four objectives. The first objective was to generate individual longitudinal trajectories of early brain development spanning infancy to school age. Prior longitudinal studies have examined trajectories of early brain development but only up to the second year of life (e.g., 35), while others have examined developmental trajectories of brain structure spanning infancy to school age but only by harmonizing cross-sectional and longitudinal datasets acquired and processed with varying methods (e.g., 36). However, to our knowledge, no purely longitudinal studies have mapped the development of brain structure, including white matter organization, from infancy to school age, using a consistent processing pipeline and one that is appropriate for early development. Indeed, methodological challenges associated with infant MRI data have restricted the number and breadth of longitudinal studies in the early developmental period. This is not only because acquiring and processing infant MRI data requires specialized procedures and tools to generate accurate brain estimates (37–42), but also because the use of different procedures and tools for different developmental stages can introduce bias in developmental analyses. However, the alternative—to use identical procedures and tools for all developmental stages—would likely cause estimates to vary in accuracy across ages and could introduce spurious effects and/or obscure true effects in developmental comparisons (43). Some infant-specific tools offer age-specific adjustments (e.g., different sets of templates) within the first two years of life, and the small number of studies that have examined longitudinal trajectories of brain development in the first two years have opted for a balanced approach, with age-appropriate adjustments to limited processing steps in a way that does not require fundamentally different techniques for different ages (e.g., additional sequences or different smoothing kernels; (35, 44). However, methods to accommodate a wider early developmental range are lacking.
In the current study, we extend this work by developing reproducible pipelines for generating longitudinal trajectories of volumetric, surface-based, and white matter organization brain measures from infancy to school age. To do this, we leveraged both infant-specific and standard MRI procedures and tools. We then compared among several candidate linear and nonlinear mixed effects models, varying according to function (e.g., linear, logarithmic) and random parameters (e.g., intercepts alone versus intercepts and slopes), to identify the model with the most parsimonious fit (45). In general, prior longitudinal studies have reported rapid growth at birth that tapers with age (35, 46–48), except for cortical thickness, which peaks between ages one and two years (49), suggesting logarithmic functions might provide a better fit compared with other functions for most measures.
Our second objective was to examine the association of individual differences in early brain development to long-term literacy development. As such, we extracted curve features (e.g., intercepts and slopes) from individual longitudinal trajectories for brain areas and tracts previously shown to relate to reading-related skill development in longitudinal studies in older children (34, 50–59) or prospective studies in infants (30–32). We then tested these curve features for correlations with preschool/early kindergarten phonological processing, a key literacy subskill. Based on converging evidence from prior neuroimaging (30–32) and genetics studies (60, 61), we hypothesized that phonological processing skill would relate to curve intercepts (i.e., brain estimates at birth) and slopes (i.e., rate of brain development). If the foundations of literacy development are largely present at birth and stable across early development, then we expect most of these brain-behavior associations to be with curve intercepts, whereas if the brain development subserving literacy subskills is more protracted or dynamic, as suggested by Roy and colleagues (34), then we expect brain-behavior associations to be with curve slopes.
Our third objective was to examine the roles of risk factors related to literacy skills in shaping longitudinal trajectories of early brain development. Reading difficulty is heritable, as 40–60% of children with a familial risk (e.g., first-degree relative with a history) of reading difficulty (FHD+) themselves develop reading difficulty (62, 63) Brain imaging studies of FHD+ preschoolers show reduced gray matter volume, activation, and fractional anisotropy in left occipito-temporal and temporo-parietal regions (16–18) and tracts (57) compared with FHD-preschoolers; these reductions overlap with those observed in children with reading difficulty (20, 64–70), suggesting that the phenotypes characteristic of reading difficulty manifest in some children before the start of formal reading instruction. Concordantly, similar alterations have been observed earlier in development, where FHD+ infants exhibited lower fractional anisotropy in left arcuate fasciculus compared with FHD-infants (71), distinguishable patterns of functional connectivity in left fusiform gyrus as shown by a support vector machine classifier (72), and alterations in neural responses to basic speech sounds as measured by event-related potentials (73–75). Possibly underlying this heritability, work in genetics, including recent large-scale genome-wide studies, has shown that reading-related skills (60) and reading difficulty (76, 77) are associated with variation in genes involved in early developmental processes, including neurogenesis and axon guidance (78).
Another risk factor repeatedly shown to affect children’s literacy skills is the home literacy environment, which includes caregiver–child shared reading and reading-related resources, in preschool literacy skills (79–84) and in infancy/toddlerhood (83, 85–90). Recent work has also identified links between the home literacy environment and brain architecture in preschoolers/kindergarteners (21, 24, 91–94) and infants (95, 96). Overall, these studies offer strong evidence that genetic and environmental factors related to the development of reading-related skills are likely to affect longitudinal trajectories of brain development starting perinatally. Therefore, we hypothesized that the FHD status and home literacy environment would significantly contribute, as covariates, to longitudinal trajectories of brain development in left hemisphere temporo-parietal, occipito-temporal, and inferior frontal regions and tracts.
Our fourth objective was to broaden the scope of our examination of early brain-literacy associations to other literacy-related subskills and subsequent reading skills. Reading acquisition is a hierarchical process in which multiple distinct but interacting subskills (including but not limited to phonological processing) converge to effect higher-order skills. First, we tested the specificity of associations with phonological processing by examining another subskill important for reading comprehension, oral language skill. Second, we tested whether phonological processing mediated relationships between curve features of early brain development and subsequent measures of decoding and word reading skill. Taken altogether, findings from this study will inform our understanding of how early brain development contributes to later reading-related skill acquisition.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Children examined in this study were obtained from two longitudinal cohorts: the New England dataset and the Calgary open-source dataset (https://osf.io/axz5r/). Only children with high-quality (i.e., post-QC, please see below) MRI data from at least two timepoints between birth and late childhood were included. Due to the multi-modal nature of the study, varying numbers of longitudinal datasets were used for structural (n = 98 with 276 observations) versus diffusion analyses (n = 128 with 396 observations). For the New England cohort, family history (i.e., first-degree relative with a history) of reading difficulty was also examined and present in 40 of 80 children. For children ages ≤ 24 months, demographic information was provided by caregivers. Please see Table 1 for overall demographic details, Supplementary Table 1 for demographic details by modality, and Supplementary Methods for participant information, including race demographics, unique to each cohort. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Boston Children’s Hospital (IRB-P00023182), Harvard University (IRB21–0916), and the University of Calgary Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board (REB13–0020). Participants’ parents gave informed consent, and children gave verbal assent if over 25 months old. Data examined here partially overlap with brain images analyzed in previous studies examining infant brain development (30–32, 36, 48, 54, 71, 72, 96–102).
Table 1.
Participant Demographics
General information | Numbers participants | observations | 137 | 441 |
Number observations per participant | 3 ± 1 | |
Age at literacy-related subskill and cognitive testing (months) | 63 ± 5.3 | |
Age at decoding/word reading testing (months) | 82 ± 6.4 | |
Covariates | Biological sex (F/M) | 73/64 |
Maternal education (years) | 17 ± 2.1 | |
Cohort ([New England]/Calgary) | 80/57 | |
Family history of reading difficulty (+/-) | 40/40 | |
Home literacy environment (a.u.) | 0.037 ± 0.41 | |
Literacy-related subskills | Phonological processing standard score | 106 ± 14 |
Oral language standard score | 113 ± 13 | |
Decoding/word reading | Word attack standard score | 112 ± 14 |
Word identification standard score | 110 ± 17 | |
Cognitive abilities | Nonverbal general cognitive ability | 106 ± 13 |
2.2. Environmental variables
Socioeconomic status was measured with maternal education, consistent with prior brain imaging studies on socioeconomic status (103–108). Maternal education measures were collected during each timepoint, although inter-time-point variability was low, and these were averaged to generate one socioeconomic status measure across the developmental window. Rather than using ordinal coding, years of education as a continuous measure ranging from 12 to 20 years were used.
For the New England cohort, parents also completed at each time point questionnaires relating to children’s home literacy environments home literacy environment, which includes the extent of parent-child shared reading and access to reading-related resources (109). Responses were indicated using ordinal scales ranging from 1 to 6. As responses were non-normally distributed (p < 0.05 according to the Shapiro-Wilk normality test; Supplementary Figure 1), except for “Time read to per week” for all timepoints and “Frequency with which family members share rhymes or jokes with the child” for one timepoint, they were normalized and then averaged at each timepoint according to procedure used previously (30). As with maternal education, home literacy environment estimates, which exhibited low inter-timepoint variability, were averaged to generate one estimate per individual across the developmental window. These overall home literacy environment estimates, which were normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk W = 0.99, p > 0.05; Supplementary Figure 2), were used in later statistical analyses.
2.3. Literacy and cognitive measures
Two literacy subskills were administered to children prior to the beginning of formal reading instruction: phonological processing and oral language. These constructs were selected as representative subskills supporting literacy development (3, 4, 8–10, 12, 110); however, they constitute a small subset of literacy-related measures collected for this cohort. No outliers were detected using the isoutlier function in MATLAB, which sets an outlier threshold at three scaled median absolute deviations from the median.
Phonological processing was measured in both New England and Calgary cohorts. For the New England cohort, the phonological processing composite was estimated from three subtests from the WJ-IV Tests of Cognitive Abilities: word access, word fluency, and substitution (111). Word access measures phonetic coding by asking children to identify words containing certain sounds. Word fluency measures speed of lexical access by asking children to name as many words as possible beginning with a certain sound in one minute. Substitution measures children’s ability to produce a new word by replacing one sound from a provided word with another sound. Importantly, while the word access and word fluency subtests require some level of lexical access, these subtests, along with the substitution subtest, are well established measures of phonological/phonemic processing/awareness. For additional details and item examples from the technical manual, please see the Supplementary Methods. Children in the Calgary cohort were administered the phonological processing subtest of the NEPSY-II, which measures phonemic awareness (112). Unlike the New England cohort, each child from the Calgary cohort completed this subtest multiple times. To harmonize phonological processing scores across the two datasets, we used NEPSY-II scores from when the child was closest in age to the average age at which the New England cohort completed the WJ-IV (64 months) and not earlier than 50 months of age.
The composite oral language, which was only measured in the New England cohort, was estimated from two subtests from the WJ-IV Tests of Oral Language: picture vocabulary and oral comprehension (113). Picture vocabulary measures lexical knowledge by asking children to specify a picture corresponding to a given word or naming an object. Oral comprehension measures oral listening, vocabulary, and reasoning by asking children to identify missing words from short passages. All assessments were administered and double-scored by testers trained by a clinical psychologist and then raw scores were converted to standard scores.
In addition, we measured decoding and word reading with two untimed subtests from the Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests III (114): word attack and word identification. For word attack, children were presented with pseudowords that they needed to decode using phonological abilities. For word identification, children were presented with individual real words that they needed to read. Word attack and word identification subtests were administered at the beginning of formal reading instruction.
Lastly, we measured children’s nonverbal general cognitive ability at preschool/early kindergarten-age using the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test: 2nd Edition (KBIT-2, (115)). Herein, children were asked to identify the piece missing from a matrix of visual images.
All raw estimates for each of the WJ-IV, NEPSY-II, and WRMT subtests were non-normally distributed (p < 0.05 according to the Shapiro-Wilk normality test), except for preschool/early kindergarten picture vocabulary and late kindergarten/grade 1 word attack (Supplementary Figure 3). All standardized (composite) estimates used in subsequent analyses were normally distributed (p > 0.05; Supplementary Figure 4).
2.4. MRI data acquisition and processing
All data were acquired on a 3.0 T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. Please see Supplementary Methods for cohort-specific acquisition parameters.
The rapid brain growth transpiring immediately after birth posed serious challenges for structural MRI processing because standard methods that are optimal for older children are suboptimal for infants and vice versa (43). To circumvent bias associated with choosing a single pipeline for multiple developmental stages, we implemented pipelines that are age-appropriate but not fundamentally different for each developmental stage.
2.4.1. Structural MRI processing and quality control
Raw magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) images were visually inspected for artifacts by a trained rater and scored as “fail,” “check,” or “pass” (116). Only images scored as “check” or “pass” underwent image processing. The standard, unmodified FreeSurfer v7.3 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) “recon” pipeline was used for brains > 50 months. Processing procedures for brains < 50 months were similar in concept to those described in (117). Namely, Infant FreeSurfer (for brains ≤ 24 months) or standard FreeSurfer (for brains 25 to 50 months) was used to extract the brain from the skull, correct for intensity inhomogeneity, and segment MPRAGE images by tissue class (gray matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid) and subcortical brain region (40); https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/infantFS). To improve tissue classification accuracy, MPRAGE images were submitted in parallel to iBEATv2.0 Docker 1.0.0 (118–120); https://github.com/iBEAT-V2/iBEAT-V2.0-Docker), has been validated for birth to age six years (118). Resulting segmentations from each software package were then hybridized using in-house MATLAB code that combined the cortical pial and white matter boundaries labelled by iBEATv2.0 with the subcortical parcellations of Infant FreeSurfer (brains ≤ 24 months) or standard Freesurfer (brains 25 to 50 months), effectively relabeling cortical gray and white matter in the FreeSurfer-style segmentation; for details, please see the Supplementary Methods. Structural processing was finalized by submitting these FreeSurfer-style hybrid segmentations to a modified version of the standard FreeSurfer v7.3 “recon” pipeline, which, for brains ≤ 24 months, incorporated elements from Infant FreeSurfer. For a schematic of the structural processing pipeline, please see Supplementary Figure 5.
Resulting white and pial surfaces were visually inspected by two (brains > 50 months) or three (brains ≤ 24 months) trained raters on a 3-point scale (0, 1, and 2) and datasets with average ratings > 1.5 were retained for subsequent analyses. Finally, parcellations were visualized to ensure accuracy of anatomical labels. For reproducibility purposes, we did not perform manual editing to correct tissue mislabeling in the remaining images; however, structural measures from edited and unedited pediatric brain images processed with FreeSurfer have been shown to be highly correlated (121), including in children (122). Measures of gray and white matter volume, surface area, cortical thickness, and mean curvature, generated in the final FreeSurfer steps, were extracted from 8 a priori left hemisphere regions delineated with the Desikan-Killiany atlas—banks of the superior temporal sulcus, fusiform gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, middle temporal gyrus, pars opercularis, pars triangularis, superior temporal gyrus, and supramarginal gyrus—based on their reported involvement in reading-related subskills (15–23, 30–32, 54, 60; for reviews, please see 123, Table 3 and 124).
2.4.2. Diffusion MRI processing and quality control
Preprocessed and tractography for all diffusion-weighted image (DWI) data, regardless of age, were performed with MRtrix3 based on the pipeline established for the Developing Human Connectome Project (41, 125). DWI data were first denoised using Marchenko–Pastur principal component analysis (126–128) and then corrected for susceptibility distortions, eddy currents, motion, and intensity inhomogeneity using FSL’s topup and eddy (with slice-to-volume correction) functions (129–133), and Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) N4 bias correction tool (134). Subsequently, three tissue response functions for spherical deconvolution were estimated using the Dhollander algorithm, a 0.1 fractional anisotropy threshold, and eight maximum harmonic degrees (135). Fiber orientation densities (FODs) were computed with multi-shell, multi-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution (136, 137) and then normalized using multi-tissue informed log-domain intensity normalization.
Two million streamlines were tracked from the normalized FOD maps using the Anatomically Constrained Tractography (ACT) technique. Importantly, ACT has been shown to greatly improve tractography, but it relies on accurate tissue segmentations, which are typically challenging to generate with infant brain data. Using the hybrid segmentations generated for brains ≤ 50 months (please see above) circumvented this challenge. Thus, FreeSurfer-style hybrid segmentations for brains ≤ 50 months and standard FreeSurfer segmentations for brains > 50 months were registered to the preprocessed DWI images using ANTs and then converted to five-tissue-type images for ACT. The remaining whole-brain tractography parameters included seeding at the gray/white matter boundary and tracking with the iFOD1 probabilistic algorithm; step size, minimum and maximum length, and maximum step angle were set to default (138, 139).
Resulting whole-brain tractography was then submitted to the open-source instantiation of Automated Fiber Quantification (AFQ; (140) for waypoint- and probabilistic-atlas-based fiber tract segmentation. The standard pyAFQ pipeline was used for brains > 24 months (141), whereas pyBabyAFQ was used for brains ≤ 24 months (142). Please see Supplementary Methods for a summary of differences between the two AFQ instantiations. Next, tracts were resampled to 100 equidistant nodes, and diffusion properties (fractional anisotropy; mean diffusivity) were quantified for each node for the left hemisphere tracts of interest—arcuate fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus, and inferior longitudinal fasciculus. Mean fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity values for each tract were used to examine model fits (please see section 2.5.1), whereas a more fine-grained, node-based approach was taken for brain-behavior analyses (please see section 2.5.2). For the latter, to better align tract cores across participants, five nodes on either end were removed, reducing the total number of nodes per tract to 90. Finally, tracts of interest were visually inspected by two trained raters on a 3-point scale (0, 1, and 2) and datasets with average ratings ≥ 1 were retained for subsequent analyses. For a schematic of the diffusion processing pipeline, please see Supplementary Figure 6.
2.5. Statistical analyses
An overview of statistical operations, all of which were performed in RStudio version 4.2.2, can be found in Supplementary Figure 7.
2.5.1. Longitudinal trajectory estimation
Prior to modeling, estimates of brain structure and white matter organization underwent a final quality control procedure to remove estimates for brain areas/tracts of interest if, for brains > 25 months, they were preceded or followed by ≥ 10% annual change (positive or negative) in any measure and for brains ≤ 24 months, inter-observation changes were negative (or positive for mean diffusivity). To mitigate data loss, we next identified which observation—earlier or later—was more likely to be inaccurate, using an outlier detection procedure (described in the Supplementary Methods). All remaining participants after these procedures had multiple observations.
To generate longitudinal trajectories (i.e., growth curves), we next submitted cleaned, longitudinal structural and white matter organization estimates to linear mixed effects models using linear, logarithmic, and quadratic functions from the R ‘lme4’ package. Intercepts and slopes were modeled as fixed and random effects, and covariates for biological sex, socioeconomic status, and cohort (New England or Calgary) were entered as fixed effects. Vijayakumar and colleagues recommends comparing model fits quantitatively (45); consequently, we computed Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) metrics and the model (i.e., function and number of random terms) that provided the best fit (i.e., lowest BIC value) was selected for subsequent analyses. Correlations between intercepts and slopes were tested to ensure associations between random terms were minimized. Most curve features were normally distributed according to the Shapiro-Wilk normality test (p > 0.05) and histograms depicting their distributions are provided in Supplementary Figures 9–15.
2.5.2. Associations between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development and phonological processing
Next, individual-level curve features (i.e., intercepts and slopes) were extracted and tested for correlations (Pearson) with literacy subskills. The extracted curve features represent individual variation in intercepts or slopes beyond what would be predicted based on the covariates (e.g., biological sex); consequently, we did not include these same covariates in the tests of correlations. To determine whether curve features of volumetric and surface-based measures were associated with phonological processing in the full sample after accounting for multiple brain regions (8 tests), a significance threshold was set to pFDR < 0.05 and applied to measures (e.g., gray matter volume) separately. As diffusion analyses were performed node-wise (90 nodes), we corrected for multiple comparisons at pFWE < 0.05 using a permutation-based, threshold-free cluster enhancement method (143), implemented in the permuco package in R (144); corrections were performed separately for each tract. These methods are similar to those used previously (24, 96, 108). When significant, individual growth curves were separated into three groups according to scores on their behavioral assessment with low < 85, 85 ≤ average ≤ 115, and high > 115, averaged by group, and then plotted.
2.5.3. Sensitivity analyses
We conducted four sensitivity analyses to test the reliability of our results. First, we performed a replication analysis on gray/white matter volume, surface area, and mean diffusivity with nonlinear mixed effects models using asymptotic functions from the R ‘nlme’ package (145), similar to that described in Alex and colleagues (36); https://github.com/knickmeyer-lab/ORIGINs_ICV-and-Subcortical-volume-development-in-early-childhood). Intercepts and asymptotes were modeled as fixed and random effects; rate constants were modeled as fixed effects. Second, instead of testing correlations between curve features and outcomes, we entered outcomes as main and interaction terms in linear mixed effects models. Third, adhering to recommendations to report both raw and TIV-corrected results (45), we recomputed brain-behavior associations for volumetric and surface-based measures using semipartial correlations (Pearson) with the random terms from longitudinal modeling with TIV as covariates of no interest. Fourth, we submitted volumetric and surface-based brain-behavior associations to semipartial correlations (Pearson) with average (across timepoint) Euler numbers, which quantifies topological defects (146). For additional details on these sensitivity analyses and Euler quantification, please see the Supplementary Methods.
2.5.4. Specificity analyses
To determine whether brain-behavior effects were specific to certain brain measures, regions, curve features, and behavioral outcomes, we performed three specificity analyses. First, we generated whole-brain maps depicting variability in associations with phonological processing according to brain measures (e.g., gray matter volume), region, and curve feature. Second, we examined whether brain-behavior associations for diffusion measures persisted in right arcuate fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus, and inferior longitudinal fasciculus. Third, limited to the New England children, we tested correlations between curve features of brain development and oral language skill, another reading subskill, and nonverbal general cognitive abilities. For subsequent testing of literacy-related covariates, we also reanalyzed brain-behavior associations with phonological processing in the New England subsample only. For volumetric and surface-based measures, FDR correction accounted for brain regions and behavioral measures (24 tests). For diffusion, cluster-level FWE correction accounted for nodes (90 tests).
2.5.5. Literacy-related factors as fixed effects in models of early brain development
We determined whether literacy-related covariates (i.e., home literacy environment and FHD status) contributed to the fit of the growth curve in two ways. First, we examined each covariate as fixed effects in models of brain development for measures and regions whose curve features related to phonological processing (please see brain-behavior associations in 2.5.2). For brain structure, a significance threshold of pFDR < 0.05 was applied to correct for the multiple brain development models that fit this criterion. Diffusion analyses, which were performed node-wise for each tract separately (90 tests), were corrected for multiple comparisons using a lenient cluster size threshold of 5 nodes, as threshold-free cluster enhancement method was not available for the statistical tests applied to covariates. Second, we compared brain development models with and without each covariate using BIC metrics. For diffusion, BIC metrics were obtained using tract averages (rather than separately on individual nodes). Correction for multiple comparisons was again set to pFDR < 0.05 and applied as described above for brain structure, this time also including statistics for average diffusion measures.
2.5.6. Indirect effects between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development and word reading outcomes via literacy subskills
As with previous work (147), indirect effects were tested when literacy subskills were related to curve features of brain development and decoding and word reading outcomes (after FDR correction). Formal indirect effects modeling was conducted using the Mediation package in R with 20,000 bootstrapped samples, and effects were determined significant when the 95% confidence interval for the average causal mediation effect did not include 0. All mediation models included covariates for maternal education, home literacy environment, and family history of reading difficulty. As in the sensitivity analyses, models were run twice, once without controlling for TIV and once including TIV curve features. Because mediation testing was limited to data with significant (after correction for multiple comparisons) brain-behavior associations, no additional correction for multiple correction was applied; this is consistent with prior literature (36, 147, 148).
2.6. Data availability
All code used to process and analyze has been made openly available at https://github.com/TeddyTuresky/Longitudinal-Trajectories-Early-Brain-Development-Language. The Calgary dataset is also freely available at https://osf.io/axz5r/. All Boston/Cambridge data used in this study will be made publicly available at upon acceptance of the manuscript.
3. Results
3.1. Longitudinal trajectories of brain structure and white matter organization in left hemisphere literacy-related brain regions and tracts
Our first objective was to generate accurate longitudinal trajectories of early brain development in regions and tracts reportedly involved in reading-related subskills (15–23, 30–32, 54, 60; for reviews, please see 123, Table 3 and 124). Accordingly, growth curves for regional estimates of gray and white matter volume, surface area, cortical thickness, and mean curvature and for tract estimates of fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity were generated from 137 children (F/M = 73/64) with 441 observations. Every child had at least two observations between birth and school age with 77 having at least one observation in infancy (Figure 1; please see Supplementary Figure 16 for age distributions by modality). Linear mixed effects models included individual-level intercepts and slopes as well as covariates for biological sex, socioeconomic status, and cohort. All structural and diffusion measures for all regions and tracts were best fit with logarithmic functions according to Bayesian Information Criterion (Supplementary Table 2), whereby rates of development were steeper perinatally and then slowed across the first ten years following birth (Figure 2; Supplementary Figures 17–22).
Figure 1.
Age distribution of longitudinal dataset from infancy to late childhood. All children had structural and/or diffusion MRI data from at least two observations (dots). Blue, New England cohort; orange, Calgary cohort.
Figure 2.
Average longitudinal trajectories from infancy to late childhood by measure. Raw estimates for each brain region examined and each measure were submitted to linear mixed effects models using a logarithmic function. Individual growth curves predicted by this model were averaged to show the overall longitudinal trajectory of the sample for each volumetric/surface-based (blue lines) and diffusion (green lines) measure. Absolute brain estimates were then converted to percent change values to visualize all brain measures along a single axis. For growth curves for separate brain regions and tracts, please see Supplementary Figures 17–22.
3.2. Associations between growth curve features and phonological processing
Our second objective was to examine whether brain development affects literacy development. Consequently, curve features (i.e., intercepts and slopes) were extracted from individual-level growth curves and tested for correlations (Pearson) with preschool/early kindergarten phonological processing scores, a subskill critical for literacy (3, 8).
For volumetric and surface-based measures, curve features of gray/white matter volume and surface area in several left hemisphere regions exhibited associations with phonological processing. Specifically, greater phonological processing was associated with (a) greater intercepts of gray matter volume in the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus, (b) greater slopes of white matter volume in left occipitotemporal, temporoparietal, and inferior frontal regions, and (c) greater surface area intercepts and slopes in inferior parietal lobule, pars triangularis, and superior temporal gyrus (Supplementary Figure 23). Next, individual growth curves corresponding to these measures and regions were separated into three groups according to phonological processing scores with low < 85, 85 ≤ average ≤ 115, and high > 115, averaged by group, and then plotted to visualize variation in longitudinal trajectories by phonological processing score. Most notably, children with lower phonological processing scores tended to have less gray matter volume in left banks of the superior temporal sulcus at birth but maintained similar rates of development compared with higher scoring children. At the same time, they had lower rates of white matter volume growth in left occipitotemporal, temporoparietal, and inferior frontal regions (Figure 3).
Figure 3.
Longitudinal trajectories of brain structure from infancy to late childhood according to phonological processing skill in preschool/early kindergarten. Graphs depict average trajectories for children with low (< 85), average (85 – 115), and high (> 115) standardized phonological processing scores for measures and regions whose (A) intercepts, (B) slopes, or (C) both intercepts and slopes correlated with phonological processing (pFDR < 0.05). Correlation statistics are reported adjacent to their corresponding plots; intercept and slope statistics for surface area averaged here for visualization purposes but reported separately in Supplemental Figure 23. As a group, children with low phonological processing in preschool/early kindergarten tended to have attenuated longitudinal trajectories, either because they began with lower estimates, as with less gray matter volume in the left banks of the superior temporal gyrus at birth (upper left graph), or because they had slower rates of development, as with white matter volume in other left hemisphere brain regions.
Curve features of white matter organization in the left arcuate fasciculus also correlated with phonological processing outcomes, as greater phonological processing skill was associated with greater slopes of mean diffusivity (Supplementary Figure 24). When distinguishing longitudinal trajectories according to literacy subskills (i.e., low < 85, 85 ≤ average ≤ 115, and high > 115), as with volumetric and surface-based measures above, children with low phonological processing scores tended to have higher mean diffusivity at birth but greater rates of mean diffusivity development (i.e., more negative slopes) compared with higher scoring children (Figure 4). No significant associations were observed for the superior or inferior longitudinal fasciculus.
Figure 4.
Longitudinal trajectories of mean diffusivity from infancy to late childhood according to phonological processing skill in preschool/early kindergarten. Graph depicts average trajectories for children with low (< 85), average (85 – 115), and high (> 115) standardized phonological processing scores for the left arcuate fasciculus nodes whose slopes correlated with phonological processing (pFWE < 0.05). Children with low phonological processing tended to exhibit faster development (i.e., more negative slope) in anterior arcuate fasciculus.
3.3. Sensitivity Analyses
We performed four sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of the observed brain-behavior relationships. For the first sensitivity analysis, we re-fit our volumetric, surface area, and mean diffusivity estimates with nonlinear mixed effects models using asymptotic functions (Supplementary Figures 25–28). This alternative model to characterize longitudinal trajectories was especially important for mean diffusivity findings, where intercepts and slopes from the main analysis were correlated for many nodes in each tract examined.
We observed that greater phonological processing was still associated with greater intercepts of gray matter volume development in the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus and of surface area in left inferior parietal lobule, pars triangularis, and superior temporal gyrus (Supplementary Table 3A, C). Interestingly, when trajectories were again split according to low (< 85), average (85 – 115), and high (> 115) standardized phonological processing scores, intercepts were saliently divided, more so than when using linear models (Supplementary Figure 3A,C). Whereas our main analyses allowed us to examine relationships between brain development and literacy subskills directly by using a random slopes term, the nonlinear model used in this sensitivity analysis replaces the random slopes term with a random asymptote term. As such, relationships between brain development and language outcomes may only be inferred where brain measures and regions exhibit asymptote-behavior correlations without corresponding intercept-behavior correlations. Models for middle and superior temporal cortices and supramarginal gyrus white matter volume did not converge. However, other brain regions whose white matter volume and surface area slopes correlated positively with phonological processing in the main analysis also exhibited significant associations between asymptotes and phonological processing. Effect sizes in were generally smaller for white matter volume and larger for surface area compared with the main analysis (Supplementary Tables 3B, C; Supplementary Figure 3B,C). Regarding white matter organization, greater phonological processing was associated with lower mean diffusivity intercepts in left arcuate fasciculus (Supplementary Tables 3D).
Our second sensitivity analysis involved adding phonological processing main and age x phonological processing interaction terms as covariate analogues to brain-behavior correlations with intercepts and slopes, respectively. All results for gray and white matter volume and mean diffusivity present in the main analysis persisted in this sensitivity analysis; however, surface area effects did not (Supplementary Table 4).
For the third sensitivity analysis, we recomputed brain-behavior associations for volumetric and surface-based measures using semipartial correlations, controlling for curve features of the longitudinal trajectory for total intracranial volume (TIV). Relative to the results of the main analysis, effect sizes were reduced when including curve features of TIV. However, associations with phonological processing remained significant for intercepts of the banks of the superior temporal sulcus gray matter and inferior parietal lobule, pars triangularis, and superior temporal gyrus surface area. Slopes of white matter volume development in inferior parietal lobule and pars triangularis also remained significant (Supplementary Table 5).
In the fourth sensitivity analysis, we again recomputed brain-behavior associations, this time controlling for Euler numbers as a reproducible alternative to manual quality control (149). On average, effect sizes showed no drop relative to the main analysis (ravg = 0.35). All brain-behavior associations significant in the main analysis remained significant with the inclusion of Euler numbers, except for the association between phonological processing and slopes of superior temporal gyrus surface area (Supplementary Table 6).
3.4. Specificity analyses
We also performed analyses to determine whether brain-behavior associations were limited to specific brain measures, regions, and curve features or contingent upon specific behavioral measures. Whole-brain analyses showed higher brain-behavior effects for white matter volume slopes compared (numerically) to other morphometric measures, especially in left inferior parietal lobule; however, effects did not appear to be specific to the left hemisphere (Supplementary Figure 32). In contrast, brain-behavior associations with mean diffusivity did not replicate in right hemisphere homologue tracts. In a subset of data, we also tested whether associations were specific to phonological processing, to other reading subskills, or to cognitive measures in general. Effects with phonological processing persisted in this subset for all brain-behavior associations except for intercepts of surface area in left inferior parietal and superior temporal cortices. However, similar effects were not observed for oral language skills or nonverbal general cognitive ability (Supplementary Table 7).
3.5. Contributions of literacy-related factors to longitudinal trajectories of brain structure and white matter organization
Next, we sought to examine whether the brain-behavior associations, observed in both the full sample and the New England cohort only, are driven by two common literacy-related factors: family history of reading difficulty and the home literacy environment. When modeled as fixed effects, neither constituted significant contributors to the longitudinal trajectories of early brain development that predicted phonological processing (Supplementary Table 8). Furthermore, BIC estimates for models with versus without literacy-related covariates were not significantly different for any brain measures or regions (Supplementary Table 9).
3.6. Indirect effects between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development and word reading outcomes via phonological processing
Finally, we examined whether literacy subskills mediated the relationship between brain development curve features (e.g., intercepts and slopes) and decoding and word reading outcomes. As a prerequisite for mediation, the mediator (i.e., phonological processing) must be associated with both the predictor (i.e., brain estimate) and outcome (i.e., word reading). We limited the potential mediator to phonological processing, which related to both decoding (r = 0.48; p < 0.005) and word reading (r = 0.53; p < 0.001), as measured by the Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests III word attack and word identification subtests (114), because brain-behavior associations with oral language were few and inconsistent in sensitivity analyses, and we limited the potential predictors to brain measures and regions/tracts surviving FDR correction in the main analysis. Indirect effects were reported when the 95% confidence intervals, based on 20,000 bootstrapped samples, for the average causal mediation effect did not include 0 (please see Methods).
Phonological processing mediated the relationship between brain and decoding and between brain and word reading for the following measures and regions: intercepts of gray matter volume development in the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus (Figure 5A). Phonological processing skill also mediated associations between decoding/word reading and slopes of white matter volume development and intercepts and slopes of surface area development in left temporo-parietal and inferior frontal regions (Figure 5B, C; Supplementary Tables 10, 11). Indirect effect sizes nominally attenuated when controlling for TIV curve features for decoding (average estimate with TIV = 0.016, average estimate without TIV = 0.020) and word reading (average estimate with TIV = 0.024, average estimate without TIV = 0.031; Supplementary Tables 12, 13). Lastly, phonological processing skill mediated associations between slopes of mean diffusivity development in left arcuate fasciculus and decoding and word reading (Figure 5D).
Figure 5.
Phonological processing skill mediates the relationship between early brain development and decoding and word reading. Indirect effects (filled arrows) were found for (A) intercepts of gray matter volume in the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus; (B) slopes of white matter volume and (C) intercepts and slopes of surface area in left temporo-parietal and inferior frontal regions; and (D) slopes of mean diffusivity in left arcuate fasciculus (nodes 6–25, green). Note: indirect effects are depicted for surface area slopes only; surface area intercept effects are reported in Supplementary Tables 10, 11.
4. Discussion
The brain regions and tracts that eventually support decoding and word reading begin to develop long before the skills themselves emerge. Here, we examined the relationship between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development and acquisition of reading-related (sub)skills in four objectives. First, we generated longitudinal trajectories of early brain structure, including white matter organization, from infancy to school age in regions and tracts previously linked to literacy development (30–32, 34, 50–58) using a novel processing and analysis pipeline appropriate for the early developmental period. Findings showed that longitudinal trajectories were best modeled using logarithmic, compared with linear and quadratic, functions. Second, we examined associations between curve features of longitudinal trajectories and a key literacy subskill: phonological processing. Results showed that curve intercepts (i.e., birth brain estimates) of gray matter volume and surface area and curve slopes (i.e., early brain development) of white matter volume, surface area, and mean diffusivity predicted phonological processing measured in preschool/early kindergarten. While effects were robust in hypothesized left temporo-parietal, occipito-temporal, and inferior frontal regions and tracts, specificity analyses suggest that these brain-behavior associations are not limited to these regions. The predominance and magnitude of slope-outcome associations in comparison with intercept-outcome associations suggests a less stable, more dynamic relationship between brain and literacy development (34). Third, we examined whether familial risk of reading difficulty and home literacy environment, two common literacy-related covariates, influenced those trajectories and found that they did not. Fourth, we expanded the scope of our inquiry to long-term literacy development, showing that phonological processing mediated associations between early brain development and decoding and word reading skills between late kindergarten and second grade. Overall, these findings suggest that the neural foundations for the subsequent development of phonological processing may be partially present at birth but are still forming in the years between birth and preschool and eventually support the development of decoding and word reading skills.
Trajectories of early brain development have been generated from longitudinal studies up to the first two years of life (e.g., 35) and from combined cross-sectional and longitudinal datasets spanning infancy to school age (e.g., 36). However, methodological challenges associated with infant MRI and longitudinal designs in general have created a relative vacuum of longitudinal MRI studies spanning infancy to school age (43). The current study fills this gap as a purely longitudinal examination of early structural brain development in which longitudinal trajectories are generated with a pipeline designed for the early developmental period. Overall, longitudinal trajectories of gray and white matter volume, surface area, fractional anisotropy, and mean diffusivity exhibited rapid postnatal growth that slowed with age, which largely comports with previous early developmental studies using cross-sectional (150–154) and combined longitudinal and cross-sectional designs (13, 36, 46, 48, 155, 156). As shown previously (49, 154), the curves for cortical thickness and mean curvature were generally less steep compared with other brain measures, both initially following birth and into childhood. Nonetheless, empirical comparisons with linear and quadratic functions showed that logarithmic models were more parsimonious for all measures examined.
After generating longitudinal trajectories, we examined how this brain development influenced literacy development by relating curve features of individual trajectories to reading-related subskills measured in preschool/early kindergarten as well as decoding and word reading skills measured between kindergarten and second grade. The finding of direct links to phonological processing and indirect links to decoding and word reading from curve intercepts of gray matter volume (i.e., gray matter volume at birth) specifically in the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus is compelling in the context of a recent large-scale genomics study showing common genetic influences on surface area in this region and reading-related skills (60), including from a gene involved in neurogenesis and axon formation (78). Combined with observed intercept-outcome associations for surface area in other left hemisphere regions and prior work showing an association between reading difficulty and left temporo-parietal sulcal patterns determined in utero (157), these findings offer convergent evidence for a mechanistic pathway through which genetic factors shape the foundations of reading development via prenatal left temporo-parietal brain development. While this intercept-outcome effect remained robust through all four sensitivity analyses, this interpretation should be viewed with caution, as specificity analyses suggest intercept-outcome effects may not be limited to the left banks of the superior temporal sulcus.
The intercept-outcome findings also support the hypothesis that school-age literacy skill builds on an early-developing foundation (33) or ‘neural scaffold’ (30–32). However, the paucity of intercept-outcome associations in contrast to the multitude and magnitude of slope-outcome associations suggests that this neural scaffold develops substantially over the first several years of life. While the mechanisms driving these divergent associations with outcomes will ultimately require further investigation, it is conceivable that they reflect links to distinct early precursors of phonological skills. For instance, the intercept-outcome associations may reflect stable relations with foundational perception skills that develop earlier (e.g., prosody, differentiating phonemes) and remain necessary for the development of more advanced phonological processing skills, whereas slope-outcome associations reflect dynamic associations with more advanced phonological processing skills such as syllable or phoneme deletion or substitution. Overall, the presence of slope-outcome associations offers insights into the early development of a neural scaffold for literacy and in doing so, underscores the importance of examining individual longitudinal trajectories (2), as opposed to cross-sectional and prospective associations.
It was also interesting that most of the slope-outcome findings were with white matter properties, and nearly all of these remained significant in all sensitivity analyses, with some effects becoming even more robust (e.g., please see inferior parietal white matter in second sensitivity analysis). While no prior study has examined the relationship between long-term literacy subskills and longitudinal trajectories of white matter development beginning in infancy (or early development of surface-based measures), there is a small corpus of literature that has linked reading-related skill performance to short-term, school-age developmental changes in left temporo-parietal white matter volume (158) and organization, most consistently in the left arcuate fasciculus (34, 56, 57, 158). This is especially compelling when considering that the slope-outcome relationship we observed with white matter organization was specific to the left arcuate fasciculus and to phonological processing, which is highly predictive of subsequent word reading and decoding performance (4, 8–10). Work from genomics might offer further explanation for the dominance of white matter associations, as genes involved in axon guidance (159), axon formation (78), and oligodendrocyte maturation (160) have also been linked to reading-related skills (60) and reading disability (76, 77). Although white matter volume and organization are not strongly associated and thought to be sensitive to different properties (161), their reliance on myelination, the primary function of oligodendrocytes, and other axonal properties suggests that they may represent another mechanism through which genetic factors may shape early brain development.
Interestingly, specificity analyses showed direct brain-behavior associations with phonological processing but not with oral language skills or nonverbal general cognitive abilities. Prior brain imaging studies in preschoolers/kindergarteners comport with this finding as phonological processing seems to exhibit more consistent associations with brain architecture (15–20), compared with oral language skills (21–23); c.f., (17, 24). While both subskills are considered critical for higher-level reading-related skills, they are thought to function on two distinguishable developmental pathways, whereby phonological processing is more involved in recognizing and decoding printed words and oral language skills are more essential for the development of reading comprehension skills (please see Scarborough’s Reading Rope; (8). It should be noted that this framework also fits with our mediation findings, which show indirect relations between early brain development and decoding and (printed) word reading via phonological processing. Furthermore, whereas phonological processing is limited in its scope, as specifically one’s ability to recognize and manipulate sounds in a word (4–7), oral language was measured in the current study as a composite of vocabulary and oral comprehension subtests, with the latter requiring children to engage with semantic and syntactic cues. As different brain regions may be more specialized for certain component language skills (e.g., pars opercularis may be more involved in syntactic processing while pars triangularis and middle temporal gyrus may be more involved in semantics; (162, 163)), aggregating distinct oral language skills may have obscured brain-behavior associations. Conversely, it is also conceivable that oral language acquisition requires additional, moderating factors that were not modeled in the present study (e.g., social interactions (164) or conversational turn-taking (21)). Nevertheless, the necessity of phonological processing for literacy development and its relation to early brain development reported here underscore the importance of examining reading-related subskills at the very beginnings of life.
Turning to literacy-related covariates, familial history of reading difficulty (FHD) was hypothesized to influence longitudinal trajectories of early brain development, based on previous findings showing FHD-related alterations in brain architecture in infancy (71, 72) and preschool (16, 18, 57). Consequently, the observation that FHD status did not influence longitudinal trajectories in any regions or tracts examined was initially unexpected. However, it is important to recognize that a child who has an older sibling or parent with a reading difficulty (FHD+) does not necessarily have a genetic susceptibility for reading difficulty (165), nor will they necessarily develop reading challenges given the multifactorial nature of reading difficulties. Rather, FHD status should be considered one of multiple risk factors that can contribute to long-term literacy development (166, 167), either through intergenerational transmission of genes or environment (168). Consistent with this, roughly half of FHD+ children develop typical reading skills (62, 63), and those who do develop typical reading skills have, as a group, been shown to recruit right- and inter-hemispheric compensatory pathways (169, 170), a pattern similar to children with reading difficulty who subsequently show improvements (171). Although the specificity analysis examining brain-behavior effects in non-a-priori brain regions did not point to literacy-related effects solely in the left hemisphere, right hemisphere regions and tracts were not thoroughly assayed in the current study. Therefore, it may behoove future studies with larger sample sizes to examine the development of right hemisphere regions and tracts in the context of FHD status and to distinguish FHD+ children who develop typical reading skills from FHD+ children who do not.
The home literacy environment also did not significantly influence longitudinal trajectories in the regions and tracts examined, despite reported links to brain structure and function in infants (95, 96) and preschoolers/kindergarteners (21, 24, 91–94). However, with few exceptions (e.g., common associations to arcuate fasciculus fractional anisotropy in infants (96) and kindergarten (24)), specific brain measures and regions/tracts linked to home literacy environment variables identified at the infant time point were not also identified at the preschool/kindergarten time point, suggesting that brain-home literacy environment relations may vary across this developmental window. Future longitudinal studies with more frequent sampling of observations will be needed to test whether this explanation is accurate. Also, as examination of home literacy was limited to longitudinal trajectories for brain measures and regions/tracts that were associated with phonological processing, it is likely that measures and regions/tracts related to the home literacy environment went untested in the current study.
This study had five main limitations. First, consistent with prior work (36), the models we fit to early brain development generated smooth longitudinal trajectories. In actuality, it is unlikely that early brain development transpires as predictably, especially during sensitive and critical periods (172) or specific learning milestones (2). Although participants in the current study were sampled over three times on average, which is preferred for modeling growth curves (173) and more than most longitudinal imaging studies (174), future studies would benefit from increased sampling, particularly around learning milestones germane to literacy development. Second, the sample size in the current study was relatively small when compared with the sample sizes used in multi-site, combined cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (e.g., 13, 36). While sensitivity analyses for the most part demonstrated the robustness of the results, future studies with larger sample sizes will be needed to confirm the findings presented here. Third, cortical thickness, mean curvature, fractional anisotropy, and mean diffusivity exhibited high correlations between random parameters (i.e., intercept-slope correlations) when modeled with logarithmic functions, which spurred concerns over the accuracy of the estimated random parameters. For the current findings, inaccurate random parameters could have generated false negatives for the former three measures and false positives for mean diffusivity. Consequently, for the former three parameters, even though they were consistently better fit with logarithmic functions, we re-analyzed intercept-outcome and slope-outcome associations using random parameters from models with quadratic functions, which had considerably lower intercept-slope correlations. Despite this, results remained non-significant. Meanwhile, the first sensitivity analysis addressed concerns for mean diffusivity by showing that nonlinear mixed effects models using asymptotic functions decoupled intercepts and slopes while maintaining the significant results observed in the main analysis. It is also important to note that this limitation does not apply to findings for gray and white matter volume or surface area. Fourth, we examined somewhat narrowly literacy-related factors that could contribute to early brain development by only including FHD status and home literacy environment, and it is likely that factors not included also contribute to the longitudinal trajectories examined (e.g., teaching quality, educational opportunities, executive functioning skills (166, 175). Fifth, our longitudinal analysis pipeline identifies and removes brain estimates preceding or following developmental changes that are too steep to occur neuroanatomically and more likely to emerge from region or tract mislabeling, despite our quality control efforts. Consequently, it is possible that estimates without steep developmental changes also suffer mislabeling but remain undetected. Overall, interpretations of findings should be considered in the context of these limitations.
In conclusion, this study examined associations between longitudinal trajectories of early brain development beginning in infancy and long-term reading acquisition, specifically literacy subskills. Longitudinal trajectories were generated using a novel, reproducible pipeline we designed specifically for examining early brain development and included familial risk of a reading difficulty and environmental covariates. Findings indicate that preschool/early kindergarten phonological processing, one of the strongest predictors of subsequent word reading development, relates to gray matter volume and surface area at birth and development of white matter volume, surface area, and mean diffusivity across early development. These results offer further evidence for a neural scaffold for literacy development, which is present at birth and continues forming across the first several years of life. The present study also provides a roadmap for future longitudinal studies to examine the relationship between early brain development and acquisition of other academic skills. Understanding when the foundations for reading emerge can deliver important insights into the development of instructional approaches and preventative, and intervention strategies.
Supplementary Material
Significance Statement.
Reading is crucial for academic, vocational, and health outcomes, but acquiring proficient reading skills is a protracted developmental process involving lower-level subskills and brain structures that undergo rapid development starting in infancy. We examined how longitudinal trajectories of early brain development support long-term acquisition of reading using a reproducible pipeline we developed specifically for infant-to-school-age longitudinal MRI data. Findings suggest that the brain bases of reading-related skills begin to develop by birth but continue building between infancy and preschool. This study emphasizes the importance of considering academic skill acquisition as a dynamic process preceding the emergence of the skill, and it offers a roadmap for future studies to examine relationships between early brain development and academic skill acquisition.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by NIH–NICHD R01 HD065762 (2016–2021), the William Hearst Fund (Harvard University), and the Harvard Catalyst/NIH (5UL1RR025758) to N.G. We would like to thank all participating families for their long-term dedication to this study. We are grateful for all additional members of the research team who contributed to data acquisition and quality control, including Victoria Hue, Ja’Kala Barber, Emily Hu, Olivia Baldi, Brooke Jordan, Jade Dunstan, Clarisa Carruthers, Carolyn King, Doroteja Rubez, Kathryn Garrisi, Ally Lee, Omeed Moini, Anaïs Colin, Zumin Chen, Colby Weiss, and Olivia Oh. We also thank Rebecca Petersen for her input on psychometric assessments, Jennifer Zuk for work on infant scanning procedures, Borjan Gagoski and Ross Mair for efforts on scanning sequences, Luke Miratrix and Fernando Aguate for guidance on (non)linear mixed effects models, and Banu Ahtam for feedback.
Footnotes
Competing interests: The authors do not have any competing interests to report.
References
- 1.Siegler R., Children’s learning. American Psychologist 6, 769–778 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bonte M., Brem S., Unraveling individual differences in learning potential: A dynamic framework for the case of reading development. Dev Cogn Neurosci [Preprint] (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 3.Kim Y.-S., Toward Integrative Reading Science: The Direct and Indirect Effects Model of Reading. J Learn Disabil 53, 469–491 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wagner R. K., Torgesen J. K., The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol Bull 101, 192–212 (1987). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wagner R., et al. , “The Nature of Prereaders’ Phonological Processing Abilities” (1987).
- 6.Goswami U., A neural oscillations perspective on phonological development and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia. Lang Linguist Compass 13 (2019).
- 7.Goswami U., “Phonological development across different languages” in Routledge International Handbook of English, Language & Literacy Teaching, (2010), pp. 98–109. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Scarborough H. S., “Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice” in Handbook of Early Literacy Research, Neuman S., Dickinson D., Eds. (Guilford Press, 2001), pp. 97–110. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Moll K., Loff A., Snowling M. J., Cognitive Endophenotypes of Dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading 17, 385–397 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- 10.Melby-Lervåg M., Lyster S.-A. H., Hulme C., Phonological Skills and Their Role in Learning to Read: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychol Bull 138, 322–352 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kendeou P., van den Broek P., White M. J., Lynch J. S., Predicting Reading Comprehension in Early Elementary School: The Independent Contributions of Oral Language and Decoding Skills. J Educ Psychol 101, 765–778 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kim Y. S. G., Why the Simple View of Reading Is Not Simplistic: Unpacking Component Skills of Reading Using a Direct and Indirect Effect Model of Reading (DIER). Scientific Studies of Reading 21, 310–333 (2017). [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bethlehem R. A. I., et al. , Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature 604, 525–533 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gilmore J., Knickmeyer R. C., Gao W., Imaging structural and functional brain development in early childhood. Nat Rev Neurosci 19, 123–137 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hoeft F., et al. , Prediction of Children’s Reading Skills Using Behavioral, Functional, and Structural Neuroimaging Measures. Behavioral Neuroscience 121, 602–613 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Raschle N. M., Chang M., Gaab N., Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. Neuroimage 57, 742–749 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Raschle N., Stering P. L., Meissner S. N., Gaab N., Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex 24, 2489–2501 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Raschle N., Zuk J., Gaab N., Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109, 2156–2161 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Saygin Z. M., et al. , Tracking the Roots of Reading Ability: White Matter Volume and Integrity Correlate with Phonological Awareness in Prereading and Early-Reading Kindergarten Children. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 13251–13258 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vandermosten M., Boets B., Wouters J., Ghesquière P., A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36, 1532–1552 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Romeo R., et al. , Language Exposure Relates to Structural Neural Connectivity in Childhood. J Neurosci 38, 7870–7877 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Romeo R., et al. , Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol Sci 29, 700–710 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Marks R. A., et al. , Spoken language proficiency predicts print-speech convergence in beginning readers. Neuroimage 201 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Davison K., et al. , Examining the relationship between shared book reading at home, white matter organization in kindergarten, and subsequent language and reading abilities: a longitudinal investigation. J Cogn Neurosci 35, 259–275 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lyytinen H., et al. , The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Ann Dyslexia 54, 184–220 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Guttorm T. K., Leppänen P. H. T., Hämäläinen J. A., Eklund K. M., Lyytinen H. J., Newborn event-related potentials predict poorer pre-reading skills in children at risk for dyslexia. J Learn Disabil 43, 391–401 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Guttorm T. K., Poikkeus A., Eklund K. M., Lyytinen P., Lyytinen H., Brain event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex 41, 291–303 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Van Zuijen T. L., Plakas A., Maassen B. A. M., Maurits N. M., Van der Leij A., Infant ERPs separate children at risk of dyslexia who become good readers from those who become poor readers. Dev Sci 16, 554–563 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Leppänen P. H. T., et al. , Infant brain responses associated with reading-related skills before school and at school age. Neurophysiologie Clinique 42, 35–41 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Yu X., et al. , Functional Connectivity in Infancy and Toddlerhood Predicts Long-Term Language and Preliteracy Outcomes. Cerebral Cortex 32, 725–736 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zuk J., et al. , White matter in infancy is prospectively associated with language outcomes in kindergarten. Dev Cogn Neurosci 50 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tang X., et al. , Longitudinal associations between language network characteristics in the infant brain and school-age reading abilities are mediated by early-developing phonological skills. Dev Cogn Neurosci 68 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cusack R., Ranzato M., Charvet C. J., Helpless infants are learning a foundation model. Trends Cogn Sci (2024). 10.1016/j.tics.2024.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 34.Roy E., et al. , White matter and literacy: A dynamic system in flux. Dev Cogn Neurosci 65 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gilmore J. H., et al. , Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cerebral Cortex 22, 2478–2485 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Alex A. M., et al. , A global multicohort study to map subcortical brain development and cognition in infancy and early childhood. Nat Neurosci 27, 176–186 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Prastawa M., Gilmore J. H., Lin W., Gerig G., Automatic segmentation of MR images of the developing newborn brain. Med Image Anal 9, 457–466 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Raschle N., et al. , Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: Challenges and practical guidelines. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1252, 43–50 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Makropoulos A., Counsell S. J., Rueckert D., A review on automatic fetal and neonatal brain MRI segmentation. Neuroimage 170, 231–248 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Zöllei L., Iglesias J. E., Ou Y., Grant P. E., Fischl B., Infant FreeSurfer: An automated segmentation and surface extraction pipeline for T1-weighted neuroimaging data of infants 0–2 years. Neuroimage 218, 116946 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pietsch M., et al. , A framework for multi-component analysis of diffusion MRI data over the neonatal period. Neuroimage 186, 321–337 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grotheer M., et al. , White matter myelination during early infancy is linked to spatial gradients and myelin content at birth. Nat Commun 13, 1–12 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Turesky T. K., Vanderauwera J., Gaab N., Imaging the rapidly developing brain: Current challenges for MRI studies in the first five years of life. Dev Cogn Neurosci 47, 100893 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gao W., et al. , Functional network development during the first year: Relative sequence and socioeconomic correlations. Cerebral Cortex 25, 2919–2928 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vijayakumar N., Mills K. L., Alexander-Bloch A., Tamnes C. K., Whittle S., Structural brain development: A review of methodological approaches and best practices. Dev Cogn Neurosci 33, 129–148 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Knickmeyer R. C., et al. , A Structural MRI Study of Human Brain Development from Birth to 2 Years. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 12176–12182 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Li G., et al. , Mapping region-specific longitudinal cortical surface expansion from birth to 2 years of age. Cerebral Cortex 23, 2724–2733 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Reynolds J., Grohs M. N., Dewey D., Lebel C., Global and regional white matter development in early childhood. Neuroimage 196, 49–58 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wang F., et al. , Developmental topography of cortical thickness during infancy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 15855–15860 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ben-Shachar M., Dougherty R. F., Deutsch G. K., Wandell B. A., “The Development of Cortical Sensitivity to Visual Word Forms” (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 51.Di Pietro S. V., Karipidis I. I., Pleisch G., Brem S., Neurodevelopmental trajectories of letter and speech sound processing from preschool to the end of elementary school. Dev Cogn Neurosci 61 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Brem S., et al. , Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter-speech sound correspondences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 7939–7944 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yu X., et al. , Emergence of the neural network underlying phonological processing from the prereading to the emergent reading stage: A longitudinal study. Hum Brain Mapp 39, 2047–2063 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Reynolds J., Long X., Grohs M. N., Dewey D., Lebel C., Structural and functional asymmetry of the language network emerge in early childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci 39 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dehaene-Lambertz G., Monzalvo K., Dehaene S., The emergence of the visual word form: Longitudinal evolution of category-specific ventral visual areas during reading acquisition. PLoS Biol 16 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yeatman J., Dougherty R. F., Ben-Shachar M., Wandell B. A., Development of white matter and reading skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109, E3045–E3053 PNAS (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wang Y., et al. , Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cerebral Cortex 27, 2469–2485 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ozernov-Palchik O., Sury D., Turesky T. K., Yu X., Gaab N., Longitudinal changes in brain activation underlying reading fluency. Hum Brain Mapp 44, 18–34 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Huber E., Mezer A., Yeatman J. D., Neurobiological underpinnings of rapid white matter plasticity during intensive reading instruction. Neuroimage 243 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Eising E., et al. , Genome-wide analyses of individual differences in quantitatively assessed reading- and language-related skills in up to 34,000 people. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 119 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Carrión-Castillo A., Paz-Alonso P. M., Carreiras M., Brain structure, phenotypic and genetic correlates of reading performance. Nat Hum Behav 7, 1120–1134 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Vogler G. P., DeFries J. C., Decker S. N., Family history as an indicator of risk for reading disability. J Learn Disabil 18, 419–421 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Snowling M. J., Melby-Lervåg M., Oral language deficits in familial dyslexia: A meta-analysis and review. Psychol Bull 142, 498–545 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hoeft F., et al. , Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, 4234–4239 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Richlan F., Kronbichler M., Wimmer H., Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage 56, 1735–42 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Richlan F., Kronbichler M., Wimmer H., Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Hum Brain Mapp 34, 3055–3065 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Richlan F., Kronbichler M., Wimmer H., Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 3299–308 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Steinbrink C., et al. , The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: Insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia 46, 3170–3178 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hasan K. M., et al. , Diffusion tensor quantification and cognitive correlates of the macrostructure and microstructure of the corpus callosum in typically developing and dyslexic children. NMR Biomed 25, 1263–1270 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Temple E., et al. , Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 12, 299–307 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Langer N., et al. , White Matter Alterations in Infants at Risk for Developmental Dyslexia. Cereb Cortex 27, 1027–1036 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Yu X., et al. , Patterns of Neural Functional Connectivity in Infants at Familial Risk of Developmental Dyslexia. JAMA Netw Open 5, E2236102 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Guttorm T. K., Leppänen P. H. T., Tolvanen A., Lyytinen H., Event-related potentials in newborns with and without familial risk for dyslexia: Principal component analysis reveals differences between the groups. J Neural Transm 110, 1059–1074 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Guttorm T. K., Leppänen P. H. T., Richardson U., Lyytinen H., Event-Related Potentials and Consonant Differentiation in Newborns with Familial Risk for Dyslexia. J Learn Disabil 34, 534–544 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.van Herten M., et al. , Differences in AERP responses and atypical hemispheric specialization in 17-month-old children at risk of dyslexia. Brain Res 1201, 100–105 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Doust C., et al. , Discovery of 42 genome-wide significant loci associated with dyslexia. Nat Genet 54, 1621–1629 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Price K. M., et al. , Hypothesis-driven genome-wide association studies provide novel insights into genetics of reading disabilities. Transl Psychiatry 12 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Watabe-Uchida M., John K. A., Janas J. A., Newey S. E., Van Aelst L., The Rac Activator DOCK7 Regulates Neuronal Polarity through Local Phosphorylation of Stathmin/Op18. Neuron 51, 727–739 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Payne Grover A. C., Andrea Angel W. L., “The Role of Home Literacy Environment in the Development of Language Ability in Preschool Children from Low-income Families” (1994).
- 80.Bus A. G., van IJzendoorn M. H., Pellegrini A. D., Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Rev Educ Res 65, 1–21 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- 81.Burgess S. R., Hecht S. A., Lonigan C. J., Relations of the home literacy environment (HLE) to the development of reading-related abilities: A one-year longitudinal study. Read Res Q 37, 408–426 (2002). [Google Scholar]
- 82.Foster M. A., Lambert R., Abbott-Shim M., McCarty F., Franze S., A model of home learning environment and social risk factors in relation to children’s emergent literacy and social outcomes. Early Child Res Q 20, 13–36 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- 83.Schmitt S. A., Simpson A. M., Friend M., A longitudinal assessment of the home literacy environment and early language. Infant Child Dev 20, 409–431 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Hamilton L. G., Hayiou-Thomas M. E., Hulme C., Snowling M. J., The Home Literacy Environment as a Predictor of the Early Literacy Development of Children at Family-Risk of Dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading 20, 401–419 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Karrass J., Braungart-Rieker J. M., Effects of shared parent-infant book reading on early language acquisition. J Appl Dev Psychol 26, 133–148 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hoff E., The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development Via Maternal Speech. Child Dev 74, 1368–1378 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Muhinyi A., Rowe M. L., Shared reading with preverbal infants and later language development. J Appl Dev Psychol 64 (2019). [Google Scholar]
- 88.Laakso M.-L., Poikkeus A.-M., Lyytinen P., Shared reading interaction in families with and without genetic risk for dyslexia: implications for toddlers’ language development. Infant Child Dev 8, 179–195 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- 89.Malin J. L., Cabrera N. J., Rowe M. L., Low-income minority mothers’ and fathers’ reading and children’s interest: Longitudinal contributions to children’s receptive vocabulary skills. Early Child Res Q 29, 425–432 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Demir-Lira Ö. E., Applebaum L. R., Goldin-Meadow S., Levine S. C., Parents’ early book reading to children: Relation to children’s later language and literacy outcomes controlling for other parent language input. Dev Sci 22 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Powers S. J., Wang Y., Beach S. D., Sideridis G. D., Gaab N., Examining the relationship between home literacy environment and neural correlates of phonological processing in beginning readers with and without a familial risk for dyslexia: an fMRI study. Ann Dyslexia 66, 337–360 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hutton J. S., Dudley J., Horowitz-Kraus T., DeWitt T., Holland S. K., Associations between home literacy environment, brain white matter integrity and cognitive abilities in preschool-age children. Acta Paediatr 109, 1376–1386 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Hutton J. S., Horowitz-Kraus T., Mendelsohn A. L., DeWitt T., Holland S. K., Home reading environment and brain activation in preschool children listening to stories. Pediatrics 136, 466–478 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hutton J. S., et al. , Shared Reading Quality and Brain Activation during Story Listening in Preschool-Age Children. Journal of Pediatrics 191, 204–211.e1 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.King L. S., Camacho M. C., Montez D. F., Humphreys K. L., Gotlib I. H., Naturalistic language input is associated with resting-state functional connectivity in infancy. Journal of Neuroscience 41, 424–434 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Turesky T. K., et al. , Home language and literacy environment and its relationship to socioeconomic status and white matter structure in infancy. Brain Struct Funct 227, 2633–2645 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Zuk J., Vanderauwera J., Turesky T., Yu X., Gaab N., Neurobiological predispositions for musicality: White matter in infancy predicts school-age music aptitude. Dev Sci 26 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Lebel C., et al. , Prepartum and Postpartum Maternal Depressive Symptoms Are Related to Children’s Brain Structure in Preschool. Biol Psychiatry 80, 859–868 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Thieba C., et al. , Factors Associated With Successful MRI Scanning in Unsedated Young Children. Front Pediatr 6, 1–5 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Paniukov D., Lebel R. M., Giesbrecht G., Lebel C., Cerebral blood flow increases across early childhood. Neuroimage 204 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Walton M., Dewey D., Lebel C., Brain white matter structure and language ability in preschool-aged children. Brain Lang 176, 19–25 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Reynolds J., Long X., Paniukov D., Bagshawe M., Lebel C., Calgary Preschool magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) dataset. Data Brief 29 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Betancourt L. M., et al. , Effect of socioeconomic status (SES) disparity on neural development in female African-American infants at age 1 month. Dev Sci 19, 947–956 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Brito N. H., Fifer W. P., Myers M. M., Elliott A. J., Noble K. G., Associations among family socioeconomic status, EEG power at birth, and cognitive skills during infancy. Dev Cogn Neurosci 19, 144–151 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Lawson G. M., Duda J. T., Avants B. B., Wu J., Farah M. J., Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Dev Sci 16, 641–652 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Merz E. C., Tottenham N., Noble K. G., Socioeconomic Status, Amygdala Volume, and Internalizing Symptoms in Children and Adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 47, 312–323 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Noble K. G., et al. , Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 18, 773–778 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Ozernov-Palchik O., et al. , The relationship between socioeconomic status and white matter microstructure in pre-reading children: A longitudinal investigation. Hum Brain Mapp 40, 741–754 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Denney M. K., English J. P., Gerber M. M., Leafstedt J., Ruz M. L., Family and home literacy practices: mediating factors for preliterate English learners at risk in Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Associations, (2001), pp. 1–17.
- 110.Kim Y. S. G., Executive Functions and Morphological Awareness Explain the Shared Variance between Word Reading and Listening Comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading 27, 451–474 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Schrank F. A., McGrew K. S., Mather N., Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Cognitive Abilities (Riverside, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- 112.Korkman M., Kirk U., Kemp S., NEPSY–Second Edition (NEPSY-II), 2nd Ed. (The Psychological Corporation, 2007). [Google Scholar]
- 113.Schrank F., Mather N., McGrew K., Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Oral Language (Riverside Publishing Co, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- 114.Woodcock R. W., Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests (WRMT-III) (NCS Pearson, 2011). [Google Scholar]
- 115.Kaufman A. S., Kaufman N. L., Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition. PsycTESTS Dataset [Preprint] (2004). [Google Scholar]
- 116.Backhausen L. L., et al. , Quality control of structural MRI images applied using FreeSurfer-a hands-on workflow to rate motion artifacts. Front Neurosci 10 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Natu V. S., et al. , Infants’ cortex undergoes microstructural growth coupled with myelination during development. Commun Biol 4 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Wang L., et al. , iBEAT V2.0: a multisite-applicable, deep learning-based pipeline for infant cerebral cortical surface reconstruction. Nat Protoc 18, 1488–1509 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Li G., et al. , Measuring the dynamic longitudinal cortex development in infants by reconstruction of temporally consistent cortical surfaces. Neuroimage 90, 266–279 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Li G., et al. , Computational neuroanatomy of baby brains: A review. Neuroimage [Preprint] (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 121.McCarthy C. S., et al. , A comparison of FreeSurfer-generated data with and without manual intervention. Front Neurosci 9 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 122.Pulli E. P., et al. , Feasibility of FreeSurfer Processing for T1-Weighted Brain Images of 5-Year-Olds: Semiautomated Protocol of FinnBrain Neuroimaging Lab. Front Neurosci 16 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 123.Ahtam B., et al. , Morphological Features of Language Regions in Individuals with Tuberous Sclerosis Complex. J Autism Dev Disord 54, 3155–3175 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Chyl K., Fraga-González G., Brem S., Jednoróg K., Brain dynamics of (a)typical reading development—a review of longitudinal studies. NPJ Sci Learn [Preprint] (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 125.Bastiani M., et al. , Automated processing pipeline for neonatal diffusion MRI in the developing Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 185, 750–763 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Cordero-Grande L., Christiaens D., Hutter J., Price A. N., Hajnal J. V., Complex diffusion-weighted image estimation via matrix recovery under general noise models. Neuroimage 200, 391–404 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Veraart J., et al. , Denoising of diffusion MRI using random matrix theory. Neuroimage 142, 394–406 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Veraart J., Fieremans E., Novikov D. S., Diffusion MRI noise mapping using random matrix theory. Magn Reson Med 76, 1582–1593 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Andersson J. L. R., Skare S., Ashburner J., How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: Application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 20, 870–888 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Andersson J. L. R., et al. , Towards a comprehensive framework for movement and distortion correction of diffusion MR images: Within volume movement. Neuroimage 152, 450–466 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Andersson J. L. R., Sotiropoulos S. N., An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125, 1063–1078 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Skare S., Bammer R., Jacobian weighting of distortion corrected EPI data in Proceedings of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, (2010).
- 133.Smith S. M., et al. , Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, 208–219 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Tustison N. J., et al. , N4ITK: Improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 29, 1310–1320 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Dhollander T., Mito R., Raffelt D., Connelly A., Improved white matter response function estimation for 3-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution in Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med, (2019).
- 136.Jeurissen B., Tournier J. D., Dhollander T., Connelly A., Sijbers J., Multi-tissue constrained spherical deconvolution for improved analysis of multi-shell diffusion MRI data. Neuroimage 103, 411–426 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Tournier J. D., Calamante F., Gadian D. G., Connelly A., Direct estimation of the fiber orientation density function from diffusion-weighted MRI data using spherical deconvolution. Neuroimage 23, 1176–1185 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Tournier J. D., Calamante F., Connelly A., MRtrix: Diffusion tractography in crossing fiber regions. Int J Imaging Syst Technol 22, 53–66 (2012). [Google Scholar]
- 139.Smith R. E., Tournier J. D., Calamante F., Connelly A., Anatomically-constrained tractography: Improved diffusion MRI streamlines tractography through effective use of anatomical information. Neuroimage 62, 1924–1938 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Yeatman J., Dougherty R., Myall N., Wandell B., Feldman H., Tract profiles of white matter properties: automating fiber-tract quantification. PLoS One 7, e49790 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Kruper J., et al. , Evaluating the Reliability of Human Brain White Matter Tractometry. Aperture Neuro 1, 1–25 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Grotheer M., et al. , Human white matter myelinates faster in utero than ex utero. (2023). 10.1073/pnas. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 143.Smith S. M., Nichols T. E., Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Frossard J., Renaud O., Permutation tests for regression, anova, and comparison of signals: The permuco package. J Stat Softw 99, 1–32 (2021). [Google Scholar]
- 145.Pinheiro J., Bates D., DebRoy S., Sarkar D., R Core Team, nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R Package Version 3 [Preprint] (2017). Available at: https://bugs.r-project.org.
- 146.Rosen A. F. G., et al. , Quantitative assessment of structural image quality. Neuroimage 169, 407–418 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Turesky T., et al. , Brain Morphometry and Diminished Physical Growth in Bangladeshi Children Growing up in Extreme Poverty: a Longitudinal Study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 52, 101029 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Turesky T. K., et al. , Brain morphometry and chronic inflammation in Bangladeshi children growing up in extreme poverty. Imaging Neuroscience 2, 1–16 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Monereo-Sánchez J., et al. , Quality control strategies for brain MRI segmentation and parcellation: Practical approaches and recommendations - insights from the Maastricht study. Neuroimage 237 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Matsuzawa J., Matsui M., Konishi T., Age-related Volumetric Changes of Brain Gray and White Matter in Healthy Infants and Children. Cerebral Cortex 11, 335–342 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Groeschel S., Vollmer B., King M. D., Connelly A., Developmental changes in cerebral grey and white matter volume from infancy to adulthood. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 28, 481–489 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Deoni S. C. L., Dean D. C., Muircheartaigh J. O., Dirks H., Jerskey B. A., Investigating white matter development in infancy and early childhood using myelin water faction and relaxation time mapping. 63, 1038–1053 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Deoni S. C. L., et al. , Breastfeeding and early white matter development: A cross-sectional study. Neuroimage 82, 77–86 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Remer J., et al. , Quantifying cortical development in typically developing toddlers and young children, 1–6 years of age. Neuroimage 153, 246–261 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Sanchez C. E., Richards J. E., Almli C. R., Neurodevelopmental MRI brain templates for children from 2 weeks to 4 years of age. Dev Psychobiol 54, 77–91 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Geng X., et al. , NeuroImage Quantitative tract-based white matter development from birth to age 2 years. Neuroimage 61, 542–557 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Im K., Raschle N., Smith S. A., Ellen Grant P., Gaab N., Atypical Sulcal Pattern in Children with Developmental Dyslexia and At-Risk Kindergarteners. Cerebral Cortex 26, 1138–1148 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Myers C. A., et al. , White Matter Morphometric Changes Uniquely Predict Children’s Reading Acquisition. Psychol Sci 25, 1870–1883 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Hannula-Jouppi K., et al. , The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genet 1, 0467–0474 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Reiche L., et al. , C21orf91 Regulates Oligodendroglial Precursor Cell Fate—A Switch in the Glial Lineage? Front Cell Neurosci 15 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Fjell A. M., et al. , The relationship between diffusion tensor imaging and volumetry as measures of white matter properties. Neuroimage 42, 1654–1668 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Friederici A. D., The cortical language circuit: From auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci [Preprint] (2012). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 163.Wang J., Rice M. L., Booth J. R., Syntactic and semantic specialization and integration in 5-to 6-year-old children during auditory sentence processing. J Cogn Neurosci 32, 36–49 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Kuhl P. K., Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nat Rev Neurosci [Preprint] (2004). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 165.Yu X., Zuk J., Gaab N., What Factors Facilitate Resilience in Developmental Dyslexia? Examining Protective and Compensatory Mechanisms Across the Neurodevelopmental Trajectory. Child Dev Perspect 12, 240–246 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Ozernov-Palchik O., Yu X., Wang Y., Gaab N., Lessons to be learned: How a comprehensive neurobiological framework of atypical reading development can inform educational practice. Curr Opin Behav Sci 10, 45–58 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Catts H., Petscher Y., Early identification of dyslexia: Current advancements and future directions. Perspect Lang Lit 44, 33–36 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 168.Hart S. A., Little C., van Bergen E., Nurture might be nature: cautionary tales and proposed solutions. NPJ Sci Learn 6, 1–12 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Zuk J., et al. , Multifactorial pathways facilitate resilience among kindergarteners at risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal behavioral and neuroimaging study. Dev Sci 24, 1–18 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Yu X., et al. , Putative protective neural mechanisms in prereaders with a family history of dyslexia who subsequently develop typical reading skills. Hum Brain Mapp 41, 2827–2845 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Hoeft F., et al. , Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, 361–366 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Nelson C. A., Gabard-Durnam L. J., Early Adversity and Critical Periods : Neurodevelopmental Consequences of Violating the Expectable Environment. Trends Neurosci 43, 133–143 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Curran P. J., Obeidat K., Losardo D., Twelve frequently asked questions about growth curve modeling. Journal of Cognition and Development 11, 121–136 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Telzer E. H., et al. , Methodological considerations for developmental longitudinal fMRI research. Dev Cogn Neurosci [Preprint] (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 175.Roy E., et al. , Differences in educational opportunity predict white matter development. Dev Cogn Neurosci 67 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All code used to process and analyze has been made openly available at https://github.com/TeddyTuresky/Longitudinal-Trajectories-Early-Brain-Development-Language. The Calgary dataset is also freely available at https://osf.io/axz5r/. All Boston/Cambridge data used in this study will be made publicly available at upon acceptance of the manuscript.