Abstract
Environmental influences on brain structure and function during early development have been well-characterized, but whether early environments are associated with the pace of brain development is not clear. In pre-registered analyses, we use flexible non-linear models to test the theory that prenatal disadvantage is associated with differences in trajectories of intrinsic brain network development from birth to three years (n = 261). Prenatal disadvantage was assessed using a latent factor of socioeconomic disadvantage that included measures of mother’s income-to-needs ratio, educational attainment, area deprivation index, insurance status, and nutrition. We find that prenatal disadvantage is associated with developmental increases in cortical network segregation, with neonates and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage showing a steeper increase in cortical network segregation with age, consistent with accelerated network development. Associations between prenatal disadvantage and cortical network segregation occur at the local scale and conform to a sensorimotor-association hierarchy of cortical organization. Disadvantage-associated differences in cortical network segregation are associated with language abilities at two years, such that lower segregation is associated with improved language abilities. These results shed light on associations between the early environment and trajectories of cortical development.
Subject terms: Development of the nervous system, Stress and resilience, Network models, Language
Early environmental factors, like disadvantage, are associated with neurocognitive development. Here, the authors find that neonates and toddlers from economically disadvantaged backgrounds show accelerated brain development, with implications for language abilities in toddlerhood.
Introduction
The first years of life are a time of rapid brain development, with intrinsic cortical organization becoming refined and large-scale brain systems developing into their adultlike configuration1,2. As children mature, intrinsic cortical networks become more segregated3,4, with sets of brain regions displaying more densely interconnected patterns of connectivity and large-scale systems becoming increasingly distinct2,5–7. This refinement of cortical organization occurs in a spatiotemporally patterned manner, with maturation occurring earlier in primary sensory regions than transmodal association regions8. The development of cortical network segregation also has implications for cognition, with higher levels of cortical network segregation associated with better cognitive abilities in adolescents and adults6,9–12. One environmental factor that is associated with the development of cortical network segregation — as well as many later life outcomes, including physical wellbeing13,14, cognitive ability15, and mental health16 — is disadvantage. Disadvantage is a broad and multifaceted construct that encompasses measures of socioeconomic status (SES) such as income, education, and occupational prestige17,18, as well as related health factors such as insurance and diet quality19.
Relations between disadvantage and the development of cortical network segregation have been predominantly examined in older children and adolescents. In youth ages 8–22 years, SES moderated age-associated increases in cortical network segregation, such that youth from more economically advantaged backgrounds started with lower cortical network segregation but showed a steeper increase in cortical network segregation during adolescence than youth from less economically advantaged backgrounds, thus ending with higher levels of cortical network segregation at 22 years20. Associations between SES and cortical network segregation were also found in youth ages 6–17 years living in low-SES neighborhoods21. Collectively, these studies suggest that disadvantage may accelerate the pace of cortical network segregation earlier in development, setting the stage for associations observed in older children and adolescents22. More broadly, associations between disadvantage and intrinsic brain organization are visible as early as the first month of life23,24, but as of yet no longitudinal studies have examined how disadvantage relates to the development of cortical network segregation during the critical first years of life.
The pace of early brain development has been associated with important risk factors and behavioral outcomes. Changes in the pace of brain development have been linked to psychiatric disorders25–27, and accelerated pubertal development is associated with poorer mental health during adolescence28–30. An accelerated pace might also result in earlier declines in brain plasticity, curtailing the development of cortical circuitry optimally suited to the environment31,32. We have specifically posited that associations between disadvantage and the pace of structural brain development might be reflected in the trajectory of functional brain development: more protracted structural brain development observed in children with less exposure to disadvantage may give rise to a longer, slower trajectory of intrinsic cortical network segregation22. If so, children with less exposure to disadvantage should show more widespread connectivity and lower cortical network segregation early in development before the rapid development of a more segregated network architecture in later childhood and adolescence, leading to more effective cortical networks in adulthood due to protracted experience-dependent influences on network segregation22. Models drawn from evolutionary developmental frameworks also suggest that the early environment might be related to the pace of maturation. These models posit that early experiences tailor organisms to anticipated conditions in the future and suggest that harsh and unpredictable environments may be associated with life-history strategies resulting in accelerated development and early maturity, while safe and nurturing environments may result in prolonged developmental strategies and extended experience-dependent learning33–35. Relatedly, the recent “change of tempo” model contends that different forms of adversity have different relations to pace: in situations of deprivation, such as inadequate nutrition or parental care, delaying maturation lowers children’s physiological requirements, while in the context of threat or abuse accelerated development may boost children’s ability to provide for immediate unmet safety needs36. We recently proposed that experiences that are both negative and chronic are likely to accelerate brain development and reduce plasticity, while exposure to positive and rare or variable experiences are most likely to decelerate brain development and enhance plasticity22. At the same time, a recent review concluded that SES may simply be associated with alterations in brain development trajectories, not specific to pace, though the authors noted the dearth of studies on intrinsic brain network development37. Limited empirical work has employed the longitudinal data necessary to model associations between disadvantage and trajectories of cortical network segregation, particularly in the earliest stages of life.
In this work, we explicitly test whether early disadvantage is associated with differences in the pace of intrinsic cortical network segregation during the first three years of life. We capitalize on a cohort of neonates and toddlers with longitudinal neuroimaging data and extensively characterized early environments19. Prenatal disadvantage was assessed using a latent factor of socioeconomic disadvantage that included measures of mother’s income-to-needs ratio, educational attainment, area deprivation index, insurance status, and nutrition. In this cohort, prenatal disadvantage is associated with brain structure and region-specific connectivity at birth38–40, however, associations between prenatal disadvantage and whole-brain function or longitudinal brain development have not yet been examined. Here, we test the theory that prenatal disadvantage is associated with differences in the pace of functional brain network development, and that neonates and toddlers from more advantaged backgrounds might show a more protracted trajectory of cortical network segregation. In a set of pre-registered analyses, we examine the development of cortical network segregation during the first three years of life, and the moderating effects of prenatal disadvantage on trajectories of cortical network segregation. We find that developmental increases in cortical functional network segregation are accelerated in neonates and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage. We take a hierarchical approach, first examining measures of cortical network segregation at the whole brain resolution, then analyzing at the level of functional brain systems, and finally at the level of individual brain regions. Based on the hypothesis that age-dependent changes in cortical plasticity might result in regional variability in associations between prenatal disadvantage and cortical network segregation, we find that environmental effects are constrained by a sensorimotor-association cortical hierarchy, with the strongest associations present in early-developing sensory systems. Finally, we show that differences in measures of cortical network segregation at 2 years of age are associated with language and cognitive abilities at the same age.
Results
Cortical network segregation increases from birth to 3 years
We first investigated the maturation of cortical functional network architecture between birth and 3 years of age by examining measures of cortical network segregation. Cortical network segregation can be measured at different scales, ranging from the local, or regional, scale to the global, or whole-brain, scale (Fig. 1a–c). Global segregation captures the extent to which sets of systems in a cortical network are distinctly partitioned, while meso-scale segregation captures the extent to which a network can be divided into distinct subnetworks, and local segregation quantifies clustered connectivity at the regional or parcel level. We fit generalized additive mixed models (GAMMs) to measures of network segregation at each of these three levels, controlling for sex, amount of fMRI data included, in-scanner motion, and average network connectivity, and including a smooth term for age, where the smooth function (model fit for age) describes the relationship between cortical network segregation and age. Across scales, cortical network segregation increases with age. Global segregation, calculated from system segregation, increased with age (Fig. 1, Fs(age) = 6.26, EDF = 2.26, p = 0.001, pFDR = 0.001), as did meso-scale segregation (Fs(age) = 3.93, EDF = 1.90, p = 0.014, pFDR = 0.014), and local segregation (Fs(age) = 43.23, EDF = 2.33, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001), indicating increasing refinement of network architecture during early development.
Next, we probed the variation in the rate of change of cortical network segregation during the first three years of development. Here, the first derivative of the smooth function of age represents the rate of change in network segregation at a given developmental time point. Analysis of the derivatives of the age trajectories showed that measures of network segregation are increasing most strongly during the first two years of life. Global segregation increased between 0–1.93 years, meso-scale segregation increased between 0–1.70 years, and local segregation increased between 0–2.13 years, consistent with the most rapid change in cortical network segregation occurring early in development.
Prenatal disadvantage moderates trajectories of cortical network segregation
As described above, theoretical models posit that environmental influences on brain development might arise by way of effects on the pace of brain development, such that brain development proceeds faster in neonates and toddlers with greater exposure to disadvantage. To test this hypothesis, we examined associations between the early environment and developmental increases in cortical network segregation. We used GAMMs to formally model age-by-disadvantage interactions, which estimate how relationships between prenatal disadvantage, our measure of early environment, and network segregation vary continuously with age. We observed significant and similar patterns of interactions between prenatal disadvantage and age across multiple scales, such that infants and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage show a faster increase in cortical network segregation than infants and toddlers with less disadvantage, ending up at a higher level of network segregation. This pattern held true for global segregation (Fig. 2a, Fs(agexSES) = 6.38, p = 0.002, pFDR = 0.002), meso-scale segregation (Fig. 2b, Fs(agexSES) = 9.86, p < 0.001, pFDR = 0.001), and local segregation (Fig. 2c, Fs(agexSES) = 13.40, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001).
Intuitively, measures of network segregation at different scales will be related: segregation at the global scale will encompass segregation at both the meso (intermediate) scale and the local scale. Correspondingly, we observe that measures of segregation are correlated with each other (global segregation – meso-scale segregation: t(417) = −6.51, p < 0.001, r = −0.30, 95% CI = [−0.39, −0.21]; meso-scale segregation – local segregation: t(417) = 19.11, p < 0.001, r = 0.68, 95% CI = [0.63, 0.73]; local segregation – global segregation: t(417) = = 18.51, p < 0.001, r = −0.67, 95% CI = [−0.72, −0.62]). To determine which effect drives the observed age-by-disadvantage associations with cortical network segregation, we conducted a series of analyses examining age-by-disadvantage associations with network segregation at each scale, controlling for network segregation at other scales. When we included average local segregation in the model for global segregation, age x disadvantage no longer significantly predicts global segregation (Fs(agexSES) = 2.62, p = 0.074). The same is true when we included average local segregation in our model for meso-scale segregation: the age-by-disadvantage effect is no longer significant (Fs(agexSES) = 0.76, p = 0.471). The inverse is not the case — including meso-scale or global segregation in our model of local segregation does not affect the significance of the age-by-disadvantage interaction (global segregation: Fs(agexSES) = 9.46, p < 0.001; meso-scale segregation; Fs(agexSES) = 4.60, p = 0.011). Therefore, we concluded that the fundamental driver of the age-by-disadvantage associations is variation in local network topology, as indexed by our measure of local segregation. Thus, in the analyses that follow we focus specifically on local segregation and associated variation in local network architecture.
Prenatal disadvantage associations are strongest in somatomotor and dorsal attention systems
To characterize associations between the environment and cortical network development in individual cortical regions, we conducted a series of exploratory (not pre-registered) analyses. We fit region-specific GAMM models to regional measures of local segregation, including a smooth term for age, and allowing age to interact with prenatal disadvantage. A total of 56% of regions showed significant age-by-disadvantage associations with local segregation, indicating that moderating associations between the environment and cortical network development are widespread across the cortex (pFDR < 0.05, Fig. 3a). To provide insight into the magnitude of moderating associations with prenatal disadvantage across cortical regions, we calculated the magnitude of variance explained by the addition of the age-by-disadvantage interaction (F-statistic). The magnitude of associations between prenatal disadvantage and developmental increases in local segregation differed across the cortex, signifying that there is variability in the association between the early environment and cortical network maturation across the developing cortex.
To better understand the pattern of variability in associations with the environment across the cortex, we asked whether moderating associations between prenatal disadvantage and age-related increases in local segregation might be more pronounced in specific functional systems. The magnitude of associations between prenatal disadvantage and developmental increases in local segregation differed across functional systems (H(12) = 90.413, eta²H = 0.25, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.19, 0.36])), with the strongest associations found in somatomotor-hand, somatomotor-mouth, dorsal attention, visual, and frontoparietal systems (Fig. 3b), consistent with environmental associations with cortical network development being most pronounced in early-developing sensorimotor regions during the first years of life. We wondered whether this was an overall principle of associations between the early environment and cortical network development, so we examined the correspondence between associations between prenatal disadvantage and developmental increases in local segregation and a canonical axis of sensorimotor-association cortical organization8. Age-by-disadvantage associations with local segregation were negatively correlated with sensorimotor-association axis ranks across regions (Fig. 3c, ρ(331) = -0.33, pspin = 0.002, 95% CI = [−0.43, −0.24]), with larger environmental associations with developmental increases in local segregation characterizing the S-A axis’s sensorimotor pole, and smaller, near zero associations at the association pole.
Associations between cognition and disadvantage-associated differences in local segregation
Finally, we sought to understand whether associations between the environment and the development of cortical network segregation relate to differences in language and cognition that we observe later in development. Disadvantage-associated differences in language (Fig. 4a, Fs(agexSES) = 7.54, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001) and cognitive (Fs(agexSES) = 12.65, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001) composite scores are evident from 2 years of age. Thus, we examined whether differences in local segregation at 2 years, when we first observe diverging trajectories of cortical network segregation associated with prenatal disadvantage, were associated with language or cognition composite scores at the same time point. There was no statistically significant association between average whole-cortex local segregation and cognition scores (t(83) = −0.30, p = 0.766, pFDR = 0.766, β = −0.03, 95% CI = [−5.66 × 10−4, −4.18−4]). Average whole-cortex local segregation was negatively associated with language scores (Fig. 4b, t(83) = −2.45, p = 0.017, pFDR = 0.033, β = −0.23, 95% CI = [−8.84 × 10−4, −9.12 × 10−5]), indicating that lower levels of cortical network segregation seen in less disadvantaged toddlers are associated with better language abilities at 2 years of age. Importantly, when examining year two language ability and additionally controlling for prenatal disadvantage, we find that both prenatal disadvantage (t(83) = −2.26, p = 0.027, β = −0.23, 95% CI = [−6.72, −0.42]) and average local segregation (t(83) = −2.43, β = −0.25, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [−217.38, −21.59]) are significantly associated with language scores, indicating that cortical network segregation is independently associated with language ability at 2 years, above and beyond the association between early disadvantage and later language abilities.
We examined whether associations between local segregation and language abilities were driven by the receptive or expressive language subscales of the language composite and found that local segregation was negatively associated with both expressive (Fig. 4c, t(83) = −2.19, β = −0.21, p = 0.032, 95% CI = [−4.52 × 10−3, −2.13 × 10−4]) and receptive language abilities (t(83) = −2.18, β = −0.21, p = 0.032, 95% CI = [−4.34 × 10−3, −1.97 × 10−4]). When controlling for prenatal disadvantage, local segregation was associated with receptive language (t(83) = −2.33, β = −0.25, p = 0.022, 95% CI = [−41.75, −3.26]), but there was no statistically significant association with expressive language (t(83) = −1.96, β = −0.21, p = 0.053, 95% CI = [−36.16, 0.24]).
While our sample size was reduced at the 3-year timepoint due to COVID-related limitations in data collection (n = 90 at year two, n = 66 at year three), we additionally examined whether differences in local segregation at age 3 years were associated with language or cognition composite scores. We found a similar pattern of effect sizes at the 3-year timepoint, with average local segregation negatively associated with language scores (t(83) = −2.84, β = −0.33, p = 0.006, pFDR = 0.012, 95% CI = [−0.001, −1.84 × 10−4]), but not cognitive scores (t(83) = −1.67, β = −0.20, p = 0.100, pFDR = 0.100, 95% CI = [−0.001, 0.0001]). Local segregation was negatively associated with both receptive (t(83) = −2.45, β = −0.29, p = 0.015, 95% CI = [−5.34 × 10−3, −5.90 × 10−4]) and expressive language abilities (t(83) = −2.94, β = −0.35, p = 0.005, 95% CI = [−6.32 × 10−3, −1.20 × 10−3]). When controlling for prenatal disadvantage at the 3-year timepoint, we find similar effect sizes for associations between local segregation and language scores as at age 2 years, but neither associations with the language composite (t(83) = −1.50, β = −0.19, p = 0.138, 95% CI = [−235.92, 33.31]), or the expressive (t(83) = −1.38, β = −0.17, p = 0.173, 95% CI = [−39.37, 7.21]) or receptive (t(83) = −1.46, β = −0.19, p = 0.148, 95% CI = [−44.15, 6.83]) language scales are significant (Fig. 4c).
Disadvantage associations with developmental increases in cortical network segregation are robust to methodological variation
To ensure that the environmental associations with changes in cortical network segregation observed were robust to methodological variation and potential confounds, we performed five sensitivity analyses. We first conducted one of our pre-registered follow-up analyses, and evaluated whether associations between prenatal disadvantage and developmental increases in cortical network segregation were accounted for by an alternate measure of the early environment. In this analysis, GAMMs were rerun with a composite SES measure of income-to-needs ratio (INR) and maternal education at birth rather than the prenatal disadvantage factor score (i.e., excluding neighborhood disadvantage, diet, and insurance status). When using this composite measure of SES, global segregation (Fs(agexSES) = 3.74, p = 0.024, pFDR = 0.033), meso-scale segregation (Fs(agexSES) = 7.78, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001), and local segregation (Fig. 5a, Fs(agexSES) = 11.61, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001) all show significant and similar patterns of interactions, such that infants and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage show a faster increase in cortical network segregation than infants and toddlers with less disadvantage. The magnitude of SES associations with developmental increases in local segregation continued to vary across the cortex (Fig. 5b), with the strongest associations found in somatomotor-hand, visual, dorsal attention, and somatomotor-mouth systems (Fig. 5c).
We also evaluated whether associations between prenatal disadvantage and developmental increases in cortical network segregation were accounted for by differences in sample composition over the study period, alterations in functional network architecture associated with head motion, changes in disadvantage over the study period, longitudinal modeling choices, or outliers. In each sensitivity analysis, associations between the environment and developmental increases in cortical network segregation closely mirrored those observed in the main analysis, with neonates and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage showing a steeper increase in cortical functional network segregation than those with less exposure to disadvantage, and environmental associations with developmental increases in cortical network segregation enriched in sensorimotor systems (see Supplementary Note 1 and Figs. 1–5). We additionally examined whether there were associations between prenatal psychosocial stress independent of prenatal disadvantage and developmental changes in cortical network segregation, however, no significant associations were found (see Supplementary Note 1 and Fig. 6). These analyses verify that findings concerning the nature and patterning of environmental associations with developmental changes in cortical functional network segregation are robust to methodological variation.
No statistically significant environmental associations with age-associated changes in network integration
The previous analyses focused on developmental trajectories of network segregation, but another property of cortical network architecture that changes markedly as children develop is cortical network integration. Cortical network integration refers to the extent to which information can be integrated across multiple brain systems, and high levels of both network integration and network segregation together constitute the unique property of small-worldness found in adult brain networks41. Thus, we also pre-registered examining developmental changes in and age-by-disadvantage associations with cortical network integration, as assessed by the average whole-brain participation coefficient. The participation coefficient is a measure of network integration that quantifies the diversity of the connections of a node across systems42, and has been linked in older children and adolescents to developmental changes in network segregation7,43,44 (Fig. 6a).
As previously, we fit a GAMM to average whole-cortex network integration with a smooth term for age, where the smooth function (model fit for age) describes the relationship between network integration and age. Average network integration increased with age (Fig. 6b, Fs(age) = 14.11, EDF = 1.68, p < 0.001, pFDR < 0.001), with the period of significant change occurring from birth to 2.27 years, consistent with the most rapid change occurring early in development. We found no statistically significant association between prenatal disadvantage and developmental changes in network integration (Fig. 6c, Fs(agexSES) = 1.71, p = 0.182, pFDR = 0.182) in these primary analyses nor in supplemental analyses examining the alternative composite measure of disadvantage described above (Fs(agexSES) = 1.53, p = 0.118, pFDR = 0.118).
Discussion
The first years of life are a critical window during which the development of the cortex proceeds in a heterochronous manner: sensory and motor cortices mature earlier than association cortices, and hierarchical refinement of plasticity-regulating features produces declines in the malleability of the cortex to environmental input, suggesting that the early environment may have a disproportionate effect on the pace of cortical development. In the current work, we show that the development of cortical brain networks during the first three years of life is strongly associated with features of the early environment, suggesting these influences may play a key role in shaping this trajectory. Specifically, we observe that developmental increases in cortical functional network segregation are accelerated in neonates and toddlers with greater exposure to prenatal disadvantage. Environmental associations with the development of cortical network segregation conform to a sensorimotor-association hierarchy of cortical organization, with the strongest associations visible in early-developing somatosensory systems that have high plasticity during this period. Differences in cortical functional network segregation associated with the early environment are also associated with language abilities at age 2 years when controlling for prenatal disadvantage.
We find that being born into a more advantaged environment is associated with a more protracted trajectory of cortical functional network development in toddlerhood and early childhood, and our robustness analyses suggest that these effects are primarily driven by variation in socioeconomic factors. Prior work has found that differences in intrinsic functional connectivity associated with SES are evident at 6 months of age24. Lower levels of cortical network segregation in children from more advantaged backgrounds at 3 years of age are also consistent with our earlier work in adolescents, where children from higher-SES backgrounds had lower levels of cortical network segregation at age 8 years20. This underscores the possibility that being born into a more advantaged household may be associated with more protracted cortical functional network development throughout childhood, with children from more advantaged backgrounds showing more widespread connectivity and thus lower segregation early in development before the rapid development of a more segregated network architecture occurring during adolescence and into adulthood22. Experiences associated with disadvantage may result in earlier declines in plasticity in a heterogenous and spatially patterned manner across the cortex. For example, one study using a putative measure of plasticity to probe how developmental variation in intrinsic cortical activity is associated with the environment finds that lower neighborhood SES is associated with reduced intrinsic activity, suggestive of decreased plasticity, in association cortex during adolescence45. Earlier reductions in plasticity associated with disadvantage might thereby result in decreased synaptic proliferation46,47 or alterations in the development of inhibitory interneurons48,49, as has been found in animal models of early environmental stress, resulting in decreased range for optimal synaptic pruning and wiring of segregated cortical networks. In our cohort, neonates from more advantaged backgrounds show increased mean diffusivity, a measure of white matter microstructure, at birth compared to neonates from less advantaged backgrounds50. As this measure of white matter microstructure typically decreases with age51, we speculate that this pattern could be indicative of more protracted white matter development in neonates from more advantaged backgrounds; accelerated development associated with higher advantage might be observable in both intrinsic cortical network segregation and also the white matter connectivity that supports intrinsic brain activity, questions that can be explored in future work.
We find that alterations in cortical network segregation associated with prenatal disadvantage are driven fundamentally by effects at the local scale. Associations between cortical network segregation and SES in older children and adolescents are also driven by alterations in local network architecture20. In youth and adolescents, associations between household SES and local segregation are evident in the prefrontal cortex specifically among youth living in low-SES neighborhoods, more evidence for environmental influences at the local scale during development21. However, adulthood SES is associated with aging-associated declines in global segregation52,53, thus one possibility is that associations between disadvantage and cortical network segregation follow a progression from local to global across the lifespan. However, the authors did not examine local or meso-scale measures of segregation, so this possibility has not yet been directly addressed; more work is needed to clarify the scale of environmental effects on measures of cortical network segregation.
We find that environmental associations with the development of cortical functional network segregation conform to a sensorimotor-association hierarchy of cortical organization, with the strongest associations with early environment visible in early-developing somatosensory systems, specifically, somatomotor, dorsal attention, and visual systems, with the frontoparietal system also showing strong environmental associations. While the dorsal attention system may not canonically be considered an early-developing system, dorsal attention regions mature dramatically between birth and 2 years of age5, are structurally and functionally connected to visual cortex54,55, and fall at the unimodal end of the primary gradient of macroscale cortical organization56. Environmental associations with cortical network segregation aligning with the sensorimotor-association hierarchy is consistent with a model where age-dependent changes in cortical plasticity allow environment effects to exert differential inputs on the human brain, depending on developmental timing, such that early plasticity in primary sensory regions allows for the largest environmental associations with intrinsic cortical activity in these regions during the first years of life.
Accumulating evidence from large-scale studies of older children and adolescents also suggests another possibility: environmental effects may be transduced through lower-level sensory systems, such that regardless of developmental stage, the strongest associations between the environment and intrinsic cortical activity are visible in somatosensory systems57. Across multiple indicators of household and neighborhood disadvantage, strong associations between disadvantage and measures of functional connectivity are consistently found in somatomotor systems in children ages 9–11 years of age58–61. In youth and adolescents ages 8–21 years, associations between SES and age-associated increases in cortical network segregation are also evident in somatomotor cortex20. It is also possible that due to the low level of social mobility in the United States population from which these studies were drawn62,63, socioeconomic position later in development measured in these studies is nearly identical to a participant’s socioeconomic position at birth, and thus the disadvantage associations measured are simply long-lasting instantiations of the effects of the birth environment on early-developing functional brain systems. Alternatively, brain areas at the sensorimotor pole of the S-A axis show the lowest variability between individuals64,65, and thus may enable the detection of effects above and beyond interindividual variability. While environmental associations were most prominent in the somatomotor system, we also observe associations between the early environment and cortical network development in the frontoparietal system. Interestingly, the environmental associations observed in the frontoparietal system are driven by strong age-by-disadvantage interaction effects in regions in the intraparietal sulcus and middle temporal gyrus, rather than in lateral prefrontal regions, suggestive of alignment with an anterior-to-posterior developmental axis.
We find that disadvantage-associated differences in cortical functional network segregation are also associated with language abilities at age 2 years, suggesting that environmental associations with the development of cortical network segregation might underlie disadvantage-associated differences in language abilities observed later in development. The association between cortical network segregation and language abilities holds even when controlling for prenatal disadvantage, suggesting that cortical network segregation is associated with language ability independent of the well-documented association between early disadvantage and later language ability. While to our knowledge no other studies have specifically examined associations between cortical network segregation and language during early development, one study of toddlers and children ages 0–6 years found that increased between-system connectivity was associated with better language performance66, and infants with more language interactions had lower within-system connectivity in the language system67. Both findings potentially indicate that better language performance and more language exposure may be associated with lower levels of network segregation. The directionality of associations between cortical network segregation and language ability we find in toddlers is broadly reversed compared with associations found in adolescents and adults, where higher levels of cortical network segregation have been found to be advantageous for language and cognition9,11,12. During late childhood, children from higher SES backgrounds show lower levels of cortical network segregation, but by early adulthood, higher SES is associated with higher cortical network segregation20, suggesting that initially low cortical network segregation might be beneficial for the development of cognitive and language abilities associated with higher SES (lower disadvantage)15. However, the directionality of associations between cortical network segregation and behavior in adults also varies based on life stage and behavioral domain examined6,10,68, underscoring the importance of understanding developmental trajectories and domain specificity when interpreting the directionality of associations.
There are several possible mechanisms by which variation in the early environment could signal to alter the tempo of brain development. One commonly proposed mechanism is deprivation, where lack of expected inputs at a developmental stage results in earlier pruning of synapses and reduced synaptic connectivity69,70. Recently, we put forth a model in which the valence and variability of experiences interact to predict maturational pace, such that experiences that are chronic and negative encourage faster maturation and restrict plasticity, and experiences that are novel and positive delay maturational processes and enhance plasticity22. Growing up in a more advantaged environment is associated with more cognitively enriching, positively valenced experiences71. This environment—the opposite of deprivation69 — may delay maturational processes and prolong plasticity: animals exposed to enriched environments as juveniles display enhanced markers of synaptic and extracellular markers of plasticity72,73, and the release of neurotransmitters associated with positive experiences increases cortical plasticity and facilitates remodeling74–76. Conversely, negative experiences such as stress might accelerate brain development through several different mechanisms77–79; higher disadvantage is consistently associated with higher stress80. Although in this work we find that prenatal socioeconomic disadvantage has stronger effects on brain development than does psychosocial stress, our measurement of stress was limited; stress may still be one of several mechanisms by which prenatal disadvantage results in changes in the tempo of brain development.
The variability of experiences might also interact with the valence of experiences to predict maturational pace. Repeated exposure to the same experience, signaling to the brain to optimize for the continued occurrence of this experience in the future, can accelerate maturation of specific circuitry81–83. This aligns with theories of stress effects on maturational pace, where repeated stress-detection and stress-regulation leads to faster maturation of the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex77,84. In contrast, rare or highly variable experiences could signal that the environment is still changing, and that plasticity is beneficial85. Computational evolutionary models suggest that individuals who have more variable experiences lose plasticity later in development than those with more consistent experiences86–88, and some evidence from the study of critical periods suggests that periods of plasticity are prolonged when environmental statistics are variable or unreliable89,90. Further work testing these models and delineating which dimensions are most important contributors to the observed effects of the early environment on the pace of brain development will be key to understanding the mechanisms by which environmental cues give rise to changes in maturational tempo.
Several limitations and possible direct extensions of this work should be highlighted. First, it is important to acknowledge that socioeconomic disadvantage is but one form of disadvantage, and other forms not examined here, such as discrimination and systemic racism, may also be associated with brain development91,92. Different environmental exposures associated with disadvantage — such as stress, cognitive stimulation, and unpredictability—are associated with brain development differently70,77,93,94, and differentiating the relative contributions of these associated exposures to the effects that we observe is a critical and urgent future direction. Second, due to the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic, sample sizes at the toddler timepoints were smaller than originally intended, and thus our analyses of behavior at the 2- and 3-year time points should be considered exploratory; we share them as grounds for future work on the topic. Third, we employed a common adult parcellation and set of systems to characterize cortical network development. It is possible that differences in the degree to which a neonate’s fine-grained cortical topography resembles that of an adult might influence measures of cortical network segregation; the continued development of neonatal parcellations95,96 will enable future work to use infant-specific parcellations and systems to characterize brain development. Fourth, here we focus specifically on changes in intrinsic cortical networks during postnatal development, but environmental factors also impact prenatal brain development; future work should investigate mechanisms of associations between the prenatal environment and cortical networks. Finally, while we observed associations between cortical intrinsic network development and language during toddlerhood, accelerated cortical network development may also have implications for the development of cognition and psychopathology that only become evident later in childhood. Longitudinal data from birth to middle childhood on both brain and behavioral development will be necessary to answer this question, data which will fortunately soon be available in the HEALthy Brain and Child Development (HBCD) study97.
The present study demonstrates that the development of intrinsic cortical brain networks during the first three years of life is associated with features of the early environment. The observed effects are consistent with an interpretation of accelerated cortical functional network development during the first years of life in neonates and toddlers exposed to greater prenatal disadvantage. Insight into the timing and directionality of environmental associations with trajectories of cortical brain development are crucial elements for understanding the optimal timing for interventions, to prevent cascading consequences of early maturation. Our results emphasize the importance of expanding and enhancing policies that provide financial support to parents of young children98–101.
Methods
Participants
Neonates were recruited as a part of the Early Life Adversity, Biological Embedding, and Risk for Developmental Precursors of Mental Health Disorders (eLABE) cohort19, whose participants were recruited under the parent March of Dimes study102. Pregnant mothers were recruited and enrolled between the second and third trimesters. Recruitment oversampled mother-infant pairs facing adversity (e.g., poverty and stress). Inclusion criteria for the study included speaking English, mother age 18 years or older, and singleton birth (see Supplementary Table 1 for detailed participant information). Women with alcohol or other substance abuse were excluded. Anatomic MR images were reviewed by a neuroradiologist (J.S.S.) and pediatric neurologist (C.D.S.). Subjects were excluded from the current analyses if they had evidence of brain injury or were born preterm (<37 weeks gestational age, GA). Additional exclusion criteria included pregnancy complications (but not gestational diabetes or hypertension) and known fetal abnormalities including intrauterine growth restriction. This study was approved by the Human Studies Committees at Washington University in St. Louis and informed consent was obtained from a parent of all participants. Participants were compensated $100–$300 per study visit at each timepoint: participants were compensated $100 for scans at the neonatal timepoint and $125 for scans at 2-year and 3-year timepoints, $100 for completion of assessments at each timepoint, and $50–100 for completion of survey measures at each timepoint.
At each timepoint, all participants with usable functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and demographic data were included. No statistical method was used to predetermine sample size. At the neonatal timepoint, 385 neonates were scanned, and participants were excluded from all timepoints for the following reasons: <37 weeks GA at birth (n = 54), brain injury (n = 17), neonatal intensive care unit stay for >7 days, required intubation or chest tube, antibiotics for >3 days, cardiac disease or metabolic disorder (n = 36), birthweight <2000 g (n = 1), and IRB exclusion (n = 1). There were 306 participants who did not meet any of these exclusion criteria (note that some met multiple exclusion criteria).
Of these participants, 261 participants (age range = 38–45 post-menstrual weeks, M = 41.3 months, 54% male) were included in the current analyses at the neonatal timepoint. Neonates were excluded for no usable T2 for registration (n = 27), no functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected or <10 min of usable fMRI data after motion censoring (n = 12), or visible artifacts in FC data (n = 7).
At the 2-year timepoint, 202 participants were scanned, of which 162 were healthy full-term neonates not subject to the exclusions above. Participants were additionally excluded for no usable T1 for registration (n = 68) or no functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected (n = 2), resulting in 92 participants (range = 1.91–2.61 years, M = 2.11 years, 59% male) included at year two. At the 3-year timepoint, 132 participants were scanned, of which 98 were healthy full-term neonates. Participants were additionally excluded for no usable T1 for registration (n = 31) or <5 min of usable fMRI data after motion censoring (n = 1) resulting in 66 participants included from year 3 (range = 2.92–3.97 years, M = 3.22 years, 64% male).
Participants who returned for data collection at year two were not significantly different than participants from whom we were not able to collect year two data in gestational age, birthweight, prenatal disadvantage, psychosocial stress, sex, neighborhood deprivation, or income-to-needs ratio (two-sided t-test, all p’s > 0.05). Participants who returned for data collection at year three were significantly more advantaged (lower prenatal disadvantage, t(358) = −2.42, p = 0.016) than participants from whom we were not able to collect year three data; there were no other significant differences between groups (p’s > 0.05).
MRI data acquisition
Imaging was performed without sedating medications at all three time points using a 3 T Prisma scanner (Siemens Corp.) and 64-channel head coil. During the scan session, structural images were collected: a T2-weighted image at the neonatal timepoint (sagittal, 208 slices, 0.8-mm isotropic resolution, echo time, TE = 563 ms, repetition time, TR = 3200 ms) and a T1-weighted image at the 2- and 3-year timepoints (sagittal, 208 slices, 0.8-mm isotropic resolution, repetition time = 2400 ms, echo time = 2.22 ms). Resting-state functional imaging data (fMRI) were collected using a blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) gradient-recalled echo-planar multiband sequence (72 slices, 2.0-mm isotropic resolution, echo time = 37 ms, repetition time = 800 ms, multiband factor = 8, 420 volumes). Spin-echo field maps were obtained (at least 1 anterior-posterior and 1 posterior-anterior) during each session with the same parameters. Between 2 and 9 5.6 min fMRI BOLD scans were acquired, depending on how the child tolerated the scan (range = 7.5–44.8 min). Framewise Integrated Real-time MRI Monitoring (FIRMM103,104) was used during scanning to monitor real time participant movement.
MRI data preprocessing
fMRI preprocessing included correction of intensity differences attributable to interleaved acquisition, bias field correction, intensity normalization of each run to a whole-brain mode value of 1,000, linear realignment within and across runs to compensate for rigid body motion, and linear registration of BOLD images to the adult Talairach isotropic atlas. Neonates were registered: BOLD to individual T2 to group-average T2 from this cohort to 711-2 N Talairach atlas. Toddlers were registered: BOLD to individual T1 to group-average T1 from this cohort to 711-2 N Talairach atlas. Field distortion correction was performed, using the FSL 6.0.4 TOPUP toolbox (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/TOPUP). Functions from the 4dfp analysis suite v0.1.0 were used for preprocessing (https://4dfp.readthedocs.io/en/latest/). Following initial processing, frames contaminated by motion were censored as described below. A surface-based neonatal parcellation approach, Melbourne Children’s Regional Brain Atlases (MCRIB), was used to generate surfaces for each neonatal subject and the volumetric resting-state BOLD timeseries were mapped to subject-specific surfaces using established procedures adapted from the Human Connectome Project as implemented in Connectome Workbench 1.2.3. Freesurfer 7.2 was used to generate surfaces for each toddler subject, and the volumetric resting-state BOLD timeseries were mapped to subject-specific surfaces using identical procedures adapted from the Human Connectome Project as implemented in Connectome Workbench 1.2.3.
After mapping to the surface, each dataset underwent resting-state fMRI preprocessing. Data were processed with the following steps: (i) nuisance regression including white matter, ventricles, extra-axial cerebrospinal fluid, and whole brain, as well as 24-parameter Friston expansion regressors derived from head motion, and (ii) band-pass filtering (0.005 Hz <f < 0.1 Hz) with demean and detrend within-run, interpolating censored frames within run.
Neonatal fMRI data were censored at FD > 0.25 mm, with the additional restriction that only epochs of at least 3 consecutive frames FD < 0.25 mm were included. This FD threshold was selected after taking into account the smaller radius of infants’ heads105 and reviewing motion traces in several subjects106,107; respiratory filtering is unsuitable for neonatal fMRI data due to the higher respiratory rate of neonates. Toddler (2-year and 3-year) fMRI data were censored based on a threshold of FDfilt > 0.2 mm, using a filtered framewise displacement trace corrected for the effect of respiration (FIRMM filtered FD108,109), with the additional restriction that only epochs of at least 3 consecutive frames FD < 0.2 mm were included.
In order to be included in the study, a minimum of 5 min (375 frames) of data retained after censoring was required, though 99% of scans across timepoints had >10 min of data retained after censoring (M = 17.4 min (1308 frames), range = 7.1–41.9 min). To account for any potential patterns of FC related to head motion or amount of data included in analyses, we calculated (i) the number of frames retained after censoring and (ii) the average FD across uncensored frames for each individual subject and included these values as subject-level covariates in all analyses. We additionally calculated (iii) the average FD across all frames for each individual subject, used as subject-level covariates in sensitivity analyses. Neither average uncensored FD nor total number of frames retained after censoring were related to prenatal disadvantage (uncensored FD: r = 0.014, p = 0.77; total number of frames: r = 0.06, p = 0.22).
fMRI data were aligned across subjects into the fs_LR32k surface space using spherical registration. Timecourses for surface data were smoothed with geodesic 2D Gaussian kernels (σ = 2.25 mm).
Network analysis
Residual mean BOLD time series from each participant at each timepoint were extracted from a 333-region cortical parcellation110. The functional connectivity matrix was represented as a graph or network111. Network edges between nodes (brain regions) were represented by the Fisher z-transformed Pearson correlation between time courses from pairs of surface regions112. Because there is not yet consensus on the spatial layout of neonatal functional networks105,113–118, we assigned nodes to 13 large-scale functional systems (also sometimes called “networks”) based on the definitions of functional systems derived in healthy adults110.
Across the cortex, we calculated three summary measures of functional network segregation, which quantify the extent to which groups or subnetworks of nodes, or brain regions, display densely interconnected patterns of connectivity (see Fig. 1). System segregation, our measure of global segregation, quantifies the difference between mean within-system connectivity and mean between-system connectivity as a proportion of mean within-system connectivity4,9, given an a priori partition of nodes into systems, in this case the 13 large-scale functional systems110. Modularity, quantified by the modularity quality index (Q), is a measure of meso-scale network segregation that estimates the extent to which the nodes of a network can be subdivided into groups or modules characterized by strong, dense intramodular connectivity and weak, sparse intermodular connectivity. Local segregation was measured using the clustering coefficient, which quantifies the amount of connectivity between a node and its strongest neighbors119–122. A node has a high clustering coefficient when a high proportion of its neighbors are also strong neighbors of each other. Local segregation at the whole-brain level was calculated as the average of the clustering coefficient across all cortical regions. In later analyses of regional specificity, we examined the clustering coefficient of individual nodes or regions. We specifically chose measures of functional network architecture that were suitable for weighted, signed networks, when possible.
We also estimated a measure of functional network integration, the participation coefficient, which quantifies the diversity of a node’s connections across systems42,123. See Supplementary Methods for further details of network analysis and equations for measures of network segregation and integration.
Demographics and socioeconomic status
Mothers completed surveys in each trimester, at delivery, and during follow up visits every 4 months to assess social background, mental health, and life experiences. Child race and ethnicity were obtained from the child’s birth certificate, options included White, Black or African American, American Indian or Alaska Native, Asian Indian, Chinese, Filipino, Japanese, Korean, Vietnamese, Other Asian, Native Hawaiian, Guamanian or Chamorro, Samoan, Other Pacific Islander, or Other for race; ethnicity was assessed by whether the child was identified as Hispanic. Sex assigned at birth was obtained from child birth certificates. Prenatal disadvantage was assessed using a latent factor of socioeconomic disadvantage from a confirmatory factor analysis using MPlus software, which included measures of mother’s income-to-needs ratio, educational attainment, area deprivation index, insurance status, and nutrition19. Maternal self-reported highest level of education and health insurance status were collected in trimester 1. Mothers reported household income and persons in the home to calculate INR in each trimester124. Home addresses were collected at delivery to obtain Area Deprivation Index percentiles; the area deprivation index is a geocoding measure that ranks neighborhoods by socioeconomic disadvantage compared with the national average based on census block data, including factors for the domains of income, education, employment, and housing quality125. Prior work in the eLABE cohort has shown that, while 26% of mothers changed addresses during pregnancy, there was no significant change in block groups during pregnancy126, and we similarly find that there was not significant social mobility (change in ADI) during the study time period in our sample (Fs(child age) = 1.44, p = 0.19). Maternal nutrition was assessed in the third trimester or at delivery using the Healthy Eating Index, a validated dietary assessment tool available through the National Institutes of Health used to measure diet quality based on U.S. Dietary Guidelines for Americans127. Higher scores on the prenatal disadvantage factor are indicative of more disadvantage.
Data on demographic and socioeconomic indicators (education, household income, insurance status, ADI) were also collected during follow-up visits. Prenatal disadvantage was highly correlated with disadvantage at years 1–3 (r’s = 0.92–0.93, p’s < 1 × 10−16). While we focus our analyses on the disadvantage factor assessed at birth, supplementary analyses investigate the contribution of disadvantage at later time points. Additionally, we examine the effect of using a composite variable of parental education and income to assess SES, rather than the disadvantage factor, for consistency with prior literature, in our sensitivity analyses.
Cognitive and language outcomes
The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-Third Edition (Bayley-III) was used to assess cognitive, language, and motor abilities at both 2 and 3 years of age. Based on prior evidence that network segregation is associated with higher-order cognitive abilities in adults9, we specifically examined age-standardized cognitive and language composite scores (M = 100, SD = 15); the language composite consists of the Receptive Language and Expressive Language subscales. At the 2-year timepoint, n = 90 children had usable Bayley assessments and fMRI data (2 children with imaging data were excluded for low-quality or missing Bayley). At the three-year timepoint, n = 66 children had usable Bayley assessments and fMRI data (no children with imaging data were excluded for low-quality or missing Bayley).
Data analysis
Our analyses relating age and prenatal disadvantage to cortical functional network architecture were pre-registered at https://aspredicted.org/eb5pd.pdf. Any deviations from the original plan or additional exploratory analyses have been fully described below.
Statistical models
To flexibly model longitudinal linear and non-linear relationships between cortical functional network segregation and age, we implemented GAMMs (generalized additive mixed models) using the mgcv package in R128. GAMMs were fit with functional network segregation as the dependent variable, age as a smooth term, a random effect of participant, and biological sex assigned at birth, in-scanner motion (average framewise displacement), number of frames of fMRI retained after censoring, and average functional network weight (average network connectivity) as linear covariates. Each GAMM estimates a smooth function (the model age fit, generated from a linear combination of weighted basis functions) that describes the relationship between functional network segregation and age, thus modeling the developmental trajectory of network segregation. Four basis functions were specified as the maximum flexibility afforded to age splines in all models (k = 4). Models were fit using thin plate regression splines as the smooth term basis set and the restricted maximal likelihood approach for smoothing parameter selection. Average network weight was included to control for global differences in connectivity strength129–131. Random effects included a random intercept per participant. To test for windows of significant change across the age range, we calculated the first derivative of the smooth function of age from the GAMM model using finite differences, and then generated a simultaneous 95% confidence interval of the derivative132 using the gratia package in R. The first derivative of this smooth function represents the rate of change in network segregation at a given developmental time point. Intervals of significant age-related change were identified as areas where the simultaneous confidence interval of the derivative does not include zero. Multiple comparisons correction was applied across models of developmental changes in cortical functional network segregation using FDR correction133.
To examine associations between prenatal disadvantage and age-related increases in functional network segregation, we allowed the smoothed age effect in the GAMM to interact with prenatal disadvantage; predictors thus included an age-by-disadvantage interaction term, a smooth term for age, and covariates including sex, in-scanner motion, number of frames of fMRI, and average network weight. We compared two interaction models: a simpler varying coefficient (linear-nonlinear) model that allows the smooth term for age to vary as a linear function of prenatal disadvantage, and a more complex non-linear interaction (bivariate smooth) model that allows the smooth term for age to vary as a fully non-linear function of prenatal disadvantage. We compared models using Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and evaluated the significance of the interaction term for the selected model. All models were best fit using the simpler varying coefficient (linear-nonlinear) model that allows the linear association between prenatal disadvantage and network segregation to vary as a smooth function of age. Interaction p-values were confirmed using a parametric bootstrap likelihood ratio test (pbkrtest package in R) for significance estimation in the mixed model context. Multiple comparisons correction was applied across models of age-by-disadvantage associations with cortical functional network segregation using FDR correction. While prenatal disadvantage was modeled continuously, for visualization purposes in plots we show trajectories from the highest and lowest deciles.
To evaluate the scale at which prenatal disadvantage is associated with measures of functional network segregation, we fit models for each of our measures of local, meso-scale, and global segregation in turn, controlling for each of the other measures of segregation. For example, we first fit a GAMM for the age-by-disadvantage effect on system segregation while including modularity as a fixed effect, then fit a GAMM for age-by-disadvantage effects on system segregation while including the clustering coefficient as a fixed effect.
To characterize regional specificity of prenatal disadvantage associations with maturational changes in local segregation, we fit region-specific GAMMs with the same interaction structure and covariates as the whole-brain models. Models were fit separately for each parcellated cortical region. For each regional GAMM, the significance of the age-by-disadvantage interaction term was assessed in a fixed degree-of-freedom context to ensure stable and accurate estimation. We corrected p-values across all region-wise GAMMs using FDR correction and set statistical significance at pFDR < 0.05. To establish the overall magnitude of the moderating effect of prenatal disadvantage on age-associated increases in local segregation, which we refer to throughout as a region’s overall age-by-disadvantage effect magnitude, we used the F-statistic for the age-by-disadvantage interaction effect. To evaluate the age-by-disadvantage effect across functional systems, we used the effect magnitude at each parcel within each system. A Kruskal–Wallis test was used to compare the magnitude of the age-by-disadvantage effects across functional systems.
Moderating effects of prenatal disadvantage on associations between age and language composite scores were also modeled with GAMMs; we allowed the smoothed age effect in the GAMM to interact with disadvantage, using the same model comparison framework as above. We tested a varying-coefficient (linear-nonlinear) model and a more complex fully non-linear interaction (bivariate smooth) model, comparing models using Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and evaluated the significance of the interaction term for the selected model; the bivariate smooth model fit the language composite best, while the linear-nonlinear model fit the cognitive composite best. Note that as these the composite scores are standardized for age, the smoothed effect of age in this model solely accounts for variation over time in language scores relative to age norms.
When examining relationships between local cortical network segregation and language outcomes at the 2-year and 3-year timepoints, due to the smaller number of datapoints available, we used linear models (results are qualitatively unchanged when using GAMs). We estimated associations between local segregation and Bayley language scaled scores, controlling for biological sex assigned at birth, in-scanner motion (average framewise displacement), number of frames of fMRI retained after censoring, and average functional network weight. Multiple comparisons correction was applied across each set of two models using FDR correction.
Comparison of cortical maps
A previously derived axis of sensorimotor-association cortical organization8 was retrieved from https://github.com/PennLINC/S-A_ArchetypalAxis. To quantify the association between S-A axis ranks and observed environmental effects on developmental increases in local segregation, we used Spearman’s rank correlations and tested for statistical significance using spin-based spatial permutation tests134,135, which account for spatial covariance structure common in neuroimaging data. We generated a null distribution based on 10,000 spherical rotations, and compared the observed value to the null.
Deviations from pre-registration
We did not conduct some analyses in the pre-registration that were deemed potential exploratory analyses and turned out to be unhelpful in elucidating the effects we found. Specifically, as the primary driver of the age-by-disadvantage effect was found to be on local segregation (the clustering coefficient) and alterations in local topology, we did not examine age-by-disadvantage effects on within- and between-system connectivity; this analysis would have been informative for probing age-by-disadvantage effects on system segregation. As our measure of local segregation, the clustering coefficient, does not rely on an a priori assignment of regions to functional systems, we deemed an analysis of functional-system-level connectivity unhelpful in investigating the effects we found here. Also, in a follow-up analysis we had planned to examine the effect of including only participants whose disadvantage factor score did not change by >1 SD between timepoints. However, as upon investigation there was little change in direct indicators of disadvantage during the study period (see Supplementary Fig. 3), and the composition of the disadvantage factor score changed from the birth to toddler timepoints (prenatal Healthy Eating Index was removed from the factor score), we decided not to pursue this analysis.
Additionally, we include here follow-up analyses that were not part of our pre-registration to probe the contributors and potential behavioral associations of the moderating associations of prenatal disadvantage with cortical functional network development trajectories that we found in the pre-registered analyses. Specifically, we examined a) which scale of segregation was the fundamental driver of age-by-disadvantage effects on cortical network segregation, b) regional variation in age-by-disadvantage effects on local segregation, and c) associations between local segregation and language and cognitive outcomes. These analyses were not pre-registered and should be considered exploratory in nature.
Finally, we initially planned to rerun any GAMMs with estimated degrees of freedom (EDF) of the age smooth <2 as linear mixed effects models, but upon visual inspection of developmental trajectories of cortical network segregation, non-linear models of age seemed appropriate even for those measures with age EDFs slightly under 2. As GAMMs can model both linear and non-linear relationships, in the case that there is no non-linear relationship between age and an outcome measure, the smooth term for age will be penalized down to a linear term. Thus, for ease of comparison and to avoid switching modeling approaches, we have instead used GAMMs for all models in the included analyses.
Citation diversity statement
Recent work in several fields of science has identified a bias in citation practices such that papers from women and other under-represented scholars are undercited relative to the number of such papers in the field136–142. We used prior methods140,143 to measure that our references contain 20.69% woman(first)/woman(last), 21.12% man/woman, 26.87% woman/man, and 31.31% man/man authors. This method is limited in that (a) names, pronouns, and social media profiles used to construct the databases may not, in every case, be indicative of gender identity and (b) it cannot account for intersex, non-binary, or transgender people. We used additional methods144,145 to measure that our references contain 11.82% author of color (first)/author of color(last), 14.11% white author/author of color, 21.51% author of color/white author, and 52.56% white author/white author. This method is limited in that (a) names, Census entries, and Wikipedia profiles used to predict racial and ethnic categories may not be indicative of racial or ethnic identity, and (b) it cannot account for Indigenous and mixed-race authors, or those who may face differential biases due to the ambiguous racialization or ethnicization of their names. We look forward to future work that could help us to better understand how to support equitable practices in science.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank the families involved with the study, and Karen Lukas, Eva Loveless, and Rich Nagel for assistance with scanning neonates. We thank the past and current team members of the March of Dimes Prematurity Research Center at Washington University in St Louis, the Early Emotional Development Program at Washington University in St Louis, and the Washington University Neonatal Development Research Group. This research was supported by the National Institute of Health (grant nos. R01 MH121877 to J.L.L. and C.E.R., R01 DA046224 to C.E.R., R01 MH113570 to C.D.S. and C.E.R., R01 MH113883 to C.D.S., J.L.L. and B.W.W., and T32 GR0029379 to U.A.T.); Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (P50 HD103525 to C.D.S., C.E.R., and J.S.S.; R01 p to D.M.B. and J.L.L.), the March of Dimes Foundation, and institutional support from St. Louis Children’s Hospital, Barnes-Jewish Hospital, and Washington University in St. Louis School of Medicine.
Author contributions
U.A.T. designed the analyses, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. A.L., J.K.K., D.A., and T.S. provided data curation and data analysis. A.N.N. and L.G. assisted with discussions of interpretation and analysis. B.B.W., J.S.S., and J.J.N. assisted with methodology and provided funding. J.L.L, D.M.B., C.E.R. and C.D.S. provided supervision, funding, and edited the manuscript. All authors commented on the manuscript at all stages.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Wei Gao, Charles Geier, and Alexis Brieant for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Data availability
The derived neural and behavioral data (processed and aggregated final data in CSV form) that can be used to replicate the findings have been deposited in Github (https://zenodo.org/doi/10.5281/zenodo.12785442). The deidentified data (metric outputs from imaging data after preprocessing and labeled spreadsheets) from the eLABE sample will be deposited into the NIMH Data Archive repository upon conclusion of the longitudinal portion of the study, as per NIH rules and regulations. Study analyses additionally made use of publicly available cortical atlases, including the Gordon 333-region parcellation (https://balsa.wustl.edu/2Vm69) and the sensorimotor-association axis (https://pennlinc.github.io/S-A_ArchetypalAxis/).
Code availability
All statistical analyses were conducted in R4.1.2 (https://www.r-project.org/) and MATLAB R2021b. Functions from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox123 were used to calculate measures of network segregation and integration. Freely available MATLAB code from https://github.com/mychan24/system_matrix_tools was used to calculate system segregation. Surfaces and regional effects were shown on cortical surfaces generated by MCRIB using the cifti and ciftiTools packages146 and Connectome Workbench 7.2. Code for all analyses presented here is publicly available at https://github.com/utooley/Tooley2023_prenatal_env_cortical_network_dev, and has been deposited into Zenodo147.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-024-52242-4.
References
- 1.Zhao, T., Xu, Y. & He, Y. Graph theoretical modeling of baby brain networks. NeuroImage10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.038 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Wen, X. et al. First-year development of modules and hubs in infant brain functional networks. NeuroImage185, 222–235 (2019). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Grayson, D. S. & Fair, D. A. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to the neuroimaging literature. NeuroImage160, 15–31 (2017). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wig, G. S. Segregated systems of human brain networks. Trends Cogn. Sci.21, 981–996 (2017). 10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gao, W. et al. The Synchronization within and Interaction between the Default and Dorsal Attention Networks in Early Infancy. Cereb. Cortex23, 594–603 (2013). 10.1093/cercor/bhs043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gu, S. et al. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA112, 13681–13686 (2015). 10.1073/pnas.1502829112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tooley, U. A. et al. The age of reason: functional brain network development during childhood. J. Neurosci.42, 8237–8251 (2022). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0511-22.2022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sydnor, V. J. et al. Neurodevelopment of the association cortices: patterns, mechanisms, and implications for psychopathology. Neuron109, 2820–2846 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 9.Chan, M. Y., Park, D. C., Savalia, N. K., Petersen, S. E. & Wig, G. S. Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA111, E4997–E5006 (2014). 10.1073/pnas.1415122111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cohen, J. R. & D’Esposito, M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J. Neurosci.36, 12083–12094 (2016). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zhang, H. & Diaz, M. T. Resting state network segregation modulates age-related differences in language production. Neurobiol. Lang. 1–22 10.1162/nol_a_00106 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 12.Nashiro, K., Sakaki, M., Braskie, M. N. & Mather, M. Resting-state networks associated with cognitive processing show more age-related decline than those associated with emotional processing. Neurobiol. Aging54, 152–162 (2017). 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Chen, E. & Matthews, K. A. Childhood socioeconomic status and adult health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.1186, 37–55 (2010). 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05334.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bundy, J. D. et al. Social determinants of health and premature death among adults in the USA from 1999 to 2018: a national cohort study. Lancet Public Health8, e422–e431 (2023). 10.1016/S2468-2667(23)00081-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kaplan, G. A. et al. Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. Int. J. Epidemiol.30, 256–263 (2001). 10.1093/ije/30.2.256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Evans, G. W. & Cassells, R. C. Childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and mental health in emerging adults. Clin. Psychol. Sci. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci.2, 287–296 (2014). 10.1177/2167702613501496 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Farah, M. J. The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron96, 56–71 (2017). 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kachmar, A. G., Connolly, C. A., Wolf, S. & Curley, M. A. Q. Socioeconomic status in pediatric health research: a scoping review. J. Pediatr.213, 163–170 (2019). 10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Luby, J. L. et al. Social disadvantage during pregnancy: effects on gestational age and birthweight. J. Perinatol.43, 477–483 (2023). 10.1038/s41372-023-01643-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tooley, U. A. et al. Associations between neighborhood SES and functional brain network development. Cereb. Cortex30, 1–19 (2020). 10.1093/cercor/bhz066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gellci, K. et al. Community and household-level socioeconomic disadvantage and functional organization of the salience and emotion network in children and adolescents. NeuroImage184, 729–740 (2019). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tooley, U. A., Bassett, D. S. & Mackey, A. P. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.22, 372–384 (2021). 10.1038/s41583-021-00457-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ramphal, B. et al. Brain connectivity and socioeconomic status at birth and externalizing symptoms at age 2 years. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 100811 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100811 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 24.Gao, W. et al. Functional network development during the first year: relative sequence and socioeconomic correlations. Cereb. Cortex25, 2919–2928 (2015). 10.1093/cercor/bhu088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kaufmann, T. et al. Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nat. Neurosci.22, 1617–1623 (2019). 10.1038/s41593-019-0471-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Karolinska Schizophrenia Project (KaSP). et al. Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nat. Neurosci.22, 1617–1623 (2019). 10.1038/s41593-019-0471-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shaw, P., Gogtay, N. & Rapoport, J. Childhood psychiatric disorders as anomalies in neurodevelopmental trajectories. Hum. Brain Mapp.31, 917–925 (2010). 10.1002/hbm.21028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mendle, J., Harden, K. P., Brooks-Gunn, J. & Graber, J. A. Development’s tortoise and hare: pubertal timing, pubertal tempo, and depressive symptoms in boys and girls. Dev. Psychol.46, 1341–1353 (2010). 10.1037/a0020205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Colich, N. L. et al. Earlier age at menarche as a transdiagnostic mechanism linking childhood trauma with multiple forms of psychopathology in adolescent girls. Psychol. Med.50, 1090–1098 (2020). 10.1017/S0033291719000953 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Deardorff, J. et al. Girls’ pubertal timing and tempo and mental health: a longitudinal examination in an ethnically diverse sample. J. Adolesc. Health68, 1197–1203 (2021). 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.01.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hensch, T. K. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.6, 877–888 (2005). 10.1038/nrn1787 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Werker, J. F. & Hensch, T. K. Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol.66, 173–196 (2015). 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ellis, B. J., Sheridan, M. A., Belsky, J. & McLaughlin, K. A. Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Dev. Psychopathol.34, 447–471 (2022). 10.1017/S0954579421001838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L. & Ellis, B. J. Beyond cumulative risk: distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Dev. Psychol.48, 662–673 (2012). 10.1037/a0024454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Frankenhuis, W. E. & Amir, D. What is the expected human childhood? Insights from evolutionary anthropology. Dev. Psychopathol.34, 473–497 (2022). 10.1017/S0954579421001401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Roubinov, D., Meaney, M. J. & Boyce, W. T. Change of pace: How developmental tempo varies to accommodate failed provision of early needs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.031 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37.Rakesh, D., Whittle, S., Sheridan, M. A. & McLaughlin, K. A. Childhood socioeconomic status and the pace of structural neurodevelopment: accelerated, delayed, or simply different? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10.1016/j.tics.2023.03.011 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Lean, R. E. et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal social disadvantage and psychosocial stress and neonatal white matter connectivity at birth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA119, e2204135119 (2022). 10.1073/pnas.2204135119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Triplett, R. L. et al. Association of prenatal exposure to early-life adversity with neonatal brain volumes at birth. JAMA Netw. Open5, e227045 (2022). 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.7045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Herzberg, M. P. et al. Maternal prenatal social disadvantage and neonatal functional connectivity: Associations with psychopathology symptoms at age 12 months. Dev. Psychol. No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified 10.1037/dev0001708 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Bassett, D. S. & Bullmore, E. T. Small-world brain networks revisited. Neuroscientist23, 499–516 (2017). 10.1177/1073858416667720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Guimerà, R. & Nunes Amaral, L. A. Functional cartography of complex metabolic networks. Nature433, 895–900 (2005). 10.1038/nature03288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Marek, S., Hwang, K., Foran, W., Hallquist, M. N. & Luna, B. The contribution of network organization and integration to the development of cognitive control. PLOS Biol.13, e1002328 (2015). 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sanders, A. F. P. et al. Age-related differences in resting-state functional connectivity from childhood to adolescence. Cereb. Cortex bhad011 10.1093/cercor/bhad011 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Sydnor, V. J. et al. Intrinsic activity development unfolds along a sensorimotor-association cortical axis in youth. Nat Neurosci.26, 638–649 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 46.Tanti, A. et al. Region-dependent and stage-specific effects of stress, environmental enrichment, and antidepressant treatment on hippocampal neurogenesis. Hippocampus23, 797–811 (2013). 10.1002/hipo.22134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bath, K. G., Manzano-Nieves, G. & Goodwill, H. Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Horm. Behav.82, 64–71 (2016). 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Manzano Nieves, G., Bravo, M., Baskoylu, S. & Bath, K. G. Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. eLife9, e55263 (2020). 10.7554/eLife.55263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Goodwill, H. L. et al. Early life stress drives sex-selective impairment in reversal learning by affecting parvalbumin interneurons in orbitofrontal cortex of mice. Cell Rep.25, 2299–2307.e4 (2018). 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lebel, C., Treit, S. & Beaulieu, C. A review of diffusion MRI of typical white matter development from early childhood to young adulthood. NMR Biomed.32, e3778 (2019). 10.1002/nbm.3778 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lebel, C. & Deoni, S. The development of brain white matter microstructure. NeuroImage182, 207–218 (2018). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chan, M. Y. et al. Long-term prognosis and educational determinants of brain network decline in older adult individuals. Nat. Aging1, 1053–1067 (2021). 10.1038/s43587-021-00125-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chan, M. Y. et al. Socioeconomic status moderates age-related differences in the brain’s functional network organization and anatomy across the adult lifespan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA115, E5144–E5153 (2018). 10.1073/pnas.1714021115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.3, 201–215 (2002). 10.1038/nrn755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Greenberg, A. S. et al. Visuotopic cortical connectivity underlying attention revealed with white-matter tractography. J. Neurosci. J. Soc. Neurosci.32, 2773–2782 (2012). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5419-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dong, H.-M., Margulies, D. S., Zuo, X.-N. & Holmes, A. J. Shifting gradients of macroscale cortical organization mark the transition from childhood to adolescence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA118, e2024448118 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Rosen, M. L., Amso, D. & McLaughlin, K. A. The role of the visual association cortex in scaffolding prefrontal cortex development: a novel mechanism linking socioeconomic status and executive function. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 100699 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100699 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 58.Rakesh, D., Seguin, C., Zalesky, A., Cropley, V. & Whittle, S. Associations between neighborhood disadvantage, resting-state functional connectivity, and behavior in the adolescent brain cognitive development study: the moderating role of positive family and school environments. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging6, 877–886 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rakesh, D., Zalesky, A. & Whittle, S. Similar but distinct—effects of different socioeconomic indicators on resting state functional connectivity: findings from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study®. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.51, 101005 (2021). 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Sripada, C. et al. Socioeconomic resources are associated with distributed alterations of the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture in youth. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.58, 101164 (2022). 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Modabbernia, A., Janiri, D., Doucet, G. E., Reichenberg, A. & Frangou, S. Multivariate patterns of brain-behavior-environment associations in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study. Biol. Psychiatry89, 510–520 (2021). 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Song, X. et al. Long-term decline in intergenerational mobility in the United States since the 1850s. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA117, 251–258 (2020). 10.1073/pnas.1905094116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Chetty, R. et al. The fading American dream: trends in absolute income mobility since 1940. Science356, 398–406 (2017). 10.1126/science.aal4617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Mueller, S. et al. Individual variability in functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Neuron77, 586–595 (2013). 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Stoecklein, S. et al. Variable functional connectivity architecture of the preterm human brain: Impact of developmental cortical expansion and maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA117, 1201–1206 (2020). 10.1073/pnas.1907892117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Bruchhage, M. M. K., Ngo, G.-C., Schneider, N., D’Sa, V. & Deoni, S. C. L. Functional connectivity correlates of infant and early childhood cognitive development. Brain Struct. Funct.225, 669–681 (2020). 10.1007/s00429-020-02027-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.King, L. S., Camacho, M. C., Montez, D. F., Humphreys, K. L. & Gotlib, I. H. Naturalistic language input is associated with resting-state functional connectivity in infancy. J. Neurosci.41, 424–434 (2021). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0779-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Yue, Q. et al. Brain modularity mediates the relation between task complexity and performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci.29, 1532–1546 (2017). 10.1162/jocn_a_01142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Sheridan, M. A. & McLaughlin, K. A. Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat. Trends Cogn. Sci.18, 580–585 (2014). 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A. & Lambert, H. K. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.47, 578–591 (2014). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., McAdoo, H. P. & Coll, C. G. The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Dev.72, 1844–1867 (2001). 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Favuzzi, E. et al. Activity-dependent gating of parvalbumin interneuron function by the perineuronal net protein brevican. Neuron95, 639–655.e10 (2017). 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Duffy, S. N. Environmental enrichment modifies the PKA-dependence of hippocampal LTP and improves hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn. Mem.8, 26–34 (2001). 10.1101/lm.36301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bao, S., Chan, V. T. & Merzenich, M. M. Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature412, 79–83 (2001). 10.1038/35083586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Vetencourt, J. F. M. et al. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science320, 385–388 (2008). 10.1126/science.1150516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Bear, M. F. & Singer, W. Modulation of visual cortical plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature320, 172–176 (1986). 10.1038/320172a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Gee, D. G. et al. Early developmental emergence of human amygdala–prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA110, 15638–15643 (2013). 10.1073/pnas.1307893110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.McEwen, B. S. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.840, 33–44 (1998). 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Snell-Rood, E. & Snell-Rood, C. The developmental support hypothesis: adaptive plasticity in neural development in response to cues of social support. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.375, 20190491 (2020). 10.1098/rstb.2019.0491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Baum, A., Garofalo, J. P. & Yali, A. M. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: Does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.896, 131–144 (1999). 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08111.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Bassett, D. S., Yang, M., Wymbs, N. F. & Grafton, S. T. Learning-induced autonomy of sensorimotor systems. Nat. Neurosci.18, 744–751 (2015). 10.1038/nn.3993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L. & Posner, M. I. Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA102, 14931–14936 (2005). 10.1073/pnas.0506897102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Romeo, R. R. et al. Beyond the “30 million word gap:” Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychol. Sci.29, 700–710 (2018). 10.1177/0956797617742725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Herringa, R. J. et al. Enhanced prefrontal-amygdala connectivity following childhood adversity as a protective mechanism against internalizing in adolescence. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging1, 326–334 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gopnik, A. Childhood as a solution to explore–exploit tensions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.375, 20190502 (2020). 10.1098/rstb.2019.0502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Frankenhuis, W. E. & Walasek, N. Modeling the evolution of sensitive periods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.41, 100715 (2020). 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Panchanathan, K. & Frankenhuis, W. E. The evolution of sensitive periods in a model of incremental development. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.283, 20152439 (2016). 10.1098/rspb.2015.2439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Frankenhuis, W. E. & Panchanathan, K. Individual differences in developmental plasticity may result from stochastic sampling. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.6, 336–347 (2011). 10.1177/1745691611412602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Chang, E. F. & Merzenich, M. M. Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science300, 498–502 (2003). 10.1126/science.1082163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Erchova, I., Vasalauskaite, A., Longo, V. & Sengpiel, F. Enhancement of visual cortex plasticity by dark exposure. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.372, 20160159 (2017). 10.1098/rstb.2016.0159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Dumornay, N. M., Lebois, L. A. M., Ressler, K. J. & Harnett, N. G. Racial disparities in adversity during childhood and the false appearance of race-related differences in brain structure. Am. J. Psychiatry180, 127–138 (2023). 10.1176/appi.ajp.21090961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Barch, D. M. & Luby, J. L. Understanding social determinants of brain health during development. Am. J. Psychiatry180, 108–110 (2023). 10.1176/appi.ajp.20220991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Callaghan, B. L. & Tottenham, N. The stress acceleration hypothesis: effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.7, 76–81 (2016). 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Short, A. K. & Baram, T. Z. Early-life adversity and neurological disease: age-old questions and novel answers. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 1–13 10.1038/s41582-019-0246-5 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 95.Myers, M. J. et al. Functional parcellation of the neonatal cortical surface. Cereb. Cortex34, bhae047 (2024). 10.1093/cercor/bhae047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wang, F. et al. Fine-grained functional parcellation maps of the infant cerebral cortex. eLife12, e75401 (2023). 10.7554/eLife.75401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Volkow, N. D., Gordon, J. A. & Freund, M. P. The Healthy Brain and Child Development Study—Shedding Light on Opioid Exposure, COVID-19, and Health Disparities. JAMA Psychiatry10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3803 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 98.Averett, S. & Wang, Y. Effects of higher EITC payments on children’s health, quality of home environment, and noncognitive skills. Public Financ. Rev.46, 519–557 (2018). 10.1177/1091142116654965 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Bitler, M., Hoynes, H. & Kuka, E. Child poverty, the great recession, and the social safety net in the United States. J. Policy Anal. Manag.36, 358–389 (2017). 10.1002/pam.21963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Sperber, J. F. et al. Unconditional cash transfers and maternal assessments of children’s health, nutrition, and sleep: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw. Open6, e2335237 (2023). 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.35237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Baltagi, B. H. & Yen, Y.-F. Welfare reform and children’s health. Health Econ.25, 277–291 (2016). 10.1002/hec.3139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Stout, M. J. et al. A multidisciplinary prematurity research cohort study. PLoS One17, e0272155 (2022). 10.1371/journal.pone.0272155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Dosenbach, N. U. F. et al. Real-time motion analytics during brain MRI improve data quality and reduce costs. NeuroImage161, 80–93 (2017). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Badke & D’Andrea, C. et al. Real-time motion monitoring improves functional MRI data quality in infants. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.55, 101116 (2022). 10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Smyser, C. D. et al. Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb. Cortex20, 2852–2862 (2010). 10.1093/cercor/bhq035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L. & Petersen, S. E. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage59, 2142–2154 (2012). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Power, J. D. et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage84, 320–341 (2014). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Kaplan, S. et al. Filtering respiratory motion artifact from resting state fMRI data in infant and toddler populations. NeuroImage247, 118838 (2022). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Fair, D. A. et al. Correction of respiratory artifacts in MRI head motion estimates. NeuroImage208, 116400 (2020). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Gordon, E. M. et al. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. NY26, 288–303 (2016). 10.1093/cercor/bhu239 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Bassett, D. S., Zurn, P. & Gold, J. I. On the nature and use of models in network neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.19, 566 (2018). 10.1038/s41583-018-0038-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Zalesky, A., Fornito, A. & Bullmore, E. On the use of correlation as a measure of network connectivity. NeuroImage60, 2096–2106 (2012). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Lin, W. et al. Functional connectivity MR imaging reveals cortical functional connectivity in the developing brain. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol.29, 1883–1889 (2008). 10.3174/ajnr.A1256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Gao, W. et al. Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA106, 6790–6795 (2009). 10.1073/pnas.0811221106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Doria, V. et al. Emergence of resting state networks in the preterm human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA107, 20015–20020 (2010). 10.1073/pnas.1007921107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Gao, W., Alcauter, S., Smith, J. K., Gilmore, J. & Lin, W. Development of human brain cortical network architecture during infancy. Brain Struct. Funct.220, 1173–1186 (2015). 10.1007/s00429-014-0710-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Eyre, M. et al. The Developing Human Connectome Project: typical and disrupted perinatal functional connectivity. Brain144, 2199–2213 (2021). 10.1093/brain/awab118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Sylvester, C. M. et al. Network-specific selectivity of functional connections in the neonatal brain. Cereb. Cortex bhac202 10.1093/cercor/bhac202 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 119.Achard, S., Salvador, R., Whitcher, B., Suckling, J. & Bullmore, E. A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J. Neurosci.26, 63–72 (2006). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Bartolomei, F. et al. Disturbed functional connectivity in brain tumour patients: evaluation by graph analysis of synchronization matrices. Clin. Neurophysiol.117, 2039–2049 (2006). 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Bassett, D. S., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Achard, S., Duke, T. & Bullmore, E. Adaptive reconfiguration of fractal small-world human brain functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA103, 19518–19523 (2006). 10.1073/pnas.0606005103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Xu, T. et al. Network analysis of functional brain connectivity in borderline personality disorder using resting-state fMRI. NeuroImage Clin.11, 302–315 (2016). 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.02.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Rubinov, M. & Sporns, O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage52, 1059–1069 (2010). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, Poverty Guidelines. U.S. Federal Poverty Guidelines Used to Determine Financial Eligibility for Certain Programs. ASPEhttps://aspe.hhs.gov/topics/poverty-economic-mobility/poverty-guidelines.
- 125.Kind, A. J. H. & Buckingham, W. R. Making neighborhood-disadvantage metrics accessible—the neighborhood atlas. N. Engl. J. Med.378, 2456–2458 (2018). 10.1056/NEJMp1802313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Brady, R. G. et al. The effects of prenatal exposure to neighborhood crime on neonatal functional connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry92, 139–148 (2022). 10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.01.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Krebs-Smith, S. M. et al. Update of the Healthy Eating Index: HEI-2015. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet.118, 1591–1602 (2018). 10.1016/j.jand.2018.05.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Wood, S. N. Stable and efficient multiple smoothing parameter estimation for generalized additive models. J. Am. Stat. Assoc.99, 673–686 (2004). 10.1198/016214504000000980 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Van Wijk, B. C. M., Stam, C. J. & Daffertshofer, A. Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One5, e13701 (2010). 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Ginestet, C. E., Nichols, T. E., Bullmore, E. T. & Simmons, A. Brain network analysis: separating cost from topology using cost-integration. PLoS One6, e21570 (2011). 10.1371/journal.pone.0021570 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Yan, C.-G., Craddock, R. C., Zuo, X.-N., Zang, Y.-F. & Milham, M. P. Standardizing the intrinsic brain: towards robust measurement of inter-individual variation in 1000 functional connectomes. NeuroImage80, 246–262 (2013). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Simpson, G. L. Modelling palaeoecological time series using generalised additive models. Front. Ecol. Evol. 6, 149 (2018).
- 133.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B Methodol.57, 289–300 (1995). 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Alexander-Bloch, A. F. et al. On testing for spatial correspondence between maps of human brain structure and function. NeuroImage178, 540–551 (2018). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Váša, F. et al. Adolescent tuning of association cortex in human structural brain networks. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. N. 199128, 281–294 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Mitchell, S. M., Lange, S. & Brus, H. Gendered citation patterns in international relations journals. Int. Stud. Perspect.14, 485–492 (2013). 10.1111/insp.12026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Dion, M. L., Sumner, J. L. & Mitchell, S. M. Gendered citation patterns across political science and social science methodology fields. Polit. Anal.26, 312–327 (2018). 10.1017/pan.2018.12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Caplar, N., Tacchella, S. & Birrer, S. Quantitative evaluation of gender bias in astronomical publications from citation counts. Nat. Astron.1, 1–5 (2017). [Google Scholar]
- 139.Maliniak, D., Powers, R. & Walter, B. F. The gender citation gap in international relations. Int. Organ.67, 889–922 (2013). 10.1017/S0020818313000209 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Dworkin, J. D. et al. The extent and drivers of gender imbalance in neuroscience reference lists. Nat. Neurosci.23, 918–926 (2020). 10.1038/s41593-020-0658-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Wang, X. et al. Gendered citation practices in the field of communication. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc.45, 134–153 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Chatterjee, P. & Werner, R. M. Gender disparity in citations in high-impact journal articles. JAMA Netw. Open4, e2114509 (2021). 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.14509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Zhou, D. et al. Gender Diversity Statement and Code Notebook v1.0. 10.5281/zenodo.3672110 (2020).
- 144.Ambekar, A., Ward, C., Mohammed, J., Male, S. & Skiena, S. Name-ethnicity classification from open sources. In Proceedings of the 15th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining 49–58 (2009).
- 145.Sood, G. & Laohaprapanon, S. Predicting race and ethnicity from the sequence of characters in a name. ArXiv Prepr. ArXiv180502109 (2018).
- 146.Pham, D., Muschelli, J. & Mejia, A. ciftiTools: a package for reading, writing, visualizing and manipulating CIFTI files in. R. NeuroImage250, 118877 (2022). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118877 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Tooley, U. Prenatal environment is associated with the pace of cortical network development over the first three years of life. Zenodo. 10.5281/zenodo.12785442 2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The derived neural and behavioral data (processed and aggregated final data in CSV form) that can be used to replicate the findings have been deposited in Github (https://zenodo.org/doi/10.5281/zenodo.12785442). The deidentified data (metric outputs from imaging data after preprocessing and labeled spreadsheets) from the eLABE sample will be deposited into the NIMH Data Archive repository upon conclusion of the longitudinal portion of the study, as per NIH rules and regulations. Study analyses additionally made use of publicly available cortical atlases, including the Gordon 333-region parcellation (https://balsa.wustl.edu/2Vm69) and the sensorimotor-association axis (https://pennlinc.github.io/S-A_ArchetypalAxis/).
All statistical analyses were conducted in R4.1.2 (https://www.r-project.org/) and MATLAB R2021b. Functions from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox123 were used to calculate measures of network segregation and integration. Freely available MATLAB code from https://github.com/mychan24/system_matrix_tools was used to calculate system segregation. Surfaces and regional effects were shown on cortical surfaces generated by MCRIB using the cifti and ciftiTools packages146 and Connectome Workbench 7.2. Code for all analyses presented here is publicly available at https://github.com/utooley/Tooley2023_prenatal_env_cortical_network_dev, and has been deposited into Zenodo147.