Abstract
An individual’s risk of substance use disorder (SUD) is shaped by a complex interplay of potent biosocial factors. Current neurodevelopmental models posit vulnerability to SUD in youth is due to an overreactive reward system and reduced inhibitory control. Having a family history of SUD is a particularly strong risk factor, yet few studies have explored its impact on brain function and structure prior to substance exposure. Herein, we utilized a network control theory approach to quantify sex-specific differences in brain activity dynamics in youth with and without a family history of SUD, drawn from a large cohort of substance-naïve youth from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. We summarize brain dynamics by calculating transition energy, which probes the ease with which a whole brain, region or network drives the brain towards a specific spatial pattern of activation (i.e., brain state). Our findings reveal that a family history of SUD is associated with alterations in the brain’s dynamics wherein: i) independent of sex, certain regions’ transition energies are higher in those with a family history of SUD and ii) there exist sex-specific differences in SUD family history groups at multiple levels of transition energy (global, network, and regional). Family history-by-sex effects reveal that energetic demand is increased in females with a family history of SUD and decreased in males with a family history of SUD, compared to their same-sex counterparts with no SUD family history. Specifically, we localize these effects to higher energetic demands of the default mode network in females with a family history of SUD and lower energetic demands of attention networks in males with a family history of SUD. These results suggest a family history of SUD may increase reward saliency in males and decrease efficiency of top-down inhibitory control in females. This work could be used to inform personalized intervention strategies that may target differing cognitive mechanisms that predispose individuals to the development of SUD.
Keywords: substance use disorder, family history, adolescence, sex differences, neuroimaging, network control theory, brain activity dynamics
1. Introduction
Substance use disorder (SUD) frequently leads to escalating consequences including familial and financial instability, poor health outcomes, and – far too often – death (1). However, only a subset of those regularly exposed to substances of abuse go on to develop SUD (2), highlighting the critical need to identify predisposing factors. Consistent with the known genetic component of SUD (2; 3), youth with a family history of SUD (FH+) are at higher risk of SUD than youth without a family history of SUD (FH−) (4). This may be due to an exacerbated developmental profile of adolescence, characterized by poor inhibitory control and a highly sensitive reward system (4; 5; 6; 7). Additionally, parental history of SUD is itself considered an adverse childhood experience and thus poses both environmental and genetic risks (8).
Recent work suggests that structural and functional signatures of SUD partly reflect genetically and environmentally-conferred predisposition for SUD, rather than sole consequences of chronic substance use (9; 10; 11). Extensive work has revealed individuals with SUD have altered dopaminergic signaling (12), white matter integrity (13; 14), functional activity (14; 15; 16), gray matter volume (14; 17), and network dynamics (18; 19). These findings are largely localized to brain regions involved in reward and inhibitory control such as the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex (20; 21). Lagging slightly behind is a growing body of parallel findings in individuals at high risk of SUD, including altered dopaminergic signaling (22), white matter integrity (23; 24; 25), functional activity (6; 26), gray matter volume (27), and network dynamics (19; 28; 29) also primarily in regions responsible for behavioral inhibition and reward processing (5; 6). However, this remains an emerging area of research with few well-powered studies in youth with no previous substance exposure. As substance use impacts the brain’s structure and function, studying FH+ youth prior to substance exposure is crucial for disentangling the driving forces from the consequences of problematic substance use.
Sex assigned at birth (hereafter, “sex”) modulates SUD at every phase of the disorder (e.g., onset, manifestation, withdrawal, craving, relapse), likely due to a confluence of biological and sociocultural correlates of sex and gender (30; 31; 32; 33). Multiple lines of evidence converge on a model in which females are more susceptible to negative reinforcers (e.g., withdrawal, stress) and males are more susceptible to positive reinforcers (e.g., pleasurable effects of drugs) of SUD (32; 34; 35; 36; 37; 38). While men are more likely to use illicit drugs and develop SUD, women escalate drug use more quickly (i.e., “telescoping” effect) and report more withdrawal and craving (32). Moreover, females are more likely to have an internalizing pathway and males more likely to have an externalizing pathway to SUD (35; 36; 37; 38). Further, sex differences in the effect of family history on adolescents have been observed in behavioral (39; 40; 41; 42) and neurobiological phenotypes (43). Yet, few neuroimaging studies have considered whether sex modifies the effect of family history of SUD.
Herein, we aim to characterize sex-specific differences in the brain’s functional activation dynamics in substance-naïve youth from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study(44). Resting-state functional MRI (rsfMRI) collected in these adolescents allows tracking of brain activity as it alternates between several commonly recurring states (45; 46). Network control theory (NCT) can be applied to model how the brain dynamically moves through these states (47; 48). NCT considers the brain to be a network of regions interconnected via white matter tracts (i.e. structural connectivity) and models brain activity through a linear time-invariant system that assumes functional activation flows via a network diffusion process. The sum of activational inputs applied during network diffusion to steer or control a trajectory from an initial state to a desired target state represent transition energy (TE)(48). In the current context, TE inputs can be thought of as the internal demands required to drive the brain towards a given brain state (49; 50), and does not necessarily reflect metabolic demands, though recent work has found TE to be related to metabolic activity (51; 52). NCT-based analyses have found TE to be i) correlated with age (53), ii) increased in heavy alcohol use in young adults (18), iii) related to neurotransmitter receptor function (50), iv) altered in psychopathology (54), and v) change with pharmacological intervention (55). The relationship between TE and a familial predisposition to SUD in youth remains unknown. We hypothesize that FH+ youth compared to FH− youth will exhibit i) increased global TE, ii) network and regional energetic differences in areas involved in cognitive control and reward sensitivity, and, finally, iii) that these differences will be sex-specific.
2. Results
2.1. Sample Characteristics
To investigate sex-specific differences in the energetic landscapes of youth with and without a family history of SUD, we use diffusion and resting state functional MRI (rsfMRI) data from the ABCD Study’s baseline assessment of a large sample of substance-naïve youth (N= 1894 individuals, ages 10.02 ± 0.63, 54% female) (Table 1). See Section 5.1.1 for exclusion criteria. We classify individuals as FH+ if they have at least one parent or two grandparents with a history of SUD and FH− if no parents or grandparents had a history (56; 57; 58). Individuals with just one grandparent with a history of SUD are classified as FH+/− and are included only in analyses of continuous associations (i.e., family history density).
Table 1.
Full Cohort | FH+ | FH− | FH+/− | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(N = 1894) | (N = 451) | (N = 1169) | (N = 274) | |
| ||||
Biological Sex, N(%) | ||||
Male | 876 (46) | 197 (44) | 542 (46) | 137 (50) |
Female | 1018 (54) | 254 (56) | 627 (54) | 137 (50) |
| ||||
Age in months, Mean (SD) | ||||
Male | 120.50 (7.51) | 119.82 (7.67) | 120.61 (7.86) | 121.03 (7.59) |
Female | 120.01 (7.44) | 119.74 (7.26) | 120.08 (7.41) | 120.23 (7.93) |
| ||||
Framewise Displacement, Mean (SD) | ||||
Male | 0.12 (0.08) | 0.13 (0.09) | 0.12 (0.08) | 0.13 (0.10) |
Female | 0.11 (0.07) | 0.12 (0.08) | 0.11 (0.07) | 0.11 (0.07) |
| ||||
MRI Scanner Model, N(%) | ||||
GE Discovery MR750 | 528 (28) | 148 (33) | 311 (27) | 69 (25) |
Siemens Prisma | 691 (36) | 145 (32) | 448 (38) | 98 (36) |
Siemens Prisma Fit | 675 (36) | 158 (35) | 410 (35) | 107 (39) |
| ||||
Household Income, N(%) | ||||
< $50,000 | 421 (22) | 157 (35) | 220 (19) | 44 (16) |
$50,000 - $100,000 | 569 (30) | 146 (32) | 333 (28) | 90 (33) |
$100,000+ | 904 (48) | 148 (33) | 616 (53) | 140 (51) |
| ||||
Parent Education, N(%) | ||||
> High School | 49 (3) | 12 (3) | 35 (3) | 2 (1) |
High School/GED | 127 (7) | 53 (12) | 66 (6) | 8 (3) |
Some College | 284 (10) | 65 (14) | 91 (8) | 28 (10) |
Associates/Bachelor | 981 (52) | 233 (52) | 607 (52) | 141 (51) |
Post-graduate | 553 (29) | 88 (20) | 370 (32) | 95 (35) |
| ||||
Race/Ethnicity, N(%) | ||||
White | 1104 (63) | 260 (58) | 749 (64) | 185 (68) |
Black | 141 (7) | 49 (11) | 74 (6) | 18 (7) |
Hispanic/Latinx | 341 (18) | 87 (19) | 206 (18) | 48 (18) |
Asian | 38 (0.2) | 0 (0) | 37 (3) | 1 (0.4) |
Other | 180 (10) | 55 (12) | 103 (9) | 22 (8) |
| ||||
In Utero Substance, N(%) | ||||
Yes | 73 (4) | 45 (10) | 15 (1) | 13 (5) |
No | 1821 (96) | 406 (90) | 1154 (99) | 261 (95) |
| ||||
Parent Mental Health, N(%) | ||||
Yes | 995 (53) | 370 (82) | 470 (40) | 155 (57) |
No | 899 (47) | 81 (18) | 699 (60) | 119 (43) |
2.2. Network Control Theory Analysis
Following previous work (47; 55), we apply k-means clustering to regional rsfMRI time series data (86-region atlas) to identify k recurring patterns of brain activity, termed “brain states” (Figure 1, a–c). For each subject, we assign each individual frame to a brain state and calculate subject-specific brain state centroids (Figure 1, d–e). We then apply NCT using a group-average structural connectome (from a subset of individuals in this dataset) to calculate the global, network, and region-level TE required to complete brain state transitions (Figure 1, f–g). At the global, network and region-levels, we calculate pairwise and mean TE (Figure 1g). Pairwise TE represents the energy a given region, network or whole brain requires to complete transitions between each pair of brain states, whereas mean TE represents the overall energetic demands of a region, network or whole brain to move through its state space (i.e., across all pairs of states). Initial TE calculations result in “pairwise regional TE” values, i.e., 86 (number of regions) kxk matrices. For each matrix, we sum pairwise regional TE of all regions belonging to each of the seven Yeo networks (59) plus subcortical and cerebellar networks to yield “pairwise network TE” values, i.e., 9 (number of networks) kxk matrices. We derive a single “pairwise global TE” kxk matrix by summing across all 86 regions pairwise TEs for each transition kxk matrix. We also average all entries in pairwise TE matrices to derive mean TE values for each region, network and globally, resulting in: 86 mean regional TE values, 9 mean network TEs, and a single value for mean global TE. See section 5.3.2 for more details.
It is important to distinguish between the four identified brain states and the nine networks for which we calculate TE values, though they are related to a common network parcellation of the 7 Yeo networks plus subcortical and cerebellar networks. Brain states refer to the four clusters identified by k-means clustering, which are each assigned to one of the nine networks based on the network whose isolated high or low amplitude activity best explain each brain state. We refer to them as “DMN+”, “DMN−”, “VIS+”, and “VIS−”, using the network name and respective indicators representing activity above (+) or below (−) regional means in the the DMN and VIS networks. These names are purely descriptive and did not influence analyses. Network TE values, on the other hand, refer to the TE values calculated by summing across regional TE values for regions assigned to each network. We reference these using the nine network names alone, e.g. “DAT”, “VIS”, “DMN”, etc.
We run a series of analysis of covariances (ANCOVAs) to investigate the effect of family history of SUD and its interaction with sex on mean and pairwise TE at the global, network, and regional levels. We include sex, age, family history of SUD (FH+ vs FH−), race/ethnicity, household income, parental education, parental mental health issues, in utero exposure to substances, MRI scanner model, in-scanner motion (i.e., mean framewise displacement), and two interaction terms: sex and family history of SUD, and income and family history of SUD as independent variables in the model. To determine the direction of effect between groups, we performed post hoc t tests on TE values found to be significant in ANCOVA models.
2.3. Brain State Identification
We found an optimal k=4 brain states, as determined using the additional explained variance <1% cut-off (Figure S1). The identified brain states consist of two pairs of anti-correlated activity patterns (i.e. meta-states), the first dominated by high and low-amplitude activity in the default mode network (DMN+/−), and the second by high and and low-amplitude activity in the visual network (VIS+/−) (Figure 2), aligning with previous work (18; 47). For replication of our results using k=5, see S8.
2.4. Global Transition Energy
We first sought to determine whether a family history of SUD and its interaction with sex have an observable global effect on the brain’s overall energetic landscape. In an ANCOVA model for mean global TE, including the covariates described above, the effect of family history of SUD was not significant; however, the interaction of sex and family history of SUD was significant (Table 2). The lack of an effect of family history of SUD on global TE is partially due to its interaction with sex, wherein an opposite effect of family history is observed in females and males. To determine the direction of this sex-specific effect, we performed within-sex unpaired t tests comparing FH+ and FH− individuals. FH+ females exhibit a significantly higher mean global TE compared to FH− females (t = 2.14, p = 0.03, pFDR = 0.04), whereas FH+ males exhibit a non-significant trend toward lower mean global TE compared to FH− males (t = −1.03, p = 0.30) (Figure 3a). Further, when including FH+/− individuals (i.e., subjects with 1 grandparent with SUD), mean global TE and family history density of SUD were found to have a weak positive trend in females (rho = 0.07, p = 0.07), while males were not correlated (rho = −0.03, p= 0.41) (Figure 3b).
Table 2.
Variable | F-statistic | p |
---|---|---|
| ||
Family History of SUD (FH+ vs FH−) | 0.17 | 0.70 |
Age | 0.05 | 0.82 |
Sex | 27.67 | 1.6e-07** |
Family History of SUD : Sex | 4.61 | 0.032* |
Household Income | 0.52 | 0.60 |
Family History of SUD : Household Income | 4.34 | 0.013* |
Parental Education | 0.13 | 0.87 |
Race/Ethnicity | 0.42 | 0.80 |
In Utero Substance Exposure | 0.019 | 0.89 |
Parental History of Mental Health | 5.01 | 0.025* |
Framewise Displacement | 0.65 | 0.42 |
MRI Scanner Model | 21.93 | 4.03e-10** |
= p-value significant,
= p-value significant after FDR correction
Notably, the effect size of sex on mean global TE was the largest out of all variables examined, with females exhibiting higher values compared to males (post hoc t= 4.7, p < 0.0001, pFDR < 0.0001). Children of parents with mental health issues exhibited higher global TE than those without parental mental health history (t = 3.08, p = 0.002, pFDR = 0.004). MRI scanner model and the interaction of family history of SUD and household income were also significant. See Supplemental Information for within-income category, within-scanner type, and single-site analyses (Figs. S12, S13, S14).
ANCOVA models for entries in the pairwise global TE matrix revealed the effect of the interaction of sex and family history of SUD was strongest in transitions to and, particularly, persistence within the VIS+/− brain states (Figure 3c). Post hoc unpaired t-tests revealed this interaction effect was primarily driven by increased pairwise global TE in FH+ females versus FH− females in transitions to the VIS+/− brain states (Figure 3d). All transitions had higher TE in FH+ females and lower TE in FH+ males compared to sex-matched FH− counterparts.
2.5. Network Transition Energy
We next investigated which networks may be driving the observed effects in global TE. For each network, we ran ANCOVAs for mean network TE using the same covariates described above. Mean network TE did not have a significant effect of family history of SUD alone in any network (Figure 4a), but significant interaction effects of sex and family history of SUD were found in the default mode (DMN), dorsal attention (DAT), and ventral attention (VAT) networks, though the VAT did not survive FDR correction (Figure 4b). ANCOVA results for all networks can be found in the Supplementary Information. To determine the direction of interaction effects in the DMN, DAT and VAT, we performed unpaired t tests to compare FH+ and FH− individuals within each sex. We found the family history-by-sex effect in the DMN was driven by higher mean network TE in FH+ females than FH− females (t = 2.44, p = 0.015, pFDR = 0.030) (Figure 4c). Further, family history density of SUD in females had a weak trending positive correlation with mean DMN TE (rho = 0.053, r = 0.090) (Figure 4f). On the other hand, family history-by-sex effects in the DAT and VAT were driven by decreased mean network TE in FH+ males compared to FH− males (DAT: t = −3.54, pFDR = 0.00025; VAT: t = −2.62, p = 0.0091, pFDR = 0.027) (Figure 4,d–e). There were also significant, though weak, negative correlations between mean network TE and FHD in males in DAT (males: rho = −0.103, p = 0.0022, pFDR = 0.011) and VAT (males: rho = −0.072, p =0.033, pFDR = 0.10), though the VAT correlation did not survive FDR correction (Figure 4,g–h).
We next investigated which pairwise state transitions drive the observed effects of family history of SUD and sex on mean network TE. Within-sex unpaired t tests revealed FH+ females exhibit increased pairwise TE of the DMN across all transitions, particularly transitions to the VIS meta-state, whereas males exhibit no significant differences (Figure 4h). FH+ males have decreased pairwise TE of the DAT for almost all transitions compared to FH− males (Figure 4i) and decreased pairwise TE of the VAT in transitions to the DMN meta-state (Figure 4j), whereas females exhibit no significant differences.
2.6. Regional Transition Energy
We next sought to identify regions contributing to observed group-by-sex differences in global and network TE. We ran ANCOVAs on mean regional TE (dependent variable) using the same terms included in the models above. ANCOVA results for all regions can be found in the Supplementary Information. Prior to FDR correction (over the 86 regions), family history of SUD had a significant effect on the regional TE of the bilateral banks of the superior temporal sulcus (banks of STS), bilateral paracentral lobules, bilateral superior temporal gyri, and right amygdala (Figure 5a). The interaction between family history of SUD and sex was significant in the left isthmus cingulate, bilateral pars orbitalis, right supramarginal gyrus, bilateral superior parietal lobules, left pericalcarine, and right cerebellum, prior to FDR correction (Figure 5d).
Unpaired post hoc t tests were performed to compare FH+ and FH− individuals’ TE for regions found to have a significant effect of family history of SUD and within each sex for regions with significant group-by-sex effects. Across all seven regions analyzed, FH+ individuals (regardless of sex) had significantly increased regional TE compared to FH− individuals (Figure 5b). Further, all regions (except the right bank of STS) were found to have a significant, though weak, positive correlation with family history density of SUD (Figure 5c).
Within-sex t tests revealed significant increases in regional TE of the right pars orbitalis and right cerebellum were found in FH+ females compared to FH− females. Bilateral superior parietal lobules and the right supramarginal were significantly decreased in FH+ males compared to FH− males (Figure 5e). The direction and effect size of correlations between family history density and regional TE of these eight regions largely recapitulated the results of within-sex t-tests. The right supramarginal (pre-FDR correction) and bilateral superior parietal lobules (post-FDR correlation) demonstrated significant, yet mild, negative correlations with family history density in males (Figure 5f). Group differences in pairwise regional TE of these regions were consistent with mean regional TE results shown here (Figures S3 & S4).
2.7. Robustness Analyses
To ensure the robustness of our results, we replicated our main findings in several ways: (i) with k=5 clustering, (ii) with individual structural connectomes (SC) in a cortex-only parcellation, and (iii) in a cohort of sex, age, and in-scanner motion-matched subjects from an external dataset (National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence; NCANDA) (60). We also re-ran ANCOVA models by stratifying our cohort in various ways: (i) a single site with the largest number of subjects, (ii) each MRI scanner model, and (iii) each income category.
We found that our main results were consistent when varying the number of clusters (k=5) (Figure S8), and when using individual SC’s (Figure S11). Our independent analysis of the NCANDA dataset showed similar trends of FH+ > FH− females and FH+ < FH− males at the global and regional levels, further supporting the generalizability of our findings across different populations (Figure S9). Subjects within the largest site (site 16) displayed significant family history-by-sex interactions on global TE (FH+ > FH− females and FH+ < FH− males) and in DMN TE (FH+ > FH− females), but not in the DAT or VAT (Figure S12. Analyzing data by MRI scanner model, we observed that our main results were largely consistent in subjects scanned with Siemens models, while results from the GE scanner differed, likely due to demographic differences and lower data quality (Figure S13). See Table S2 for subject demographics by MRI model. Previous ABCD analyses found GE scanners have lower reproducibility in diffusion MRI metrics (61), higher distinguishability after site-normalization in rsfMRI data (62), and higher non-compliance to imaging protocols across modalities (63) compared to Siemens. Further, Siemens scanners implement real-time motion monitoring while GE scanners do not (64); motion correction is critical in this dataset of young adolescents. When stratifying the cohort by income level, our primary findings were consistent mainly in the highest income group (i.e., largest subgroup) (Figure S14). In the ABCD cohort, previous work has found higher income is related to greater curiosity and availability of substances, higher rates of early substance use, and greater odds of substance use in this cohort (65; 66; 67). Overall, these analyses confirm the robustness and reliability of our findings across various conditions and datasets, but indicate a possible influence of socioeconomic and demographic factors.
3. Discussion
Using a network control theory approach that models the brain as a networked, complex, dynamical system, we investigated differences in the brain activity dynamics of substance-naïve youth with and without a family history of SUD. Our findings indicate that a family history of SUD was associated with increased TE in certain brain regions, and that the effect of family history varies by an individuals’ sex at all levels of investigation, including global, network, and regional. Specifically, we found females with a family history of SUD tended to have increased TE compared to females without a family history, while the opposite was true for males. Interestingly, increases in FH+ females were driven by the default mode network, while decreases in FH+ males were driven by dorsal and ventral attention networks.
3.1. Family history of SUD increases regional transition energies in specific regions, independent of sex.
Family history of SUD was associated with increased TE in several regions: bilaterally in the banks of the STS, paracentral lobules and superior temporal gyri, and in the right amygdala. Functional and structural abnormalities of these regions have been observed in individuals with SUD or high risk of SUD (17; 58; 68; 69; 70; 71; 72). A meta-analysis by Pando-Naude et al. (17) found individuals with various types of SUDs consistently exhibit reductions in the volume of the superior temporal gyrus, a region shown to have increased engagement during craving in SUD (73; 74; 75). Studies in substance-naïve FH+ youth from the ABCD baseline assessment have reported reduced cortical thickness in left bank of STS and left paracentral lobule, as well as greater left paracentral activation during successful inhibition (58; 76). Further, family history of alcohol use disorder (AUD) is consistently associated with reduced volume of the amygdala (68; 69; 70), a key region in withdrawal and craving symptoms of SUD (77). We found a significant effect of family history in the right amygdala, but not the left, perhaps reflecting functional lateralization of emotional processing in the amygdalae (78). Taken together, our findings of increased TE in these regions may be the result of structural and/or functional alterations and reflect heightened risk of SUD.
3.2. Family history of SUD in females increases regional and default mode network transition energies.
FH+ females exhibit higher TE in the DMN, a network linked to self-generated thought, monitoring the internal state, and higher-order cognition (79; 80; 81; 82; 83). Atypical DMN activity has been previously observed in individuals with parental history of SUD (51) and with ongoing SUD (84; 85; 86; 87). Recent models suggest the DMN, positioned at the top of the brain’s network hierarchy (88), plays are role in optimizing prediction errors and exerts top-down inhibitory control over attentional and sensory systems (82; 83; 88). Given that increases in DMN TE of FH+ females are driven by transitions to the VIS meta-state, our findings suggest greater difficulty in carrying out “top-down” transitions theoretically important for inhibitory control and supports the idea that SUD results from disrupted top-down communication (89; 90). These findings provide a mechanistic explanation for FH+ females’ heightened propensity for rapid habit formation (32). Additionally, abnormal DMN activity, linked to withdrawal and craving in SUD (84; 91), may cause rumination on negative internal states, aligning with observations of females’ susceptibility to negative reinforcers of substance use (32; 92).
Regionally, FH+ females exhibit increased TE of the right pars orbitalis and right cerebellum. The pars orbitalis, a region of the DMN involved in inhibition of inappropriate motor responses and regulation of craving (93; 94; 95), has been linked to future substance use through lower gray matter volume in the ABCD cohort (96). Reductions in cerebellar gray matter volume are frequently observed in SUD (97; 98; 99; 100). Moreover, FH+ youth show reduced cerebellar activation during risky decision-making (101). While the majority of studies have not considered group-by-sex effects (93; 94; 95; 97; 98; 99), some studies note female-specific structural and functional alterations of these regions in SUD or a family history of SUD (76; 102; 103).
3.3. Family history of SUD in males decreases regional and attentional network transition energies.
FH+ males exhibit a distinct pattern of decreased TE in attentional networks, which appears to align with findings of heightened risk of SUD in youth with behavioral attention deficits (104), atypical functional activity in the DAT and VAT of individuals with SUD (19; 105; 106; 107), and a sustained attention network that predicts future substance use (29). Regionally, FH+ males exhibit decreased TE bilaterally in the superior parietal lobules and in the right supramarginal gyrus, which are engaged in goal-directed attention (108) and cognition and language (109), respectively. Altered structural and functional neurophenotypes have been observed in youth from the ABCD cohort with a parental history of AUD (58; 76), where family history-by-sex effects were either not found (76) or not considered (58). Further, a study in substance-naïve youth identified decreased cortical thickness of the left supramarginal gyrus as the top predictor and being male as second in predicting future alcohol use (110). Our findings suggest reduced energetic demand in attention networks and regions prior to substance use may lead an individual to more readily attend to the rewarding effects of substances once exposed. This is particularly evident in FH+ males’ decreased pairwise TE of the VAT, a network responsible for stimulus-driven allocation of attention, being driven by transitions to the DMN+/− brain states, representing “bottom-up” state transitions.
3.4. Female and male FH+ youth exhibit distinct halves of a common neurodevelopmental model of SUD.
How differences in the resting brain’s transition energies relate to SUD predisposition remains unclear. One possibility is that higher or lower energetic demand of a network/region makes it less or more dominant, respectively, in driving brain state transitions during action and processing of the environment. This theory, based on the assumption of energy consumption minimization (52; 111), coupled with our current findings, suggests a reduced ability of FH+ females to reach states that support impulse inhibition by the DMN, and an increased propensity of FH+ males to occupy states driven by reward saliency via the DAT/VAT. Put another way, FH+ females have a harder time “stepping on the brakes”, whereas FH+ males more readily “step on the gas”. This aligns with a common disease model of addictive behaviors, which suggests a strengthened impulsive/reactive system (bottom-up) and a diminished reflective/deliberative system (top-down) (21). While acute and chronic effects of drugs may have both effects, our findings suggest FH+ females and males premorbidly exhibit distinct halves of this model. Additionally, abnormality in a reflective/deliberative system in females and an impulsive/reactive system in males aligns with the sexes’ respective higher probabilities for internalizing and externalizing pathways to SUD (35; 36; 37; 38).
Baseline sex differences in the functional dynamics of these networks may explain sex-modulated manifestation of familial predisposition to SUD (112; 113). Further, activity of the DMN and DAT are anti-correlated, meaning when one is active, the other is suppressed (88; 114; 115), a phenomenon evident from early infancy and increasing with maturity (116; 117). Given the inverse functional relationship of these networks and the opposite direction of effect in FH+ females and males (increased DMN TE and decreased DAT TE, respectively), predisposition to SUD in females and males may be underpinned by distinct mechanisms with similar behavioral phenotypes.
3.5. SUD vulnerability manifests in functional dynamics of the cortex prior to substance use.
Notably, the majority of regions and networks implicated here are cortical, with the exception of the cerebellum and amygdala. Our findings contrast a body of work implicating the subcortical dopaminergic system in SUD predisposition (6; 118; 119; 120; 121). However, these studies largely utilized older subjects who had likely initiated substance use (6; 118; 121). Differences in subcortical regions may emerge in late adolescence/early adulthood through developmental processes or in response to the onset of substance use (118; 122). Moreover, our findings echo that of Pando-Naude et al. (17) who propose addiction is characterized by both cortical and subcortical pathologies, whereas occasional use (risk factor for future dependence) is characterized by cortical pathology only. Recent findings applying NCT in young adults with current heavy alcohol use identified increased global TE and, importantly, increased TE in transitions from subcortical to higher-order states (18). Taken together, our results posit cortical abnormalities are present in FH+ youth prior to substance initiation, with perhaps further cortical and subcortical pathologies resulting from active and/or chronic substance use. Furthermore, our findings pinpoint functional alterations as more prominently altered in this cohort of individuals. Using individual structural connectomes, we found the majority of our main results are replicated (S11) and global TE metrics were highly correlated as those from a group-average SC. These findings align with recent observations of a lack of differences in white matter integrity between FH+ and FH− drug-naive adolescents, suggesting familial predisposition to SUD is manifested primarily in functional dynamics (26).
3.6. Limitations and Future Directions
The age range of cohort our represents a period of major neurodevelopmental changes, which vary by sex (53; 104; 123; 124; 125; 126). Thus, our findings need to be validated over the entire window of development through leveraging the longitudinal nature of the ABCD and NCANDA studies. Notably, we did find our mean global TE results replicated in the NCANDA dataset in individuals aged 12–22 years old. Further, exclusion of subjects with in-scanner motion may have implications on reported findings given that head motion is a stable and heritable phenotype in adults (127; 128) and a phenotypic marker of impulsivity (129). Increased head movement (even within acceptable limits) was shown to be an important variable in a machine learning study predicting future alcohol use (5). Therefore, our cohort may not represent the full range of extremity of SUD predisposition. Additionally, our analyses did not examine differences by gender identity due to the limited gender diversity in the ABCD baseline assessment. As the cohort matures, examining the effect of sex, gender and sexual orientation on SUD predisposition will be critical, particularly due to higher rates of SUD in lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and queer (LGBTQ+) youth (130; 131; 132).
4. Conclusion
Our study reveals sex-specific effects of a family history of SUD with distinct network alterations in male and female youths with a family history: FH+ males exhibit decreased transition energy in the attentional networks and FH+ females show heightened transition energy in the DMN. We posit that this may translate to FH+ males more readily “stepping on the gas” and FH+ females having a harder time “stepping on the brakes” in terms of substance use patterns. These findings suggest that mechanisms underlying SUD predisposition are complex and influenced by sex-specific neurodevelopmental trajectories that may ultimately result in a common constellation of behavioral phenotypes. Recognizing these mechanistic differences is crucial for understanding the onset of SUD and developing targeted intervention strategies.
5. Methods
5.1. Sample Characteristics
The Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) study is longitudinally tracking the brain development and health of a nationally representative sample of children aged 9 to 11 years old (at the time of enrollment) from 21 centers across the United States (https://abcdstudy.org). Parents’ full written informed consent and all children’s assent were obtained by each center. Research protocols were approved by the institutional review board of the University of California, San Diego (no. 160091), and the institutional review boards of the 21 data collection sites (133). The current study utilized neuroimaging data from the 2.0.1 release and non-imaging instruments from the baseline assessment updated to ABCD Data Release 5.1.
5.1.1. Exclusions
From the original ABCD cohort of N=11,868, we excluded youth who: i) did not survive strict MRI quality control and/or exclusion criteria as established by Chen et al. (134) (N = 9506), ii) were scanned on Philips scanners (N = 2), iii) did not meet criteria for group definitions of FH+ or FH− (see Section 5.1.2; N = 109), iv) had previously used substances (see Section 5.1.3; N = 54), v) were adopted (N = 32), vi) had a mismatch between reported sex and their sex determined by salivary samples (N = 20), vii) had missing household income information (see Section 5.1.4, N = 119), or viii) had missing information on parental mental health issues (5.1.2, N = 79). Further, 53 subjects were excluded (after k-means clustering and TE calculations) due to outlier mean global TE values (see Section 5.3.3). See Table S1 for excluded subject demographics.
5.1.2. Family History of Substance Use Disorder and Mental Illness
We utilized the baseline assessment of the Family History Module Screener (FHAM-S) (135; 136) in which parents reported psychopathology and substance use problems within the first and second degree biological family members of each child. Although family history of alcohol and drug problems were obtained separately, in analyses we considered cross-substance family history of substance use (FH+) as having family members with a history of either alcohol or drug use problems. This choice is supported by recent evidence for the heritability of a general vulnerability factor for cross-substance addiction (3) and neuroimaging findings of abnormalities across SUDs to a common brain network that is similar across imaging modalities and substances (137). In the FHAM-S, problems with drugs or alcohol may include marital, work, school, arrests or driving under influence, health, rehabilitation programs, heavy use, or social issues.
Following previous work (56; 57; 58), we categorize individuals as FH+ if they have at least one parent or a minimum of two grandparents with a history of SUD. Individuals with no parents or grandparents with a history of SUD are classified as family history negative (FH−). Individuals with just one grandparent with a history of SUD are classified as FH+/− and are only included in analyses that rely on continuous associations (i.e., family history density) as these individuals likely have minimal genetic load of previous generations (56), yet could not be classified as FH−.
In addition to FH+ or FH− group assignments, a more granular measure of family history density (FHD) was calculated based on the sum of positive reports of problems from biological parents (+1) and biological grandparents (+0.5). FHD scores could thus range from 0 to 4, with a score of 0 indicating no family history of SUD and a score or 4 indicating SUD in both parents and all four grandparents (58). Participants with only one grandparent with a history of SUD, who were excluded from categorical analyses (FH+ vs FH−), were included in FHD analyses. As in previous work (57; 58), other second-degree relatives (i.e., aunts and uncles) were not considered due to missingness and less accurate account of distal relatives.
Given the high comorbidity of substance use problems and mental illness, we also included a binary variable for parental history of mental illness, other than SUD, as a variable in our analyses. The FHAM-S evaluates parental history of mental health issues including: suicide, depression, mania, anti-social personality, schizophrenia, and other emotional/mental heath issues. If one or more parents of a subject were reported as experiencing one or more of these mental health issues, a subject met criteria for having a parental history of mental health issues.
5.1.3. Exposure to Substances
Childhood substance use.
To isolate the effects of family history of SUD from substance use itself, we excluded youth who had initiated substance use according to parents or children themselves (N = 54). Subjects were excluded if they self-reported lifetime use of more than a sip of alcohol, more than a puff of a cigarette/e-cigarettes, or any use of nicotine products, cannabis products, synthetic cannabinoids, cocaine, cathinones, methamphetamine, ecstasy/MDMA, ketamine, gamma-hydroxybutyrate, heroin, psilocybin, salvia, other hallucinogens, anabolic steroids, inhalants, or prescription misuse of stimulants, sedatives, opioid pain relievers or over-the-counter cough/cold medicine (138). Parents were also asked if their child had exhibited drinking problems (consumed three or more drinks a day, consumed two drinks in the last 12 months) or used drugs (cocaine, marijuana, solvents, stimulants, tobacco, opioids, hallucinogens, sedatives, or other). If parents endorsed any of these, children were excluded from analyses.
In utero substance exposure.
Given the known impact of maternal substance use during pregnancy on brain development (139; 140), we included in utero substance exposure as a binary variable in our ANCOVA models. Prenatal substance use exposure was reported based on caregiver recall in the ABCD Developmental History Questionnaire (136). Consistent with previous work using dichotomous analyses (141), we considered exposure as either present or absent based on if mothers reported maternal use of over 7 drinks of alcohol per week (142), any tobacco, marijuana, cocaine, opiates, or other drugs after pregnancy was recognized.
5.1.4. Household Income and Parental Education
We included two measures typically associated with socio-economic status (SES): household income (HI) and parental education (PE). Both of these variables are considered essential for mental health research by the National Institute of Mental Health (143) and have a known relation to substance use disorders and their consequences (144; 145). In the ABCD study, both HI and PE were obtained in parent-reported demographic questionnaires (136). In order to simplify our statistical models without losing nuance, we followed previous work and re-coded the PE and HI variables into fewer groups (146; 147). The PE survey responses, for which there were originally 21 choices, was re-coded into 5 levels: less than high school, high school graduate/GED, some college, associate’s/bachelor’s degree, and postgraduate degree. We used the highest of the two parents PE. For HI, the original nine response options were re-coded into three levels: (1) less than $50,000, (2) more than $50,000 and less than $100,000, and (3) over $100,000. Individuals missing PE or HI data were excluded from analyses.
5.1.5. Sex Assigned at Birth
Our analysis of sex differences focuses on sex assigned at birth, which we refer to as “sex”. We did not control for or assess gender-based differences in brain dynamics in the present study. We excluded individuals whose sex assigned at birth did not match their sex as determined in a salivary sample as this could imply clerical errors in reporting sex assigned at birth. However, we did not control for or assess gender-based differences in the present study. While “male” and “female” brain features exist in both sexes, some features may be more prevalent in one sex than the other (123; 148; 149) and, further, recent work has suggested sex and gender may be mapped on to different brain networks (150). In our current study we have found sex to be a useful, easily obtained, and important biomarker as we have identified meaningful variance within and between each sex.
5.2. Neuroimaging Data
5.2.1. Parcellation
In our main results, we present analyses in which the rsfMRI timeseries and group-average structrual connectome (SC) were parcellated using an 86-region atlas derived from FreeSurfer (FS86) combining the 68 region Desikan-Killiany (DK68) gyral atlas (34 cortical regions per hemisphere) with 16 subcortical structures (8 per hemisphere, excluding brainstem) and 2 cerebellar structures (1 per hemisphere) to render a whole brain anatomically-defined parcellation for each subject (151; 152). Each cortical region was assigned to one of seven networks from the functionally-defined Yeo seven-network parcellation (59). Subcortical regions were assigned to a subcortical network and cerebellar regions to a cerebellar network. The rsfMRI time-series and individual structural connectome (SC) were also extracted in the DK68 cortical atlas. See Section 5.2.3 for more information on atlas choice.
5.2.2. Resting-state Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (rs-fMRI)
We analyzed rsfMRI collected during the baseline assessment of the multi-site ABCD study. Minimally processed rsfMRI data underwent pre-processing and quality control as described in (134; 153) and summarized here. Subjects whose scans were obtained on Philips scanners were excluded due to incorrect post-processing per recommendations of ABCD Consortium. Scans underwent removal of initial frames and alignment with the T1 images using boundary-based registration (BBR). Volumes (as well as one volume before and two volumes after) were marked as outliers if they had framewise displacement (FD) > 0.3 mm or DVARS > 50. Uncensored segments of data containing fewer than five contiguous volumes were also censored. Functional runs were excluded if over half of the volumes were censored or BBR costs were >0.6. Nuisance covariates (global signal, motion correction parameters, average ventricular signal, average white matter signal, and their temporal derivatives) were regressed out of the fMRI time-series using non-censored volumes to estimate regression coefficients. Brain scans were band-pass filtered (0.009 ≤ f ≤ 0.08 Hz), projected onto FreeSurfer fsaverage6 surface space, and smoothed using a 6-mm full-width half-maximum kernel. Upon receiving this pre-processed data, we subsequently removed censored volumes from the time-series and normalized the BOLD time series by the mean gray matter BOLD signal (pre-spatial and temporal filtering). After censoring outlier frames, the average number of remaining rsfMRI frames (across all scans) was 1155.88±291.12 (mean ± std).
Given the known scanner effects in the rs-fMRI data of the ABCD dataset (62), we included the scanner model in all ANCOVA models as a covariate of no interest. As Philips scanners were excluded, our dataset consisted of three scanner model types: Prisma (Siemens), Prisma Fit (Siemens), and Discovery MR750 (GE).
5.2.3. Structural Connectomes
In the ABCD study, diffusion MRI was collected at the baseline assessment (154). Pre-processed dMRI was further processed by deterministic tractography with SIFT2 global streamline weighting and regional volume normalization. Structural connectomes (SC) were extracted in the FS86 (cortical and subcortical) for 149 subjects and DK68 (cortical only) for 2080 subjects. The SC matrices are symmetric with the diagonal (self-connections) set equal to zero. Global TE values using a group-average SC and individual SC for N subjects were found to be highly correlated (rho = 0.99, p < 0.0001). Given this high correlation and the known relevance of subcortical regions in SUD literature (6; 118; 119; 120; 121), we chose to utilize the group-average SC for the FS86 atlas. This choice is further supported by previous NCT work (50) and recent observations of a lack of differences in white matter integrity between FH+ and FH− drug-naive adolescents, suggesting familial predisposition is manifested primarily in functional dynamics (26). Main results were replicated in the DK68 parcellation using individual, cortex-only SCs (Figure S11).
5.2.4. Framewise Displacement
Given the concern for head motion during neuroimaging of a pediatric cohort, we controlled for subjects’ tendency to move their head during rest in the scanner by calculating the average framewise displacement (FD) across all timepoints of all scans per subject (censored and uncensored). We included mean FD as a covariate of no interest in all ANCOVA models.
5.3. Network Control Theory Analyses
5.3.1. Extraction of brain states
Following Cornblath et al. (47), all subjects’ fMRI time series were concatenated in time and k-means clustering was applied to identify clusters of brain activation patterns, or brain states. Pearson correlation was used as the distance metric and clustering was repeated 10 times with random initializations before choosing the solution with the best separation of the data. To further assess the stability of clustering and ensure our partitions were reliable, we independently repeated this process 10 times and compared the adjusted mutual information (AMI) between each of the 10 resulting partitions. The partition which shared the greatest total AMI with all other partitions was selected for further analysis. In general, we found that the mutual information shared between partitions was very high (>0.99), suggesting consistent clustering across independent runs. We chose the number of clusters textk via the elbow criterion, i.e. by plotting the gain in explained explained across clusterings for k=2 through k=14 and identifying the “elbow” of the plot, which was at k=4 (Figure S1). In addition, k=4 fell below 1% of variance explained by clustering, a threshold used previously for determining k (47; 55). Thus, we chose k=4 for its straightforward and symmetric interpretation and replicated main results for k=5 in Supplementary Information. For interpretability, each cluster centroid was named via one of nine a priori defined canonical resting-state networks (RSN) (59) plus subcortical and cerebellar networks, by the cosine similarity between the centroid and binary representations of each RSN. Because the mean signal from each scan’s BOLD time series was removed during bandpass filtering, positive values in the centroid reflect activation above the mean (high-amplitude) and negative values reflect activation below the mean (low-amplitude). Subject-specific brain state centroids were calculated for each individual across all included timepoints of their rsfMRI scans.
5.3.2. Transition Energy Calculations
Following similar procedures as described elsewhere (47; 55), we summarize briefly here. We employ a linear time-invariant model:
where is a representative (group average) NxN structural connectivity matrix obtained as described above using deterministic tractography from a subset of ABCD subjects (see Section 3.2.3). is normalized by its maximum eigenvalue plus 1 and subtracted by the identity matrix to create a continuous system. is a vector of length N containing the regional activation at time t. is an NxN matrix of control points, in this case,the identity matrix is used for uniform control. is the external input into the system. N is the number of regions in our parcellation, where N = 86 for our main results. We selected a time-horizon of T = 1.501 as it had the strongest negative correlation with state-pair transition probability measures (across range T = 0.001 to 10), or the likelihood of a transition to occur, as done in previous studies (47; 55). To compute the minimum control energy required to drive the system from an initial brain state to a target state over T, we compute an invertible controllability Gramian for controlling the network A from N nodes.
Using the above methodology to define brain states (see Section 3.3.1), we calculate the regional transition energy for a given transition between each pair of brain states and persistence within each state (pairwise regional TE). To calculate pairwise regional TE, we integrate u(t) over the time-horizon to yield the total amount of input signals into each region necessary to complete each transition, resulting in a kxk matrix for each region (where k= 4 in our main results). We calculate the global transition energy (pairwise global TE), a kxk matrix, by summing all pairwise regional TE for the transition. We calculate the transition energy required of a network (pairwise network TE) by summing all pairwise regional TE for those regions assigned to a network, resulting in a kxk matrix for each network. To calculate mean global TE (1 constant), mean network TE (vector of length equal to the number of networks) and mean regional TE (vector of length equal to number of regions), we average across all pairwise TEs at each respective level of analysis.
5.3.3. Defining Outlier Subjects
After calculating mean global TE values, we excluded N = 53 individuals from further analyses due to a mean global TE more than 3 scaled median absolute deviations (MAD) from the sample median (155). While within acceptable limits, these subjects were found to have significantly higher mean framewise displacement compared to non-outlier subjects (t = 3.58, p = 3.5e-04). Sample characteristics of all excluded subjects can be found in Supplementary Table S1. Importantly, the reported results are largely similar when including outlier subjects.
5.4. Statistics
Between-group comparisons were made using ANCOVA models including age, sex assigned at birth (“sex”), family history of SUD, race/ethnicity, parental education, household income, in utero exposure to substances, parental mental health issues, MRI scanner model, in-scanner motion (i.e., mean framewise displacement), and two interaction terms: family history of SUD:sex, and family history of SUD:household income. Full ANCOVA results can be found in the Supplementary Information. For metrics (global, network and regional TEs) found to be significant in ANCOVA models, unpaired t tests were performed post hoc between FH+ and FH− subjects across all subjects (significant for family history of SUD) and within each sex separately (significant for interaction between family history of SUD and sex) to determine the direction of effect. Correlations between average TE and family history density were calculated using Spearman’s rank correlations. P-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg False Discovery Rate (q=0.05) procedure where indicated (pFDR) (156).
Supplementary Material
6. Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Thomas Yeo’s lab, specifically Leon Qi Rong Ooi, for providing the processed MRI data.
LS was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number T32 285 DA03980. KMP was supported by R01 DA057567 and U24 AA021697. CT was supported with a Career Transition Award (TA-2204-39428) and a postdoctoral fellowship (FG-2008-36976) from the National Multiple Sclerosis Society. AK was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number RF1 MH123232.
7. Data Availability
Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. Processed neuroimaging data by (153) used in this study were uploaded by the original authors to the NDA. Researchers with access to the ABCD data will be able to download the data: https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=1368. The ABCD imaging data used in this report came from https://doi.org/10.15154/1504041 and non-imaging data was from the 5.1 release (http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/z563-zd24). These data were used in the analyses described in https://doi.org/10.15154/c9z7-ng36. The ABCD data are publicly available via the NIMH Data Archive (NDA).
Collection and distribution of the NCANDA data were supported by NIH funding AA021681, AA021690, AA021691, AA021692, AA021695, AA021696, AA021697. Researchers with access to the NCANDA data will be able to download the data via https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=4513.
References
- 1.Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2021 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. PEP22-07-01-005, NSDUH Series H-57). Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2022). [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nestler E. J. & Lüscher C. The Molecular Basis of Drug Addiction: Linking Epigenetic to Synaptic and Circuit Mechanisms. Neuron 102, 48–59, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.016 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hatoum A. S. et al. Multivariate genome-wide association meta-analysis of over 1 million subjects identifies loci underlying multiple substance use disorders. Nat. mental health 1, 210–223, DOI: 10.1038/s44220-023-00034-y (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Whitesell M., Bachand A., Peel J. & Brown M. Familial, Social, and Individual Factors Contributing to Risk for Adolescent Substance Use. J. Addict. 2013, e579310, DOI: 10.1155/2013/579310 (2013). Publisher: Hindawi. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Squeglia L. M. & Cservenka A. Adolescence and Drug Use Vulnerability: Findings from Neuroimaging. Curr. opinion behavioral sciences 13, 164–170, DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.12.005 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tervo-Clemmens B., Quach A., Calabro F. J., Foran W. & Luna B. Meta-analysis and review of functional neuroimaging differences underlying adolescent vulnerability to substance use. NeuroImage 209, 116476, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116476 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ernst M., Pine D. S. & Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol. Medicine 36, 299–312, DOI: 10.1017/S0033291705005891 (2006). Publisher: Cambridge University Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cronholm P. F. et al. Adverse Childhood Experiences: Expanding the Concept of Adversity. Am. J. Prev. Medicine 49, 354–361, DOI: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.02.001 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hatoum A. S. et al. Polygenic risk scores for alcohol involvement relate to brain structure in substance-naïve children: Results from the ABCD study. Genes, Brain Behav. 20, e12756, DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12756 (2021). _eprint: 10.1111/gbb.12756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Baranger D. A. et al. Convergent Evidence for Predispositional Effects of Brain Gray Matter Volume on Alcohol Consumption. Biol. Psychiatry 87, 645–655, DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.08.029 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Luciana M. Risks versus consequences of adolescent and young adult substance use: A focus on executive control. Curr. addiction reports 7, 453–463, DOI: 10.1007/s40429-020-00341-9 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Volkow N. D., Fowler J. S. & Wang G.-J. Role of dopamine in drug reinforcement and addiction in humans: results from imaging studies. Behav. Pharmacol. 13, 355 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tondo L. P. et al. White matter deficits in cocaine use disorder: convergent evidence from in vivo diffusion tensor imaging and ex vivo proteomic analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 252, DOI: 10.1038/s41398-021-01367-x (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Moeller S. J. & Paulus M. P. Toward biomarkers of the addicted human brain: Using neuroimaging to predict relapse and sustained abstinence in substance use disorder. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biol. Psychiatry 80, 143–154, DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.03.003 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tolomeo S. & Yu R. Brain network dysfunctions in addiction: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Transl. Psychiatry 12, 41, DOI: 10.1038/s41398-022-01792-6 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Yip S. W., Kiluk B. & Scheinost D. Toward addiction prediction: An overview of cross-validated predictive modeling findings and considerations for future neuroimaging research. Biol. psychiatry. Cogn. neuroscience neuroimaging 5, 748–758, DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.11.001 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pando-Naude V. et al. Gray and white matter morphology in substance use disorders: a neuroimaging systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 1–18, DOI: 10.1038/s41398-020-01128-2 (2021). Number: 1 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Singleton S. P. et al. Altered Structural Connectivity and Functional Brain Dynamics in Individuals With Heavy Alcohol Use Elucidated via Network Control Theory. Biol. Psychiatry: Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.05.006 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zilverstand A., Huang A. S., Alia-Klein N. & Goldstein R. Z. Neuroimaging Impaired Response Inhibition and Salience Attribution in Human Drug Addiction. A systematic review. Neuron 98, 886–903, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Volkow N. D., Michaelides M. & Baler R. The Neuroscience of Drug Reward and Addiction. Physiol. Rev. 99, 2115–2140, DOI: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2018 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lüscher C., Robbins T. W. & Everitt B. J. The transition to compulsion in addiction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 247–263, DOI: 10.1038/s41583-020-0289-z (2020). Number: 5 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dalley J. W. et al. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Sci. (New York, N.Y.) 315, 1267–1270, DOI: 10.1126/science.1137073 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Acheson A. et al. Assessment of whole brain white matter integrity in youths and young adults with a family history of substance-use disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5401–5413, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22559 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Herting M. M., Fair D. & Nagel B. J. Altered fronto-cerebellar connectivity in alcohol-naïve youth with a family history of alcoholism. NeuroImage 54, 2582–2589, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.030 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Waters A. B., Sawyer K. S. & Gansler D. A. White matter connectometry among individuals with self-reported family history of drug and alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Dependence 206, 107710, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.107710 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.He X. et al. White matter integrity and functional connectivity in adolescents with a parental history of substance use disorder. Neuroimage Reports 1, 100037, DOI: 10.1016/j.ynirp.2021.100037 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Weiland B. J. et al. Substance abuse risk in emerging adults associated with smaller frontal gray matter volumes and higher externalizing behaviors. Drug alcohol dependence 137, 68–75, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.01.005 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ersche K. D. et al. Brain networks underlying vulnerability and resilience to drug addiction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. United States Am. 117, 15253–15261, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2002509117 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Weng Y. et al. A robust brain network for sustained attention from adolescence to adulthood that predicts later substance use. bioRxiv 2024.04.03.587900, DOI: 10.1101/2024.04.03.587900 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Becker J. B. & Chartoff E. Sex differences in neural mechanisms mediating reward and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 166–183, DOI: 10.1038/s41386-018-0125-6 (2019). Number: 1 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Becker J. B., McClellan M. L. & Reed B. G. SEX DIFFERENCES, GENDER AND ADDICTION. J. neuroscience research 95, 136–147, DOI: 10.1002/jnr.23963 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Westermeyer J. & Boedicker A. E. Course, Severity, and Treatment of Substance Abuse Among Women Versus Men. The Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abus. 26, 523–535, DOI: 10.1081/ADA-100101893 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McHugh R. K., Votaw V. R., Sugarman D. E. & Greenfield S. F. Sex and Gender Differences in Substance Use Disorders. Clin. psychology review 66, 12–23, DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.10.012 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Potenza M. N. et al. Neural Correlates of Stress-Induced and Cue-Induced Drug Craving: Influences of Sex and Cocaine Dependence. The Am. journal psychiatry 169, 406–414, DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020289 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Heitzeg M. M., Hardee J. E. & Beltz A. M. Sex differences in the developmental neuroscience of adolescent substance use risk. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 23, 21–26, DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.020 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hussong A. M., Jones D. J., Stein G. L., Baucom D. H. & Boeding S. An Internalizing Pathway to Alcohol and Substance Use Disorders. Psychol. addictive behaviors : journal Soc. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 25, 390–404, DOI: 10.1037/a0024519 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nolen-Hoeksema S., Grayson C. & Larson J. Explaining the Gender Difference in Depressive Symptoms. . [DOI] [PubMed]
- 38.Hicks B. M. et al. Gender Differences and Developmental Change in Externalizing Disorders From Late Adolescence to Early Adulthood: A Longitudinal Twin Study. J. abnormal psychology 116, 433–447, DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.433 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Weafer J., De Arcangelis J. & de Wit H. Sex Differences in Behavioral Impulsivity in At-Risk and Non-Risk Drinkers. Front. Psychiatry 6, 72, DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00072 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hasson R. & Fine J. G. Gender differences among children with ADHD on continuous performance tests: a meta-analytic review. J. Atten. Disord. 16, 190–198, DOI: 10.1177/1087054711427398 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Riley A. L., Hempel B. J. & Clasen M. M. Sex as a biological variable: Drug use and abuse. Physiol. & Behav. 187, 79–96, DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.10.005 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cycowicz Y. M. et al. Sex differences in stress responses among underrepresented minority adolescents at risk for substance use disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 191, 42–48, DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.07.003 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cornish J. L. & Prasad A. A. Sex Differences in Substance Use Disorders: A Neurobiological Perspective. Front. Glob. Women’s Heal. 2, DOI: 10.3389/fgwh.2021.778514 (2021). Publisher: Frontiers. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Volkow N. D. et al. The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 4–7, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.002 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cabral J., Kringelbach M. L. & Deco G. Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Prog. Neurobiol. 114, 102–131, DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.12.005 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tambini A., Ketz N. & Davachi L. Enhanced Brain Correlations during Rest Are Related to Memory for Recent Experiences. Neuron 65, 280–290, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cornblath E. et al. Temporal sequences of brain activity at rest are constrained by white matter structure and modulated by cognitive demands. Commun. Biol. 3, 261, DOI: 10.1038/s42003-020-0961-x (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gu S. et al. Controllability of structural brain networks. Nat. Commun. 6, 8414, DOI: 10.1038/ncomms9414 (2015). Number: 1 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Karrer T. M. et al. A practical guide to methodological considerations in the controllability of structural brain networks. J. Neural Eng. 17, 026031, DOI: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab6e8b (2020). Publisher: IOP Publishing. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Braun U. et al. Brain network dynamics during working memory are modulated by dopamine and diminished in schizophrenia. Nat. Commun. 12, 3478, DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-23694-9 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.He X.-S. et al. Uncovering the biological basis of control energy: Structural and metabolic correlates of energy inefficiency in temporal lobe epilepsy. Sci. advances 8, eabn2293, DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abn2293 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ceballos E. G. et al. The control costs of human brain dynamics, DOI: 10.1101/2024.01.24.577068 (2024). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Parkes L. et al. Asymmetric signaling across the hierarchy of cytoarchitecture within the human connectome. Sci. Adv. 8, eadd2185, DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.add2185 (2022). Publisher: American Association for the Advancement of Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zöller D. et al. Structural control energy of resting-state functional brain states reveals less cost-effective brain dynamics in psychosis vulnerability. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 2181–2200, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25358 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Singleton S. P. et al. Receptor-informed network control theory links LSD and psilocybin to a flattening of the brain’s control energy landscape. Nat. Commun. 13, 5812, DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-33578-1 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Gonçalves P. D. et al. Associations Between Family History of Alcohol and/or Substance Use Problems and Frontal Cortical Development From 9 to 13 Years of Age: A Longitudinal Analysis of the ABCD Study. Biol. Psychiatry Glob. Open Sci. 4, 100284, DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsgos.2023.100284 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cservenka A. Neurobiological phenotypes associated with a family history of alcoholism. Drug Alcohol Dependence 158, 8–21, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.021 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lees B. et al. Problems experienced by children from families with histories of substance misuse: An ABCD Study®. Drug alcohol dependence 218, 108403, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108403 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Thomas Yeo B. T. et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165, DOI: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 (2011). Publisher: American Physiological Society. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Brown S. A. et al. The National Consortium on Alcohol and NeuroDevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA): A Multisite Study of Adolescent Development and Substance Use. J. Stud. on Alcohol Drugs 76, 895–908, DOI: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.895 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Pan Y. et al. A site-wise reliability analysis of the ABCD diffusion fractional anisotropy and cortical thickness: impact of scanner platforms, DOI: 10.1101/2024.02.21.581460 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Nielson D. et al. Detecting and harmonizing scanner differences in the ABCD study - annual release 1.0 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 63.Sinha H. & Raamana P. R. Solving the Pervasive Problem of Protocol Non-Compliance in MRI using an Open-Source tool mrQA. Neuroinformatics DOI: 10.1007/s12021-024-09668-4 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hagler D. J. et al. Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. NeuroImage 202, 116091, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116091 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lisdahl K. M. et al. Substance use patterns in 9–10 year olds: Baseline findings from the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Drug Alcohol Dependence 227, 108946, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.108946 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Martz M. E. et al. Individual-, peer-, and parent-level substance use-related factors among 9- and 10-year-olds from the ABCD Study: Prevalence rates and sociodemographic differences. Drug Alcohol Dependence Reports 3, 100037, DOI: 10.1016/j.dadr.2022.100037 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Sullivan R. M. et al. Substance use patterns in 9 to 13-year-olds: Longitudinal findings from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Drug Alcohol Dependence Reports 5, 100120, DOI: 10.1016/j.dadr.2022.100120 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Comstock S. M., Vaidya J. G. & Niciu M. J. Neurophysiological Correlates and Differential Drug Response in Subjects With a Family History of an Alcohol Use Disorder. Chronic Stress. 3, 2470547019865267, DOI: 10.1177/2470547019865267 (2019). Publisher: SAGE Publications Inc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Dager A. D. et al. Shared Genetic Factors Influence Amygdala Volumes and Risk for Alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 412–420, DOI: 10.1038/npp.2014.187 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hill S. Y. & O’Brien J. Psychological and Neurobiological Precursors of Alcohol Use Disorders in High Risk Youth. Curr. addiction reports 2, 104–113, DOI: 10.1007/s40429-015-0051-1 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hill S. Y. et al. Right amygdala volume in adolescent and young adult offspring from families at high risk for developing alcoholism. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 894–905, DOI: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01088-5 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Benegal V., Antony G., Venkatasubramanian G. & Jayakumar P. N. IMAGING STUDY: Gray matter volume abnormalities and externalizing symptoms in subjects at high risk for alcohol dependence. Addict. Biol. 12, 122–132, DOI: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00043.x (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Hartwell K. J. et al. Neural Correlates of Craving and Resisting Craving for Tobacco in Nicotine Dependent Smokers. Addict. Biol. 16, 654–666, DOI: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00340.x (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Charboneau E. J. et al. Cannabis cue-induced brain activation correlates with drug craving in limbic and visual salience regions: Preliminary results. Psychiatry research 214, 122–131, DOI: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.005 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Park M.-S. et al. Brain substrates of craving to alcohol cues in subjects with alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Alcohol. 42, 417–422, DOI: 10.1093/alcalc/agl117 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lees B. et al. Parental family history of alcohol use disorder and neural correlates of response inhibition in children from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Alcohol. clinical experimental research 44, 1234–1244, DOI: 10.1111/acer.14343 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Koob G. F. & Volkow N. D. Neurocircuitry of Addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 217–238, DOI: 10.1038/npp.2009.110 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Baas D., Aleman A. & Kahn R. S. Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Brain Res. Rev. 45, 96–103, DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.02.004 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Buckner R. L., Andrews-Hanna J. R. & Schacter D. L. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals New York Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38, DOI: 10.1196/annals.1440.011 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Greicius M. D., Krasnow B., Reiss A. L. & Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. United States Am. 100, 253–258, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Menon V. 20 years of the default mode network: A review and synthesis. Neuron 111, 2469–2487, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.04.023 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Brandman T., Malach R. & Simony E. The surprising role of the default mode network in naturalistic perception. Commun. Biol. 4, 1–9, DOI: 10.1038/s42003-020-01602-z (2021). Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Dohmatob E., Dumas G. & Bzdok D. Dark control: The default mode network as a reinforcement learning agent. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 3318–3341, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25019 (2020). _eprint: 10.1002/hbm.25019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Zhang R. & Volkow N. D. Brain default-mode network dysfunction in addiction. NeuroImage 200, 313–331, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.036 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Ma N. et al. Abnormal Brain Default-Mode Network Functional Connectivity in Drug Addicts. PLOS ONE 6, e16560, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016560 (2011). Publisher: Public Library of Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Chanraud S., Pitel A.-L., Pfefferbaum A. & Sullivan E. V. Disruption of Functional Connectivity of the Default-Mode Network in Alcoholism. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2272–2281, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhq297 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Hernández-Álvarez D. M. et al. Default Mode Network Efficiency Is Correlated With Deficits in Inhibition in Adolescents With Inhalant Use Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 11, 209, DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00209 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Carhart-Harris R. L. & Friston K. J. The default-mode, ego-functions and free-energy: a neurobiological account of Freudian ideas. Brain 133, 1265–1283, DOI: 10.1093/brain/awq010 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Renteria R., Baltz E. T. & Gremel C. M. Chronic alcohol exposure disrupts top-down control over basal ganglia action selection to produce habits. Nat. Commun. 9, 211, DOI: 10.1038/s41467-017-02615-9 (2018). Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Sjoerds Z. et al. Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e337–e337, DOI: 10.1038/tp.2013.107 (2013). Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Li Q. et al. Disrupted Default Mode Network and Basal Craving in Male Heroin-Dependent Individuals: A Resting-State fMRI Study. The J. Clin. Psychiatry 77, 4560, DOI: 10.4088/JCP.15m09965 (2016). Publisher: Physicians Postgraduate Press, Inc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Bobzean S. A. M., DeNobrega A. K. & Perrotti L. I. Sex differences in the neurobiology of drug addiction. Exp. Neurol. 259, 64–74, DOI: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.01.022 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Swick D., Ashley V. & Turken A. U. Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 9, 102, DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Tabibnia G. et al. Common prefrontal regions activate during self-control of craving, emotion, and motor impulses in smokers. Clin. psychological science : a journal Assoc. for Psychol. Sci. 2, 611–619, DOI: 10.1177/2167702614522037 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Kober H. et al. Prefrontal–striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 14811–14816, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Miller A. P. et al. Neuroanatomical variability associated with early substance use initiation: Results from the ABCD Study. medRxiv 2024.03.06.24303876, DOI: 10.1101/2024.03.06.24303876 (2024). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Chanraud S. et al. Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 32, 429–438, DOI: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301219 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Boutte D. et al. Association of genetic copy number variations at 11 q14.2 with brain regional volume differences in an alcohol use disorder population. Alcohol 46, 519–527, DOI: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2012.05.002 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Sutherland M. T. et al. Chronic cigarette smoking is linked with structural alterations in brain regions showing acute nicotinic drug-induced functional modulations. Behav. Brain Funct. 12, 16, DOI: 10.1186/s12993-016-0100-5 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Kühn S. et al. Brain grey matter deficits in smokers: focus on the cerebellum. Brain Struct. Funct. 217, 517–522, DOI: 10.1007/s00429-011-0346-5 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Cservenka A. & Nagel B. J. Risky decision-making: an FMRI study of youth at high risk for alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 604–615, DOI: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01650.x (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Inhóf O. et al. Internet addiction associated with right pars opercularis in females. J. Behav. Addict. 8, 162–168, DOI: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.135 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rossetti M. G. et al. Gender-related neuroanatomical differences in alcohol dependence: findings from the ENIGMA Addiction Working Group. NeuroImage: Clin. 30, 102636, DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102636 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Harakeh Z. et al. The association between neurocognitive functioning and smoking in adolescence: The TRAILS study. Neuropsychology 26, 541–550, DOI: 10.1037/a0029217 (2012). Place: US Publisher: American Psychological Association. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Song Z., Chen J., Wen Z. & Zhang L. Abnormal functional connectivity and effective connectivity between the default mode network and attention networks in patients with alcohol-use disorder. Acta Radiol. 62, 251–259, DOI: 10.1177/0284185120923270 (2021). Publisher: SAGE Publications. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Zehra A. et al. Neural correlates of visual attention in alcohol use disorder. Drug Alcohol Dependence 194, 430–437, DOI: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.10.032 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Kelly C. et al. Reduced Interhemispheric Resting State Functional Connectivity in Cocaine Addiction. Biol. psychiatry 69, 684–692, DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.022 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Lanssens A., Pizzamiglio G., Mantini D. & Gillebert C. R. Role of the dorsal attention network in distracter suppression based on features. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 37–46, DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2019.1683525 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Stoeckel C., Gough P. M., Watkins K. E. & Devlin J. T. Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex; a journal devoted to study nervous system behavior 45, 1091–1096, DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Squeglia L. M. et al. Neural Predictors of Initiating Alcohol Use During Adolescence. The Am. J. Psychiatry 174, 172–185, DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15121587 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Shine J. M. & Poldrack R. A. Principles of dynamic network reconfiguration across diverse brain states. NeuroImage 180, 396–405, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.010 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Dumais K. M., Chernyak S., Nickerson L. D. & Janes A. C. Sex differences in default mode and dorsal attention network engagement. PLOS ONE 13, e0199049, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199049 (2018). Publisher: Public Library of Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Murray L. et al. Sex differences in functional network dynamics observed using coactivation pattern analysis. Cogn. neuroscience 12, 120–130, DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2021.1880383 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Huang Z., Zhang J., Wu J., Mashour G. A. & Hudetz A. G. Temporal circuit of macroscale dynamic brain activity supports human consciousness. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaz0087, DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz0087 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Zhou X. & Lei X. Wandering Minds with Wandering Brain Networks. Neurosci. Bull. 34, 1017–1028, DOI: 10.1007/s12264-018-0278-7 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Gao W. et al. The Synchronization within and Interaction between the Default and Dorsal Attention Networks in Early Infancy. Cereb. Cortex 23, 594–603, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhs043 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Anderson J. S., Ferguson M. A., Lopez-Larson M. & Yurgelun-Todd D. Connectivity Gradients Between the Default Mode and Attention Control Networks. Brain Connect. 1, 147–157, DOI: 10.1089/brain.2011.0007 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Yau W.-Y. W. et al. Nucleus Accumbens Response to Incentive Stimuli Anticipation in Children of Alcoholics: Relationships with Precursive Behavioral Risk and Lifetime Alcohol Use. The J. Neurosci. 32, 2544–2551, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-11.2012 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Blum K. et al. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J. Psychoact. Drugs 32 Suppl, i–iv, 1–112, DOI: 10.1080/02791072.2000.10736099 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Morales A. M., Jones S. A., Ehlers A., Lavine J. B. & Nagel B. J. Ventral striatal response during decision making involving risk and reward is associated with future binge drinking in adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 1884–1890, DOI: 10.1038/s41386-018-0087-8 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Heitzeg M. M., Nigg J. T., Yau W.-Y. W., Zucker R. A. & Zubieta J.-K. Striatal dysfunction marks preexisting risk and medial prefrontal dysfunction is related to problem drinking in children of alcoholics. Biol. psychiatry 68, 287–295, DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.020 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Braams B. R., Duijvenvoorde A. C. K. v., Peper J. S. & Crone E. A. Longitudinal Changes in Adolescent Risk-Taking: A Comprehensive Study of Neural Responses to Rewards, Pubertal Development, and Risk-Taking Behavior. J. Neurosci. 35, 7226–7238, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4764-14.2015 (2015). Publisher: Society for Neuroscience Section: Articles. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Cornblath E. J. et al. Sex differences in network controllability as a predictor of executive function in youth. NeuroImage 188, 122–134, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.048 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Blakemore S. & Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 296–312, DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Farrant K. & Uddin L. Q. Asymmetric development of dorsal and ventral attention networks in the human brain. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 165–174, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.001 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Lenroot R. K. et al. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. NeuroImage 36, 1065–1073, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Couvy-Duchesne B. et al. Heritability of head motion during resting state functional MRI in 462 healthy twins. NeuroImage 102 Pt 2, 424–434, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.010 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Van Dijk K. R. A., Sabuncu M. R. & Buckner R. L. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage 59, 431–438, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Kong X.-Z. et al. Individual differences in impulsivity predict head motion during magnetic resonance imaging. PloS One 9, e104989, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104989 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Marshal M. P. et al. Sexual orientation and adolescent substance use: a meta-analysis and methodological review. Addict. (Abingdon, England) 103, 546–556, DOI: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02149.x (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Day J. K., Fish J. N., Perez-Brumer A., Hatzenbuehler M. L. & Russell S. T. Transgender Youth Substance Use Disparities: Results from a Population-Based Sample. The J. adolescent health : official publication Soc. for Adolesc. Medicine 61, 729–735, DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.06.024 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Fish J. N., Bishop M. D. & Russell S. T. Developmental Differences in Sexual Orientation and Gender Identity–Related Substance Use Disparities: Findings From Population-Based Data. J. Adolesc. Heal. 68, 1162–1169, DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.10.023 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Auchter A. M. et al. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 8–15, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.003 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Chen J. et al. Shared and unique brain network features predict cognitive, personality, and mental health scores in the ABCD study. Nat. Commun. 13, 2217, DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-29766-8 (2022). Number: 1 Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Rice J. P. et al. Comparison of Direct Interview and Family History Diagnoses of Alcohol Dependence. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 1018–1023, DOI: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb00983.x (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Barch D. M. et al. Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 55–66, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Stubbs J. L. et al. Heterogeneous neuroimaging findings across substance use disorders localize to a common brain network. Nat. Mental Heal. 1, 772–781, DOI: 10.1038/s44220-023-00128-7 (2023). Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Lisdahl K. M. et al. Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 80–96, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.007 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Jacobson J. L. & Jacobson S. W. Effects of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure on Child Development. Alcohol Res. & Heal. 26, 282–286 (2002). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Mareckova K., Marecek R., Andryskova L., Brazdil M. & Nikolova Y. S. Prenatal exposure to alcohol and its impact on reward processing and substance use in adulthood. Transl. Psychiatry 14, 1–9, DOI: 10.1038/s41398-024-02941-9 (2024). Publisher: Nature Publishing Group. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Roffman J. L. et al. Association of adverse prenatal exposure burden with child psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. PLOS ONE 16, e0250235, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250235 (2021). Publisher: Public Library of Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Flak A. L. et al. The Association of Mild, Moderate, and Binge Prenatal Alcohol Exposure and Child Neuropsychological Outcomes: A Meta-Analysis. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 214–226, DOI: 10.1111/acer.12214 (2014). _eprint: 10.1111/acer.12214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Barch D. M. et al. Common Measures for National Institute of Mental Health Funded Research. Biol. psychiatry 79, e91–e96, DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.006 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Collins S. E. Associations Between Socioeconomic Factors and Alcohol Outcomes. Alcohol Res. : Curr. Rev. 38, 83–94 (2016). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Hiscock R., Bauld L., Amos A., Fidler J. A. & Munafò M. Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review. Annals New York Acad. Sci. 1248, 107–123, DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06202.x (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Dennis E., Manza P. & Volkow N. D. Socioeconomic status, BMI, and brain development in children. Transl. Psychiatry 12, 33, DOI: 10.1038/s41398-022-01779-3 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Assari S., Mistry R., Caldwell C. H. & Bazargan M. Protective Effects of Parental Education Against Youth Cigarette Smoking: Diminished Returns of Blacks and Hispanics. Adolesc. Heal. Medicine Ther. 11, 63–71, DOI: 10.2147/AHMT.S238441 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Joel D. & McCarthy M. M. Incorporating Sex As a Biological Variable in Neuropsychiatric Research: Where Are We Now and Where Should We Be? Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 42, 379–385, DOI: 10.1038/npp.2016.79 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Joel D. et al. Sex beyond the genitalia: The human brain mosaic. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 15468–15473, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1509654112 (2015). Publisher: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Dhamala E., Bassett D. S., Yeo B. & Holmes A. J. Functional brain networks are associated with both sex and gender in children. Sci. Adv. 10, eadn4202, DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adn4202 (2024). Publisher: American Association for the Advancement of Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Desikan R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Fischl B. et al. Whole Brain Segmentation. Neuron 33, 341–355, DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Ooi L. Q. R. et al. Comparison of individualized behavioral predictions across anatomical, diffusion and functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage 263, 119636, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119636 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Casey B. et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 43–54, DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Leys C., Ley C., Klein O., Bernard P. & Licata L. Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 764–766, DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 (2013). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Benjamini Y. & Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. Royal Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodological) 57, 289–300 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- 157.Goutte C., Toft P., Rostrup E., Nielsen F. & Hansen L. K. On Clustering fMRI Time Series. NeuroImage 9, 298–310, DOI: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0391 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Müller-Oehring E. M. et al. Influences of Age, Sex, and Moderate Alcohol Drinking on the Intrinsic Functional Architecture of Adolescent Brains. Cereb. Cortex (New York, NY) 28, 1049–1063, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhx014 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Rohlfing T., Zahr N. M., Sullivan E. V. & Pfefferbaum A. The SRI24 multichannel atlas of normal adult human brain structure. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 798–819, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20906 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Cetin-Karayumak S. et al. Harmonized diffusion MRI data and white matter measures from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. bioRxiv: The Prepr. Serv. for Biol. 2023.04.04.535587, DOI: 10.1101/2023.04.04.535587 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. Processed neuroimaging data by (153) used in this study were uploaded by the original authors to the NDA. Researchers with access to the ABCD data will be able to download the data: https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=1368. The ABCD imaging data used in this report came from https://doi.org/10.15154/1504041 and non-imaging data was from the 5.1 release (http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/z563-zd24). These data were used in the analyses described in https://doi.org/10.15154/c9z7-ng36. The ABCD data are publicly available via the NIMH Data Archive (NDA).
Collection and distribution of the NCANDA data were supported by NIH funding AA021681, AA021690, AA021691, AA021692, AA021695, AA021696, AA021697. Researchers with access to the NCANDA data will be able to download the data via https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=4513.