Skip to main content
BMC Psychology logoLink to BMC Psychology
. 2024 Nov 21;12:683. doi: 10.1186/s40359-024-02180-z

The relationship between autistic traits and problematic smartphone use in adolescents: The serial mediating role of anxiety and executive dysfunction

Siyuan Zhou 1,2,#, Zongping Chen 1,2,#, Ya Liu 1,2,
PMCID: PMC11583648  PMID: 39574179

Abstract

Background

Based on the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model, this study aimed to explore the relationship between autistic traits and problematic smartphone use (PSU) among Chinese adolescents and to examine the serial mediation effect of anxiety and executive dysfunction in the association between autistic traits and PSU.

Methods

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient, Smartphone Addiction Scale, the trait version of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory, and Dysexecutive Questionnaire were administered to a sample comprising 412 senior high school students (average age = 17.05 years, SD = 0.65). Structural equation models were utilized to explore the simple and serial mediating role of anxiety and executive dysfunction played in the association between autistic traits and PSU.

Results

This study found that social rather than non-social autistic traits were positively associated with anxiety, executive dysfunction, and PSU. Furthermore, after controlling for gender, anxiety and executive dysfunction acted as sequential mediators in the connection between social autistic trait and PSU. However, non-social autistic trait did not predict anxiety, executive dysfunction, or PSU.

Conclusion

This study supports the I-PACE model and deepens understanding of PSU formation. Furthermore, the findings underscore the importance of addressing social challenges faced by adolescents with high autistic traits, providing a viable potential intervention pathway to promote healthy smartphone use in this population.

Keywords: Autistic traits, Problematic smartphone use, Anxiety, Executive dysfunction, Serial mediation, Structural equation models

Introduction

As of June 2024, the number of smartphone Internet users in China mainland has reached 1.096 billion, which is nearly 75.9% of the Chinese population [1]. Despite of convenience of smartphones [2], a growing number of studies find the adverse effects of excessive smartphone use [3]. Excessive smartphone use is linked to social anxiety [4, 5], poor communication skills [6], social relationship and loneliness [7], lower mindfulness [8], poor emotion regulation [9], sleep problems [10, 11], and more psychological distress [12].

Some scholars have described excessive smartphone use as "mobile phone addiction" or "smartphone addiction", highlighting features of behavioral addiction like salience, withdrawal, and tolerance [5, 13]. However, others have argued that labeling frequent smartphone use alongside substances like cocaine or alcohol may diminish the severity of true addiction and overpathologize typical behavior [14]. Therefore, the more appropriate terminology "problematic smartphone use" was used in this study.

In light of the aforementioned adverse consequences of PSU, and considering that adolescents have lower impulse control than adults and are more susceptible to the adverse effects of PSU [15], research on predictors of PSU among adolescents and the underlying mechanisms is essential for improving the understanding of PSU among adolescents and development of effective interventions.

Although the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model was originally proposed to explain development and maintenance of Internet use disorders [16, 17], it has also been applied to understanding PSU [18]. This model was applied to test factors that could contribute to PSU in this study. Autistic traits, as personality traits, were tested as an antecedent of PSU; anxiety and executive dysfunction, as affective and cognitive factors, respectively, were tested as intervening variables that would mediate this association. Following the I-PACE model, the current study would address how autistic traits influence PSU.

The relationship between autistic traits and PSU

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a group of neurodevelopmental disorders charactered by poor social skills, damaged communications, and restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (RRBIs) [19]. These traits not only exist in clinical samples, but also in the general population with a continuous distribution [20, 21]. According to the continuum theory of autism, autistic traits are a subclinical set of characteristics that are milder but qualitatively similar to the diagnosed autism phenotype [22, 23]. Higher levels of autistic traits are associated with increased sensitivity, attention to detail, and intense focus on specific tasks, and also associated with poorer affect recognition, impaired social interactions, decreased fantasy life, and poorer communication skills [23, 24]. These social challenges may lead individuals with higher autistic traits to seek out alternative means of communication that are less demanding than face-to-face interactions. Online communication could represent a more accessible form of interaction for such individuals, as the Internet can reduce the constraints they might experience in offline social settings [25]. Previous studies have investigated the relationship between autistic traits and Internet addiction and found a significant correlation [2628]. PSU was considered as a specific form of internet addiction [29, 30]. The convenience of online communication with smartphones, which are an easily accessible online medium, makes them potentially highly attractive to individuals with high autistic traits. Additionally, a recent review showed that autistic traits and behavior addiction share some common characteristics, such as extreme preoccupations and rigid habits [31], which has found a positive correlation between autistic traits and behavior addiction in both clinical and general population. Thus, it seems that different aspects of autistic traits are relevant to PSU, including characteristics in social and non-social domains [30]. However, few studies have investigated the relationship between autistic traits and PSU. To the best of our knowledge, only one study among college students found a significant association between autistic traits and PSU [4]. Furthermore, this association was mediated by the social interaction characteristics of autistic traits (social anxiety and loneliness). It is still unclear whether other characteristics of autistic traits are relevant to PSU.

Most prior studies have investigated autistic traits as a unitary construct, while growing data reveals the dissociation of social and non-social autistic traits in both clinical and general population. This dissociation is observed at the behavioral [3235], genetic [36, 37], cognitive [38, 39], and neural levels [38]. Social and non-social traits are thought to represent different processes, as social traits are conceptualized by deficits in social abilities, communication, and recognition of social cues, while non-social traits are mainly conceptualized by systemizing [33]. In sum, the understanding of the relationship between autistic traits and PSU is limited, and it is necessary to fractionate social and non-social autistic traits in the current study, which could deepen our understanding of the relationship between autistic traits and PSU.

The mediating role of anxiety between autistic traits and PSU

Anxiety is one of the most common comorbidities of ASD in children [40, 41], adolescents [41, 42], and adults [35, 43]. Comorbid anxiety disorders have been reported in approximately 40% of ASD individuals [44]. A recent review has revealed that higher trait anxiety scores in autism compared to control groups [45]. Individuals with higher level of autistic traits seem likely not only lack social skills and social knowledge, but also lack the initiative to engage in interpersonal interaction [44], which may be associated with anxiety [4650]. This supports the idea of a close relation between autistic traits and anxiety.

Additionally, previous research has found that individuals with high anxiety typically use their smartphones more excessively [5155]. This is also in line with Brand et al.'s (2016) I-PACE Model, which is a theoretical model that suggests that a number of core characteristics of an individual are susceptibility factors of PSU, such as personality traits, biological factors, and psychopathological traits (e.g., anxiety, depression), which play an important role in the early developmental stages of PSU [16]. Individuals with higher levels of anxiety may tend to use smartphones to fulfil the need to interact in a less direct way than in real-life interactions [56]. Using smartphone can reduce an individual's anxiety when interacting with others because it provides more opportunity to think about each response in a dialogue [57]. A recent review on the relationship between anxiety, including trait anxiety, social anxiety, and generalized anxiety, with smartphone addiction, reveled a small to moderate correlation between anxiety and smartphone addiction [58]. Moreover, existing studies have revealed that social autistic traits are strongly positively correlated with anxiety and other psychiatric conditions, while non-social autistic traits are irrelevant to these variables [44, 59]. Therefore, anxiety may be a mediator in the relation between autistic traits and PSU.

The mediating role of executive dysfunction between autistic traits and PSU

Executive function is a cognitive control system responsible for integration and coordination of information in the service of goal-oriented behaviors [60]. This umbrella term is consisted of three core domains: working memory, inhibition control, and switching [60, 61]. Executive dysfunction theory of ASD advocated that autism is usually associated with deficits in executive function [39, 62]. Executive dysfunction underlies characteristic symptomatology of ASD, both in the social and non-social domains. Further, executive function deficits are found in non-clinical sample high in autistic traits. A growing body of research has revealed a link between autistic traits and executive dysfunction among general individuals [6365].

Additionally, RRBIs in ASD individuals have been hypothesized to be linked with executive dysfunction [38]. Research has demonstrated that RRBIs are related to three core executive function (i.e., cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory) impairments [66]. Contrarily, some research found that social autistic traits but not RRBIs are associated with executive dysfunction [67, 68]. Another research showed that both social autistic traits and RRBIs are associated with executive dysfunction [69]. Few studies have examined the association between autistic traits and executive dysfunction with distinguishing between social and non-social traits in the general population.

In addition, executive dysfunction is a robust predictor of PSU [16, 17]. Following the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model, executive dysfunction is crucially involved in the development and maintenance of PSU [16, 17]. Existing empirical studies indicated that executive dysfunction is correlated with PSU [7073]. Therefore, executive dysfunction may be another mediator in the relation between autistic traits and PSU.

The serial mediating effect of anxiety and executive dysfunction

It has been shown that high level of anxiety can lead to impaired executive function (i.e., executive dysfunction) [37, 62, 7476]. The attentional control theory (ACT) [74] proposed that anxiety impairs inhibition and shifting (two core components of executive function). This can be explained as anxiety exhausting cognitive resources and leading to executive dysfunction [74, 77].

Previous studies have examined the mediation mechanism in the relation between anxiety and PSU. The mediators examined include distress tolerance [53], mindfulness [53], rumination [51, 52], fear of missing out [51, 54, 55], and boredom proneness [52, 54]. However, few empirical studies explored the mediation role of executive dysfunction between them, which is an essential factor contributing to the development of PSU [78]. A study found a mediating role of executive dysfunction between anxiety and PSU in college students [72].

Therefore, individuals with higher autistic traits may have more anxiety, which could be associated to greater executive dysfunction, in turn relating to the tendency to engage in PSU. In other words, anxiety and executive dysfunction serially mediate the association between autistic traits and PSU.

The present study

This study explored the potential mediation mechanisms underlying the relationship between autistic traits and PSU [16, 17]. According to the I-PACE model, some core characteristics of individuals, such as personality traits, are susceptibility factors for PSU, while affective and cognitive factors mediate the association between these core characteristics and PSU. Individuals with higher levels of autistic traits are more likely to lack social stimulation and social skills, and may feel uncomfortable interacting with others face-to-face, which may cause them to feel anxious in social situations [44], and this anxiety can in turn exacerbate executive dysfunction [37, 62, 7476]. Thus, in order to cope with negative emotions and stress, individuals experiencing anxiety and executive dysfunction may turn to increased smartphone use as a coping way to enhance comfort or entertainment while socialising. Based on this, we proposed that the relationship between autistic traits and PSU would be serially mediated by anxiety and executive dysfunction.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the relationship between social and non-social autistic traits and PSU in adolescents and to explore the mediation role of anxiety and executive dysfunction in this relationship. Based on the aforementioned literature review, we established a hypothesized model presented in Fig. 1, and tested the following hypotheses: (1) Autistic traits are positively correlated with PSU among adolescents. (2) Anxiety acts as a simple mediator between autistic traits and PSU. (3) Executive dysfunction acts as a simple mediator between autistic traits and PSU. (4) Autistic traits exacerbate PSU through the serial mediation role of anxiety and executive dysfunction. Furthermore, based on previous studies, social autistic traits may be more predictive of PSU than non-social autistic traits due to their inherent social nature.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The hypothesized serial mediation model

Method

Participants

Using a convenient sampling method, a total of 470 senior high school students in Sichuan, Chongqing and Hunan province in China completed the online questionnaires (Survey Star, www.wjx.cn) during the July–August 2023. Data from 58 participants were excluded due to short response times, regular responses, or incorrect answers to lie detector questions. Since participants were asked to make forced choices for each question, there were no missing values. The data provided by a total of 412 participants were thus included in subsequent analyses, for a response rate of 87.7%. The age distribution of the subjects was 15–19 years old, with an average age of 17.05 ± 0.65. Among them 111 were male and 301 were female. This study was approved by the local Ethics. All participants provided written informed consent. The participants were fully informed about the purpose and procedures of the study. The consent form clearly indicated their right to withdraw from the study at any time without facing any negative consequences. Participants' privacy and confidentiality were protected. The researchers assured the participants that their personal information would remain confidential and that the data would be anonymized.

Measurement

Autistic traits

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) is a self-report, 50-item questionnaire designed to measure autistic traits in the general population [20] and assessing five different areas (10 items each): social skills, attention to details, attention switching, communication, and imagination. Each item is scored on a four-point scale: “definitely disagree", “slightly disagree", “slightly agree", and “definitely agree". The AQ total score ranges from 50 to 200, with a higher AQ score meaning greater autistic traits for an individual. The Chinese version of the AQ has been confirmed to have good internal consistency (Cronbach α = 0.81) [79]. In this study, the Cronbach's α for the AQ total scale was 0.66.

To assess both social and non-social autistic traits, we utilized the two-factor model of the Autism Spectrum Quotient [20]. Specifically, we distinguished between a "Social Interaction" factor (representing AQ social traits) and an "Attention to Detail" factor (representing AQ non-social traits). Higher scores on these factors indicate greater challenges in social interaction and a stronger inclination towards attention to detail, respectively [33]. The Cronbach's α for the AQ social traits subscale was 0.76, while AQ non-social traits subscale was 0.60.

Problematic smartphone use

Problematic smartphone use was measured by the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS) [2] which consists of 33 items. The scale includes six components: daily life, disturbance, positive anticipation, withdrawal, cyberspace-oriented relationship, overuse, and tolerance. Each item is rated on a six-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 6 (“strongly agree”). A higher score indicates a more severe PSU. The Chinese version of the SAS was used in this study, and the scale had a good reliability with Cronbach's α coefficient of 0.91 [80]. The Cronbach’s α coefficient of the SAS in this study was 0.95.

Anxiety

Anxiety was measured using the trait version of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory [81]. The questionnaire consists of 20 items. Each item was rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (always), higher scores indicate higher anxiety. The Chinese version of the STAI-T was used in this study, and the scale had a good reliability with a Cronbach’s α of 0.89 in the general population [82]. In the current study, Cronbach’s α of the STAI-T was 0.90.

Executive dysfunction

Executive dysfunction was measured by the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) [83]. This questionnaire consists of 20 items and includes five subscales: inhibition, intention, executive memory, positive affect, and negative affect. Each item in this scale was rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). A higher score indicates a more impaired executive function. The Chinese version of the DEX was used in this study, which has been confirmed to be a reliable measurement for both clinical and non-clinical Chinese population [84, 85]. The Cronbach’s α coefficient of the DEX in this study was 0.93.

Data analysis

This study utilized SPSS 15.0 and Mplus 7.4 software for data analysis. We performed a descriptive statistics analysis to identify the distribution of all variables and Pearson’s correlation analysis to estimate the correlation coefficients between all study variables. Subsequently, a Harman single-factor test was carried out to examine potential common method bias [86]. Finally, based on previous studies [87], all variables were standardized, and then serial mediating effect analysis was conducted under the control of gender and age with structural equation modeling technology. In this model, 5000 bootstrap samples and 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs) were implemented. The goodness of fit was assessed using various indices, including the χ2 test statistic, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI).

Autistic traits (both social and non-social traits) were independent variables (X); PSU was dependent variable (Y); anxiety was considered the first mediated variable (M1); and executive dysfunction was the second mediator (M2). An effect is considered significant when 95% CI does not contain zero.

Results

Common method bias test

Since all study variables were measured by self-reported questionnaires, it is necessary to evaluate the extent of common method bias [86]. A Harman single-factor test was conducted. Specifically, all items of each questionnaire were included in a factor analysis adopting principal component analysis method to extract factors. The unrotated exploratory factor analysis extracted a total of 29 factors with an eigenvalue > 1, the first factor explained only 18.37% of the total variance, which is less than the critical standard of 40%. Therefore, common method bias was not a major problem in this study. In addition, this study also examined the common method bias by controlling for the effects of an unmeasured latent methods factor, which showed that after controlling for the common method variance factor, the change in each of the fit indicators did not exceed 0.05, thus indicating that there is no serious problem of common method bias in this study [88].

Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis

The means and standard deviations of study variables and the correlations among them are presented in Table 1. All study variables were significantly correlated each other. Moreover, the correlation coefficients between social and non-social autistic trait with anxiety, executive dysfunction, as well as PSU were calculated. Results revealed that social autistic trait was positively and significantly correlated with anxiety, executive dysfunction, as well as PSU, while non-social autistic trait exhibited a negative correlation with these variables. Additionally, the correlation coefficient of social and non-social autistic traits was − 0.331 (p < 0.001). Further details are given in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficient matrix of all study variables

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Autistic traits 119.91 8.62 1
2. Social autistic trait 93.07 9.00 0.920*** 1
3. Non-social autistic trait 24.78 3.37 0.051  − 0.331*** 1
4. Anxiety 45.38 8.97 0.496*** 0.515***  − 0.100* 1
5. Executive dysfunction 36.17 14.46 0.445*** 0.475***  − 0.122* 0.581*** 1
6. Problematic smartphone use 85.36 21.85 0.262*** 0.298***  − 0.119* 0.470*** 0.537*** 1

N = 412. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001

Serial mediation analysis

Firstly, we examined the serial mediation with the overall autistic traits. Figure 2 presents the path coefficients of the mediation analysis. The total effect of autistic traits on PSU is c = 0.389, p < 0.001. The mediation effect analysis was carried out according to the hypothesized model and the model fit indices are listed as follows: χ2/df = 2.57, RMSEA = 0.065, CFI = 0.932, TLI = 0.916, SRMR = 0.060. According to Hu and Bentler (1995)’s recommendation, if χ2/df < 3, RMSEA < 0.08, SRMR < 0.10, and TLI and CFI > 0.90, the model is acceptable. When anxiety and executive dysfunction were included, the direct effect of autistic traits on smartphone addiction was not significant (c′ =  − 0.072, p = 0.329). Thus, anxiety and executive dysfunction exerted a completely mediated effect in the relationship between autistic traits and PSU. The total mediation effect was significant (ab = 0.461, 95% CI = [0.358,0.580], see Table 2). The simple mediation effect of anxiety (a1b1 = 0.169, 95% CI = [0.056,0.303]) and executive dysfunction (a2b2 = 0.209, 95% CI = [0.086,0.251]) was significant. The serial mediation effect of anxiety and executive dysfunction was also significant (a1db2 = 0.127, 95% CI = [0.065,0.200]).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Serial mediation model of the relationship between autistic traits and problematic smartphone use. Solid lines mean significant pathways. Dotted line means non-significant pathway. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001

Table 2.

Serial mediation analysis results of the relationship between autistic traits and problematic smartphone use

Effect BootSE 95% CI
Lower Upper
c Autistic traits → problematic smartphone use (total effect) 0.389 0.057 0.272 0.492
c Autistic traits → problematic smartphone use (direct effect)  − 0.072 0.074  − 0.215 0.072
ab Total indirect effect 0.461 0.042 0.358 0.251
a1b1 Autistic traits → anxiety → problematic smartphone use 0.169 0.064 0.056 0.303
a2b2 Autistic traits → executive dysfunction → problematic smartphone use 0.165 0.042 0.056 0.303
a1db2 Autistic traits → anxiety → executive dysfunction → problematic smartphone use 0.127 0.065 0.200 0.269

Then, social and non-social autistic trait were divided and included in the hypothesized model. The total effect of social autistic trait on PSU is c1 = 0.401, p < 0.001, while non-social autistic trait had no significant effect on PSU, c2 =  − 0.032, p = 0.638. Therefore, the following mediation analysis excluded non-social autistic trait. Figure 3 presents the path coefficients of the mediation analysis with social autistic trait. The results of social autistic trait model are analogical to the overall autistic traits model and the model fit indices are listed as follows: χ2/df = 2.64, RMSEA = 0.063, CFI = 0.941, TLI = 0.926, SRMR = 0.057. When anxiety and executive dysfunction were included, the direct effect of social autistic trait on PSU was not significant (c1′ =  − 0.086, p = 0.267). Thus, anxiety and executive dysfunction exerted a completely mediated effect in the relationship between social autistic trait and PSU. The total mediation effect was significant (ab = 0.487, 95% CI = [0.378,0.618], see Table 3). The simple mediation effect of anxiety (a1b1 = 0.178, 95% CI = [0.058,0.314]) and executive dysfunction (a2b2 = 0.183, 95% CI = [0.101,0.289]) was significant. The serial mediation effect of anxiety and executive dysfunction was also significant (a1db2 = 0.125, 95% CI = [0.069,0.209]).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Serial mediation model of the relationship between social autistic trait and problematic smartphone use. Solid lines mean significant pathways. Dotted line means non-significant pathway. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001

Table 3.

Serial mediation analysis results of the relationship between social autistic trait and problematic smartphone use

Effect BootSE 95% CI
Lower Upper
c1 Social autistic trait → problematic smartphone use (total effect) 0.401 0.057 0.282 0.503
c1' Social autistic trait → problematic smartphone use (direct effect)  − 0.086 0.077  − 0.230 0.072
ab Total indirect effect 0.487 0.061 0.378 0.618
a1b1 Social autistic trait → anxiety → problematic smartphone use 0.178 0.066 0.058 0.314
a2b2 Social autistic trait → executive dysfunction → problematic smartphone use 0.183 0.047 0.101 0.289
a1db2 Social autistic trait → anxiety → executive dysfunction → problematic smartphone use 0.125 0.035 0.069 0.209

Discussion

This study extends the application of the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model by examining its relation to PSU among adolescents. Notably, our research differentiates between social and non-social autistic traits, revealing their distinct relationships with PSU. Specifically, we found that social autistic traits were significantly and positively correlated with PSU, anxiety, and executive dysfunction, while non-social autistic traits showed no such association. Results from the mediation model indicated that anxiety and executive dysfunction played a serial mediating role in the relationship between social autistic traits and PSU. Social autistic traits indirectly increased PSU through three pathways: anxiety, executive dysfunction, and the anxiety–executive dysfunction serial path. In contrast, non-social traits had no effect on PSU, anxiety, or executive dysfunction. These findings demonstrate the complexity of PSU, provide insights into how social autistic traits drive PSU and suggest pathways for targeted interventions.

In summary, this research advances the field by offering new insights into the relationship between autistic traits and PSU, and provide implications for future studies. These findings have important implications for developing interventions that address the social challenges faced by adolescents with high autistic traits and promote healthy smartphone use habits in this population.

The relationship between autistic traits and problematic smartphone use

The findings of this study reveal a significantly positive correlation between autistic traits and PSU. Particularly, social autistic traits show a stronger association with PSU. This finding is consistent with previous research, indicating that individuals with high autistic traits may be more inclined to use smartphones as a means to avoid the discomfort of social interactions, as digital communication reduces the constraints they might experience in offline social settings [25]. This may be why social autistic trait has a higher correlation with PSU. That is, social autistic trait focuses on the characteristic of difficulty in social interaction.

However, it is noteworthy that non-social autistic traits show no prediction to PSU. This may suggest that while non-social autistic traits are associated with executive dysfunction, it may not be sufficient to lead to PSU. It may also imply that different dimensions of autistic traits may have varying impacts on PSU, with social autistic traits potentially playing a larger role in PSU.

These findings underscore the importance to address social challenges for adolescents high in autistic traits, and provide viable intervention pathways to promote healthy smartphone use habits for this population. Future research can further explore how different dimensions of autistic traits independently or collectively affect PSU and how to design targeted prevention and intervention measures to reduce PSU among adolescents with high autistic traits.

The simple mediating role of anxiety

Results confirm that anxiety is a simple mediator between autistic traits and PSU in adolescents. Consistent with previous studies [44, 45, 49, 59, 79], this study observed that individuals with high autistic traits tend to experience higher level of anxiety. According to the compensatory internet use theory, individuals may excessively rely on smartphones to alleviate their anxiety, thus leading to PSU [89]. This finding coincides with prior research on college students [5155]. Therefore, it is reasonable to conclude that adolescents with high levels of autistic traits are more susceptible to developing PSU due to elevated anxiety.

In parallel with prior research [44, 59], results support the notion that social rather than non-social autistic trait is associated with anxiety within the general population. Because of their poor social skills and difficulties in communication, individuals with high autistic traits may experience feelings of embarrassment in social situations [49] as well as heightened loneliness [90]. These deficits in social ability may explain why people with high autistic traits are more inclined to choose non-face-to-face interactions [4]. Smartphone and online communication give them the opportunities to get rid of embarrassing occasions. This is consistent with the positive correlation between autistic traits and social anxiety in general student samples [4, 91, 92].

The simple mediating role of executive dysfunction

Moreover, results confirmed that executive dysfunction plays a simple mediating role between autistic traits and smartphone addiction in adolescents. This finding is in line with prior studies which have robustly demonstrated the association between executive dysfunction and smartphone addiction [7173]. The result that autistic traits were positively linked with executive dysfunction is consistent with the executive dysfunction theory of ASD [39, 62]. Previous studies revealed that individuals with high autistic traits have inferior attentional shifting compared to their counterparts with low autistic traits [63, 92]. Working memory and attentional control have also been demonstrated to be decreased in individuals with high autistic traits [63, 93]. Additionally, executive dysfunction is a shared feature in ASD and internet addiction [94], executive dysfunction could aggravate the development of internet addiction [9597]. Individuals with worser working memory capacity and lower attentional control may suffer more distraction from digital media such as the Internet and mobile phones [98]. It is therefore reasonable to conclude that adolescents with high autistic traits are prone to develop PSU due to their executive dysfunction.

It would be worth noting that social but not non-social autistic traits predicted executive dysfunction. It is inconsistent with the hypothesis that executive dysfunction underlies RRBIs [38]. Non-social autistic trait did not predict executive dysfunction may be because the “Attention Switching” subscale was classified into social autistic trait in this study. This subscale is conceived a non-social feature of ASD in previous studies. However, Palme and coworkers [99] argued that attention switching is a trait that benefits social behavior. The dual properties of “Attention Switching” may underestimate the relationship between non-social autistic trait and other study variables (e.g., executive dysfunction, anxiety).

The serial mediating role of anxiety and executive dysfunction

More importantly, this study found that anxiety and executive dysfunction exerts a serial mediating role in the association between autistic traits and PSU in adolescents. Prior studies on the mediation mechanism in the relationship between anxiety and smartphone addiction mainly focused on the mediated role of FoMO, rumination and boredom proneness [58, 100, 101]. The finding that anxiety predicted executive dysfunction is consistent with the attention control theory [74]. Prior studies have found that anxiety impairs executive function across all aspects [62, 75, 76]. Indeed, a recent study has already found executive dysfunction madidates the relationship between anxiety and PSU [72]. According to the I-PACE model [16, 17], poor executive function could aggravate the development of addiction behavior. Hence, it is reasonable to conclude that individuals with high autistic traits have a strong tendency to develop PSU due to their impaired executive function caused by anxiety.

The relationship between social and non-social autistic traits within general population

Another noteworthy point to mention is that when autistic traits were divided into social and non-social autistic trait, social autistic trait showed positive correlations with other study variables, while non-social autistic trait showed weak though significant negative correlations. Moreover, these two factors had a moderately significant negative correlation. In past studies, the clinical individuals scored higher than the control group on all subscales of AQ [20, 102]. This suggests that the results cannot be interpreted as detail-orientation irrelevant to autistic traits. In contrast, these findings should support the notion that social and detail-oriented autistic trait do not tend to co-occur in the general population as revealed by prior cluster analysis [99]. Our results demonstrated that social autistic traits caused PSU, which is consistent with previous study [4]. Therefore, intervention program of PSU for individuals with high autistic traits should focus on their deficits and difficulties in social interaction.

Theoretical implications

This study advances the theoretical understanding of PSU by the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model in an adolescent sample. It builds on prior research by fractionating autistic traits into social and non-social domains, showing that social autistic traits are more strongly associated with PSU through the mediating role of anxiety and executive dysfunction. These findings support and expand the I-PACE model by demonstrating the interplay between personality traits (autistic traits), affect (anxiety), and cognition (executive dysfunction) in shaping PSU. This fractionation approach suggests that social difficulties rather than detail-oriented behaviors are the primary drivers of PSU in individuals with high autistic traits, offering a more refined theoretical understanding of the link between autistic traits and behavioral addictions. By highlighting the mediating mechanisms of anxiety and executive dysfunction, this study underscores the importance of considering affective and cognitive factors when studying technology addiction in specific populations.

Practical implications

Findings provide several practicable insights for reducing PSU among adolescents with high levels of social autistic traits. First, interventions aimed at mitigating PSU should prioritize addressing the social challenges faced by these individuals, as their preference to use smartphones may stem from difficulties in face-to-face social interactions. Targeted programs could focus on social skills, social anxiety, and alternative coping mechanisms for managing negative emotions. Additionally, improving executive functioning through cognitive training or mindfulness practices may be beneficial in reducing the reliance on smartphones, a maladaptive coping strategy. This research also informs educators and mental health professionals to provide tailored intervention programs that cater to specific needs of adolescents with high social autistic traits, ultimately promoting healthy digital habits in this vulnerable population.

Limitations of the study and further research

A few limitations of the current study should be acknowledged. Firstly, the sample in this study is Chinese adolescents.Future research could consider how cultural factors influence the expression of autistic traits and the development of PSU, as well as the potential impact of cultural backgrounds on anxiety and executive functioning.

Secondly, this is a cross-sectional study and thus cannot uncover causal relation. To address this limitation, future research should adopt longitudinal studies to track changes over time and establish causal relationships between autistic traits, anxiety, executive dysfunction, and PSU. Longitudinal designs could allow researchers to observe how these variables influence each other over time and provide stronger evidence for the direction of these relationships. Finally, this study did not explore which core aspects of executive function are related to PSU. This study utilized only self-report questionnaire to measure executive function and attention to detail subscale to represent non-social autistic trait which makes results limited. Future research should examine which specific core aspects of executive function—such as working memory, inhibitory control, or cognitive flexibility—are most closely related to PSU. This could be achieved by incorporating more comprehensive and objective measures of executive functioning, such as behavioral tasks or neuropsychological assessments. Additionally, using a more detailed approach to assess non-social autistic traits, beyond attention to detail, could provide a clearer understanding of their role in PSU development.

Conclusion

Anxiety and executive dysfunction acted as sequential mediators in the connection between social autistic trait and smartphone addiction. However, non-social autistic trait did not predict anxiety, executive dysfunction, or smartphone addiction. Our results confirmed that social autistic traits are of greater concern than non-social autistic traits in PSU, provided implications for PSU interventions among individuals with high autistic traits.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the high school students for agreeing to participate in the study.

Abbreviations

PSU

Problematic smartphone use

I-PACE

Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution model

ASD

Autism spectrum disorder

RRBIS

Restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests

ACT

Attentional control theory

AQ

Autism Spectrum Quotient

SAS

Smartphone Addiction Scale

DEX

Dysexecutive Questionnaire

SD

Standardized deviation

Boot SE

The standard error of 95% Bootstrap confidence interval

RMSEA

Root mean square error of approximation

SRMR

Standardized root mean square residual

CFI

Comparative fit index

TLI

Tucker-Lewis index

Authors’ contributions

ZSY conducted data collection and analysis under the supervision of YL and completed the first draft of the thesis. YL has revised the first draft and gave many valuable suggestions. CZP did most of the critical work in the revision process. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by Graduate Research Innovation Project of Chongqing Normal University (YKC24034) awarded to Siyuan Zhou, Graduate Research Innovation Project of Chongqing, China (CYS240377) awarded to Zongping Chen, and Humanity and Social Science Youth Foundation Project of the Ministry of Education, the People’s Republic of China (17XJC190003) awarded to Ya Liu.

Data availability

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The design of this study followed the guidelines and regulations of the Declaration of Helsinki and has been approved by the local research Ethics Committee of Chongqing Normal University (May 30, 2023; CNU-EDU-20230530–002). All participants signed informed consent. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Siyuan Zhou and Zongping Chen contributed equally to this work and share the first authorship.

References

  • 1.China Internet Information Center. The 54th China statistical report on internet development. 2024. https://www.cnnic.net.cn/n4/2024/0829/c88-11065.html. Accessed 13 Oct 2024.
  • 2.Kwon M, Kim D-J, Cho H, Yang S. The Smartphone Addiction Scale: Development and Validation of a Short Version for Adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e83558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Busch PA, McCarthy S. Antecedents and consequences of problematic smartphone use: A systematic literature review of an emerging research area. Comput Hum Behav. 2021;114:106414. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lu M, Pang F, Wang R, Liu Y, Peng T. The association between autistic traits and excessive smartphone use in Chinese college students: The chain mediating roles of social interaction anxiety and loneliness. Res Dev Disabil. 2022;131:104369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ran G, Li J, Zhang Q, Niu X. The association between social anxiety and mobile phone addiction: A three-level meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav. 2022;130:107198. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cerit B, Bilgin NC, Ak B. Relationship between smartphone addiction of nursing department students and their communication skills. Contemp NURSE. 2018;54:532–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yayan EH, Dag YS, Duken ME. The effects of technology use on working young loneliness and social relationships. Perspect Psychiatr CARE. 2019;55:194–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Volkmer SA, Lermer E. Unhappy and addicted to your phone? – Higher mobile phone use is associated with lower well-being. Comput Hum Behav. 2019;93:210–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Huang W, Liang F, Alexander S. Smartphone addiction and cross-cultural adjustment among overseas Chinese students: The role of emotion regulation beliefs and strategies. Front Psychol. 2022;13:1009347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Demirci K, Akgonul M, Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. J Behav Addict. 2015;4:85–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yuan Y, He X, He Q, Jia Y, Xu Z, Li M. Problematic mobile phone use and time management disposition in Chinese college students: the chain mediating role of sleep quality and cognitive flexibility. BMC Psychol. 2023;11:440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lei LYC, Ismail MAA, Mohammad JAM, Yusoff MSB. The relationship of smartphone addiction with psychological distress and neuroticism among university medical students. BMC Psychol. 2020;8:97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Horvath J, Mundinger C, Schmitgen MM, Wolf ND, Sambataro F, Hirjak D, et al. Structural and functional correlates of smartphone addiction. Addict Behav. 2020;105:106334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Panova T, Carbonell X. Is smartphone addiction really an addiction? J Behav Addict. 2018;7:252–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang P, Zhao M, Wang X, Xie X, Wang Y, Lei L. Peer relationship and adolescent smartphone addiction: The mediating role of self-esteem and the moderating role of the need to belong. J Behav Addict. 2017;6:708–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Woelfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:252–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brand M, Wegmann E, Stark R, Mueller A, Woelfling K, Robbins TW, et al. The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;104:1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brailovskaia J, Stirnberg J, Rozgonjuk D, Margraf J, Elhai JD. From low sense of control to problematic smartphone use severity during Covid-19 outbreak: The mediating role of fear of missing out and the moderating role of repetitive negative thinking. PLoS ONE. 2021;16:e0261023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. DSM-5TM. 5th ed. Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population - A twin study. Arch Gen PSYCHIATRY. 2003;60:524–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ruzich E, Allison C, Smith P, Watson P, Auyeung B, Ring H, et al. Measuring autistic traits in the general population: a systematic review of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Mol Autism. 2015;6:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mandy W, Pellicano L, St Pourcain B, Skuse D, Heron J. The development of autistic social traits across childhood and adolescence in males and females. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59:1143–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Zhao X, Li X, Song Y, Shi W. Autistic traits and prosocial behaviour in the general population: Test of the mediating effects of trait empathy and state empathic concern. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:3925–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Finkenauer C, Pollmann MMH, Begeer S, Kerkhof P. Brief report: Examining the link between autistic traits and compulsive internet use in a non-clinical sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:2252–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chen YL, Chen SH, Gau SSF. ADHD and autistic traits, family function, parenting style, and social adjustment for Internet addiction among children and adolescents in Taiwan: A longitudinal study. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;39:20–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Concerto C, Rodolico A, Avanzato C, Fusar-Poli L, Signorelli MS, Battaglia F, et al. Autistic traits and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms predict the severity of internet gaming disorder in an italian adult population. Brain Sci. 2021;11:774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Zhang Y, Sindermann C, Kendrick KM, Becker B, Montag C. individual differences in tendencies toward internet use disorder, internet literacy and their link to autistic traits in both China and Germany. Front PSYCHIATRY. 2021;12:638655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Choi S-W, Kim D-J, Choi J-S, Ahn H, Choi E-J, Song W-Y, et al. Comparison of risk and protective factors associated with smartphone addiction and Internet addiction. J Behav Addict. 2015;4:308–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Normand CL, Fisher MH, Simonato I, Fecteau S-M, Poulin M-H. A Systematic Review of Problematic Internet Use in Children, Adolescents, and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2022;9:507–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kervin R, Berger C, Moon SJ, Hill H, Park D, Kim JW. Behavioral addiction and autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Res Dev Disabil. 2021;117:104033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Greenberg DM, Warrier V, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Testing the empathizing-systemizing theory of sex differences and the extreme male brain theory of autism in half a million people. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115:12152–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Cath DC, Boomsma DI. Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): a study in Dutch population and patient groups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1555–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Shuster J, Perry A, Bebko J, Toplak ME. Review of factor analytic studies examining symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:90–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Svedholm-Hakkinen AM, Halme S, Lindeman M. Empathizing and systemizing are differentially related to dimensions of autistic traits in the general population. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2018;18:35–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ronald A, Happé F, Plomin R. The genetic relationship between individual differences in social and nonsocial behaviours characteristic of autism. Dev Sci. 8:444–58. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Warren SL, Heller W, Miller GA. The structure of executive dysfunction in depression and anxiety. J Affect Disord. 2021;279:208–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Happé F, Ronald A. The ‘fractionable autism triad’: a review of evidence from behavioural, genetic cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18:287–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:26–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.van Steensel FJA, Heeman EJ. Anxiety levels in children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. J CHILD Fam Stud. 2017;26:1753–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.White SW, Oswald D, Ollendick T, Scahill L. Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29:216–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Uljarevic M, Richdale AL, Evans DW, Cai RY, Leekam SR. Interrelationship between insistence on sameness, effortful control and anxiety in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Mol AUTISM. 2017;8:36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hollocks MJ, Lerh JW, Magiati I, Meiser-Stedman R, Brugha TS. Anxiety and depression in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2019;49:559–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Baiano C, Raimo G, Zappullo I, Cecere R, Rauso B, Positano M, et al. Anxiety sensitivity domains are differently affected by social and non-social autistic traits. J AUTISM Dev Disord. 2022;52:3486–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Jolliffe R, Adams D, Simpson K. Trait anxiety in individuals on the autism spectrum: a systematic review. Rev J AUTISM Dev Disord. 2023;10:523–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McArthur GE, Lee E, Laycock R. Autism traits and cognitive performance: Mediating roles of sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53:4560–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rosbrook A, Whittingham K. Autistic traits in the general population: What mediates the link with depressive and anxious symptomatology? Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2010;4:415–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kunihira Y, Senju A, Dairoku H, Wakabayashi A, Hasegawa T. ‘Autistic’ traits in non-autistic japanese populations: relationships with personality traits and cognitive ability. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:553–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kanne SM, Christ SE, Reiersen AM. Psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial difficulties in young adults with autistic traits. J AUTISM Dev Disord. 2009;39:827–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Galvin J, Howes A, McCarthy B, Richards G. Self-compassion as a mediator of the association between autistic traits and depressive/anxious symptomatology. Autism. 2021;25:502–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vally Z, Alghraibeh AM, Elhai JD. Severity of depression and anxiety in relation to problematic smartphone use in the United Arab Emirates: The mediational roles of rumination and fear of missing out. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2021;3:423–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang Y, Yang H, Montag C, Elhai JD. Boredom proneness and rumination mediate relationships between depression and anxiety with problematic smartphone use severity. Curr Psychol. 2022;41:5287–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Elhai JD, Levine JC, O’Brien KD, Armour C. Distress tolerance and mindfulness mediate relations between depression and anxiety sensitivity with problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;84:477–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wolniewicz CA, Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Boredom proneness and fear of missing out mediate relations between depression and anxiety with problematic smartphone use. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2020;2:61–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Elhai JD, Rozgonjuk D, Liu T, Yang H. Fear of missing out predicts repeated measurements of greater negative affect using experience sampling methodology. J Affect Disord. 2020;262:298–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lee S, Tam CL, Chie QT. Mobile phone usage preferences: The contributing factors of personality, social anxiety and loneliness. Soc Indic Res. 2014;118:1205–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Konan N, Celik OT. The Mediator Role of Interaction Anxiety in the Relationship between Social Support Perception and Smartphone Addiction. J Educ Future-EGITIM VE GELECEK Derg. 2019;:63–75.
  • 58.Elhai JD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. The relationship between anxiety symptom severity and problematic smartphone use: A review of the literature and conceptual frameworks. J ANXIETY Disord. 2019;62:45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Warrier V, Toro R, Won H, Leblond CS, Cliquet F, Delorme R, et al. Social and non-social autism symptoms and trait domains are genetically dissociable. Commun Biol. 2019;2:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Diamond A. Executive Functions. In: Fiske ST, editor. ANNUAL REVIEW OF PSYCHOLOGY, vol. 64. Palo Alto: Annual Reviews; 2013. p. 135–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41:49–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Demetriou EA, DeMayo MM, Guastella AJ. Executive function in autism spectrum disorder: history, theoretical models, empirical findings, and potential as an endophenotype. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Christ SE, Kanne SM, Reiersen AM. Executive function in individuals with subthreshold autism traits. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:590–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Mason LA, Zimiga BM, Anders-Jefferson R, Paap KR. Autism traits predict self-reported executive functioning deficits in everyday life and an aversion to exercise. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:2725–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tsai TH, Chen YL, Gau SSF. Relationships between autistic traits, insufficient sleep, and real-world executive functions in children: a mediation analysis of a national epidemiological survey. Psychol Med. 2021;51:579–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, Lai Z. Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of Autistic Disorder. J AUTISM Dev Disord. 2005;35:445–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bishop DVM, Norbury CF. Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology - I: Generativity. Autism. 2005;9:7–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Dichter GS, Lam KSL, Turner-Brown LM, Holtzclaw TN, Bodfish JW. Generativity abilities predict communication deficits but not repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:1298–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Van Eylen L, Boets B, Steyaert J, Wagemans J, Noens I. Executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders: influence of task and sample characteristics and relation to symptom severity. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24:1399–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Chen J, Liang Y, Mai C, Zhong X, Qu C. General deficit in inhibitory control of excessive smartphone users: Evidence from an event-related potential study. Front Psychol. 2016;7:511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Gao L, Zhang J, Xie H, Nie Y, Zhao Q, Zhou Z. Effect of the mobile phone-related background on inhibitory control of problematic mobile phone use: An event-related potentials study. Addict Behav. 2020;108:106363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ge J, Liu Y, Cao W, Zhou S. The relationship between anxiety and depression with smartphone addiction among college students: The mediating effect of executive dysfunction. Front Psychol. 2023;13:1033304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Hadlington LJ. Cognitive failures in daily life: Exploring the link with Internet addiction and problematic mobile phone use. Comput Hum Behav. 2015;51:75–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7:336–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Derakshan N, Ansari TL, Hansard M, Shoker L, Eysenck MW. Anxiety, inhibition, efficiency, and effectiveness an investigation using the antisaccade task. Exp Psychol. 2009;56:48–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Derakshan N, Smyth S, Eysenck MW. Effects of state anxiety on performance using a task-switching paradigm: An investigation of attentional control theory. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16:1112–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mitchell RLC, Phillips LH. The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:617–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dominguez-Salas S, Diaz-Batanero C, Martin Lozano-Rojas O, Verdejo-Garcia A. Impact of general cognition and executive function deficits on addiction treatment outcomes: Systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive pathways. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:772–801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wallace GL, Budgett J, Charlton RA. Aging and autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from the broad autism phenotype. Autism Res. 2016;9:1294–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Li L, Niu Z, Griffiths MD, Mei S. The smartphone addiction scale: Psychometric properties, invariance, network perspective, and latent profile analysis among a sample of Chinese university students. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2024;22:24–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. In: Weiner IB, Craighead WE, editors. The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. 1st ed. Wiley; 2010. p. 1–1. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Cheng C, He J, Yu Y, Zhong X, Li C, Zhang X, et al. Measurement invariance of Chinese version of State-Trait anxiety inventory form y-all databases. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2021;29:68–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Chan RCK. Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: A study with the use of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire. Br J Psychol. 2001;92:551–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Yang Z-Y, Sun S-F, Lui SSY, Shi H-S, Xie D-J, Xie W-L, et al. An attempt at revisiting the factor structure of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire in the Chinese setting. PSYCH J. 2018;7:25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Zhu J, Fan J, Gan J, Liu W, Zheng H, Lei H, et al. Application of Chinese version of the Dysexecutive questionnaire in obsessive-compulsive disorder-all databases. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2018;26:1127–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J-Y, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. 2003;88:879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Wen Z, Fang J, Xie J, Ouyang J. Methodological research on mediation effects in China’s mainland-All Databases. Methodol Res Mediat Eff China’s Mainl. 2022;30:1692–702. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zhou H, Long L. Statistical remedies for common method biases. Adv Psychol Sci. 2004;12:942–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Kardefelt-Winther D. A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Comput Hum Behav. 2014;31:351–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Li J, Zhan D, Zhou Y, Gao X. Loneliness and problematic mobile phone use among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: The roles of escape motivation and self-control. Addict Behav. 2021;118:106857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Freeth M, Bullock T, Milne E. The distribution of and relationship between autistic traits and social anxiety in a UK student population. Autism. 2013;17:571–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Liew SM, Thevaraja N, Hong RY, Magiati I. The relationship between autistic traits and social anxiety, worry, obsessive-compulsive, and depressive symptoms: specific and non-specific mediators in a student sample. J AUTISM Dev Disord. 2015;45:858–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Godoy PBG, Shephard E, Milosavljevic B, Johnson MH, Charman T. Brief Report: Associations Between Cognitive Control Processes and Traits of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Anxiety in Children at Elevated and Typical Familial Likelihood for ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:3001–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Coutelle R, Weiner L, Paasche C, Pottelette J, Bertschy G, Schröder CM, et al. Autism spectrum disorder and video games: restricted interests or addiction? Int J Ment Health Addict. 2022;20:2243–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Ge J, Liu Y, Cao W, Zhou S. The relationship between anxiety and depression with smartphone addiction among college students: The mediating e ect of executive dysfunction. Front Psychol. 2023;13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 96.Zhou Z, Zhu H, Li C, Wang J. Internet addictive Individuals share impulsivity and executive dysfunction with alcohol-dependent patients. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 97.Zhou Z, Zhou H, Zhu H. Working memory, executive function and impulsivity in Internet-addictive disorders: a comparison with pathological gambling. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2016;28:92–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Unsworth N, McMillan BD, Brewer GA, Spillers GJ. Everyday attention failures: An individual differences investigation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2012;38:1765–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Palmer CJ, Paton B, Enticott PG, Hohwy J. ‘Subtypes’ in the presentation of autistic traits in the general adult population. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:1291–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Elhai JD, Dvorak RD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. J Affect Disord. 2017;207:251–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Elhai JD, Yang H, Montag C. Cognitive- and emotion-related dysfunctional coping processes: Transdiagnostic mechanisms explaining depression and anxiety’s relations with problematic smartphone use. Curr Addict Rep. 2019;6:410–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Zhang L, Sun Y, Chen F, Wu D, Tang J, Han X, et al. Psychometric properties of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient in both clinical and non-clinical samples: Chinese version for mainland China. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.


Articles from BMC Psychology are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES