Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Dec 5;19(12):e0311905. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0311905

Uncovering the essence of moving experiences in Japanese culture: Development and validation of a kando reaction scale

Haruka Shoda 1,5,*, Shoko Yasuda 2,5, Ai Uemiya 3,5, Atsushi Yuhaku 4,5, Tadao Isaka 4,5
Editor: Shanjida Chowdhury6
PMCID: PMC11620437  PMID: 39636820

Abstract

Kando is a Japanese term referring to a spectrum of reactions associated with feeling moved experiences. This study developed an instrument to measure an individual’s degree of kando and its associated reactions and explored how Japanese people experience kando in their lives. As part of a large-scale survey, we analyzed data from 4,690 Japanese participants aged 20–69. In the survey, participants recalled and described their most significant kando events and rated their experiences on a provisional kando reaction scale consisting of 43 items. The results indicated that the most significant kando events could be grouped into eight clusters: family issues, interpersonal relationships, arts, sports, travel/nature, negative issues, achievements, and religion/disaster. Using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, we constructed a 33-item kando reaction scale with a correlated 11-factor structure. The degree of kando and its relevant reactions differed as functions of event clusters and population characteristics such as sex and age. For example, kando on family issues generated multiple, somewhat contrasting, responses such as happiness and hardship. Intense arousal when experiencing kando tended to increase depending on the individual’s age. Our kando reaction scale can be a useful instrument for describing and exploring psychological mechanisms relevant to kando.

Introduction

When we hold a newborn baby in our arms or see someone winning a sports event after overcoming a slump, we experience unique feelings that move us. In Japanese, this psychological phenomenon is known as “kando” [1]. Western countries have explored concepts relevant to kando in studies on kama muta, being moved, and awe. This study aimed to understand the structure of kando among the Japanese. By doing so, we provide a fundamental basis for exploring the universal and cross-cultural underpinnings of the experiences described as kando (in Japan), kama muta, being moved, and awe (in Western countries).

Psychological research on kando has been conducted in Japan since the late 1990s, including a series of studies by Tokaji (e.g., [2, 3]). Tokaji [3] described kando as “the state of being emotionally moved.” However, kando is not just a psychological phenomenon captured within the conventional framework of emotion research such as the basic emotion theory [4] and the dimensional theory of emotion [5]. Tokaji [3] argued that kando occurs in association with both positive (e.g., happiness and joy) and negative (e.g., sadness) emotions. For example, sad experiences associated with kando (e.g., bereavement and death) can be accompanied by positive psychological aspects that enable one to overcome poignant experiences. In addition, kando can be elicited at various intensities from subtle daily events (e.g., background music on the street) to major life accomplishments (e.g., marriage and childbirth) [6]. The degree of arousal in relation to kando ranges from weak to strong and people can experience kando with gentle tears or severe goosebumps.

When kando is experienced, physical reactions occur alongside the emotional responses. Previous studies [7] have demonstrated that kando while listening to music is accompanied by five physical reactions: “goosebumps,” “lump in the throat,” “shiver down the spine,” “tears,” and “arousal.” These psychophysiological reactions have been also reported in the studies of kama muta, being moved, and awe [8]. In the following section, we discuss these concepts, summarize the relevant literature, and identify their similarities and differences.

Kama muta is a Sanskrit word meaning “moved by love” [9]. Fiske et al. [9] suggested that kama muta occurs when communal sharing relationships (hereafter CSRs) with other people or things suddenly intensify. CSRs refer to relationships in which people feel a sense of connection through love, unity, fusion, harmony, patriotism, and identity. For example, when CSRs are rapidly strengthened by witnessing others’ kind behavior, kama muta can be experienced. In other words, the trigger for the kama muta experience is primarily prosocial or altruistic behavior [10].

Research on the experience of being moved among German nationals identified six event categories that trigger emotional responses: human relationships, significant life events, political and social events, nature-related events, art-related events, and the others (misc.) [11]. Theoretically, such experiences can occur when positive core values fundamentally important for humans are prominent (e.g., brotherhood, solidarity, peace, health, excellence, and beauty) [12]. In addition, Menninghaus et al. [11] showed that events evoking moving experiences are characterized by low-to-moderate levels of arousal, whereas the experience itself is powerful, regardless of the arousal level of the event.

Awe is a psychological state that occurs when one is exposed to a great work of art, intellectual inspiration, the beauty of nature, etc. [13]. It is characterized by its central concepts of “vastness” and “accommodation” [14]. Vastness refers not only to size in the physical sense but also to size in the social sense, including fame, authority, and prestige. In other words, vastness refers to one’s perception of the degree of greatness compared to the presence of the self or internal standards. Accommodation refers to the need to change and adapt one’s cognitive framework when confronting such an amazing entity [15]. Awe, which is a powerful emotion that can change one’s cognitive framework, arises from feelings experienced when encountering an extraordinary presence [16].

The concepts of kama muta, being moved, and awe broadly overlap with kando. For example, kando can be triggered by prosocial or altruistic behavior [17] in the same manner as kama muta. We sometimes experience and express kando as a strong psychological reaction such as being moved or feeling a sense of awe. However, Yasuda et al. [1] noted that kando differs from these concepts. For example, kando frequently occurs in contexts that do not involve social or interpersonal relationships such as listening to unfamiliar music or experiencing the majesty of nature. In addition, while the reactions to being moved and awe are powerful, kando occurs with a wide range of intensities, from gentle heartwarming kando to powerful heart wrenching kando. Therefore, despite some overlaps, it is more reasonable to regard kando as an umbrella concept that includes kama muta, being moved, and awe, as suggested by Yasuda et al. [1].

The present study bases its theory of kando on the work of Zickfeld et al. [18], who constructed the KAMMUS II (the kama muta multiplex scale) to measure kama muta, surveyed 19 countries, and found that kama muta is applicable across various cultures and languages. In the KAMMUS II, the “emotion labels” subscale consists of the items “It was heartwarming,” “I was moved,” and “I was touched,” which can be used to represent a general response to kama muta. Interestingly, in the KAMMUS II, the item “I was moved” was translated into Japanese using the word “kando”; however, the emotion labels score for the Japanese sample was the lowest among the countries in the study. One possible reason is that the concepts of being moved in English and kando in Japanese are not concordant. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct a more in-depth assessment of kando and comprehend its conceptual framework. This will enable us to determine whether the findings of Zickfeld et al. [18] were attributable to semantic discrepancies between being moved and kando or to differences in cognitive processes among participants from Japan and other countries.

As aforementioned, kando involves various concepts such as kama muta, being moved, and awe. An instrument that measures the experience of kando and the relevant concepts is required to confirm whether kando is an umbrella term that encompasses these concepts. Therefore, the primary purpose of this study is to construct a kando reaction scale (KRS) that measures the broad concept of kando, thereby enabling us to understand the characteristics and relevance of its concepts. The literature on kando among the Japanese population has focused on particular targets such as films [3], novels [19], and music [7]. However, the KRS should be comprehensively applied to broader situations. We would like to understand how the characteristics of kando vary according to the events and targets that trigger them in order to broaden the scope of kando that can be measured and consequently comprehend the “general” and “event-specific” components of kando in further work.

Our survey had three key aims. First, we sought to examine the types of events that Japanese people memorize as significant kando experiences. While we acknowledge that kando can be experienced through small, everyday occurrences, we addressed the participants’ “most significant kando events” in this study to focus on representative reactions to kando experiences. Second, we constructed a KRS to measure the degree of kando and the relevant reactions. Finally, we examined the KRS scores across different event categories and demographic factors, including generation and sex. Based on our findings, we developed a suitable instrument to describe and explore the mechanisms of action of kando.

Methods

This study was part of a large-scale research project comprising four quarterly surveys conducted from November 18, 2021 to September 6, 2022. All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The survey was approved by the Ethics Review Committee for Research Involving Human Subjects (Humanities and Social Sciences) of Ritsumeikan University (No. Kinugasa-Human-2021–84). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Participants

A total of 13,971 Japanese adults aged 20–69 years participated in one of the four surveys, for which generations (i.e., 20s to 60s) and sexes (i.e., men and women) were assigned equivalently. This study reported the data obtained from the third and fourth surveys, in which a complete set of questionnaire items was incorporated. A survey company conducted the surveys online and obtained data from 6,915 participants in the third (n = 3, 445) and fourth surveys (n = 3, 470).

Exclusion criteria

Some participants in online surveys may be careless because they answer questions without reading the instructions or items carefully (e.g., [20]). Therefore, we included four attention check items (e.g., “Please choose ‘3’ for this item.”) to determine whether participants responded appropriately. Participants with incorrect responses to any of the four items were excluded.

Furthermore, in the surveys, we asked participants to describe their memories of the most significant kando experiences (See the procedure for more details). Two assistants individually reviewed all of the descriptions and judged their relevance to kando. If an assistant judged the data to be irrelevant, the participants were excluded.

In addition, we asked the participants to choose categories from the 19 items that they felt best represented in their descriptions. We excluded participants who chose seven (i.e., M + 3SD) or more items because selecting an excessively large number of categories may not accurately represent a single event.

Characteristics of the study sample

Data from 2,308 participants (67.0%) in the third survey and 2,382 participants (68.6%) in the fourth survey (N = 4, 690) were analyzed. The participants ranged in age from 20 to 69 years (M = 45.80, SD = 13.73), and there were 2,049 men and 2,641 women. Table 1 provides detailed descriptive statistics of the participants’ ages across sexes and generations.

Table 1. Characteristics of the study sample (N = 4, 690).
Generation Sex n Age
M (SD)
20s Men 354 25.64 (2.73)
Women 449 25.51 (2.76)
30s Men 375 35.19 (2.82)
Women 524 34.55 (2.84)
40s Men 422 45.03 (2.92)
Women 548 44.72 (2.79)
50s Men 410 54.70 (2.86)
Women 563 53.81 (2.81)
60s Men 488 64.27 (2.88)
Women 557 63.49 (2.70)
Total 4690 45.80 (13.73)

Measures

Kando reaction scale

The provisional KRS consists of 43 items and was created in Japanese. S1 Table lists all of the items and their English equivalents. The items included general reactions when experiencing kando (e.g., “I felt kando,” “It stirred my heart,” “It touched my heart”) and adjectives representing emotions (e.g., “It was pleasant,” “I was excited,” “I was happy,” “I was sad”). In addition, considering that the transition from negative/neutral to positive affective states could be relevant to kando [1], we included items representing affective transitions (e.g., “While it was happening, I had anxieties and worries, but they were gone by the end”). We also included subjective and physiological reactions reported in the previous studies of kando [7] and kama muta [18]. Examples of items related to lacrimation are “I cried” and “I was close to tears”; items related to pilomotor reflex are “I had goosebumps” and “I felt a shiver”; and items related to emotional warmth included “There was a warm sensation in my chest” and “It was heartwarming.” Furthermore, regarding kando-related concepts [1], we included items related to awe (e.g., “I felt awe”) and spiritual experiences (“I felt a mysterious power”).

KAMMUS II

To examine the validity of the KRS, we also included items from the KAMMUS II [18]. The KAMMUS II originally consisted of five sections: sensations (e.g., “moist eyes”), appraisal (e.g., “I observed an incredible bond”), motivation (e.g., “I wanted to hug someone”), valence (e.g., “positive feelings”), and emotion labels (“heartwarming”). However, some of the kando events might not be relevant to interpersonal bonds or relationships. In this case, the appraisal and motivation items could not be answered; hence, we excluded them from our survey. The remaining 16 KAMMUS II items overlapped with the provisional KRS because they included the concept of kama muta. We translated the KAMMUS II (English version) by ourselves, while Zickfeld et al. [18] provided the Japanese version of KAMMUS II. This is because “being moved” was translated as “kando” for the Japanese equivalent, which was inappropriate considering our conceptualization of kando. The validity of the translation was confirmed using a professional back-translation service. Owing to this back-translation process, the English expressions in S1 Table differ from the original English version of the KAMMUS II.

Procedure

First, we asked the participants to recall the most significant kando events in their lives. Specifically, we asked them to describe a particular episode and its reactions in over 300 Japanese letters (Detailed analyses of episodes and reactions will be reported in another study). Participants then selected one or more item(s) from the following categories to describe the event: “marriage or romantic relationships,” “pregnancy/childbirth,” “childcare,” “family,” “animals/pets,” “illness/accidents,” “bereavement,” “examination,” “job acquiring,” “sport,” “literary works,” “visual works/arts (including cinemas and television programs),” “music,” “nature/universe,” “disasters,” “travel/cross-cultural experience,” “religion,” “destiny,” and “others (with additional description of details).” These categories were chosen according to the current authors’ discussion based on the categories of Maeura et al. [15], who compared the experiences of awe and kando. Subsequently, we asked the participants to complete 13 questionnaires, including the KRS and KAMMUS II. The remaining questionnaires, which mainly examined psychological and subjective well-being, and cultural aspects of human cognition, were used for other studies. The KRS and KAMMUS II were rated on a 7-point Likert scale with the following instruction: “We would like to ask you about what you felt in connection to the most significant kando events you have experienced. Please rate the extent to which you felt the following feelings between 1 and 7, with one being ‘Did not feel at all’ and seven being ‘Incredibly strong.”’ The survey was projected to be completed within approximately 15 minutes.

Data analysis

First, we analyzed the categories selected as participants’ most significant kando events. Participants were given the option to choose one or more categories, and their responses were recorded as either 0 or 1. Specifically, a value of 1 was assigned to participants who chose a category, and a value of 0 was assigned to those who did not. We calculated the Jaccard distance between the selected event categories based on coding; that is, the dissimilarity statistic between two binary variables (0 or 1). We conducted a hierarchical cluster analysis (Ward’s method) using Jaccard distances to group similar events, which were then aggregated according to generation and sex.

Next, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to refine the KRS items. To avoid performing the EFA and CFA on the same sample, we randomly divided the sample (N = 4, 690) into two subsamples where the two subsamples had comparable distributions of generations (i.e., 20s–60s) and sexes (men and women). Before performing EFA, we estimated the number of factors using a parallel analysis with squared multiple correlations. This allowed us to estimate the possible maximum number of factors [21] by considering the complex multifaceted nature of kando. We then conducted an EFA using the maximal likelihood method and calculated oblimin solutions (oblique rotation). Among the items for which neither factor loading scored > .40, the item with the maximum complexity value was omitted to achieve a simple factor structure. This procedure was repeated until the maximum pattern value for each item reached > .40. We also examined the internal consistency of each factor by computing Cronbach’s α coefficient. The EFA was conducted using the psych package [22] in R.

To examine the KRS factor structure in detail, we conducted a CFA in which we compared four CFA models examined in recent studies (e.g., [23]): (a) one-factor, (b) correlated factors, (c) second-order, and (d) bifactor models (Fig 1). As shown in the schematic (Fig 1), the one-factor model (a) is based on the hypothesis that one general factor (g) determines all items. Correlated-factor model (b) indicates that the correlated factors determine the items. The items assigned to each factor were based on the factor patterns determined by the EFA. The second-order model (c) hypothesizes that uncorrelated factors are determined by a second-order general factor (g). The bifactor model (d) also hypothesized uncorrelated factors, but the general factor (g) loaded onto all the items. Information criteria and fit indices were compared to determine the best-fit model. The cut-off criteria for acceptable fit were based on Hu and Bentler [24]: RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation) < .06, SRMR (standardized root mean squared residual) < .08, CFI (comparative fit index) > .95, and TLI (Tucker Lewis index) > .95. We assessed the validity of the KRS by examining its correlation with the KAMMUS II.

Fig 1. Schematic pictures for four confirmatory factor analysis models.

Fig 1

Error variables not depicted.

Finally, we analyzed the impact of the cluster of kando events on the KRS factor scores to gain insights into the nature of kando across various events. As an additional analysis, we examined the effects of participants’ generation and sex on the factor scores to determine whether the degree of kando differed depending on the participant demographics. The dataset used in the analyses is available in S1 Dataset.

Results

Clustering of most significant kando events

First, we conducted hierarchical cluster analysis (using the Jaccard distance and Ward method) for the categories chosen as the participants’ most significant kando events (hereafter called “event categories,” S1 Fig). Based on the dendrogram (Fig 2), we obtained eight clusters, which we named based on the type of event categories: “family issues,” “interpersonal relationships,” “arts,” “sport,” “travel/nature,” “negative issues,” “achievement,” and “religion/disaster.”

Fig 2. The dendrogram obtained by hierarchical cluster analysis for event categories.

Fig 2

We created a cross-table (Table 2) to examine how the distribution of clusters differed according to population characteristics (i.e., generation and sex). In this table, clusters are sorted according to their total frequencies. We conducted a χ2 test for each cluster to examine the distribution differences among the ten groups (i.e., the combination of five generations and two sexes). Here, we show the categories in each cluster along with the results of χ2 tests and subsequent residual analyses.

Table 2. Cross-tabulation of event clusters by generation (20s to 60s) and sex (M for male, W for female): Results of χ2 test and residual analysis.

Event Cluster 20s 30s 40s 50s 60s Total
M W M W M W M W M W M W
Family Issues 47 149 110 262+ 134 255+ 109 259+ 158 261+ 558 1186
Interpersonal Relationships 119+ 142+ 100 139 95 107 110 125 132 132 556 645
Arts 67 122+ 72 81 55 80 64 78 59 75 317 436
Sport 79+ 21 78+ 27 83+ 47 98+ 54 103+ 42 441 191
Travel/Nature 38 37 41 31 45 78 55 92+ 72 100+ 251 338
Negative Issues 37 39 29 42 42 65 44 92+ 64 88+ 216 326
Achievement 49+ 34 28 31 40 39 41 26 43 29 201 159
Religion/ Disaster 5 0 5 5 9 5 9 10 15+ 10 43 30
Others 21 24 19 18 32 23 28 21 32 26 132 112

significantly less (p < .05); +significantly more (p < .05)

The “family issues” cluster (i.e., “family,” “childcare,” and “pregnancy/childbirth”) was chosen the most frequently (27.2% of men and 44.9% of women). The χ2 test showed a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 227.88, p < .001. Furthermore, the residual analysis revealed that there were significantly more women aged 30 years or older in this cluster than the other population groups.

The next cluster (“interpersonal relationships”) was composed of “destiny,” “friendship,” and “marriage/romantic relationships” (27.1% of men and 24.4% of women). In this context, the category of “destiny” refers to accidental, fortuitous, or serendipitous encounters with someone important. Thus, we named this cluster “interpersonal relationships.” The χ2 test found a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 39.02, p < .001. According to the residual analysis, men and women in their 20s belonged to this cluster significantly more than the other population groups.

The “literary works,” “visual works (including cinemas and television programs),” and “music” comprised an “art” cluster. The χ2 test showed a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 60.49, p < .001. The residual analysis indicated that significantly more women in their 20s were in this cluster than the other population groups.

The “sport” category comprised a cluster on its own. The χ2 test showed a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 213.80, p < .001. According to the residual analysis, men of all ages belonged to this cluster significantly more frequently than women.

The categories of “nature/universe” and “travel/cross-cultural experience” comprised a “travel/nature” cluster. The χ2 test showed a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4690) = 57.95, p < .001. The residual analysis showed that women in their 50s and 60s belonged to this cluster significantly more often than the other population groups.

The “negative issues” cluster comprised “bereavements,” “illness/accidents,” and “animal/pets.” We found a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 40.80, p < .001. The residual analysis showed that women in their 50s and 60s belonged to this cluster more often than the other population groups.

The “achievement” cluster was comprised of “job acquiring” and “examination.” The χ2 test yielded a significant association between frequency and group, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 39.72, p < .001. The residual analysis showed that men in their 20s were significantly more common in this cluster than in other population groups.

Finally, the “religion/disaster” category was identified. Japan frequently experiences natural disasters such as large earthquakes and typhoons. These natural disasters are strong forces that influence the lives of the Japanese people and can be connected to religious thought. The frequency of this cluster showed a significant association with the groups χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 19.72, p = .02. According to the residual analysis, men in their 60s belonged to this cluster significantly more often than the other population groups.

Participants who were not assigned to any cluster were labeled “others” (5.2% [n = 243]). Examples of “others” included “volunteer,” “birthday,” “computer games,” “job,” and “hobbies,” hobbies. The association between the frequency of “others” and groups was not significant, χ2(9, N = 4, 690) = 16.52, p = .06.

In sum, eight clusters were identified for the participants’ most significant kando events. The responses to each cluster differed as a function of the participants’ sex and generation. Sex differences were found in broad generations for the “family issues” and “sport” clusters, which were significantly higher for women (except those in their 20s) and men, respectively. Women of younger ages (20s) responded more to the “arts,” whereas men in this generation did so for “achievements.” Women from the older generations (50s and 60s) responded more to “travel/nature” and “negative issues,” and men in their 60s responded more to the “religion/disaster” cluster.

Exploratory factor analyses

EFA was conducted to refine the provisional version of the KRS with 43 items. In EFA, we used the first subsample (n = 2, 345) chosen from all the samples (N = 4, 690). We used a parallel analysis with squared multiple correlations, whereby the number of factors is estimated to be 11. The final pattern matrices are listed in Table 3. The fit indices showed a good fit: χ2 = 196.81, p = .87, RMSR = .01, TLI = .98, RMSEA = .024 (90% CI of the RMSEA [.022,.027]).

Table 3. Pattern matrix computed by exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and standardized coefficients computed by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) based on the correlated 11-factor model.

Pattern Matrix by Exploratory Factor Analysis
F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 F7 F8 F9 F10 F11 H2a Comb CFAc
F1 Positive Emotions (α = .91 [.90,.91])
P1 .90 .01 -.01 .01 .01 -.01 .01 .03 .00 .04 .02 .86 1.01 .90
P2 .80 -.01 -.04 .05 .02 .07 .07 -.02 .04 -.02 .02 .77 1.05 .88
P3 .67 .11 .01 -.01 .01 .16 .00 .01 -.06 .01 .02 .72 1.20 .85
F2 Exhilaration (α = .84 [.83,.85])
E1 -.04 .83 -.04 .04 .00 .00 -.02 .05 .04 .00 .01 .73 1.02 .79
E2 -.01 .73 .04 .03 .02 .01 .07 .03 -.01 .00 .01 .61 1.04 .70
E3 .29 .49 -.03 -.08 -.02 .11 -.11 .05 -.08 .07 .09 .60 2.17 .75
E4 .19 .45 .07 .02 .05 -.08 .23 .01 -.04 .15 -.10 .50 2.54 .61
E5 .21 .43 -.13 .06 -.08 .16 .00 .07 .06 -.07 .07 .56 2.39 .75
F3 Hardship (α = .78 [.77,.80])
H1 .06 -.02 .77 .02 -.01 .00 .08 -.05 -.03 .05 -.03 .62 1.06 .75
H2 .00 -.01 .77 .01 -.03 -.01 -.09 .08 .08 -.01 .00 .61 1.08 .74
H3 -.06 .03 .57 -.14 .04 -.03 -.07 -.01 .06 -.06 .09 .42 1.31 .66
H4 -.18 -.01 .55 .04 .06 -.03 .24 -.03 .00 -.04 .01 .53 1.67 .74
F4 kando (α = .80 [.78,.81])
K1 -.01 .08 .01 .68 .03 .09 -.01 -.01 .01 .03 .02 .62 1.07 .77
K2 -.02 -.06 -.05 .50 .06 .16 -.06 .14 .04 .01 .09 .52 1.57 .71
K3 .31 -.04 -.05 .49 .08 .01 .02 .09 .01 -.01 -.05 .53 1.93 .67
K4 -.09 .11 .01 .42 .05 .17 .05 .00 .01 .07 .09 .40 1.84 .61
K5 .12 .13 .01 .41 .04 -.03 .05 .02 .02 .08 .15 .43 1.86 .59
F5 Tears (α = .88 [.88,.89])
Te1 .01 .01 -.01 -.01 .98 -.05 -.01 -.02 .01 -.01 -.01 .90 1.01 .87
Te2 .01 -.01 .00 .03 .79 .10 -.01 .04 -.02 .02 .00 .74 1.04 .91
F6 Warmth (α = .83 [.82,.84])
W1 .02 .01 -.02 .05 .03 .78 .02 -.01 .00 .05 -.02 .73 1.02 .83
W2 .08 .01 .00 .05 .03 .74 .00 -.01 -.03 .02 -.01 .69 1.04 .85
F7 Overcome (α = .72 [.70,.74])
O1 .01 .02 -.03 .01 .00 .00 .84 .03 .00 -.02 .02 .71 1.01 .80
O2 .08 .01 .09 .01 .00 .02 .70 -.02 .01 .09 -.06 .61 1.11 .80
O3 -.06 .01 .02 -.11 .07 .17 .42 .06 .09 -.13 .22 .31 2.62 .45
F8 Goosebumps (α = .68 [.65,.70])
G1 .01 -.01 .01 .06 .04 .00 .00 .74 -.05 .06 .01 .61 1.04 .72
G2 .04 .13 -.02 -.03 -.05 -.05 .03 .65 .07 .03 .03 .57 1.16 .75
G3 -.17 -.05 .15 -.03 .12 .10 .09 .41 .13 -.05 .06 .35 2.58 .41
F9 Awe (α = .72 [.69,.74])
A1 .04 -.02 .10 -.02 -.01 -.02 -.01 -.02 .81 .01 -.02 .70 1.04 .84
A2 -.03 .04 -.11 .05 .02 .00 .02 .02 .70 .05 .01 .51 1.09 .68
F10 Transcendence (α = .70 [.67,.72])
Tr1 .02 -.02 -.01 -.02 .02 .09 -.01 .07 .08 .68 -.01 .57 1.09 .68
Tr2 -.01 .10 .01 .12 .03 .04 .05 .12 .06 .50 .08 .57 1.48 .77
F11 Surprise (α = .58 [.54,.61])
S1 .13 .02 .01 .09 -.04 -.05 -.03 .12 .02 -.01 .57 .47 1.29 .54
S2 -.04 .01 .06 .07 .07 -.02 .06 -.02 .02 .38 .45 .52 2.18 .73
Inter-factor Correlationsd
F1
F2 .54
F3 -.28 -.15
F4 .36 .28 -.16
F5 .07 .03 .09 .40
F6 .57 .30 -.20 .57 .44
F7 .17 .28 .34 .12 .24 .17
F8 .19 .50 .01 .38 .25 .17 .09
F9 -.11 .08 .36 .11 .09 -.03 .11 .35
F10 .29 .30 .10 .37 .26 .30 .19 .36 .28
F11 .10 .26 .08 .28 .12 .17 .06 .44 .24 .27

Cronbach’s α values with 95% confidence intervals are also shown.

aH2 indicates commonality.

bCom indicates complexity.

cCFA indicates the standardized coefficients computed by confirmatory factor analysis.

dInter-factor correlations were computed among factor scores using exploratory factor analysis.

In the following, we describe each item using a combination of the first letter of the factor name and the number (e.g., item 1 of the “positive emotions” factor will be described as “P1”). The list of items is presented in English along with their Japanese originals in S1 Table.

F1 (positive emotions) consisted of P1 (I was joyful), P2 (I was happy), and P3 (I was smiling), indicating positive experiences with the kando. F2 (exhilaration) was also positive, but its meaning implied a transition from a negative or neutral mood to a positive one: E1 (I felt exhilarated), E2 (I felt refreshed), E3 (I was cheerful and my feelings were elevated), E4 (I felt a sense of accomplishment), and E5 (It was pleasant). By contrast, F3 (hardship) consisted of the following negative emotions: H1 (I felt anxious), H2 (I felt scared), H3 (It was unpleasant), and H4 (It was emotionally difficult). F4 (kando) reflects the general reaction of kando: K1 (It stirred my heart), K2 (It touched my heart), K3 (I felt kando), K4 (There was some manner of sensation in my chest), and K5 (It was even more moving than initially expected). F5 (tears) indicated physical reactions relevant to tears: Te1 (I cried) and Te2 (I was close to the tears). F6 (warmth) was a construct of heart-warming experiences: W1 (There was a warm sensation in my chest) and W2 (It was heartwarming). F7 (overcome) directly denoted the negative-to-positive changes during an event: O1 (While it was happening, it was difficult and painful, but in the end, it was positive), O2 (While it was happening, I had anxieties and worries, but they were gone by the end), and O3 (Sadness changed to joy). F8 (goosebumps) indicated strong bodily reactions: G1 (I had goosebumps), G2 (I shivered), and G3 (I lost my breath). F9 (awe) reflected the experience of awe: A1 (I felt awe) and A2 (I felt veneration). F10 (transcendence) indicated the experience of a higher power exerting its influence: Tr1 (I felt a mysterious power) and Tr2 (I felt a great power). Finally, F11 (surprise) consisted of two items indicating surprise reactions: S1 (I was surprised) and S2 (It was beyond my understanding).

We also calculated the Cronbach’s α coefficients as internal consistency indices (see Table 3) and found that nine of the 11 factors satisfied the cutoff criterion of α > .70 as a conventional rule of thumb (e.g., [25]). However, for F8 (goosebumps) and F11 (surprise), the α values were lower than.70, perhaps because the α coefficients tended to underestimate the internal consistency of the scales consisting of only a few items. In the content analysis, G1 (I had goosebumps), G2 (I shivered), and G3 (I lost my breath) represented strong bodily reactions when experiencing kando. The nuances seem to be independent of other factors that measure bodily reactions: F5 (tears) and F6 (warmth). S1 (I was surprised) and S2 (It was beyond my understanding) represented experiences that surpassed participants’ initial expectations. This can be interpreted as internally consistent, expressing a surprise reaction when experiencing a kando. The items for the goosebump and surprise factors seem to be internally consistent in their nuances. Therefore, these factors should be included as independent variables in the KRS.

Confirmatory factor analysis and correlations with the KAMMUS II

CFAs were conducted under the four models shown in Fig 1 using the second subsample of 2,345 participants to examine the validity of the EFA factor structure. We computed the information criteria and fit indices for each model (see Table 4). According to the information criteria (Akaike Information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information criterion (BIC)), the correlated 11-factor model indicated the best fit among the four models. For this model (Fig 1b, Table 4b), RMSEA (< .06) and SRMR (< .08) indicated a good fit, whereas the CFI (< .95) and TLI (< .95) indicated an unsatisfactory fit. However, as Perry et al. [26] showed, CFI and TLI are unlikely to meet these criteria when applying a complex model. We believe that the correlated 11-factor model is acceptable for our data and satisfies the combined rule of RMSEA < .06 and SRMR < .09 [24].

Table 4. Fit indices and information criteria for four confirmatory factor analysis models.

χ 2 χ2/df RMSEA [90%CI] CFI TLI SRMR AIC BIC
(a) One-factor 20,892 1.29 .12 [.11,.12] .43 .39 .14 290,212 290,592
(b) Correlated 11-factor 3,844 1.27 .05 [.05,.05] .90 .89 .06 273,274 273971
(c) Second-order factor 7,534 1.26 .07 [.07,.07] .80 .79 .11 276,875 277,319
(d) Bifactor 7,122 1.24 .07 [.07,.07] .81 .79 .11 276,507 277,078

Due to the large sample size of this study, we examined the possible biasness of the CFA model. We conducted CFA for the correlated 11-factor model and computed goodness-of-fit indices (Table 5) for each of the ten population groups (i.e., men and women for the age groups of 20s–60s). As shown in Table 5, the fit indices were mostly equivalent to the CFA model for the overall data (see Table 4b), suggesting the least model biasness.

Table 5. Fit indices computed for each of the population groups (generation and sex).

M for men and W for women.

RMSEA [90%CI] CFI TLI SRMR
20s M .06 [.06,.07] .89 .86 .07
W .06 [.06,.07] .89 .87 .07
30s M .06 [.06,.07] .89 .87 .07
W .06 [.06,.07] .89 .86 .07
40s M .06 [.05,.06] .91 .89 .06
W .07 [.06,.07] .87 .84 .08
50s M .06 [.05,.06] .91 .90 .07
W .06 [.05,.06] .91 .90 .06
60s M .06 [.06,.07] .89 .87 .07
W .06 [.06,.07] .89 .87 .07

Next, we examined the validity of the KRS by computing the Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the KRS and KAMMUS II. Hereafter, we report analyses using the merged sample (N = 4, 690). The KRS scores were computed as factor scores under the 11-factor CFA model (see S2 Table for KRS inter-factor correlations). For KAMMUS II, we computed subscale scores by calculating the mean values of the member items for each factor. Owing to the large sample size (N = 4, 690), even negligible correlation coefficients were significant; therefore, we do not report the coefficients’ significance tests here. In the following, we regard |r| > .40 as a substantial correlation under the assumption that these two questionnaires are aimed at measuring the similar concepts of kando and kama muta. Note that all 16 items of the KAMMUS II overlapped with those of the KRS; therefore, some of the correlation coefficients were very close to 1.00 when the factors shared the same items.

Table 6 shows Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the KRS factor scores and KAMMUS II subscale scores. The kando factor (F4) showed positive correlations with all but the choke-up (r = .36) subscales in KAMMUS II, indicating that the kando factor is generally relevant to the reactions measured by KAMMUS II. Similarly, the KAMMUS II emotion labels score (i.e., the composition of “being moved,” “being touched,” and “heartwarming”) correlated with most factors in the KRS, except hardship, overcome, goosebumps, and awe.

Table 6. Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the KRS and the KAMMUS II.

KRS KAMMUS II
Sensations
Tears Chills Warmth Choked up Exhilaration Valence Emotion label
F1 Positive emotions .15 .24 .53 -.08 .84 .91 .54
F2 Exhilaration .09 .55 .45 .12 .87 .64 .46
F3 Hardship .09 .01 -.17 .33 -.32 -.35 -.24
F4 Kando .52 .46 .79 .36 .57 .50 .91
F5 Tears .99 .23 .47 .40 .17 .16 .50
F6 Warmth .47 .17 .82 .12 .63 .65 .84
F7 Overcome .28 .17 .28 .23 .36 .28 .23
F8 Goosebumps .29 .94 .33 .56 .44 .20 .39
F9 Awe .06 .36 .03 .42 -.04 -.14 .04
F10 Transcendence .36 .56 .53 .45 .50 .36 .58
F11 Surprise .29 .59 .44 .51 .39 .25 .50

Values in bold indicate |r| > .40.

In terms of subscale correlations, the KRS scores for positive feelings measured by positive emotions (F1), exhilaration (F2), and warmth (F6) were highly correlated with those in the KAMMUS II (i.e., warmth, exhilaration, and valence), as initially predicted. KRS exhilaration (F2) and warmth (F6) also correlated with the KAMMUS II chills and tears, respectively. Regarding subjective bodily reactions, KRS tears (F5) is highly correlated with KAMMUS II tears, warmth, and choked up, whereas KRS goosebumps (F8) was highly correlated with KAMMUS II chills, choked up, and exhilaration. KRS awe (F9) was correlated only with the KAMMUS II choke-up. KRS transcendence and surprise exhibited similar tendencies and were highly correlated with KAMMUS II chills, warmth, and choked up. KRS transcendence also showed a high correlation with the KAMMUS II exhilaration. In contrast, the KRS hardship (F3) and overcome (F7) factors were not highly correlated with any of the KAMMUS II factors. This indicates that these factors are not necessarily concordant with positive feelings (i.e., warmth, exhilaration, valence) or other factors in kama muta.

We developed the KRS with 33 items under the 11 correlated factors model, which showed an acceptable fit to the current data. Some of the factor scores showed high correlations with the relevant scales in kama muta (KAMMUS II), demonstrating the validity of the KRS in measuring the concept of kando regarding feeling moved. In contrast, the KRS hardship, overcome, and awe factors were not highly correlated with the KAMMUS II, indicating differences between the KRS and the KAMMUS II. Furthermore, the KRS transcendence and surprise factors were not included in the KAMMUS II, and these factors showed broad correlations with multiple subscales in the KAMMUS II. This indicates that the KRS extends the KAMMUS II and can be used to measure the degree of kando and its relevant reactions, including kama muta, awe, and being moved.

The KRS factor scores across event clusters

We examined how the KRS factor scores differed as a function of event clusters. We computed the mean values and their 95% confidence intervals to examine the scores across the different event clusters (Fig 3). Note that participants belonged to multiple event clusters; for example, some participants could be assigned to the “family issues” and “arts” clusters when describing their children’s events relevant to music. In other words, the data for different event clusters were not independent; therefore, an analysis of variance (ANOVAs) could not be performed. Alternatively, we conducted one-sample t-tests against “0” for each of the event clusters because the factor score is relative to the grand mean (“0”). To adjust for the possibility of a Type I error, we treated α = .01 as statistically significant. We primarily reported the KRS factor scores that were significantly higher than the grand mean (“0”) in Table 7.

Fig 3. Mean factor scores of each of the KRS factors computed for each event cluster.

Fig 3

Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of means.

Table 7. Results of one-sample t-tests for the KRS factor scores for each event cluster.

KRS Interpersonal
Family Issues Relationship Arts
(n = 1744) (n = 1201) (n = 753)
t p d t p d t p d
F1 Positive emotions 11.25** <.001 0.27 2.09 .04 0.06 -8.57** <.001 -0.37
F2 Exhilaration -2.33 .02 -0.06 -1.23 .22 -0.04 -2.11 .04 -0.35
F3 Hardship 4.48** <.001 0.11 -2.18 .03 -0.06 -3.52** <.001 -0.45
F4 kando 6.12** <.001 0.15 0.45 .66 0.01 3.18* .002 0.03
F5 Tears 13.84** <.001 0.33 2.07 .04 0.06 3.06* .002 0.24
F6 Warmth 15.73** <.001 0.38 4.53** <.001 0.13 -4.37** <.001 -0.08
F7 Overcome 10.85** <.001 0.26 -0.01 .99 -0.00 -10.63** <.001 -0.23
F8 Goosebumps -8.62** <.001 -0.21 -4.65** <.001 -0.13 9.44** <.001 0.05
F9 Awe -2.27 .02 -0.05 -4.62** <.001 -0.13 3.51** <.001 0.46
F10 Transcendence 7.21** <.001 0.17 -4.45** <.001 -0.13 0.07 .95 0.18
F11 Surprise 1.46 .14 0.04 -2.77* .006 -0.08 2.14 0.03 0.23
KRS Sport Nature Negative Issues
(n = 632) (n = 589) (n = 542)
t p d t p d t p d
F1 Positive emotions 3.40 .001 0.14 -0.47 .64 -0.02 -10.07** <.001 -0.43
F2 Exhilaration 12.84** <.001 0.51 6.18 <.001 0.26 -10.70** <.001 -0.46
F3 Hardship -4.34** <.001 -0.17 -4.36 <.001 -0.18 9.41** <.001 0.40
F4 kando 2.12 .03 0.08 -1.15 .25 -0.05 -3.83** <.001 -0.17
F5 Tears -1.10 .27 -0.04 -12.40** <.001 -0.51 3.74** <.001 0.16
F6 Warmth -3.41* .001 -0.14 -6.36** <.001 -0.26 -4.40** <.001 -0.19
F7 Overcome 4.05** <.001 0.16 -8.51** <.001 -0.35 3.37* .001 0.15
F8 Goosebumps 16.26** <.001 0.65 6.45** <.001 0.27 -6.12** <.001 -0.26
F9 Awe 1.92 .06 0.08 7.51** <.001 0.31 3.87** <.001 0.17
F10 Transcendence 3.16* .002 0.13 5.27** <.001 0.22 -1.98 .05 -0.09
F11 Surprise 4.46** <.001 0.18 5.81** <.001 0.24 -0.50 .62 -0.02
KRS Achievement Disaster Others
(n = 360) (n = 73) (n = 244)
t p d t p d t p d
F1 Positive emotions 3.85** <.001 0.20 -3.18* .002 -0.37 -0.13 .89 -0.01
F2 Exhilaration 7.38** <.001 0.39 -2.98* .004 -0.35 1.63 .10 0.10
F3 Hardship 1.38 .17 0.07 3.87** <.001 0.45 -1.89 .06 -0.12
F4 kando -4.90** <.001 -0.26 0.26 .80 0.03 -3.81** <.001 -0.24
F5 Tears -3.94** <.001 -0.21 2.01 .05 0.24 -6.06** <.001 -0.39
F6 Warmth -3.53** <.001 -0.19 -0.70 .49 -0.08 -4.23** <.001 -0.27
F7 Overcome 11.86** <.001 0.63 1.92 .06 0.23 -3.03* .003 -0.19
F8 Goosebumps -0.55 .58 -0.03 -0.42 .67 -0.05 -0.53 .60 -0.03
F9 Awe -3.30* .001 -0.17 3.90** <.001 0.46 -2.10 .04 -0.13
F10 Transcendence -4.89** <.001 -0.26 1.52 .13 0.18 -5.21** <.001 -0.33
F11 Surprise -4.89** <.001 -0.26 1.99 .05 0.23 -3.04* .003 -0.20

*p < .01,

**p < .001

For the “family issues” cluster, higher scores than the grand mean were shown broadly in positive emotions, hardship, kando, tears, warmth, overcome, and transcendence factors. In contrast, the “interpersonal relationships” cluster scored significantly high only for the warmth factor. For the “arts” cluster, the scores for kando, tears, goosebumps, and awe were significantly high. The “sport” cluster also scored significantly high in a variety of factors: positive emotions, exhilaration, overcome, goosebumps, transcendence, and surprise. The “travel/nature” cluster showed similar tendencies in that the cluster scored significantly high for exhilaration, goosebumps, transcendence, and surprise; this cluster also scored high for the awe factor. In the “negative issues” cluster, the scores were significantly high for hardship, tears, overcome, and awe. The “achievement” cluster was similar to the “sport” and “travel/nature” in that the cluster scored significantly high for positive emotions, exhilaration, and overcome. The “religion/disaster” cluster exhibited high scores for hardship and awe. None of the factors were significantly high for “others.” In summary, we demonstrate that KRS factors can differentiate participants’ experiences among different event clusters, indicating that KRS can describe the characteristics of kando for different life events.

KRS factor scores across populational characteristics

Finally, we examined how KRS factor scores differed according to the participants’ generation and sex. Fig 4 illustrates the mean values of each KRS factor score by plotting the interactions between generation and sex. We conducted two-way between-subjects ANOVAs with generation and sex as independent variables and each KRS factor score as a dependent variable (Table 8). We also regarded α = .01 as the significance level because of the large sample size (N = 4, 690).

Fig 4. Mean factor scores of the KRS factors across age and sex.

Fig 4

Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of means.

Table 8. Results of analyses of variance for the KRS factor scores dealing the sex and generation as between-subject factors.

KRS Generation Sex Generation × Sex
F(4, 4, 680) p ηp2 F(1, 4, 680) p ηp2 F(4, 4, 680) p ηp2
F1 Positive emotions 5.51** <.001 .005 31.21** <.001 .007 0.82 .51 <.001
F2 Exhilaration 10.50** <.001 .009 9.63* .002 .002 1.06 .38 <.001
F3 Hardship 3.74* .005 .003 3.16 .08 <.001 1.62 .17 .001
F4 kando 2.75 .03 .002 34.79** <.001 .007 0.59 .67 <.001
F5 Tears 1.82 .12 .002 123.56** <.001 .026 0.84 .50 <.001
F6 Warmth 1.31 .27 .001 131.52** <.001 .027 0.54 .71 <.001
F7 Overcome 1.32 .26 .001 11.42** <.001 .002 1.31 .26 .001
F8 Goosebumps 7.78** <.001 .007 57.69** <.001 .012 0.56 .69 <.001
F9 Awe 24.37** <.001 .020 20.05** <.001 .006 1.80 .05 .002
F10 Transcendence 11.64** <.001 .010 14.64** <.001 .003 1.55 .18 .001
F11 Surprise 9.75** <.001 .008 0.67 .41 <.001 0.89 .47 <.001

*p < .01,

**p < .001

The ANOVAs showed that the two-way interactions were not significant (ps > .01) for either factor. The main effects of generation were significant for the factors of positive emotions (F1), exhilaration (F2), hardship (F3), goosebumps (F8), awe (F9), transcendence (F10), and surprise (F11). However, the effect sizes were small or negligible (ηp2.01), except for the small to medium effect of awe (ηp2=.02). For the factors excluding hardship (F3), values increased in individuals in their 20s–50s and plateaued or decreased in individuals in their 60s (Fig 4). According to multiple comparisons using Bonferroni’s correction, participants in their 20s showed significantly lower scores for positive emotions (than those in their 40s and 50s), exhilaration, and goosebumps (than those in their 50s and 60s), as well as awe, transcendence, and surprise (compared to those in their 40s, 50s, and 60s). Participants in their 30s also exhibited lower scores for exhilaration and goosebumps (compared to those in their 50s and 60s), awe (compared to those in their 40s, 50s, and 60s), transcendence, and surprise (compared with those in their 50s). Participants in their 40s scored significantly lower for exhilaration and goosebumps than those in their 50s but scored significantly higher for hardship than those in their 50s and 60s. The scores were not significantly different between participants in their 20s and 30s. The differences between those in their 50s and 60s were not significant, except that the surprise score was significantly higher for those in their 50s than for those in their 60s. These results show that the generational effects in the kando-related reactions of positive emotions, exhilaration, goosebumps, awe, transcendence, and surprise generally increase with age, peaking around the 50s. The hardship experience was highest for those in their 40s and decreased for those in their 50s and 60s.

The main effects of sex were significant for positive emotions (F1), exhilaration (F2), kando (F4), tears (F5), warmth (F6), overcome (F7), goosebumps (F8), awe (F9), and transcendence (F10). However, the effect sizes were small or negligible (ηp2.01) for most factors, except for the small-to-medium effects of tears (ηp2=.026), warmth (ηp2=.027), and goosebumps (ηp2=.012). As shown in Fig 4, men scored significantly higher on exhilaration, goosebumps, and awe, whereas women scored significantly higher on positive emotions, tears, warmth, overcome, and transcendence.

Discussion

This study explored the types of events that Japanese people memorized as their most significant kando experience and examined how they reacted to them according to the newly developed KRS. We developed the KRS with 11 correlated factors by applying EFA and CFA, which included emotional states (i.e., positive emotions, exhilaration, hardship, overcome, surprise); subjective physical reactions (i.e., goosebumps, warmth, tears); and the general experience or perception of the event (i.e., kando, transcendence, awe). We also showed that the KRS scores differed as a function of the event’s characteristics: family issues, interpersonal relationships, arts, sports, travel/nature, negative issues, achievements, religion/disaster, and others. The generational effects on kando were not overly significant, but kando-related reactions with strong arousal (i.e., positive emotions, exhilaration, goosebumps, awe, transcendence, and surprise) were likely to be gradually enhanced for participants in their 20s and 50s. The effects of sex were found to be relatively stronger for subjective physical reactions (warmth, tears, and goosebumps) than for psychological reactions.

Notably, we examined the various events experienced by more than 4,000 people. The existing literature on kando (e.g., [2, 3, 7, 19]) has focused only on a specific target (e.g., music, novels, stories); therefore, researchers have not examined the reactions among different domains. The current study enabled us to compare reactions among different domains through participants’ recall of events. For example, we found that “interpersonal relationships” and “family issues,” both in the domain of human-to-human relationships, differed in the evaluations of our KRS. This indicates that the KRS is suitable for describing the characteristics of kando, at least when dealing with experiences stored in autobiographical memories.

The event categories were grouped into eight clusters. Menninghaus, et al. [11] qualitatively categorized experiences of being moved into six groups among German nationals: “relationship,” “critical life,” “political event,” “nature-related,” “art-related,” and “others (misc.).” Our eight categories overlap those of Menninghaus et al. [11], suggesting that events triggering kando and being moved are analogous between Japan and Germany. However, differences between the two studies were also observed in the distribution of categories. For example, the “art-related” category was chosen as the fifth most prominent of six categories in Germany [11]. In contrast, it was the third most prominent factor among the eight clusters in the present study. Although this may be due to differences in concepts between kando and being moved, the degree of kando can vary cross-culturally, even if the same events can generate kando.

We demonstrated that the KRS effectively differentiated the participants’ reactions among event categories. As in KAMMUS II, the KRS contains emotional and physiological reactions, and several new factors in the KRS were identified. First, the KRS contains hardship as a negative emotion, extracted independently of positive emotions. As discussed in the literature on affective well-being [27], negative emotions are not the opposite of positive ones. Tokaji [3] suggested that kando is associated with complex and mixed emotions. Similar to our data, family issues scored high in both the positive emotions and hardship factors, providing evidence that positive and negative emotions (i.e., hardship) should be measured independently to describe the entire kando experience.

In addition, the overcome, surprise, and transcendence factors were identified for the first time in this study and have not been included in existing scales, such as the KAMMUS II. For events involving relatively long processes, such as family issues, sports, negative issues, and achievements, people responded more strongly to the overcome factor, which represents an affective transition from negative to positive experiences, evidencing that kando is positive overall despite occasional negative events [1]. Although kama muta includes these affective transitions, as represented by the phrase “tears of joy” [8], we explicitly included this as a overcome factor in the KRS.

The transcendence factor was uniquely extracted in this study. We identified the “religion/disaster” category as one type of event triggering kando, suggesting that the meaning of kando also encompasses the unknown or religious issues (i.e., “I felt a mysterious power,” “I felt a great power”). In the literature on strong music experiences, people in Western countries feel connected to God through music [28]. To the best of our knowledge, such a transcendence factor has not been examined in the research on being moved or kama muta. Using a cross-cultural study, it may be beneficial to explore whether this factor is unique to kando in Japan (or Asian/Buddhist countries) or whether the mental mechanism also exists in people in Western countries.

Despite the smaller effect sizes, we identified differences in the KRS based on age and sex. Generational effects were particularly prominent in the KRS factors associated with strong arousal (i.e., positive emotions, exhilaration, goosebumps, awe, transcendence, and surprise). Research on emotions and memory has shown that people tend to remember high-arousal experiences more frequently than low-arousal ones [29]. Moreover, memories are likely to be reinterpreted upon recall, and people tend to transform negative experiences into positive ones (i.e., autobiographical reasoning [30]). Therefore, participants in their 20s and 50s may be more likely to interpret events positively through repeated recall of memories.

This study also highlighted that sex differences were greater for the subjective physical reactions of tears, warmth, and goosebumps than for psychological reactions. Considering women’s higher biological vulnerability to adverse and stressful events [31], it is noteworthy that they are more responsive to kando events with tears and warmth. In contrast, goosebumps was rated higher by men. The meaning of goosebumps (torihada in Japanese) originates from piloerection owing to low temperatures and has been used analogically when describing fear, disgust, nervousness, and anxiety [32] and acquired positive connotations in the lexicon after 1990s. The term is frequently used in sports research [32]. Considering that men are more likely to choose the sports event cluster and that the term “goosebumps” is commonly used in sports, higher ratings of goosebumps by men might be influenced by sociocultural factors.

Considering the scope of this study, we constructed a CFA model using covariance-based structural equation modeling (CB-SEM). However, further exploration is possible by addressing the causal relationships among the factors using partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) [33]. By doing so, we can explore the mental structure of how the overall evaluation of kando is determined by the psychological and physical components. These recent statistical developments will enable us to understand the direct and indirect indicators explaining the degree of kando. This may also allow us to refine the KRS into its validated, shorter version.

Finally, this study has several limitations. First, our data were based on people’s memories; therefore, it remains unclear whether the KRS can measure direct reactions to stimuli, especially in a laboratory setting. In addition, because we asked participants to recall the “most significant kando event,” we cannot know whether the KRS can be used for recently experienced events. Furthermore, this study was conducted among a Japanese population; therefore, the applicability of the KRS to other populations remains unknown. We suggest cultural roles in the experience of kando so that multi-linguistic studies (e.g., [34]) can be conducted in the future using validated translations of the KRS. This would allow us to understand the role of culture in the experience of kando.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Frequency of categories selected as the most significant kando events.

(EPS)

pone.0311905.s001.eps (1.7MB, eps)
S1 Table. The kando reaction scale in Japanese alongside its English equivalents.

(PDF)

pone.0311905.s002.pdf (132.2KB, pdf)
S2 Table. Inter-factor correlations of the kando reaction scale computed using confirmatory factor analysis.

(PDF)

pone.0311905.s003.pdf (47.5KB, pdf)
S1 Dataset. Raw data used in all analyses.

(XLSX)

pone.0311905.s004.xlsx (1.1MB, xlsx)

Acknowledgments

We thank Yuta Goto and Kaori Takasu for their assistance with data cleaning.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

The authors declare that this study received funding from Yamaha Motor Co., Ltd. The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

References

  • 1. Yasuda S, Shoda H, Uemiya A, Isaka T. A review of psychological research on kando as an inclusive concept of moving experiences. Frontiers in Psychology. 2022;13:974220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.974220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Tokaji A. Mechanisms for evoking emotional responses of “kandoh” (The state of being emotionally moved). Cognitive Studies: Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society. 2001;8(4):360–368. doi: 10.11225/jcss.8.360 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Tokaji A. Research for determinant factors and features of emotional responses of “kandoh” (the state of being emotionally moved). Japanese Psychological Research. 2003;45(4):235–249. doi: 10.1111/1468-5884.00226 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Ekman P. Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review. 1992;99(3):550–553. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1980;39(6):1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Oode S, Imai A, Ando A, Taniguchi T. Classification of words expressing the emotional affects based on the subjective similarity measures. Journal of Natural Language Processing. 2007;14(3):81–97. doi: 10.5715/jnlp.14.3_81 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Yasuda S, Nakamura T. A psychological study of strong experiences due to listening to music based on a subjective measurement of physical reactions. The Japanese Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2008;6(1):11–19. doi: 10.5265/jcogpsy.6.11 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Zickfeld JH, Arriaga P, Santos SV, Schubert TW, Seibt B. Tears of joy, aesthetic chills and heartwarming feelings: Physiological correlates of kama muta. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(12):e13662, 1–26. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Fiske AP, Seibt B, Schubert T. The sudden devotion emotion: Kama muta and the cultural practices whose function is to evoke it. Emotion Review. 2019;11(1):74–86. doi: 10.1177/1754073917723167 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Zickfeld JH, Schubert TW, Seibt B, Fiske AP. Moving through the literature: What is the emotion often denoted being moved? Emotion Review. 2019;11(2):123–139. doi: 10.1177/1754073918820126 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Menninghaus W, Wagner V, Hanich J, Wassiliwizky E, Kuehnast M, Jacobsen T. Towards a psychological construct of being moved. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(6):1–33. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Cova F, Deonna JA. Being moved. Philosophical Studies. 2014;169(3):447–466. doi: 10.1007/s11098-013-0192-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Shiota MN, Keltner D, Mossman A. The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cognition & Emotion. 2007;21(5):944–963. doi: 10.1080/02699930600923668 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition & Emotion. 2003;17(2):297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Maeura N, Nakayama M, Uchida Y. The discriminability and commonality between awe and “kando” (being emotionally moved) in Japan. Cognitive Studies: Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society. 2020;27(3):262–279. doi: 10.11225/cs.2020.036 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Chirico A, Yaden DB, Riva G, Gaggioli A. The potential of virtual reality for the investigation of awe. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Hashimoto I, Ogura T. Relationships between emotionally moved experiences and empathic tendency in adolescence. Bulletin of the Faculty of Education (Educational Sciences), Ehime University. 2002;48(2):57–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Zickfeld JH, Schubert TW, Seibt B, Blomster JK, Arriaga P, Basabe N, et al. Kama muta: Conceptualizing and measuring the experience often labelled being moved across 19 nations and 15 languages. Emotion. 2019;19:402–424. doi: 10.1037/emo0000450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Kato J, Murata K. The effect of the narrative depiction of separation in the novel portrayed friendship on being moved: Focusing on the mediating effect of uncovering the social values. Japanese Journal of Research on Emotions. 2017;24(2):82–91. doi: 10.4092/jsre.24.2_82 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Ward MK, Meade AW. Dealing with careless responding in survey data: Prevention, identification, and recommended best practices. Annual Review of Psychology. 2023;74:577–596. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-040422-045007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Reise SP, Waller NG, Comrey AL. Factor analysis and scale revision. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12(3):287–297. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.3.287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Revelle W. psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality Research; 2022. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.
  • 23. Dunn KJ, McCray G. The place of the bifactor model in confirmatory factor analysis Investigations into construct dimensionality in language testing. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020; p. 1357. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Taber KS. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Research in Science Education. 2018;48:1273–1296. doi: 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. John L Perry PJC Adam R Nicholls, Crust L. Assessing Model Fit: Caveats and Recommendations for Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science. 2015;19(1):12–21. doi: 10.1080/1091367X.2014.952370 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Mroczek DK, Kolarz CM. The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75(5):1333–1349. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Gabrielsson A. Strong experiences with music: Music is much more than just music. Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Holland AC, Kensinger EA. Emotion and autobiographical memory. Physics of Life Reviews. 2010;7(1):88–131. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. McLean KC, Fournier MA. The content and processes of autobiographical reasoning in narrative identity. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42(3):527–545. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Bianchin M, Angrilli A. Gender differences in emotional responses: A psychophysiological study. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;105(4):925–932. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.10.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Wu L. A research on the usage of “Torihada ga tatsu” using a newspaper article database (in Japanese). Research Journal of the Graduate School of Humanities and Human Sciences. 2016;15:93–108. doi: 10.14943/rjgsl.15.l93 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Hair JF Jr, Matthews LM, Matthews RL, Sarstedt M. PLS-SEM or CB-SEM: Updated guidelines on which method to use. International Journal of Multivariate Data Analysis. 2017;1(2):107–123. doi: 10.1504/IJMDA.2017.087624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Aqeel M, Jami H, Ahmed A. Translation, adaptation, and cross-language validation of student: Thinking about my homework scale (STP). International Journal of Human Rights in Healthcare. 2017;10(5):296–309. doi: 10.1108/IJHRH-05-2017-0019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Fig. Frequency of categories selected as the most significant kando events.

(EPS)

pone.0311905.s001.eps (1.7MB, eps)
S1 Table. The kando reaction scale in Japanese alongside its English equivalents.

(PDF)

pone.0311905.s002.pdf (132.2KB, pdf)
S2 Table. Inter-factor correlations of the kando reaction scale computed using confirmatory factor analysis.

(PDF)

pone.0311905.s003.pdf (47.5KB, pdf)
S1 Dataset. Raw data used in all analyses.

(XLSX)

pone.0311905.s004.xlsx (1.1MB, xlsx)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.


Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES