Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2024 Aug 5;45(9):1183–1211. doi: 10.1177/02724316241271327

The Relationship between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements in Adolescent Girls from London

Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer 1,, Jessica Evans 1, Katy Ratcliffe 1, Kaushalya Janaarthanan 1, Saz Ahmed 2, Willem Kuyken 3, Tim Dalgleish 4, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore 1,2
Editors: Dorottya Kisfalusi, Károly Takács
PMCID: PMC12443375  PMID: 40969154

Abstract

Understanding ourselves within our peer environment is an important component of self-development during adolescence, the period of life between the onset of puberty and adulthood (between ages 10 and 24 years). We used a self-appraisal paradigm to investigate cross-sectionally the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements in adolescent girls. One hundred and sixty-three girls (9–15 years), recruited from London, United Kingdom, rated how well a set of positive and negative adjectives described themselves, or a chosen familiar other. Participants also completed a self-report friendship quality questionnaire. Higher perceived friendship quality predicted lower negative self-judgements and higher positive self-judgements. These relationships did not change across the age range tested, but there was an overall decrease in positivity effect (higher positive judgements compared to negative judgements) with age. These findings highlight the importance of investigating how different components of peer relationships are related to self-concept development in adolescence.

Keywords: adolescence, friendship quality, self-judgements, self-concept, self-appraisal task

Introduction

Adolescence is a period of life when belonging to peer groups and social evaluative concerns become particularly salient (Pfeifer & Berkman, 2018; Sawyer et al., 2018; Tomova et al., 2021). During this period, young people are developing their sense of self and use their increasing ability to reflect on the mental states and behaviours of other people (Dumontheil et al., 2010) to learn about their own goals, behaviour and social roles (Crone et al., 2022; Moses-Payne et al., 2021; Sebastian et al., 2008). The quality of peer relationships is a significant determinant of self-worth (Bagwell & Schmidt, 2011; Maunder & Monks, 2019) and adaptive psychosocial functioning in adolescence (van Harmelen et al., 2017, 2021), while peer rejection is associated with increased depression and anxiety symptoms (Pickering et al., 2020). Most socioemotional disorders emerge during adolescence (Blakemore, 2019), and adolescent girls are particularly vulnerable to developing mental health problems (Campbell et al., 2021) and negative self-appraisals (Pfeifer et al., 2013). Therefore, it is important to understand how peer environments are related to how adolescent girls think about themselves. This cross-sectional study sought to investigate how the quality of one’s friendships is related to self-judgements in adolescent girls aged 9–15 years, and how this relationship is associated with age.

The maturation of biological and cognitive processes, along with changes in adolescents’ social roles and environments, makes adolescence an important stage in the refinement of one’s self-concept: the multifaceted socio-cognitive construct encompassing knowledge and beliefs about the self (Pfeifer & Allen, 2021). Although ideas about selfhood emerge in early childhood (Cunningham et al., 2014; Harter, 2012), self-concepts become increasingly complex and abstract throughout adolescence. For example, young people’s descriptions of themselves become more domain-specific (e.g., academic, social, physical; Harter, 2012; Shapka & Khan, 2011; van der Cruijsen et al., 2023). In addition, the likelihood of adolescents endorsing negative self-judgements increases throughout early adolescence, peaking in mid-late adolescence (15–17 years) and decreasing in early adulthood (McArthur et al., 2019; van der Aar et al., 2018; van der Cruijsen et al., 2018). It has been proposed that cognitive and social processes associated with earlier pubertal development in girls when compared to boys are related to increased endorsement of negative self-appraisals in girls (Pfeifer & Allen, 2021; Pfeifer et al., 2013). In turn, the increase in negative self-evaluations during early and mid-adolescence in girls has been suggested to explain age and gender differences associated with poor mental-health and well-being outcomes, including internalising and externalising behaviours (Ybrandt, 2008), self-consciousness and social anxiety (Moses-Payne et al., 2022), and depressive symptoms (Bone et al., 2021; Shapka & Keating, 2005).

The development of social-cognitive processing abilities during adolescence might contribute to the rise of negative self-judgements in adolescent girls (Jankowski et al., 2014). For example, the continued development of the ability to ascertain the mental states of others (Dumontheil et al., 2010) allows adolescents to be more aware of the opinions of their peers, and subsequently reflect on how they think others perceive them (Crone & Steinbeis, 2017; Sebastian et al., 2008). In fact, this form of reflected self-concept becomes more central across adolescence. Studies show increased activation of brain regions that process social information during self-evaluation tasks (Güroğlu, 2022) and increasing similarities in self-evaluations in behavioural tasks using one’s own perspective and the perspective of others (Pfeifer et al., 2009; Van der Cruijsen et al., 2019; van der Cruijsen et al., 2023). Furthermore, increased awareness of the behaviours and abilities of peers has been related to an increased use of social comparisons to understand adolescents’ own behaviours and abilities (Crone et al., 2022; Pfeifer & Peake, 2012). In line with this, early adolescents internalise peer rejection, such that they view themselves more negatively after being rejected by peers (Rodman et al., 2017). In addition, one study found that girls (aged 9–13 years) engage in more self-critical social comparison processes, such as upward contrast (i.e., self-evaluation becoming more negative as a result of comparisons with someone judged as having better abilities; Gerber et al., 2018), compared to boys (Valls, 2022). Engaging in self-critical social comparisons is related to negative socioemotional responses (Noon & Meier, 2019; Pfeifer & Peake, 2012), including lower self-esteem (Orth et al., 2018; Rieger et al., 2016; Somerville et al., 2010) and higher self-consciousness (Rankin et al., 2004; Somerville, 2013; Somerville et al., 2013), particularly in early adolescence compared with other ages (Roeder et al., 2014).

Early adolescent friendships are marked by being relatively dynamic, with bonds between friends forming and breaking in short periods of time and becoming more stable across mid- and late adolescence (Burnett Heyes et al., 2015; Flannery & Smith, 2021; Stadtfeld et al., 2020). At the same time, sociocognitive development enables a myriad of relational behaviours to become more central in adolescent friendships compared to those of children, including reciprocity and loyalty, intimacy, self-disclosure, emotional support and conflict resolution (Flannery & Smith, 2017a; Güroğlu, 2022; Poulin & Chan, 2010). In turn, adolescents increasingly prefer to incorporate the views of close friends in their social decision-making (Slagter et al., 2023) and show similarities in self-evaluations and evaluations of close friends (van Buuren et al., 2020). Similarly, the preference to direct prosocial behaviour (e.g. in a resource allocation game) to friends rather than to socially distant peers increases with age during this period (Güroglu et al., 2014), particularly if these friendships are reciprocal (Burnett Heyes et al., 2015). Literature on the association between social relationships and self-cognitions in adolescence has typically focussed on the detrimental effect of peer victimisation on negative self-cognitions (e.g. Cole et al., 2016; Norrington, 2021; Roeder et al., 2014), as this is an important risk factor for developing mental health difficulties (Christina et al., 2021; Goemans et al., 2023; Roeder & Cole, 2018). In the current study, we focus instead on perceived friendship quality, which is a significant predictor of adolescent friendship stability and dissolution, psychosocial functioning, self-esteem and self-worth (Bagwell & Schmidt, 2011; Flannery & Smith, 2021; Harris & Orth, 2020), with high friendship quality being a protective factor against mental health problems (van Harmelen et al., 2021).

High friendship quality can be defined as a combination of high levels of positive interpersonal features, such as pro-social behaviour, self-disclosure, trust, intimacy and encouragement, alongside low levels of negative features, such as conflict and rivalry (Berndt, 2002). It has been proposed that friendship quality provides a space for the development of secure attachments, which are characterised by safety, trust and self-disclosure, and enable identity formation, positive self-esteem and self-worth (Gorrese & Ruggieri, 2013; Vijayakumar & Pfeifer, 2020). In addition, relationships characterised by low conflict promote feelings of social self-competence (Flannery & Smith, 2017a; Roeder et al., 2014) and are more likely to be maintained later in adolescence (Flannery & Smith, 2021). This suggests that there might be a bidirectional relationship between friendship and self-appraisals. For example, self-esteem and positive self-concepts are both linked to increased trust and self-disclosure in relationships (Tajmirriyahi & Ickes, 2020; Vijayakumar & Pfeifer, 2020). At the same time, conflict in peer relationships can lead to depressive symptoms and aversive behaviours, which in turn negatively impact relationships (e.g. interpersonal theory of depression; Rudolph, 2017; Stewart & Harkness, 2017).

The role of friendship quality on self-evaluation is likely to change as social and cognitive processes develop throughout adolescence. For example, heightened salience of socio-emotional contextual information (Albert & Newhouse, 2019; Goddings et al., 2012), coupled with relatively unstable self-concepts and social networks (Crone et al., 2022; Stadtfeld et al., 2020), could mean that the positive salience of good quality friendships plays a particularly important role in affective processes guiding self-evaluation in early adolescence (Rapee et al., 2019; Somerville, 2013). Conversely, as sociocognitive processes related to the recognition and attribution of mental states (e.g. social perspective taking) become more sophisticated (Crone & Steinbeis, 2017; Dumontheil et al., 2010; Flannery & Smith, 2017b; Van der Graaff et al., 2014), and quality of friendships becomes more stable and reciprocal (Burnett Heyes et al., 2015; Flannery & Smith, 2017a), it is possible that perceived friendship quality becomes a better predictor of adjustment and positive self-concepts later in adolescence.

In this cross-sectional study, we investigated the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements in adolescent girls and age-related differences in this relationship in early and mid-adolescence. We employed a self-appraisal task to measure the impact of positive and negative information on the self-judgements of girls between the ages of 9 and 15 years, compared to judgements of a chosen familiar other. This experimental task arguably provides a more sensitive measure of self-attributions than questionnaire-based measures of self-esteem and self-worth. In addition, we used a self-report questionnaire of friendship quality (van Harmelen et al., 2016), which was designed to measure both positive (e.g. trust, self-disclosure and closeness) and negative (e.g. conflict) experiences of close social relationships. We focused on girls as girls are more sensitive than boys to peer rejection (Joinson et al., 2012; Mendle et al., 2020) and tend to have more mental health problems in early adolescence than boys (Joinson et al., 2012). As peer relationships play an important role in the development of self-judgements, we firstly hypothesised that perceived friendship quality would be related to increased positive self-judgements (hypothesis 1a) and decreased negative self-judgements (hypothesis 1b) (both relative to judgements made about a chosen familiar other). In addition, given the biological, cognitive and socio-emotional development that occurs throughout the period of adolescence, we explored age-related differences in the strength of the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements (exploratory hypothesis 2).

Methods

Sample

This study used cross-sectional data collected between October 2019 and February 2020 for a larger study (Ahmed et al., 2024). The data from the larger study comprised 183 early adolescent girls between the ages of 9 and 15 years (M = 13.5, SD = 1.0), recruited from eight schools in London (see supplemental material SM1 for more details on school selection). Participants completed the study on individual computers in group sessions of between 2 and 26 pupils. In the current study, data from 20 participants were excluded due to technical difficulties during the self-appraisal task. Therefore, the final sample comprised data from 163 girls (aged 9–15 years, M = 13.1, SD = 1.0). Upon reviewer request, we have included an a posteriori power simulation in supplemental material SM1. The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee and carried out in accordance with General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR). Informed consent from parents and assent from all participants were obtained. Participants were compensated £10 in vouchers for taking part in a 1-h testing session. De-identified data and scripts are available on OSF at https://osf.io/dq9p3/.

Procedure

We used a self-appraisal task to measure self-judgements, which was presented in Gorilla (https://gorilla.sc/), and the Cambridge Friendship Questionnaire (van Harmelen et al., 2016), which was presented in Qualtrics (https://qualtrics.com/), to measure perceived friendship quality. We also used the Pubertal Development Scale (Petersen et al., 1988) and an abbreviated version of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices test (Bilker et al., 2012) to allow adjustments for the potential confounding effects of differences in pubertal development and non-verbal reasoning abilities in this age range. Participants completed other cognitive tasks and questionnaires as part of the larger study, which are described elsewhere (Ahmed et al., 2024).

Self-Appraisal Task

Self-judgements were measured using a task based on the self-appraisal paradigm described in Moses-Payne et al. (2022). In this task, participants were asked about positive or negative stimuli that referred to the participants themselves or another person. Participants chose a ‘familiar other’ whom they did not know personally (e.g. a celebrity or fictional character), as this has been suggested to offer greater differences in depth of processing (Sui & Humphreys, 2015; van Buuren et al., 2020). The 2 (word valence: positive, negative; within-subjects) x 2 (social condition: self, other; within-subjects) repeated measures design of the self-appraisal task is optimised to compare positive and negative judgements of self to those of a chosen familiar other (social control condition).

Participants had to rate how well a set of trait-descriptive adjectives described themselves (‘Does this word describe you?’) or the chosen other (‘Does this word describe [e.g., Hermione Granger]?’). Participants were given 7 s to judge the descriptiveness of the word from zero (‘Does not describe me/her at all’) to 10 (‘Totally describes me/her’). Participants judged a total of 64 words based on how well they described themselves (32 words: 18 positive, 14 negative) or their chosen familiar other (32 words: 18 positive, 14 negative). The order of the words was pseudo-randomised across social condition and word valence (see supplemental material SM2 for details about word allocation and randomisation). This task additionally included a memory component during which participants were asked whether they recognised the words presented in the task (described in Ahmed et al., 2024). We did not analyse this data here as the current analysis focused on self-evaluation (and not memory).

Descriptiveness ratings of positive and negative trait-descriptive adjectives for oneself provided during the task were used as measures of positive self-judgements and negative self-judgements. Self-judgements therefore refer to the extent to which an individual believes they are accurately represented by trait-descriptive adjectives (Moses-Payne et al., 2022). Negative and positive judgements about the chosen other were measured in the same way.

Friendship Quality Questionnaire

Perceived friendship quality was measured using the Cambridge Friendship Questionnaire (van Harmelen et al., 2016), an 8-item self-report questionnaire derived from a semi-structured interview on social relationships (Goodyer et al., 1989). This questionnaire asks participants to rate the extent to which they agree with a set of indicators of closeness of friendships and conflict behaviours (e.g., ‘How often do you see your friends outside of school?’, ‘Do you have arguments with your friends that upset you?’) on a four or six-point Likert scale. This questionnaire does not measure structural components of friendships, such as the number of friends in the participant’s network. Items are added to produce a score ranging from zero to 32, with higher scores indicating greater perceived friendship quality. The questionnaire has good measurement invariance and external validity (van Harmelen et al., 2016).

Pubertal Development Scale

To enable adjustment for the potential confounds of pubertal development, participants completed the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen et al., 1988). This is a self-report scale that assesses five general indicators of development (growth in height, skin changes, growth of body, breast development and menarche), for example: “Have your breasts begun to grow? 1 = no, 2 = yes barely, 3 = yes definitely, 4 = development completed”. The scores of these five questions were averaged to give a continuous score ranging from 1 (pre-pubertal) to 4 (completed pubertal development). The question about onset of menarche was rated on a 3-point scale (1 = no, 2 = yes barely & 3 = yes definitely) but was recoded so that ‘no’ was scored as 1 and both ‘yes barely’ and ‘yes definitely’ were scored as 4 (in line with previous work; see Carskadon & Acebo, 1993). Pubertal stage of the participants ranged from 1.4 to 3.8 (M = 2.8, SE = .6) and no participant reported complete pubertal maturation (a score of 4).

Non-Verbal Reasoning

To enable adjustment for the potential confounds of non-verbal reasoning, participants completed a nine-item abbreviated version of the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test, which is related to IQ (Bilker et al., 2012) The number of correct answers was summed to give a continuous score from zero to 9. Non-verbal reasoning scores of the participants ranged from 1.0 to 9.0 (M = 6.0, SE = 1.7). One participant did not have non-verbal reasoning data due to computer issues.

Statistical Analysis

The dependent variable was participant judgements, measured by taking each descriptiveness rating for the trait-adjectives presented in the self-appraisal task. The social condition and word valence factors of the self-appraisal task were used as independent variables, as well as friendship quality (as determined by the sum of the friendship quality questionnaire) and participant age. Both friendship quality and age were modelled as continuous linear variables and were mean-centred to meet assumptions of multicollinearity. We used decimal age (rounded to the nearest 100th).

Raw trial-level judgements were modelled using a linear mixed effects model (lmerTest package version 3.1–3; Kuznetsova et al., 2017) in the R programming environment (R version 4.3.2; R Core Team, 2023). To test whether perceived friendship quality cross-sectionally predicts differences in self-judgements (H1), the first model regressed judgements on the three-way interaction between friendship quality, social condition and word valence, as well as their two-level interactions and main effects. To test whether the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements differed cross-sectionally with age (EH2), the second model additionally included age (and all possible three-way interactions among this and the other variables of the model). To obtain more parsimonious models, we progressively excluded nonsignificant higher-level interactions via nested model comparison. Both models clustered data by participant (as a random intercept) and included random slopes for within-subject factors as random effects (social condition and word valence). Main effects and interactions of the best-fitting model were inspected using Type III Wald F-tests with Satterthwaite approximations for degrees of freedom. We converted F values of significant main effects and interactions to estimated effect sizes of ηp2 (partial eta-squared; confidence interval = 95%) using the effectsize package (version 0.8.6; Ben-Shachar et al., 2020). Post hoc comparisons were performed using the emmeans package (version 1.8.5; Lenth et al., 2022) and were Bonferroni-corrected for two comparisons (social condition and word valence).

We tested the robustness of the main effects and interactions by running a set of sensitivity analyses including additional covariates to the models described above, which we refer to here as control models. To control for differences in pubertal development in this age group, which is related to sensitivity to social-emotional processing (Pfeifer & Peake, 2012), the first control model additionally included pubertal development as a covariate (CM1). The second control model additionally included non-verbal abilities as a covariate (CM2). We additionally controlled for testing group size (i.e. number of participants taking part in the same testing session, CM3) as peer presence could affect social information processing (Breiner et al., 2018). Finally, we controlled for single gender schools (CM4). Pubertal development, non-verbal reasoning and testing group size were all modelled as continuous linear variables, whereas single gender school was modelled as a binary factor (single gender, co-education). See supplemental material SM3 for full details about fixed- and random-effect structure of all models. Data and scripts are available on OSF at https://osf.io/dq9p3/.

Results

Overall, descriptive statistics of the self-appraisal task replicated previous findings relating to mean self-appraisal and appraisals of a chosen other in this age group (self-judgements: Mpositive = 6.0, SDpositive = 2.7, Mnegative = 3.6, SDnegative = 2.9; other judgements: Mpositive = 6.3, SDpositive = 2.9, Mnegative = 3.0, SDnegative = 3.0; Moses-Payne et al., 2022; van der Cuijsen et al., 2018; van der Aar et al., 2018, see Figure 1). Self-reported friendship quality was comparable to the previous literature using this scale (M = 24.0, SD = 4.0, IQR = 22 - 27; van van Harmelen et al., 2016; van Harmelen et al., 2017; van Harmelen et al., 2021) and was not correlated with age (r = −.04, p = .617).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Self-appraisal task. Participants rated whether a set of 64 words were descriptive of themselves (32 words: 18 positive, 14 negative) or of a chosen familiar other (e.g., Hermione Granger; 32 words: 18 positive, 14 negative) on a scale from zero (Does not describe me/them at all) to 10 (Totally describes me/them). Participants had 7 s to provide their rating. Words were pseudo-randomised (see supplemental material SM1 for details).

Hypothesis 1. Effect of Perceived Friendship Quality on Judgements

The results of the linear mixed effects model showed a main effect of word valence (F (1,161) = 411.61, p < .001; η p 2 = .72, CI [.65 .77]), such that positive judgements were higher than negative judgements, which has been referred to as a positivity effect (contrast negative - positive = −2.79, SE = .14, p < .001; Moses-Payne et al., 2022). There was no main effect of social condition (F (1,161) = 2.41, p = .123). The association between social condition and judgements was dependent on word valence: there was a two-way interaction between social condition and word valence (F (1,161) = 19.82, p < .001; η p 2 = .11, CI [.04 .21]), which was driven by higher negative self-judgements than negative judgements about the chosen other (contrast other - self = −.58, SE = .13, pBonf < .001), and lower positive self-judgements than positive judgements about the chosen other (contrast other - self = .37, SE = .13, pBonf = .007, see Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Interaction between word valence and social condition on judgements. The violin plots represent kernel probability density of judgements (0 – 10; descriptiveness ratings from the self-appraisal task) grouped by word valence (positive on the left and negative on the right) and social condition (self-judgements in purple and judgements of the chosen other in orange). Dots represents participant-level mean judgements. Squares show group mean judgements as estimated by the linear mixed effects model and error bars show the 95% confidence intervals (negligible). Asterisks represent *** pBonf < .001, ** pBonf < .01.

There was no main effect of perceived friendship quality (F (1,161) = 0.12, p = .729). Instead, there was a two-way interaction between perceived friendship quality and word valence (F (1,161) = 10.43, p = .002; η p 2 = .06, CI [.01 .14]): higher perceived friendship quality was related to increased positive judgements (slope = .05, SE = .02, p Bonf = .020) and decreased negative judgements (slope = −.06, SE = .02, p Bonf = .012). In addition, there was a significant three-way interaction between perceived friendship quality, word valence and social condition (F (1,161) = 10.59, p = .001; η p 2 = .06, CI [.01 .15]; see Figure 3). In support of hypothesis 1, simple effects showed that higher friendship quality predicted higher positive self-judgements (slope = .09, SE = .02, pBonf = .001; Figure 3, left panel, blue line) as well as lower negative self-judgements (slope = −.11, SE = .03, pBonf < .001; Figure 3, left panel, green line). Positive judgements for the chosen other (slope = .02, SE = .03, pBonf = 1; Figure 3, right panel, blue line) and negative judgements for the chosen other (slope = −.01, SE = .03, pBonf = 1; Figure 3, right panel, green line) were not significantly different across perceived friendship quality. Post-hoc comparisons showed that the difference between negative self-judgements and negative judgements for the chosen other become smaller with increasing perceived friendship quality (contrast other - self = .11, SE = .03, pBonf = .002). The difference between positive self-judgements and positive judgements for the chosen other followed a similar trend but was not statistically significantly (contrast other - self = −.07, SE = .03, pBonf = .060). These effects were robust to all sensitivity analyses (see supplemental material SM4).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Relationship between perceived friendship quality and judgements depends on social condition (self in the left; other in the right) and word valence (positive in blue; negative in green). The plot shows judgements (0 – 10; descriptiveness ratings from the self-appraisal task) as a function of participant friendship quality (0 – 32), social condition (self or chosen other) and word valence (positive or negative). Data points are participant-level mean judgements. A linear mixed effects model showed that mean positive self-judgements (blue line) increased with perceived friendship quality and mean negative self-judgements (green line) decreased with perceived friendship quality (left panel). The right panel shows that positive (blue line) and negative judgements (green line) for the chosen other did not change significantly with perceived friendship quality (right panel). Shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks show Bonferroni corrected p-values: ***pBonf < .001; **pBonf < .01.

Exploratory Hypothesis 2: Effect of age on the relationship between Perceived Friendship Quality and Judgements

In addition to the terms described above, a second linear mixed effects model additionally showed a main effect of age (F (1,160) = 11.33, p < .001; η p 2 = .07, CI [.01 .15]) and a two-way interaction between age and word valence (F (1,160) = 5.63, p = .019; η p 2 = .03, CI [0 .11]; see Figure 4). While higher judgements overall in the self-appraisal task were associated with older age (slope = .31, SE = .08, p < .001), simple effects showed that this effect was driven by higher negative judgements in the self-appraisal task being associated with older age (slope = .32, SE = .09, pBonf < .001). This was not true for positive judgements, which did not differ significantly as a function of age (slope = 0, SE = .08, pBonf = 1). This resulted in a smaller positivity effect associated with older age, whereby the difference between positive judgements and negative judgements was smaller at older ages (contrast negative - positive = .33, SE = .14, pBonf = .035, see Figure 4). As age and word valence did not further interact with the social condition of the self-appraisal task (i.e., there was no three-way interaction), the results suggest that the age-related increase in negative judgements was not specific to self-judgements, but rather general across both self-judgements and judgements for the chosen other. These effects were robust to all sensitivity analyses (see supplemental material SM4). Notably, a model including higher-level interactions of age and friendship quality did not fit the data better than this model (χ2 (6) = 2.16, p = .904). This means that there is not sufficient evidence to suggest that there were age-related differences in the relationship between friendship quality and judgements in our sample.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Positivity effect (higher positive compared to negative judgements) decreases with age. The plot shows mean judgements (0 – 10; average computed from trait-descriptive adjectives rated during the self-appraisal task) as a function of participant age and word valence (positive or negative). Data points are participant-level mean judgements. A linear model showed that mean negative judgements increased with age (green line), while mean positive judgements did not change significantly with age (blue line). Shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals). Asterisks show Bonferroni corrected p-value: ***pBonf < .001.

Discussion

This study used a self-appraisal task and self-reported friendship quality to cross-sectionally investigate the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements in adolescent girls aged 9–15 years. We found that higher self-reported friendship quality was related to lower negative self-judgements and higher positive self-judgements. In contrast, this relationship was not found for judgements of a chosen familiar other. In addition, we did not find evidence for an age-related difference in the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements. However, we found that negative judgements in the self-appraisal task, but not positive judgements, were associated with older age, suggesting that a positivity effect in judgements, whereby positive judgements are higher than negative judgements, was smaller in older adolescents.

The Relationship between Perceived Friendship Quality and Positive and Negative Self-Judgements

In support of our first hypothesis, we found that higher self-reported friendship quality was related to higher positive self-judgements and lower negative self-judgements. This is in line with previous work suggesting that the quality of interpersonal relationships during adolescence is related to increased well-being and psychosocial functioning (Alsarrani et al., 2022; van Harmelen et al., 2017), and reduced feelings of anxiety and depression (van Harmelen et al., 2016), particularly in girls (Rueger et al., 2010). Self-esteem has been suggested to be an important factor for how social experiences are integrated into the self-concept (Crone et al., 2022), and therefore, one mechanism by which perceived friendship quality is related to positive self-concepts might be increased feelings of self-worth and self-esteem (Harris & Orth, 2020; Maunder & Monks, 2019). For example, friendship quality is defined by experiencing security, closeness and intimacy in friendships (Bagwell & Schmidt, 2011), and could be related to self-esteem through increased feelings of social acceptance and belonging (Tomova et al., 2021). In addition, self-esteem could be influenced by direct positive feedback from friends (e.g., validation and compliments) and fewer negative social experiences, such as receiving hurtful criticism or being victimised (Bagwell & Schmidt, 2011; Duru et al., 2019; Güroğlu, 2022; van Harmelen et al., 2021). Given that conflict in peer relationships and victimisation are consistently related to depressive cognitions (e.g. Cole et al., 2016; Norrington, 2021), and low self-esteem is a predictor of mental health difficulties (Rieger et al., 2016), understanding how friendship quality is related to self-esteem and positive self-cognitions could be a fruitful avenue for understanding links between self-processing and mental well-being.

Another explanation is that having good quality friendships might reduce negative biases in self-evaluative processing when compared to evaluative processing of others. Good quality friendships could be related to perceived similarity with close friends, which might be beneficial for self-evaluation (van Buuren et al., 2020). Research on social comparisons has suggested that comparing oneself to similar others has a greater influence on self-esteem and self-judgements than comparing ourselves to more socially distant individuals (e.g., Tarrant et al., 2006). In addition, literature on the self-enhancement model in young adults suggests that evaluations between the self and others are more similar in high quality relationships (Morry & Sucharyna, 2019), and that people are motivated to include close friends in self-concepts to expand the self (e.g., Aron et al., 2022; Gardner et al., 2002). Friendship quality has also been linked to increased social perspective taking in mid- and late adolescence (Flannery & Smith, 2017b; Rokeach & Wiener, 2022), which could be related to the use of similar strategies to make self-judgements and judgements about other people. In line with this, our results show that negative self-judgements and negative judgments of chosen others become more similar with higher perceived friendship quality. In fact, social network research suggests that stable networks are characterised by peer similarity and shared characteristics (Stadtfeld et al., 2020), which could mean that people choose to associate with individuals who are similar to them (Kandel, 2017). This contributes to a bidirectional relationship between the formation of stable social networks and the role of social comparisons in self-evaluations. Note, however, that our self-appraisal task uses a socially distant familiar other as a control social condition, as this has been suggested to offer greater differences in depth of processing (Sui & Humphreys, 2015; van Buuren et al., 2020). Future research could investigate the relationship between friendship quality and self-judgements compared to those of socially close others (e.g. a best friend). It is important to acknowledge that favourable opinions of friends and social desirability bias might influence judgements to be at ceiling (positive) or at floor (negative). In sum, further studies could investigate the role of perceived similarity in friendship groups, and how this is related to different strategies employed to make judgements about the self, friends and larger peer groups.

Age-Related Differences in the Relationship between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements

In general, our results are consistent with previous literature on age-related differences in self-judgements during adolescence in girls: negative self-judgements increase during adolescence, but positive self-judgements remain relatively stable in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (Moses-Payne et al., 2022; van der Aar et al., 2018; van der Cruijsen et al., 2018). This was true for both self-judgements and judgements about the chosen others. This suggests that the positivity effect consistently found in evaluations in children, young people and adults (Beer, 2014; Moses-Payne et al., 2022), whereby we view ourselves and others more positively than negatively, might be less pronounced during early and mid-adolescence. It has been suggested that self-concepts transition away from a positivity effect in the process of general self-evaluations becoming more domain-specific (Crone et al., 2022). For example, the onset of puberty has been suggested to mark a turning point in the neurocognitive mechanisms that make young people more sensitive to their social-emotional context during self-evaluation (Barendse et al., 2020; Pfeifer & Peake, 2012). In line with this, young people might use different strategies to inform self-concepts across development: while children might rely on parental relationships for informing self-judgements, adolescents might rely on using information from their social environment, and potentially shift to rely more on self-relevant memory in adulthood (Cunningham & Turk, 2017; Pfeifer et al., 2009; Yoon et al., 2018). Our results show that a decrease in positivity effect across adolescence remains after taking pubertal development into account, which suggests that sociocultural contexts and structures might also play a role in forming evaluative self-judgements across early and mid-adolescence.

We hypothesised that there would be an age-related difference in the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements throughout early to mid-adolescence. It is possible that having good quality friendships would be particularly important for self-evaluation during early adolescence, when self-concepts are more unstable and potentially more sensitive to contextual influences. In contrast, another possibility was that an improved ability to engage in reflected self-appraisals and social comparisons with good friends during mid-adolescence would be more significant for self-evaluation during that period. However, our results suggested that the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements did not differ between early and mid-adolescence. There are at least two explanations for these null findings. On the one hand, friendship quality increases with age throughout adolescence (Poulin & Chan, 2010; van Harmelen et al., 2021), and, therefore, the age range of our sample (9–15 years) might not be large enough to observe a difference in the relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements. In addition, our self-report friendship quality questionnaire might not have been sensitive enough to detect nuances in good quality friendships. For example, differences in trust, intimacy and communication, or factors related to structure of networks, such as quantity and stability of peer relationships, might be independently related to self-evaluation processes in early and mid-adolescence (Barzeva et al., 2022). In fact, the self-reported friendship quality questionnaire does not consider whether a young person might have no friends. Future studies could develop methods to measure greater nuances in the quality of friendships and peer relationships, leading to work showing how these might individually predict self-evaluations in early, mid and late adolescence.

Limitations and Future Research Directions

The findings of this study should be interpreted considering certain conceptual and methodological limitations. First, as has been discussed throughout the paper, due to the cross-sectional nature of our data, our results cannot discern any causal or longitudinal relationship between perceived friendship quality and self-judgements. An alternative interpretation of our results is that the girls in our sample with positive self-concepts are better able to maintain high quality friendships. In fact, self-esteem and positive self-concept have been linked to increased self-disclosure (Tajmirriyahi & Ickes, 2020; Vijayakumar & Pfeifer, 2020), the action of sharing personal information about oneself with others, which is thought to be a component of friendship quality (Cuadros & Berger, 2022; Towner et al., 2022). In addition, low conflict in high quality friendships is related to increased social self-competence and use of conflict resolution strategies (Flannery & Smith, 2017a, 2021). Therefore, the link between friendship quality and self-evaluation is likely to be complex and bidirectional, possibly reflecting a feedback loop over time (Harris & Orth, 2020). We were unable to explore this dynamic relationship using data from a second timepoint, given that the data collection of the larger study was interrupted by the outbreak of the coronavirus pandemic in 2020, and the friendship quality measure in the second timepoint was heavily confounded by social distancing restrictions imposed by the pandemic (e.g., questionnaire items asking participants to report how many times they see their friends). This also resulted in data collection being halted for a group of 9- and 15-year-old girls, which means that our findings might be most generalisable to girls between 10 and 14 years of age. Longitudinal studies across early and mid-adolescence should be conducted to understand how friendship group dynamics are important for self-development, as well as how particular self-perceptions could be related to how young people navigate their social environments.

Finally, there were practical limitations associated with using data from a larger study. First, the larger study only included data from girls. There are several reasons why we might expect to find gender differences in the relationship between friendship quality and self-judgements across adolescence: girls tend to have greater negative self-concepts (van der Cruijsen et al., 2018) and mental health problems (Bone et al., 2021), have an earlier onset of puberty on average (Dorn, 2006) and show higher neural sensitivity to socioemotional information (Pfeifer et al., 2013). In addition, girls tend to show, on average, greater social perspective taking and empathic concern in close relationships (Flannery & Smith, 2017b), and these gender differences are exacerbated in environments that adopt more traditional gender roles (Van der Graaff et al., 2014). Notably, single gender schools have been suggested to challenge traditional gender expectations and increase mathematics and science-related self-beliefs in girls (see review in Robinson et al., 2021). This highlights the potential in considering the school context in adolescent friendship dynamics. Second, while we explored age-related differences in the relationship between friendship quality and self-appraisals, the larger study did not include other factors that could potentially modify this relationship. For example, girls from poorer households and from ethnic minorities are at greater risk of experiencing mental health difficulties (Patalay & Fitzsimons, 2017), which might be associated with unique social pressures and negative self-appraisals (e.g. Deardorff et al., 2019). The larger study prioritised recruitment from schools that had continuing education from primary school and secondary school, which resulted in the majority of schools being independently funded, and therefore our sample is not likely to be generalisable to the UK population. Taken together, future studies are needed to understand whether our results are generalisable to young people who have different experiences related to their identity or socioeconomic background. In addition, given that the larger study was not designed for the analyses employed in this study, future studies should replicate these findings using a study design optimised for detecting meaningful effects using mixed effects models.

Conclusion

The current study suggests that the quality of friendships could be an important component in self-evaluations in early- and mid-adolescence in girls. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the relationship between friendship quality and positive self-judgements - such as positive feedback, increased perceived similarity with close friends, and reductions of cognitive biases - could inform current literature exploring self-concept training as a tool for integrating positive experiences into one’s self-concept (Van der Aar et al., 2022). In addition, we found a decrease in positivity effect from early adolescence and into mid-adolescence (Beer, 2014), which could be related to changes in the neurocognitive strategies underlying heightened negative self-appraisals during adolescence, such as the use of peer comparisons (Crone et al., 2022). Future studies should investigate how larger social and cultural structures (e.g., social inequalities and social media use) can amplify vulnerabilities related to neurocognitive development and mental health in young people (Choudhury et al., 2023).

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material - The Relationship Between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements in Adolescent Girls from London

Supplemental Material for The Relationship Between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements in Adolescent Girls from London by Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer, Jessica Evans, Katy Ratcliffe, Kaushalya Janaarthanan, Saz Ahmed, Willem Kuyken, Tim Dalgleish, and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore in The Journal of Early Adolescence

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the children and families who took part in the study.

Author Biographies

Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer is a third year PhD student at the Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge. Her research investigates the relationship between social inequalities and mental health difficulties in young people, with a particular interest in understanding the role of self-appraisals and social connectedness in this relationship.

Jessica Evans is a recent graduate from the University of Cambridge in Psychological and Behavioural Sciences. Her final year research project investigated the association between perceived friendship quality and negative self-judgements in adolescent girls.

Katy Ratcliffe is a recent graduate from the University of Cambridge in Psychological and Behavioural Sciences. Her final year research project investigated the association between perceived friendship quality and negative self-judgements in adolescent girls.

Kaushalya Janaarthanan is a fourth-year medical student at the University of Cambridge. Her third year research project investigated the association between perceived friendship quality and negative self-judgements in adolescent girls.

Saz Ahmed is a Research Manager in Mental Health at the Wellcome Trust and an honorary Research Fellow at the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL. Her research focuses on adolescent social cognition and mental health.

Professor Willem Kuyken is the Ritblat Professor of Mindfulness and Psychological Science at the University of Oxford and Director of the University of Oxford Mindfulness Research Group. His research group focus on psychological mechanisms and psychological approaches to depression, mental health and flourishing.

Tim Dalgleish is a clinical psychologist. He works both as a research scientist for the Medical Research Council and as a practitioner in the NHS. His research focuses on understanding and developing psychological preventions and treatments for common mental health problems, such as depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress. He adopts a translational approach, seeking to utilise insights from basic cognitive neuroscience to enhance clinical interventions for these conditions.

Sarah-Jayne Blakemore is Professor of Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience and leader of the Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Group at the University of Cambridge. Her group's research focuses on the development of social cognition and decision making in the human adolescent brain, and adolescent mental health.

Author Contributions: S.A. and S.-J.B. designed the study; S.A. and B.P.P. organised testing and collected data; B.P.P., J.E., K.R. and K.J. participated in the analysis of the data; all authors participated in the interpretation of the data, as well as in drafting and revising the manuscript, and have approved the submitted version.

The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Declarations of interest are regularly updated on the Blakemore Lab website (https://sites.google.com/site/blakemorelab/code-of-conduct/declarations-of-interest).

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by the Wellcome Trust WT107496/Z/15/Z (SA, WK, TD, S-JB), the UK Medical Research Council (BPP), the Gates Cambridge Foundation (BPP), the Jacobs Foundation (S-JB), the Wellspring Foundation (S-JB) and the University of Cambridge (S-JB).

Rights of Use: For the purpose of open access, the authors have applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

Supplemental Material: Supplemental material for this article is available online

ORCID iDs

Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6707-9943

Katy Ratcliffe https://orcid.org/0009-0001-4216-7883

Data Availability Statement

The analysis scripts and data used in this study can be found in the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/dq9p3/).

References

  1. Ahmed S. P., Piera Pi-Sunyer B., Moses-Payne M. E., Goddings A. L., Speyer L. G., Kuyken W., Dalgleish T., Blakemore S. J. (2024). The role of self-referential and social processing in the relationship between pubertal status and difficulties in mental health and emotion regulation in adolescent girls in the UK. Developmental Science, 27(4), e13503. 10.1111/DESC.13503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albert K. M., Newhouse P. A. (2019). Estrogen, stress, and depression: Cognitive and biological interactions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 399–423. 10.1146/ANNUREV-CLINPSY-050718-095557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alsarrani A., Hunter R. F., Dunne L., Garcia L. (2022). Association between friendship quality and subjective wellbeing among adolescents: A systematic review. BMC Public Health 22(1), 2420–2437. 10.1186/S12889-022-14776-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aron A., Lewandowski G., Branand B., Mashek D., Aron E. (2022). Self-expansion motivation and inclusion of others in self: An updated review. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 39(12), 3821–3852. 10.1177/02654075221110630 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bagwell C., Schmidt M. E. (2011). Friendships in childhood & adolescence (389). Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  6. Barendse M. E. A., Cosme D., Flournoy J. C., Vijayakumar N., Cheng T. W., Allen N. B., Pfeifer J. H. (2020). Neural correlates of self-evaluation in relation to age and pubertal development in early adolescent girls. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 44, 100799. 10.1016/J.DCN.2020.100799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barzeva S. A., Richards J. S., Veenstra R., Meeus W. H. J., Oldehinkel A. J. (2022). Quality over quantity: A transactional model of social withdrawal and friendship development in late adolescence. Social Development, 31(1), 126–146. 10.1111/SODE.12530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beer J. S. (2014). Exaggerated positivity in self-evaluation: A social neuroscience approach to reconciling the role of self-esteem protection and cognitive bias. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(10), 583–594. 10.1111/SPC3.12133 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ben-Shachar M. S., Lüdecke D., Makowski D. (2020). Effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. The Journal of Open Source Software, 5(56), 2815. 10.21105/joss.02815 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berndt T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 7–10. 10.1111/1467-8721.00157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bilker W. B., Hansen J. A., Brensinger C. M., Richard J., Gur R. E., Gur R. C. (2012). Development of abbreviated nine-item forms of the Raven’s standard progressive Matrices test. Assessment, 19(3), 354–369. 10.1177/1073191112446655 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blakemore S. J. (2019). Adolescence and mental health. Lancet (London, England), 393(10185), 2030–2031. 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)31013-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bone J. K., Lewis G., Roiser J. P., Blakemore S. J., Lewis G. (2021). Recall bias during adolescence: Gender differences and associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 282, 299–307. 10.1016/J.JAD.2020.12.133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Breiner K., Li A., Cohen A. O., Steinberg L., Bonnie R. J., Scott E. S., Taylor-Thompson K., Rudolph M. D., Chein J., Richeson J. A., Dellarco D. V., Fair D. A., Casey B. J., Galván A. (2018). Combined effects of peer presence, social cues, and rewards on cognitive control in adolescents. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(3), 292–302. 10.1002/DEV.21599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Burnett Heyes S., Jih Y. R., Block P., Hiu C. F., Holmes E. A., Lau J. Y. F. (2015). Relationship reciprocation modulates resource allocation in adolescent social networks: Developmental effects. Child Development, 86(5), 1489–1506. 10.1111/CDEV.12396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Campbell O. L. K., Bann D., Patalay P. (2021). The gender gap in adolescent mental health: A cross-national investigation of 566,829 adolescents across 73 countries. SSM - Population Health, 13, 100742. 10.1016/J.SSMPH.2021.100742 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carskadon M. A., Acebo C. (1993). A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 14(3), 190–195. 10.1016/1054-139X(93)90004-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Choudhury S., Piera Pi-Sunyer B., Blakemore S.-J. (2023). A neuroecosocial perspective on adolescent development. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 5(1), 285–307. 10.1146/ANNUREV-DEVPSYCH-120321-011511 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Christina S., Magson N. R., Kakar V., Rapee R. M. (2021). The bidirectional relationships between peer victimization and internalizing problems in school-aged children: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 85, 101979. 10.1016/J.CPR.2021.101979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cole D. A., Zelkowitz R. L., Nick E., Martin N. C., Roeder K. M., Sinclair-McBride K., Spinelli T. (2016). Longitudinal and incremental relation of cybervictimization to negative self-cognitions and depressive symptoms in young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(7), 1321–1332. 10.1007/s10802-015-0123-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crone E. A., Green K. H., van de Groep I. H., van der Cruijsen R. (2022). A neurocognitive model of self-concept development in adolescence. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 273–295. 10.1146/ANNUREV-DEVPSYCH-120920-023842 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Crone E. A., Steinbeis N. (2017). Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(3), 205–215. 10.1016/J.TICS.2017.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cuadros O., Berger C. (2022). Self-disclosure, self-concept, and friendship’s perceived functions among aggressive and popular adolescents. Youth and Society, 55(8), 1415–1437. 10.1177/0044118X221093394 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cunningham S. J., Brebner J. L., Quinn F., Turk D. J. (2014). The self-reference effect on memory in early childhood. Child Development, 85(2), 808–823. 10.1111/CDEV.12144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cunningham S. J., Turk D. J. (2017). Editorial: A review of self-processing biases in cognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(6), 987–995. 10.1080/17470218.2016.1276609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Deardorff J., Hoyt L. T., Carter R., Shirtcliff E. A. (2019). Next steps in puberty research: Broadening the lens toward understudied populations. Journal of Research on Adolescence: The Official Journal of the Society for Research on Adolescence, 29(1), 133–154. 10.1111/JORA.12402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dorn L. D. (2006). Measuring puberty. Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 39(5), 625–626. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dumontheil I., Apperly I. A., Blakemore S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338. 10.1111/J.1467-7687.2009.00888.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Duru E., Balkis M., Turkdoğan T. (2019). Relational violence, social support, self-esteem, depression and anxiety: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(9), 2404–2414. 10.1007/s10826-019-01509-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Flannery K. M., Smith R. L. (2017. a). Are peer status, friendship quality, and friendship stability equivalent markers of social competence? Adolescent Research Review, 2(4), 331–340. 10.1007/s40894-016-0042-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Flannery K. M., Smith R. L. (2017. b). The effects of age, gender, and gender role ideology on adolescents’ social perspective-taking ability and tendency in friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 34(5), 617–635. 10.1177/0265407516650942 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Flannery K. M., Smith R. L. (2021). Breaking up (with a friend) is hard to do: An examination of friendship dissolution among early adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 41(9)1368–1393. 10.1177/02724316211002266 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gardner W. L., Gabriel S., Hochschild L. (2002). When you and I are ‘we,’ you are not threatening: The role of self-expansion in social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(2), 239–251. 10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gerber J. P., Wheeler L., Suls J. (2018). A social comparison theory meta-analysis 60+ years on. Psychological Bulletin, 144(2), 177–197. 10.1037/BUL0000127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goddings A. L., Burnett Heyes S., Bird G., Viner R. M., Blakemore S. J. (2012). The relationship between puberty and social emotion processing. Developmental Science, 15(6), 801–811. 10.1111/J.1467-7687.2012.01174.X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Goemans A., Viding E., McCrory E. (2023). Child maltreatment, peer victimization, and mental health: Neurocognitive perspectives on the cycle of victimization. Trauma, Violence and Abuse, 24(2), 530–548. 10.1177/15248380211036393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Goodyer I. M., Wright C., Altham P. M. E. (1989). Recent friendships in anxious and depressed school age children. Psychological Medicine, 19(1), 165–174. 10.1017/S0033291700011119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gorrese A., Ruggieri R. (2013). Peer attachment and self-esteem: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 559–568. 10.1016/J.PAID.2013.04.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Güroğlu B. (2022). The power of friendship: The developmental significance of friendships from a neuroscience perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 16(2), 110–117. 10.1111/CDEP.12450 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Güroglu B., Bos W., Crone E. A. (2014). Sharing and giving across adolescence: An experimental study examining the development of prosocial behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(APR), 291. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Harris M. A., Orth U. (2020). The link between self-esteem and social relationships: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(6), 1459–1477. 10.1037/PSPP0000265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Harter S. (2012). Emerging self-processes during childhood and adolescence. The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Jankowski K. F., Moore W. E., Merchant J. S., Kahn L. E., Pfeifer J. H. (2014). But do you think I’m cool? Developmental differences in striatal recruitment during direct and reflected social self-evaluations. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 40–54. 10.1016/J.DCN.2014.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Joinson C., Heron J., Araya R., Paus T., Croudace T., Rubin C., Marcus M., Lewis G. (2012). Association between pubertal development and depressive symptoms in girls from a UK cohort. Psychological Medicine, 42(12), 2579–2589. 10.1017/S003329171200061X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kandel D. B. (2017). Hom ophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent Friendships1. Interpersonal Development, 84(2), 249–258. 10.4324/9781351153683-14 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kuznetsova A., Brockhoff P. B., Christensen R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. 10.18637/JSS.V082.I13 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lenth R. V., Bolker B., Buerkner P., Giné-Vázquez I., Herve M., Jung M., Love J., Miguez F., Riebl H., Singmann H. (2022). Package ‘lsmeans’ title least-squares means. 10.1080/00031305.1980.10483031 [DOI]
  48. Maunder R., Monks C. P. (2019). Friendships in middle childhood: Links to peer and school identification, and general self-worth. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 211–229. 10.1111/BJDP.12268 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McArthur B. A., Burke T. A., Connolly S. L., Olino T. M., Lumley M. N., Abramson L. Y., Alloy L. B. (2019). A longitudinal investigation of cognitive self-schemas across adolescent development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(3), 635–647. 10.1007/s10964-018-00981-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mendle J., Beam C. R., McKone K. M. P., Koch M. K. (2020). Puberty and transdiagnostic risks for mental health. Journal of Research on Adolescence: The Official Journal of the Society for Research on Adolescence, 30(3), 687–705. 10.1111/JORA.12552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Morry M. M., Sucharyna T. A. (2019). Relationship social comparisons in dating and marital relationships: Adding relationship social comparison interpretations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159(4), 398–416. 10.1080/00224545.2018.1498826 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moses-Payne M. E., Chierchia G., Blakemore S. J. (2022). Age-related changes in the impact of valence on self-referential processing in female adolescents and young adults. Cognitive Development, 61, 101128. 10.1016/J.COGDEV.2021.101128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moses-Payne M. E., Habicht J., Bowler A., Steinbeis N., Hauser T. U. (2021). I know better! Emerging metacognition allows adolescents to ignore false advice. Developmental Science, 24(5), Article e13101. 10.1111/DESC.13101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Noon E. J., Meier A. (2019). Inspired by friends: Adolescents’ network homophily moderates the relationship between social comparison, envy, and inspiration on instagram. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 22(12), 787–793. 10.1089/CYBER.2019.0412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Norrington J. (2021). Adolescent peer victimization, self-concept, and psychological distress in emerging adulthood. Youth and Society, 53(2), 273–295. 10.1177/0044118x20910938 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Orth U., Erol R. Y., Luciano E. C. (2018). Development of self-esteem from age 4 to 94 years: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1045–1080. 10.1037/BUL0000161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Patalay P., Fitzsimons E. (2017). Mental ill-health among children of the new century. https://www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/
  58. Petersen A. C., Crockett L., Richards M., Boxer A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–133. 10.1007/BF01537962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pfeifer J. H., Allen N. B. (2021). Puberty initiates cascading relationships between neurodevelopmental, social, and internalizing processes across adolescence. Biological Psychiatry, 89(2), 99–108. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2020.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pfeifer J. H., Berkman E. T. (2018). The development of self and identity in adolescence: Neural evidence and implications for a value-based choice perspective on motivated behavior. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 158–164. 10.1111/CDEP.12279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pfeifer J. H., Kahn L. E., Merchant J. S., Peake S. J., Veroude K., Masten C. L., Lieberman M. D., Mazziotta J. C., Dapretto M. (2013). Longitudinal change in the neural bases of adolescent social self-evaluations: Effects of age and pubertal development. Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(17), 7415–7419. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4074-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pfeifer J. H., Masten C. L., Borofsky L. A., Dapretto M., Fuligni A. J., Lieberman M. D. (2009). Neural correlates of direct and reflected self-appraisals in adolescents and adults: When social perspective-taking informs self-perception. Child Development, 80(4), 1016–1038. 10.1111/J.1467-8624.2009.01314.X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pfeifer J. H., Peake S. J. (2012). Self-development: Integrating cognitive, socioemotional, and neuroimaging perspectives. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 55–69. 10.1016/J.DCN.2011.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pickering L., Hadwin J. A., Kovshoff H. (2020). The role of peers in the development of social anxiety in adolescent girls: A systematic review. Adolescent Research Review, 5(4), 341–362. 10.1007/s40894-019-00117-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Poulin F., Chan A. (2010). Friendship stability and change in childhood and adolescence. Developmental Review, 30(3), 257–272. 10.1016/J.DR.2009.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rankin J. L., Lane D. J., Gibbons F. X., Gerrard M. (2004). Adolescent self-consciousness: Longitudinal age changes and gender differences in two cohorts. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14(1), 1–21. 10.1111/J.1532-7795.2004.01401001.X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rapee R. M., Oar E. L., Johnco C. J., Forbes M. K., Fardouly J., Magson N. R., Richardson C. E. (2019). Adolescent development and risk for the onset of social-emotional disorders: A review and conceptual model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 123, 103501. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2019.103501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. R Core Team . (2023). R: The R project for statistical computing. https://www.r-project.org/
  69. Rieger S., Göllner R., Trautwein U., Roberts B. W. (2016). Low self-esteem prospectively predicts depression in the transition to young adulthood: A replication of Orth, robins, and roberts (2008). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(1), e16–e22. 10.1037/PSPP0000037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Robinson D. B., Mitton J., Hadley G., Kettley M. (2021). Single-sex education in the 21st century: A 20-year scoping review of the literature. Teaching and Teacher Education, 106, 103462. 10.1016/J.TATE.2021.103462 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rodman A. M., Powers K. E., Somerville L. H. (2017). Development of self-protective biases in response to social evaluative feedback. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(50), 13158–13163. 10.1073/pnas.1712398114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Roeder K. M., Cole D. A. (2018). Prospective relation between peer victimization and suicidal ideation: Potential cognitive mediators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(6), 769–781. 10.1007/s10608-018-9939-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Roeder K. M., Cole D. A., Sinclair K. R., Dukewich T. L., Preacher K. J., Felton J. W., Jacky A., Tilghman-Osborne C. (2014). Sensitive periods for the effect of peer victimization on self-cognition: Moderation by age and gender. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4 Pt 1), 1035–1048. 10.1017/S0954579414000601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rokeach A., Wiener J. (2022). Predictors of friendship quality in adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Mental Health, 14(2), 328–340. 10.1007/S12310-022-09508-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rudolph K. D. (2017). Advances in conceptual and empirical approaches to understanding the interpersonal context of youth depression: Commentary. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 51, 65–69. 10.1016/j.appdev.2017.05.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rueger S. Y., Malecki C. K., Demaray M. K. (2010). Relationship between multiple sources of perceived social support and psychological and academic adjustment in early adolescence: Comparisons across gender. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(1), 47–61. 10.1007/s10964-008-9368-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sawyer S. M., Azzopardi P. S., Wickremarathne D., Patton G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. The Lancet. Child and adolescent health, 2(3), 223–228. 10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sebastian C., Burnett S., Blakemore S. J. (2008). Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 441–446. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Shapka J. D., Keating D. P. (2005). Structure and change in self-concept during adolescence. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement, 37(2), 83–96. 10.1037/H0087247 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shapka J. D., Khan S. (2011). Self-perception. In Encyclopedia of Adolescence. New York: Springer. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1695-2_481 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. Slagter S. K., Gradassi A., van Duijvenvoorde A. C. K., van den Bos W. (2023). Identifying who adolescents perfer as source of information within their social network. Scientific Reports, 13. 10.1038/s41598-023-46994-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Somerville L. H. (2013). Special issue on the teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 121–127. 10.1177/0963721413476512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Somerville L. H., Jones R. M., Ruberry E. J., Dyke J. P., Glover G., Casey B. J. (2013). The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion in adolescence. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1554–1562. 10.1177/0956797613475633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Somerville L. H., Kelley W. M., Heatherton T. F. (2010). Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal cortical responses to evaluative social feedback. Cerebral Cortex, 20(12), 3005–3013. 10.1093/CERCOR/BHQ049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Stadtfeld C., Takács K., Vörös A. (2020). The emergence and stability of groups in social networks. Social Networks, 60, 129–145. 10.1016/J.SOCNET.2019.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Stewart J. G., Harkness K. L. (2017). Testing a revised interpersonal theory of depression using a laboratory measure of excessive reassurance seeking. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 331–348. 10.1002/JCLP.22338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sui J., Humphreys G. W. (2015). The integrative self: How self-reference integrates perception and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 719–728. 10.1016/J.TICS.2015.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Tajmirriyahi M., Ickes W. (2020). Self-concept clarity as a predictor of self-disclosure in romantic relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(6), 1873–1891. 10.1177/0265407520911131 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tarrant M., MacKenzie L., Hewitt L. A. (2006). Friendship group identification, multidimensional self-concept, and experience of developmental tasks in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 29(4), 627–640. 10.1016/J.ADOLESCENCE.2005.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tomova L., Andrews J. L., Blakemore S. J. (2021). The importance of belonging and the avoidance of social risk taking in adolescence. Developmental Review, 61, 100981. 10.1016/J.DR.2021.100981 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Towner E., Grint J., Levy T., Blakemore S. J., Tomova L. (2022). Revealing the self in a digital world: A systematic review of adolescent online and offline self-disclosure. Current Opinion in Psychology, 45, 101309. 10.1016/J.COPSYC.2022.101309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Valls M. (2022). Gender differences in social comparison processes and self-concept among students. Frontiers in Education, 6, 815619. 10.3389/feduc.2021.815619 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. van Buuren M., Walsh R. J., Sijtsma H., Hollarek M., Lee N. C., Bos P. A., Krabbendam L. (2020). Neural correlates of self- and other-referential processing in young adolescents and the effects of testosterone and peer similarity. NeuroImage, 219, 117060. 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.117060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Van der Aar L. P. E., Peters S., Becht A. I., Crone E. A. (2022). Better self-concept, better future choices? Behavioral and neural changes after a naturalistic self-concept training program for adolescents. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 22(2), 341–361. 10.3758/s13415-021-00946-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. van der Aar L. P. E., Peters S., Crone E. A. (2018). The development of self-views across adolescence: Investigating self-descriptions with and without social comparison using a novel experimental paradigm. Cognitive Development, 48, 256–270. 10.1016/J.COGDEV.2018.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. van der Cruijsen R., Blankenstein N. E., Spaans J. P., Peters S., Crone E. A. (2023). Longitudinal self-concept development in adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 18(1), Article nsac062. 10.1093/SCAN/NSAC062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. van der Cruijsen R., Peters S., van der Aar L. P. E., Crone E. A. (2018). The neural signature of self-concept development in adolescence: The role of domain and valence distinctions. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 1–12. 10.1016/J.DCN.2017.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Van der Cruijsen R., Peters S., Zoetendaal K. P. M., Pfeifer J. H., Crone E. A. (2019). Direct and reflected self-concept show increasing similarity across adolescence: A functional neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 129, 407–417. 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2019.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Van der Graaff J., Branje S., De Wied M., Hawk S., Van Lier P., Meeus W. (2014). Perspective taking and empathic concern in adolescence: Gender differences in developmental changes. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 881–888. 10.1037/A0034325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. van Harmelen A. L., Blakemore S. J., Goodyer I. M., Kievit R. A. (2021). The interplay between adolescent friendship quality and resilient functioning following childhood and adolescent adversity. Adversity and Resilience Science, 2(1), 37–50. 10.1007/s42844-020-00027-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. van Harmelen A. L., Gibson J. L., St Clair M. C., Owens M., Brodbeck J., Dunn V., Lewis G., Croudace T., Jones P. B., Kievit R. A., Goodyer I. M. (2016). Friendships and family support reduce subsequent depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents. PLoS One, 11(5), Article e0153715. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0153715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Van Harmelen A. L., Kievit R. A., Ioannidis K., Neufeld S., Jones P. B., Bullmore E., Dolan R., NSPN Consortium. Fonagy P., Goodyer I. (2017). Adolescent friendships predict later resilient functioning across psychosocial domains in a healthy community cohort. Psychological Medicine, 47(13), 2312–2322. 10.1017/S0033291717000836 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Vijayakumar N., Pfeifer J. H. (2020). Self-disclosure during adolescence: Exploring the means, targets, and types of personal exchanges. Current Opinion in Psychology, 31, 135–140. 10.1016/J.COPSYC.2019.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Ybrandt H. (2008). The relation between self-concept and social functioning in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 31(1), 1–16. 10.1016/J.ADOLESCENCE.2007.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Yoon L., Somerville L. H., Kim H. (2018). Development of MPFC function mediates shifts in self-protective behavior provoked by social feedback. Nature Communications, 9(1), 3086–3110. 10.1038/s41467-018-05553-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material - The Relationship Between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements in Adolescent Girls from London

Supplemental Material for The Relationship Between Perceived Friendship Quality and Self-Judgements in Adolescent Girls from London by Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer, Jessica Evans, Katy Ratcliffe, Kaushalya Janaarthanan, Saz Ahmed, Willem Kuyken, Tim Dalgleish, and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore in The Journal of Early Adolescence

Data Availability Statement

The analysis scripts and data used in this study can be found in the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/dq9p3/).


Articles from The Journal of Early Adolescence are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES