Skip to main content
Brain Communications logoLink to Brain Communications
. 2025 Aug 28;7(5):fcaf309. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaf309

Sleep deprivation affects memory function, depression and anxiety-like behaviours in rats and mice: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Xiaofan Zhang 1,2,3, Cheng-wei Liu 4, Xin Sheng 5, Yifan Jiang 6, Sheng Zhang 7, XiaoYan Mo 8, Yuan Yang 9,10,, Fengfei Ding 11,
PMCID: PMC12455040  PMID: 40994825

Abstract

Sleep deprivation paradigms have been employed in rat and mouse models to elucidate the function of sleep. The effects of sleep deprivation on memory function, as well as changes in depression- and anxiety-like behaviours, have been extensively investigated; however, the findings have often been inconsistent. In the present study, we conducted a comprehensive literature review of researches utilizing sleep deprivation paradigms in both rats and mice. A total of 164 original studies were analysed to extract results from behavioural tests concerning memory function and depression- and anxiety-like behaviours in wild-type rats or mice before and after sleep deprivation. The meta-analysis revealed that sleep deprivation consistently impaired memory function, irrespective of the paradigms, durations and species involved [P = 0.000, SMD (standardized mean difference) 95% CI (confidence intervals at 95%): −0.73 (−0.89, −0.57) for sleep deprivation; P = 0.000, SMD (95% CI): −0.75 (−0.93, −0.57) for rapid eye movement sleep deprivation]. Similar, albeit less pronounced, effects were observed on depression-like behaviours [P = 0.000, SMD (95% CI): −0.41 (−0.52, −0.29) for sleep deprivation; P = 0.000, SMD (95% CI): −0.60 (−0.79, −0.42) for rapid eye movement sleep deprivation]. The impact of sleep deprivation on anxiety-like behaviours was more variable. When considering both mice and rats, sleep deprivation generally exhibited anxiogenic effects [P = 0.049, SMD (95% CI): −0.19 (−0.39, −0.00) for sleep deprivation; P = 0.705, SMD (95% CI): 0.04 (−0.18, 0.27) for rapid eye movement sleep deprivation]. However, subgroup analyses indicated that rodent species and sleep durations demonstrated distinct responses to sleep deprivation. This study provides critical insights for selecting optimal paradigms, durations, species and behavioural tests in experimental designs.

Keywords: sleep deprivation, memory, depression, anxiety, species


Zhang et al. report that sleep deprivation consistently impairs memory function but generates diverse effects on depression and anxiety-like behaviours due to factors such as species, sex, age, or interventional paradigms, etc.

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract.

Graphical Abstract

Introduction

Sleep is a fundamental biological process that is highly conserved across various species. The mechanisms and functions of sleep remain subjects of active investigation, with numerous scientific questions still unanswered.1 In humans, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep occur in five to six periodic episodes throughout the course of a single night, each serving distinct physiological functions.2 Although the circadian rhythms in response to light are reversed in rodents such as rats or mice compared with humans, both NREM and REM sleep exhibit remarkable conservation between these groups.3 Experimental protocols for sleep deprivation (SD), which simulate sleep loss or restriction, have been extensively utilized to unravel the complexities of sleep.4,5 Commonly employed paradigms for sleep manipulation include total sleep deprivation (TSD), which keeps animals awake for a specified duration; rapid eye movement sleep deprivation (REMSD), which primarily suppresses REM sleep; and sleep fragmentation (SF), which induces intermittent awakenings during sleep periods.6-8

In healthy individuals experiencing SD, impaired memory, as well as symptoms of anxiety and depression, are commonly observed, which attract the attention of clinicians.9-11 This area of research is rapidly expanding, focusing on the functions of sleep and the effects of sleep insufficiency on behavioural outcomes.12 However, clinical studies often rely on self-reported questionnaires completed by insomnia patients to assess sleep loss, and they typically implemented acute, short-term interventions for SD or restriction in healthy individuals for ethical considerations.13-15 Animal models play crucial roles in elucidating the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying SD. Specifically, these models can be utilized for acute, chronic or selective SD, particularly in the context of REMSD.16-18

In recent decades, the volume of research on SD has increased dramatically revealing a wide array of behavioural outcomes in response to similar SD paradigms.19-21 These discrepancies may be attributed to variations in experimental paradigms, modelling durations and the species of animals utilized in SD studies. Additionally, the differences in SD protocols and behavioural assessments conducted by various researchers under diverse conditions may lead to divergent conclusions, potentially arising from systematic errors. Factors such as room temperature, humidity, seasonal variations and the sex of the rats and mice, among numerous others, may significantly influence the results of behavioural tests.22-24

In this study, we conducted a comprehensive literature review of experimental studies employing SD paradigms in rat or mouse models. TSD and SF can reduce or disrupt both NREM and REM sleep. Given that REM sleep can only occur following deep NREM sleep, disturbances in NREM sleep inevitably impact REM sleep. REMSD is a straightforward method that predominantly suppresses REM sleep. In this review, we categorized the studies utilizing REMSD alongside other methods to summarize the behavioural outcomes associated with REMSD and mixed studies that disrupt both REM and NREM sleep. Initially, we performed a global analysis to summarize the behavioural outcomes of experimental SD on memory function, as well as depression- and anxiety-like behaviours in wild-type adult rats and mice under both REMSD and SD (including all paradigms). Furthermore, we conducted subgroup analyses of REMSD and SD protocols to evaluate the isolated effects of species (rats versus mice) and experimental duration (short-term versus long-term protocols) on various behavioural paradigms. We also discussed potential confounding factors that may bias the behavioural outcomes. Finally, we compared the effect sizes of typical parameters from the behavioural tests to identify those with optimal detecting performance.

Materials and methods

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Search strategy

A comprehensive search of studies using experiments on SD was conducted using three databases: Pubmed, Scopus and Embase. The search strategy employed in PubMed was as follows: (((‘Sleep Deprivation”[Mesh]) OR (sleep restriction)) AND (animal [Filter])) AND (rodent OR rat OR mice OR mouse) AND (animal [Filter])). Filters applied included Other Animals and English language. Experimental SD was defined as housing the animals under specific conditions or in SD apparatuses, while control animals were maintained in a standard feeding environment. The search strategies utilized in Scopus and Embase are detailed in Supplementary Table 1. All types of literature published prior to July 2023 were included. Articles were selected through a two-round process. In the first round, titles and abstracts were screened for relevance to the study's aims; in the second round, full-text articles were selected for data extraction. The screening process was conducted by Xin Sheng and verified by Chengwei Liu, Xiaofan Zhang and Fengfei Ding. Any disagreements were resolved through discussion among the authors.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

A comprehensive search of studies involving SD experiments was conducted using three databases: PubMed, Scopus and Embase. The behavioural tests employed to assess memory function include the fear conditioning test (FC), Morris water maze test (MWM), novel object recognition test (NOR), novel location recognition test (NLR), passive avoidance test (PAT), plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (PMDAT) and radial arm water maze task (RAWM).25-30 Behavioural tests used to evaluate depression-like behaviours comprise the forced swimming test (FST), sucrose preference test (SPT) and tail suspension test (TST).31-33 Additionally, the behavioural tests assessing anxiety-like behaviours include the elevated plus maze test (EPM), open field test (OF) and PMDAT.34-36 The parameters of each behavioural test are presented in Supplementary Table 2. Included studies should identify SD as the independent variable and behavioural changes as the dependent outcomes. For studies incorporating additional variables beyond SD, such as transgenic manipulation or pharmacological treatments, data from littermates or vehicle controls subjected to SD protocols were also considered. Studies excluded from our analysis included: (i) reviews/systematic reviews; (ii) studies involving other animal species; (iii) those reporting only title/abstract; and (iv) studies with incompatible designs, including those lacking data for calculating standardized mean values and SDs, those without a defined primary outcome or control group, those that did not provide sample sizes and those exhibiting heterogeneity in behavioural experiments.

Data extraction and processing

Data were primarily extracted directly from written results in the text. If necessary, we inquired about the raw data from the authors via email. Subsequently, we manually measured the histograms using a digital ruler (Image J) to extract the mean and standard deviation values for studies that did not respond to our requests for results. For each experiment, detailed information regarding the SD protocols was compiled, including species information (mouse or rat), duration of SD and sample size (n). In cases where the sample size was reported as a range rather than exact numbers, we used the minimum value of the range as the sample size. The data were presented as the standard error of the mean (SEM), and the standard deviation was calculated using the formula: Standard deviation = SEM ×n.

To investigate the global effect of experimental SD on behavioural tests, we combined data from the same experimental setup across different articles into a single group. The details of the algorithm are provided in the supplementary methods.37

Statistical analysis

Meta-analysis

We conducted multiple meta-analyses with four clusters of datasets. Initially, all included studies were aggregated to assess the behavioural outcomes of SD, encompassing all paradigms. For each behavioural test parameter, we integrated the sample sizes and measurements from various studies to derive an overall sample size, mean and standard deviations, as detailed in the Supplementary material. Subsequently, we stratified the datasets into subgroups to examine factors influencing the results. Further meta-analyses were performed within these subgroups to compare REMSD versus SD protocols, short-term versus long-term protocols and species differences between mice and rats.

The analysis encompassed the first three clusters of datasets. Cluster one examines the overall effects of SD (including all paradigms) and REMSD protocols on memory function, as well as depression- and anxiety-like behaviours. Cluster two investigates the impact of deprivation protocols (SD or REMSD) on behavioural outcomes across species, specifically mice or rats. Cluster three evaluates the effects of deprivation protocols (SD or REMSD) on outcomes over varying durations, distinguishing between short-term and long-term deprivation. Protocols exceeding 72 h were categorized as long-term, while those lasting <72 h were classified as short-term.

We subsequently conducted a meta-analysis to identify behavioural tests and their respective parameters with sufficient sensitivity for detecting behavioural alterations following SD. We calculated the standardized mean difference (SMD) for each parameter of the behavioural tests across different studies using the Cohen method (Cluster Four).38 Unlike clusters one to three, which utilized combined data on sample size, mean and standard deviation from multiple studies, the analysis in cluster four relied on the extracted data from individual research studies.

We employed a DerSimonian and Laird random effects weighted mean difference meta-analysis to calculate the overall effect size for each dataset cluster.39 Data were presented as SMD with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI). Results were deemed significant if the confidence interval did not include zero and the associated Cochran's Q P-value was <0.05. All calculations were conducted using Stata 17 software (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). The effect size was further analysed using ANOVA in IBM SPSS version 25.0, with a P-value below 0.05 considered indicative of statistical significance.

Risk of bias and quality assessment

The selection bias was evaluated using SYRCLE's Risk of Bias (RoB) tool.40 To mitigate the influence of sample size in the subsequent subgroup meta-analysis, we generated a funnel plot based on the combined data from the global effects of SD analysis to assess publication bias. The publication bias of all selected articles was evaluated through visual analysis of the funnel plot (meta funnel plot, STATA Corporation, College Station, TX, USA).41 This funnel plot was constructed using the study's precision (standard error) and effect size. We employed Egger's regression test to assess potential publication bias.42 Visual inspection of the funnel plot revealed that effect sizes were evenly distributed across the top of the funnel, indicating no significant publication bias in the meta-analyses (Fig. 1A). A quantitative assessment using Begg's test further confirmed the absence of publication bias (P = 0.783, Fig. 1B). The shortlisted studies exhibited either low or unclear risk of bias in most assessment categories, according to SYRCLE's RoB tool (Fig. 1C). Given the absence of publication bias in the meta-analysis of global effects, we disregarded publication bias in the subsequent subgroup meta-analysis.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Publication bias with global effects of sleep deprivation. (A) The funnel plot was constructed to illustrate the relationship between the study's precision [standard error of the standardized mean difference, S.E. (SMD)] and effect sizes (SMD) (n = 32). (B) Begg's funnel plot was utilized to assess publication bias concerning the global effects of sleep deprivation. The horizontal line in the funnel plot represents the random-effects summary estimate, while the sloping lines indicate the expected 95% CI for the standard error (n = 32). (C) A summary of outcomes derived from the SYRCLE's Risk of Bias (RoB) assessment tool is presented (n = 164). Each dot in Fig. 1A and B represents the combined data from the analysis of the global effects of sleep deprivation. n, sample size; SMD, standardized mean difference.

Results

The study selection process is illustrated in Fig. 2. A total of 2813 articles were screened based on their abstracts, with full-text articles consulted as necessary. Following the initial screening, 2725 original studies underwent a comprehensive evaluation, resulting in the acquisition of full texts for 247 articles for data extraction. Articles featuring rats or mice younger than 8 weeks or older than 60 weeks were excluded, leading to the inclusion of 164 articles in the final meta-analysis. Among these, 17 articles focused exclusively on females, 134 on males, 9 on both genders and 4 did not specify gender. Some articles reported multiple behavioural assessments and SD paradigms, yielding a total of 371 indices for the current meta-analysis. These indices were derived from 164 articles (REMSD: n = 104, TSD: n = 44, Sleep Restriction: n = 5, Sleep Fragmentation: n = 6, REMSD and TSD: n = 2, REMSD and Sleep Restriction: n = 2, REMSD and Sleep Fragmentation: n = 1; Supplementary Table 2) and encompassed 306 experiments. Of the articles reviewed, 62 utilized mice and 102 utilized rats, with the following mouse strains represented: BALB/c (n = 3), C57BL/6 (n = 37), CD-1 (n = 1), DBA/2J (n = 1), ICR (n = 2), Swiss (n = 17) and 1 article did not disclose strain information. For rats, the strains included Long Evans (n = 4), Sprague Dawley (n = 24), Wistar (n = 72) and two articles did not disclose strain information.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Flowchart of the articles selection process.  n, sample size.

Among these, 109 articles and 207 experiments specifically investigated REMSD. A comprehensive description of these articles and experiments, including the details of the rat and mouse strains, duration of the studies and behavioural categories, is presented in Table 1. Additionally, the specifics of the variables included in the meta-analysis can be found in Supplementary Table 2.

Table 1.

The number of articles or experiments for subgroups

SD REMSD
Articles (n) Exp (N) Articles (N) Exp (N)
Total 164 306 109 207
Species
 Mice 62 130 30 71
 Rats 102 176 79 136
Duration
 Long-term 59 110 39 84
 Short-term 113 196 70 123
Memory function
 FC 24 28 8 9
 MWM 53 56 36 40
 NOR 28 35 17 22
 NLR 9 11 7 7
 PAT 14 23 12 18
 PMDAT 4 6 2 3
 RAWM 16 35 17 35
Depressive-like behaviour
 FST 22 35 12 20
 SPT 8 14 6 12
 TST 7 8 3 3
Anxiety-like behaviour
 EPM 20 20 12 15
 OF 24 31 16 22
 PMDAT 3 4 1 1

A single article may have employed more than one behavioural assessment. EPM, elevated plus maze test; FC, fear conditioning test; FST, forced swim test; MWM, Morris water maze test; n, sample size; NLR, novel location recognition test; NOR, novel object recognition test; OF, open field test; PAT, passive avoidance test; PMDAT, plus-maze discriminative avoidance task; RAWM, radial arm water maze test; REMSD, rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; SD, sleep deprivation; SPT, sucrose preference test; TST, tail suspend test.

Both SD and REMSD induced memory disruption and depression-like behaviours; only SD induced anxiety-like behaviour

Through a comprehensive meta-analysis of all parameters extracted from behavioural tests, we found that both SD (encompassing all SD paradigms) and REMSD significantly disrupted behaviours across all selected experiments involving rats and mice [P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.59 (−0.73, −0.44)for SD; P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.60 (−0.79, −0.42) for REMSD]. The subgroup meta-analysis revealed that SD was associated with the induction of anxiety-like behaviours [P = 0.049, SMD (95% CI): −0.19 (−0.39, −0.00)], depression-like behaviours [P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.41 (−0.52, −0.29)] and memory loss [P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.73 (−0.89, −0.57), Fig. 3]. In contrast, REMSD was found to induce only depression-like behaviours [P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.60 (−0.79, −0.42)] and memory loss [P < 0.001, SMD (95% CI): −0.75 (−0.93, −0.57)] but not anxiety-like behaviours [P = 0.705, SMD (95% CI): 0.04 (−0.18, 0.27)] in rats and mice (Fig. 4).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Forest plot for the effects of SD on anxiogenesis, depression and memory function loss, respectively. Memory function: control group sample size (Con-n) = 2514, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 2533. Depression-like behaviours: control group sample size (Con-n) = 588, experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 596. Anxiety-like behaviours: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 735, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 719. The black square and horizontal line represent each study's odds ratio and 95% CI, respectively, with a point estimate.43 The black diamond indicates the effect size of each study. The summary effect for each subgroup and overall is visualized as the diamond at the bottom of the plot.44 CI, confidence interval; EPM, elevated plus maze test; FC, fear conditioning test; FST, forced swim test; MWM, Morris water maze test; n, sample size; NLR, novel location recognition test; NOR, novel object recognition test; OF, open field test; PAT, passive avoidance test; PMDAT, plus-maze discriminative avoidance task; RAWM, radial arm water maze test; SD, sleep deprivation; SMD, standardized mean difference; SPT, sucrose preference test; TST, tail suspend test.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Forest plot for the effects of REMSD on anxiogenesis, depression and memory function loss, respectively. Memory function: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 1745, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 1768. Depression-like behaviours: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 371, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 372. Anxiety-like behaviours: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 438, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 446. The meanings of the black square, horizontal line and black and diamonds in the figure are consistent with those in Fig. 3. CI, confidence interval; EPM, elevated plus maze test; FC, fear conditioning test; FST, forced swim test; MWM, Morris water maze test; n, sample size; NLR, novel location recognition test; NOR, novel object recognition test; OF, open field test; PAT, passive avoidance test; PMDAT, plus-maze discriminative avoidance task; RAWM, radial arm water maze test; REMSD, rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; SMD, standardized mean difference; SPT, sucrose preference test; TST, tail suspend test.

Only SD-induced anxiety-like behaviour in rats

We conducted a meta-analysis based on studies involving different species to compare the effects of SD on anxiety-like behaviours between rats and mice. Our findings indicated that the SD protocols led to the emergence of anxiety-like behaviours in rats [P = 0.030, SMD (95% CI): −0.31 (−0.60, −0.03)] but not in mice [P = 0.088, SMD (95% CI): −0.27 (−0.57, 0.04), Fig. 5A]. In contrast, neither mice nor rats exhibited anxiolytic-like behaviours following REMSD [P = 0.453, SMD (95% CI): −0.17 (−0.61, 0.27) for mice; P = 0.844, SMD (95% CI): 0.06 (−0.54, 0.66) for rats, Fig. 5B]. Additionally, both species displayed depressive-like behaviours and memory loss following treatments with either SD or REMSD (Table 2).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Forest plots the effects of SD (A) and REMSD (B) on anxiety-like behaviours between mice and rats. (A) The overall effects of SD on anxiety-like behaviours in different species. (B) The overall effect of REMSD on anxiety-like behaviours in different species. Anxiety-like behaviours: SD: Mice: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 307, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 283; Rats: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 428, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 436. REMSD: Mice: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 224, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 233; Rats: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 214, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 213. The meanings of the black square, horizontal line and black and diamonds in the figure are consistent with those in Fig. 3. CI, confidence interval; EPM, elevated plus maze test; n, sample size; OF, open field test; PMDAT, plus-maze discriminative avoidance task; REMSD, rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; SD, sleep deprivation; SMD, standardized mean difference.

Table 2.

The pooled effects of SD and REMSD by different rodent species and deprivation protocol durations

Categories SMD 95%CI Random effects P-value
Rodent species
Memory function
 SD on mice −0.56 −0.82, −0.30 0.000***
 SD on rats −0.84 1.02, −0.67 0.000***
 REMSD on mice −0.56 −0.90, −0.22 0.001***
 REMSD on rats −0.86 −1.08, −0.65 0.000***
Depression-like behaviours
 SD on mice −0.36 −0.49, −0.23 0.000***
 SD on rats −0.93 −1.59, −0.27 0.005**
 REMSD on mice −1.23 −2.18, −0.28 0.011*
 REMSD on rats −0.93 −1.60, −0.27 0.006**
Anxiety-like behaviours
 SD on mice −0.27 −0.57, 0.04 0.088
 SD on rats −0.39 −0.70, −0.09 0.012*
 REMSD on mice −0.17 −0.61, 0.27 0.453
 REMSD on rats 0.06 −0.54, 0.66 0.844
Protocol duration
Memory function
 Long-term SD −0.99 −1.20, −0.78 0.000***
 Short-term SD −0.66 −0.83, −0.48 0.000***
 Long-term REMSD −1.10 −1.30, −0.89 0.000***
 Short-term REMSD −0.67 −0.89, −0.44 0.009**
Depression-like behaviours
 Long-term SD −0.25 −0.47, −0.03 0.003**
 Short-term SD −1.31 −2.19, −0.44 0.026*
 Long-term REMSD −1.36 −2.07, −0.65 0.000***
 Short-term REMSD −0.61 −0.94, −0.28 0.000***
Anxiety-like behaviours
 Long-term SD −0.04 −0.42, 0.34 0.829
 Short-term SD −0.24 −0.47, 0.00 0.054
 Long-term REMSD 0.03 −0.36, 0.43 0.548
 Short-term REMSD 0.12 0.27, 0.50 0.870

REMSD, rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; SD, sleep deprivation; SMD, the standardized mean difference; ∗P ≤ 0.05, ∗∗P ≤ 0.01, ∗∗∗P ≤ 0.001.

Short-term SD-induced anxiogenesis

Similarly, the analysis of pooled samples revealed that both long-term and short-term SD and REMSD induced depression-like behaviours and memory deficits in rats or mice (Table 2). Notably, only short-term SD was associated with increased anxiety levels in these animals, as indicated by a trend towards significance [short-term SD: P = 0.054, SMD (95% CI): −0.24 (−0.47, 0.00); long-term SD: P = 0.829, SMD (95% CI): −0.04 (−0.42, 0.34), as illustrated in Fig. 6A]. In contrast, neither short-term nor long-term REMSD produced significant alterations in anxiety-like behaviours [short-term REMSD: P = 0.548, SMD (95% CI): 0.12 (−0.27, 0.50); long-term REMSD: P = 0.870, SMD (95% CI): 0.03 (−0.36, 0.43), as depicted in Fig. 6B].

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Forest plot of long-term and short-term SD (A) and REMSD (B) on anxiety-like behaviours. (A) The overall effects of SD with different duration on anxiety-like behaviours. (B) The overall effects of REMSD with different duration on anxiety-like behaviours. SD: Long-term: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 296, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 292; Short-term: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 439, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 427. REMSD: Long-term: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 246, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 254; Short-term: Control group sample size (Con-n) = 192, Experimental group sample size (Exp-n) = 192. The meanings of the black square, horizontal line and black and diamonds in the figure are consistent with those in Fig. 3. CI, confidence interval; EPM, elevated plus maze test; n, sample size; OF, open field test; PMDAT, plus-maze discriminative avoidance task; REMSD, rapid eye movement sleep deprivation; SD, sleep deprivation; SMD, standardized mean difference.

The best parameters to detect memory disruption, depression and anxiety-like behaviours due to SD and REMSD

In the meta-analysis of Cluster four, we calculated the SMD for the individual parameters of the behavioural tests to identify those with adequate sensitivity for detection. The FC, MWM, NOR, NLR, PAT and RAWM are effective in detecting cognitive decline associated with both SD and REMSD through observable behavioural changes related to memory function. For assessing depression-like behaviours, the FST, SPT and TST proved to be reliable indicators of depressive behaviours following SD or REMSD. In terms of evaluating anxiety-like behaviours, the EPM successfully identified anxiety-like responses in both SD and REMSD conditions. Conversely, the PMDAT was limited to detecting changes specifically related to REMSD. Notably, the OF failed to identify anxiety-like behaviours in either SD or REMSD conditions (Supplementary Table 3).

The parameters with a P-value <0.05 in the meta-analysis (Supplementary Table 3) were plotted alongside their SMD, which reflect the sensitivity of detection regarding the behavioural outcomes of either SD or REMSD. Given that SD and REMSD exhibited similar effects across different species and experimental durations on memory function and depression-like behaviours, we focused solely on parameters derived from SD paradigms for these behavioural tests. The sensitivity of the parameters did not show significant differences among the memory function tests [F(14, 217) = 0.809, P = 0.659]. Notably, the number of platform crossings and the time spent (both in percentage and seconds) in the target quadrant during the MWM test demonstrated the highest average SMD among the parameters for detecting memory impairments induced by SD and REMSD (Fig. 7A). In terms of depression-like behaviours, the immobility score from the TST exhibited the most robust performance in detecting these behaviours in both SD and REMSD conditions (Fig. 7B). However, the sensitivity of the parameters did not reveal significant differences among the tests for depression-like behaviours [F(2, 54) = 2.587, P = 0.085].

Figure 7.

Figure 7

The effect size of the parameters to evaluate memory function and depressive-like behaviours after sleep deprivation. The differences were assessed using one-way ANOVA. The sensitivity of the parameters revealed no significant differences in (A) memory function tests [F(14, 217) = 0.809, P = 0.659] and (B) depressive-like behaviour tests [F(2, 54) = 2.587, P = 0.085]. The effect sizes of the selected parameters that demonstrated statistical significance were derived from the meta-analysis of cluster four (Supplementary Table 3). These parameters were obtained from behavioural tests evaluating memory function and depressive-like behaviours, including FC, FST, MWM, NLR, NOR, PAT, RAWM, TST and SPT. (A) Memory Function Tests: Freezing% context fear (FC), n = 25; Freezing% cue fear (FC), n = 9; Escape Latency(s) (MWM), n = 24; Distance% in target quadrant (MWM), n = 13; Number of platform crossing (MWM), n = 21; Time spent in target quadrant(s) (MWM), n = 13; Time% in target quadrant (MWM), n = 29; Discrimination index% (NLR), n = 9; Discrimination index% (NOR), n = 22; Recognition index% (NOR), n = 12; Long-term memory (PAT), n = 17; Time% in aversive enclosed arm (PMDAT), n = 3; Long-term memory 24 h (RAWM), n = 7; Long-term memory 5 h (RAWM), n = 11; Short-term memory 30 min (RAWM), n = 17. (B) Depressive-like Behaviour Tests: Immobility (S) (FST), n = 35; Sucrose preference% (SPT), n = 14; immobility (S) (TST), N = 8. Each dot represents the effect size generated from one experiment. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. FC, fear conditioning; FST, forced swim test; MWM, Morris water maze; n, sample size; NLR, novel location recognition; NOR, novel object recognition; PAT, passive avoidance test; RAWM, radial arm water maze; SD, sleep deprivation; TST, tail suspend test; SPT, the sucrose preference test.

The parameter representing the number of entries in the closed arm of the EPM exhibited comparable efficacy in detecting anxiety-like behaviours under both SD and REMSD conditions [one-way ANOVA: F(2, 27) = 0.111, P = 0.896; Supplementary Fig. 1]. The anxiety-like effects observed were sufficiently robust to indicate that they were independent of rodent species, as demonstrated by a two-way ANOVA revealing no significant effects for parameters [F(2, 31) = 0.513, P = 0.726) or species (F(1, 31) = 0.685, P = 0.414]. Furthermore, the duration of SD did not significantly influence anxiety-like behaviours, as indicated by the two-way ANOVA results for parameters [F(2, 31) = 0.040, P = 0.961) and duration (F(1, 31) = 0.003, P = 0.957; Supplementary Fig. 1].

However, the immobility score was found to be above zero in rats subjected to REMSD (Supplementary Fig. 2). In contrast to the findings observed in cluster two, REMSD appears to exert a mild anxiolytic effect in rats, with scores nearly above zero, while simultaneously eliciting an anxiogenic effect in mice (Supplementary Fig. 2A). Furthermore, this parameter demonstrated slightly enhanced sensitivity in detecting anxiety-like behaviours during short-term REMSD compared with long-term REMSD (Supplementary Fig. 2B). These findings underscore the importance of focusing on these parameters when evaluating the behavioural outcomes associated with SD.

Discussion

Through meta-analysis, we synthesized the experimental findings regarding the behavioural outcomes following SD interventions in both rats or mice. Our analysis indicates that SD protocols consistently lead to deficits in memory function, irrespective of the specific SD paradigms, modelling durations, or species involved (Figs 3 and 4 and Table 2). When comparing effect sizes, the MWM test, which assesses spatial memory function in rodents,45 emerged as the most sensitive behavioural assessment for memory function among the tests evaluated (Fig. 7). From an ethical standpoint, intensive SD interventions are typically limited to healthy young individuals for a maximum duration of a few days.46-48 Research has shown that a five-day sleep restriction impairs spatial working memory function without affecting verbal working memory or declarative memory in humans.49 Additionally, one study implementing mild sleep restriction over six weeks reported subsequent impairments in spatial orientation and vigilant attention.50 Our findings align with these observations in humans, highlighting the strong association between sleep loss, cognitive deficits and neurodegeneration.51,52 Both rodent and human studies have demonstrated that SD negatively impacts brain interstitial waste clearance.53-55 Therefore, the conclusions drawn from our research support the rationale for utilizing rat and mouse models to elucidate the mechanisms underlying sleep loss and to serve as valuable disease models for therapeutic development.

It was surprising to discover that both short-term and long-term SD or REMSD could induce depression-like behaviours in healthy rats and mice (Table 2). Traditional animal models of depression, such as chronic unpredictable mild stress and social isolation, typically require 1 to 2 months for effective modelling.56,57 This finding aligns with clinical observations indicating that chronic sleep loss can lead to depression in healthy individuals.58,59 Interestingly, some studies have reported that acute SD may produce an antidepressant effect in depressed patients,60-62 although these individuals often relapse into depression following a recovery night.63 In our analysis, we found that the immobility time measured by the TST exhibited the best detection performance compared with the sucrose preference percentage in the SPT and the immobility time in the TST (Fig. 7B). The TST is a commonly used assessment for screening potential antidepressant drugs.33 Additionally, SD protocols also yielded positive findings in both the FST and SPT. We speculate that the results of these assessments may be influenced by reduced locomotion due to sleep debt, particularly following acute SD. Therefore, caution is warranted when correlating depression-like behaviours in rodents with actual ‘depression’ following acute SD, as these models lack subjective emotional assessments.64 A recent study reported similar depression-like behavioural outcomes in the FST, SPT and TST following SD.65 While they reviewed the literature concerning REMSD, SD and various rodent species, they did not compare factors such as mice versus rats or REMSD versus SD, which would highlight the differential effects of various SD protocols.65

In comparison to the deficits in memory function and the presence of depression-like behaviours, the anxiety-like behaviours resulting from SD or REMSD were less consistent (Fig. 5). SD was found to induce anxiety-like behaviours in rats (Fig. 5A and Table 2), particularly with short-term SD, which demonstrated an overall anxiogenic effect (Fig. 6A; Table 2). Conversely, REMSD did not yield significant results across the overall battery of anxiety-like behaviours (Figs 5B and 6B;  Table 2), a finding that aligns with previous studies conducted on both mice and rats.13 Research on SD-induced anxiety-like behaviour in animal models has not mirrored the anxiety observed in humans following SD.4 Consequently, experimental SD has been deemed to lack translational value in this context.66-68 It is important to note that the mood-elevating effects of SD reported in some studies are not observed in healthy human volunteers.59 The subjects in our study were all wild-type rats and mice, receiving no additional treatments aside from the SD intervention; therefore, data on animal models of mood disorders remain limited. Furthermore, the response criteria used in clinical trials often rely on categorical judgments (e.g. ‘was helpful’ versus. ‘was not helpful’), which are based on personal subjective assessments, or they may refer to minimal differences in absolute scores or percentages from questionnaires.69,70 In contrast, response criteria in rodent studies primarily focus on behavioural phenotypes.71 This discrepancy in rating practices may contribute to the heterogeneity observed between clinical trial outcomes and experimental animal study results. Additionally, diverse behavioural outcomes reported in both rodents and humans, such as ‘hyperactivity,’ impulsivity, aggression and ‘manic’ states, could be associated with anxiety.72-77 REMSD has been shown to alter levels of various neurotransmitters, including dopamine, serotonin, norepinephrine and γ-aminobutyric acid, all of which are linked to adverse behavioural changes and other health consequences, including mania and various psychiatric disorders.78

Pires et al.4 summarized the anxiety-like behaviours resulting from SD, and their findings regarding the EPM, OF and PMDAT were consistent with our current analysis. However, contrary to the anxiolytic effects of REMSD reported in Pires’s study, we did not observe any significant impacts of REMSD on anxiety-like behaviours. It is worth noting that TSD may elicit a more pronounced stress response, with anxiogenic effects predominantly associated with TSD.79 In our study, 46 articles utilized TSD (Supplementary Table 2), while only 11 articles in Pires’s review involved TSD. This disparity suggests that TSD may play a significant role in the anxiogenesis observed in our findings. Moreover, our study incorporated multiple variables within each behavioural test paradigm, whereas Pires’s study restricted each analysis to a single variable category, potentially leading to a reduced sample size that could bias their results. Both our study and Pires et al. identified inconsistencies in anxiety-like behaviours resulting from SD. Specifically, we found that SD protocols induced anxiety-like behaviours in rats but not in mice [P = 0.030, SMD (95% CI): −0.31 (−0.60, −0.03) for rats; P = 0.088, SMD (95% CI): −0.27 (−0.57, 0.04) for mice; Fig. 5A]. This conclusion was based on the overall effects observed across all anxiety-like behavioural tests (EPM, PMDAT and OF). Notably, only the EPM effectively detected anxiety-like behaviours in the context of SD (Supplementary Table 3). We speculate that the limitations of certain studies may hinder the achievement of statistically significant results. Nevertheless, the findings in mice approached significance, with the upper limit of the 95% CI being 0.04, which is nearly zero. Consistently, parameters utilized in the EPM test indicated an anxiogenic effect in mice (Supplementary Fig. 2A). The non-significant result for mice suggests that some behavioural tests may lack sufficient sensitivity to assess anxiety-like behaviours in mice subjected to SD manipulation. Our study offers insights that may help clarify the inconsistencies observed in behavioural outcomes across different studies.

Similar to depression-like behaviours, evaluating anxiety-like behaviours in animal models is even more complex than in humans, primarily due to numerous confounding factors that can influence behavioural assessments, such as genetic background, sex and environmental conditions.80 In our review, we identified 14 articles that focused on anxiety-like behaviours (non-REMSD: n = 9, REMSD: n = 5) utilizing C57BL/6 mice, while seven articles (non-REMSD: n = 1, REMSD: n = 6) employed Swiss mice. Although C57BL/6 mice are generally recognized as being less active and exhibiting higher anxiety levels compared with Swiss mice, we are unable to draw definitive conclusions regarding the impact of mouse strains on the results due to the disproportionate representation of REMSD manipulations (Supplementary Table 2). Moreover, the timing of behavioural experiments was not specified in the majority of the studies we reviewed, which could represent a potential source of variability in the results. Despite these limitations, our analysis still indicated that species and the duration of SD might also act as confounding factors (Figs 5 and 6). Additionally, it is important to note that SD protocols involving consistent rotating bars or platforms may induce stress in both rats and mice, further complicating the interpretation of anxiety-like behaviours.81-83

In contrast, other SD protocols, such as manual gentle handling, tend to be less stressful.84 Therefore, SD can serve as a significant source of stress, particularly concerning the duration and intensity of the manipulation. It is nearly impossible to avoid inducing stress, especially during short-term SD interventions. Consequently, we may interpret the anxiety-like behaviours observed in this study as a result of the interplay between short-term SD and stress. Furthermore, animal models used in anxiety research often incorporate behavioural procedures designed to elicit a cognitive or affective state, such as learned helplessness.85 It is important to note that learned helplessness may manifest as symptomatic behaviour in rodents, particularly under the long-term effects observed in anxiety-like behavioural studies. Additionally, the concept of sleep debt, as previously discussed, should also be factored into the interpretation of anxiety-like behavioural test results following SD protocols. Both the EPM and OF tests can be influenced by changes in locomotor capabilities, which may introduce bias into the results.35,86 The relationship between sleep and anxiety is complex, making it challenging to draw clear conclusions in rat and mouse models.

Limitations

This meta-analysis concentrated on animal behavioural studies, yet many critical details regarding randomization, blinding, housing conditions and timing of outcome assessments were frequently unreported. Additionally, the studies exhibited varying methodological quality across different laboratories, which significantly impacts the overall quality of the findings. Our analysis included a minimum of three articles per comparison; however, sample size calculations are often absent in the animal literature, leading to the likelihood that many studies are underpowered. Consequently, potential selection bias was unavoidable. The research protocols for behavioural assessments have not been fully standardized, resulting in considerable heterogeneity in the pooled estimates. Furthermore, this meta-analysis did not account for the effects of gender on SD in rats and mice. Previous investigations have explored gender differences in stress responses to SD, revealing that behaviours are closely linked to sex hormone levels.87 However, the sample sizes of the included articles (16 focusing on females and nine on both genders, as shown in Supplementary Table 2) were insufficient (15.24%, 25/164) to adequately evaluate the impact of gender across various SD paradigms. Therefore, we opted for a mixed-gender approach in the current meta-analysis. Similarly, due to sample size limitations (as noted in Supplementary Table 4), we were unable to conduct a comprehensive meta-analysis encompassing total SD, sleep restriction and sleep fragmentation. To confirm our findings, large-scale prospective studies examining the effects of SD are still necessary in the future.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we assert that cognitive impairment induced by SD represents a stable phenotype warranting further mechanistic exploration. The depression- and anxiety-like behaviours resulting from SD are crucial for understanding the neurobiological processes underlying sleep, the pathogenesis of emotional disorders and their comorbidity with sleep disturbances. This study also offers valuable insights for selecting appropriate experimental models to evaluate the efficacy of therapeutics aimed at treating insomnia and its associated complications.

Supplementary Material

fcaf309_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgement

We thank Dr. WenHua Liu (statistician) for consultation with statistic analysis and Dr. Shijing Zhang for consultation with the literature search strategy.

Contributor Information

Xiaofan Zhang, Department of Pharmacology, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Shanghai Medical College, Fudan University, Shanghai 200032, China; Department of Neurology, Tongji Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430030, China; Hubei Key Laboratory of Neural Injury and Functional Reconstruction, Huazhong University of Science and Technology Wuhan 430030, China.

Cheng-wei Liu, Department of Pharmacology, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Shanghai Medical College, Fudan University, Shanghai 200032, China.

Xin Sheng, Department of Neurology, Tongji Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430030, China.

Yifan Jiang, Department of Neurology, Tongji Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430030, China.

Sheng Zhang, Center for Rehabilitation Medicine, Department of Neurology, Provincial People's Hospital, People's Hospital of Hangzhou Medical College, Hangzhou 310014, China.

XiaoYan Mo, Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, The Second People's Hospital of Kunming, Rehabilitation Hospital Affiliated to Kunming University, Kunming 650021, China.

Yuan Yang, Department of Neurology, Tongji Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430030, China; Hubei Key Laboratory of Neural Injury and Functional Reconstruction, Huazhong University of Science and Technology Wuhan 430030, China.

Fengfei Ding, Department of Pharmacology, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Shanghai Medical College, Fudan University, Shanghai 200032, China.

Data availability

The data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its supplementary materials. Any additional data or materials can be requested from the corresponding author. All calculations were conducted using Stata 17 software, following the guidelines outlined in the Stata meta-analysis reference manual (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX). For further details, please refer to the following link: https://www.stata.com/manuals17/meta.pdf.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Funding

This study was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology (2021YFC2502200), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82422028, 82371488, 82071542, 82090034) and the Shanghai Technology Innovative Action Plan (20ZR1403500, 21QA1401100).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  • 1. Scammell  TE, Arrigoni  E, Lipton  JO. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron. 2017;93(4):747–765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. McKillop  LE, Vyazovskiy  VV. Sleep and ageing: From human studies to rodent models. Curr Opin Physiol. 2020;15:210–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Van Twyver  H. Sleep patterns of five rodent species. Physiol Behav. 1969;4(6):901–905. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Pires  GN, Bezerra  AG, Tufik  S, Andersen  ML. Effects of experimental sleep deprivation on anxiety-like behaviour in animal research: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:575–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Matenchuk  BA, Mandhane  PJ, Kozyrskyj  AL. Sleep, circadian rhythm, and gut microbiota. Sleep Med Rev. 2020;53:101340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Mehta  R, Khan  S, Mallick  BN. Relevance of deprivation studies in understanding rapid eye movement sleep. Nat Sci Sleep. 2018;10:143–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Reynolds  AC, Banks  S. Total sleep deprivation, chronic sleep restriction and sleep disruption. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:91–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Bhagavan  SM, Sahota  PK. Sleep fragmentation and atherosclerosis: Is there a relationship?  Mo Med. 2021;118(3):272–276. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Cousins  JN, Fernández  G. Chapter 2—The impact of sleep deprivation on declarative memory. In: Van Dongen  HPA, Whitney  P, Hinson  JM, Honn  KA, Chee  MWL, eds. Progress in brain research. Elsevier; 2019:27–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Kim  T, Kim  S, Kang  J, Kwon  M, Lee  SH. The common effects of sleep deprivation on human long-term memory and cognitive control processes. Front Neurosci. 2022;16:883848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Perotta  B, Arantes-Costa  FM, Enns  SC, et al.  Sleepiness, sleep deprivation, quality of life, mental symptoms and perception of academic environment in medical students. BMC Med Educ. 2021;21(1):111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Mason  GM, Lokhandwala  S, Riggins  T, Spencer  RMC. Sleep and human cognitive development. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;57:101472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Pires  GN, Bezerra  AG, Tufik  S, Andersen  ML. Effects of acute sleep deprivation on state anxiety levels: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 2016;24:109–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Newbury  CR, Crowley  R, Rastle  K, Tamminen  J. Sleep deprivation and memory: Meta-analytic reviews of studies on sleep deprivation before and after learning. Psychol Bull. 2021;147(11):1215–1240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Scott  AJ, Webb  TL, Rowse  G. Does improving sleep lead to better mental health? A protocol for a meta-analytic review of randomised controlled trials. BMJ Open. 2017;7(9):e016873. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Senzai  Y, Scanziani  M. A cognitive process occurring during sleep is revealed by rapid eye movements. Science. 2022;377(6609):999–1004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Iranzo  A. The REM sleep circuit and how its impairment leads to REM sleep behaviour disorder. Cell Tissue Res. 2018;373(1):245–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Hammer  M, Jung  F, Brankačk  J, Yanovsky  Y, Tort  ABL, Draguhn  A. Respiration and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep substructure: Short versus long episodes. J Sleep Res. 2023;32(3):e13777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Aleisa  AM, Helal  G, Alhaider  IA, et al.  Acute nicotine treatment prevents REM sleep deprivation-induced learning and memory impairment in rat. Hippocampus. 2011;21(8):899–909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Takatsu-Coleman  AL, Zanin  KA, Patti  CL, et al.  Short-term sleep deprivation reinstates memory retrieval in mice: The role of corticosterone secretion. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(10):1967–1978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Hu  B, Liu  C, Mou  T, et al.  Meta-analysis of sleep deprivation effects on patients with depression. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:783091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ferrara  M, Bottasso  A, Tempesta  D, Carrieri  M, De Gennaro  L, Ponti  G. Gender differences in sleep deprivation effects on risk and inequality aversion: Evidence from an economic experiment. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0120029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Halperin  D. Environmental noise and sleep disturbances: A threat to health?  Sleep Sci. 2014;7(4):209–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Hutchinson  E, Avery  A, Vandewoude  S. Environmental enrichment for laboratory rodents. Ilar J. 2005;46(2):148–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Pace-Schott  EF, Germain  A, Milad  MR. Effects of sleep on memory for conditioned fear and fear extinction. Psychol Bull. Jul 2015;141(4):835–857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lissner  LJ, Wartchow  KM, Toniazzo  AP, Gonçalves  CA, Rodrigues  L. Object recognition and Morris water maze to detect cognitive impairment from mild hippocampal damage in rats: A reflection based on the literature and experience. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2021;210:173273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Denninger  JK, Smith  BM, Kirby  ED. Novel object recognition and object location behavioural testing in mice on a budget. J Vis Exp. 2018;(141):10.3791/58593. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Zarrindast  MR, Khalilzadeh  A, Rezayat  SM, Sahebgharani  M, Djahanguiri  B. Influence of intracerebroventricular administration of histaminergic drugs on morphine state-dependent memory in the step-down passive avoidance test. Pharmacology. 2005;74(2):106–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Leão  AH, Medeiros  AM, Apolinário  GK, et al.  Hippocampal-dependent memory in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task: The role of spatial cues and CA1 activity. Behav Brain Res. 2016;304:24–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Buresová  O, Bures  J, Oitzl  MS, Zahálka  A. Radial maze in the water tank: An aversively motivated spatial working memory task. Physiol Behav. 1985;34(6):1003–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Kraeuter  AK, Guest  PC, Sarnyai  Z. The forced swim test for depression-like behaviour in rodents. Methods Mol Biol. 2019;1916:75–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Liu  MY, Yin  CY, Zhu  LJ, et al.  Sucrose preference test for measurement of stress-induced anhedonia in mice. Nat Protoc. 2018;13(7):1686–1698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Can  A, Dao  DT, Terrillion  CE, Piantadosi  SC, Bhat  S, Gould  TD. The tail suspension test. J Vis Exp. 2012;(59):e3769. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Kraeuter  AK, Guest  PC, Sarnyai  Z. The elevated plus maze test for measuring anxiety-like behaviour in rodents. Methods Mol Biol. 2019;1916:69–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Seibenhener  ML, Wooten  MC. Use of the Open Field Maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behaviour in mice. J Vis Exp. 2015;(96):e52434. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Silva  RH, Frussa-Filho  R. The plus-maze discriminative avoidance task: A new model to study memory-anxiety interactions. Effects of chlordiazepoxide and caffeine. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;102(2):117–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Altman  DG, Machin  D, Bryant  TN, Gardner  MJ. Statistics with confidence: Confidence intervals and statistical guidelines. J R Stat Soc Ser A-Stat Soc. 1990;153:256–256. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Rev. ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; xv, 474. 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. DerSimonian  R, Laird  N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials revisited. Contemp Clin Trials. 2015;45(Pt A):139–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Hooijmans  CR, Rovers  MM, de Vries  RB, Leenaars  M, Ritskes-Hoitinga  M, Langendam  MW. SYRCLE's risk of bias tool for animal studies. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2014;14:43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Ahmed  I, Sutton  AJ, Riley  RD. Assessment of publication bias, selection bias, and unavailable data in meta-analyses using individual participant data: A database survey. Bmj. 2012;344:d7762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Hayashino  Y, Noguchi  Y, Fukui  T. Systematic evaluation and comparison of statistical tests for publication bias. J Epidemiol. 2005;15(6):235–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Wadhwa  M, Prabhakar  A, Ray  K, et al.  Inhibiting the microglia activation improves the spatial memory and adult neurogenesis in rat hippocampus during 48 h of sleep deprivation. J Neuroinflammation. 2017;14(1):222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
  • 44. Kim  DJ, Lee  HP, Kim  MS, et al.  The effect of total sleep deprivation on cognitive functions in normal adult male subjects. Int J Neurosci. 2001;109(1-2):127–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Alhola  P, Polo-Kantola  P. Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2007;3(5):553–567. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Killgore  WD. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog Brain Res. 2010;185:105–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Hennecke  E, Lange  D, Steenbergen  F, et al.  Adverse interaction effects of chronic and acute sleep deficits on spatial working memory but not on verbal working memory or declarative memory. J Sleep Res. 2021;30(4):e13225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Smith  MG, Wusk  GC, Nasrini  J, et al.  Effects of six weeks of chronic sleep restriction with weekend recovery on cognitive performance and wellbeing in high-performing adults. Sleep. 2021;44(8). [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Shamim  SA, Warriach  ZI, Tariq  MA, Rana  KF, Malik  BH. Insomnia: Risk factor for neurodegenerative diseases. Cureus. 2019;11(10):e6004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Dauvilliers  Y. Insomnia in patients with neurodegenerative conditions. Sleep Med. 2007;8  Suppl 4:S27–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Achariyar  TM, Li  B, Peng  W, et al.  Glymphatic distribution of CSF-derived apoE into brain is isoform specific and suppressed during sleep deprivation. Mol Neurodegener. 2016;11(1):74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Holth  JK, Fritschi  SK, Wang  C, et al.  The sleep-wake cycle regulates brain interstitial fluid tau in mice and CSF tau in humans. Science. 2019;363(6429):880–884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Shokri-Kojori  E, Wang  GJ, Wiers  CE, et al.  β-Amyloid accumulation in the human brain after one night of sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(17):4483–4488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Pałucha-Poniewiera  A, Podkowa  K, Rafało-Ulińska  A, Brański  P, Burnat  G. The influence of the duration of chronic unpredictable mild stress on the behavioural responses of C57BL/6J mice. Behav Pharmacol. 2020;31(6):574–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Noguchi  T, Saito  M, Aida  J, et al.  Association between social isolation and depression onset among older adults: A cross-national longitudinal study in England and Japan. BMJ Open. 2021;11(3):e045834. [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Kalmbach  DA, Arnedt  JT, Song  PX, Guille  C, Sen  S. Sleep disturbance and short sleep as risk factors for depression and perceived medical errors in first-year residents. Sleep. 2017;40(3). [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Gerner  RH, Post  RM, Gillin  JC, Bunney  WE, Jr. Biological and behavioural effects of one night's sleep deprivation in depressed patients and normals. J Psychiatr Res. 1979;15(1):21–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Pflug  B, Tölle  R. Disturbance of the 24-hour rhythm in endogenous depression and the treatment of endogenous depression by sleep deprivation. Int Pharmacopsychiatry. 1971;6(3):187–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Boland  EM, Rao  H, Dinges  DF, et al.  Meta-analysis of the antidepressant effects of acute sleep deprivation. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017;78(8):e1020–e1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Gottlieb  JF, Goel  N, Chen  S, Young  MA. Meta-analysis of sleep deprivation in the acute treatment of bipolar depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2021;143(4):319–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Wu  JC, Bunney  WE. The biological basis of an antidepressant response to sleep deprivation and relapse: Review and hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147(1):14–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Söderlund  J, Lindskog  M. Relevance of rodent models of depression in clinical practice: Can we overcome the obstacles in translational neuropsychiatry?  Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;21(7):668–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Hu  B, Liu  C, Lv  T, et al.  Meta-analysis of sleep deprivation effects on depression in rodents. Brain Res. 2022;1782:147841. [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Pires  GN, Tufik  S, Andersen  ML. Relationship between sleep deprivation and anxiety--experimental research perspective. Einstein (Sao Paulo ). 2012;10(4):519–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Pires  GN, Tufik  S, Andersen  ML. Grooming analysis algorithm: Use in the relationship between sleep deprivation and anxiety-like behaviour. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2013;41:6–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Silva  RH, Kameda  SR, Carvalho  RC, et al.  Anxiogenic effect of sleep deprivation in the elevated plus-maze test in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl ). 2004;176(2):115–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Lin  Y. Robust inference for responder analysis: Innovative clinical trial design using a minimum p-value approach. Contemp Clin Trials Commun. 2016;3:65–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Draak  THP, de Greef  BTA, Faber  CG, Merkies  ISJ. The minimum clinically important difference: Which direction to take. Eur J Neurol. 2019;26(6):850–855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Saré  RM, Lemons  A, Smith  CB. Behaviour testing in rodents: Highlighting potential confounds affecting variability and reproducibility. Brain Sci. 2021;11(4). [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Anderson  C, Platten  CR. Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behav Brain Res. 2011;217(2):463–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Kamphuis  J, Meerlo  P, Koolhaas  JM, Lancel  M. Poor sleep as a potential causal factor in aggression and violence. Sleep Med. 2012;13(4):327–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Albert  I, Cicala  GA, Siegel  J. The behavioural effects of REM sleep deprivation in rats. Psychophysiology. 1970;6(5):550–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Colombo  C, Benedetti  F, Barbini  B, Campori  E, Smeraldi  E. Rate of switch from depression into mania after therapeutic sleep deprivation in bipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 1999;86(3):267–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Young  JW, Henry  BL, Geyer  MA. Predictive animal models of mania: Hits, misses and future directions. Br J Pharmacol. 2011;164(4):1263–1284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Gessa  GL, Pani  L, Fadda  P, Fratta  W. Sleep deprivation in the rat: An animal model of mania. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1995;5  Suppl:89–93. [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Siddique  SA, Tamilselvan  T, Vishnupriya  M, Balamurugan  E. Evaluation of neurotransmitter alterations in four distinct brain regions after Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Deprivation (REMSD) induced mania-like behaviour in Swiss albino mice. Neurochem Res. 2018;43(6):1171–1181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Minkel  JD, Banks  S, Htaik  O, et al.  Sleep deprivation and stressors: Evidence for elevated negative affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1015–1020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Harro  J. Animals, anxiety, and anxiety disorders: How to measure anxiety in rodents and why. Behav Brain Res. 2018;352:81–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Deurveilher  S, Shewchuk  SM, Semba  K. Homeostatic sleep and body temperature responses to acute sleep deprivation are preserved following chronic sleep restriction in rats. J Sleep Res. 2021;30(5):e13348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Alzoubi  KH, Al-Jamal  FF, Mahasneh  AF. Cerebrolysin prevents sleep deprivation induced memory impairment and oxidative stress. Physiol Behav. 2020;217:112823. [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Nollet  M, Wisden  W, Franks  NP. Sleep deprivation and stress: A reciprocal relationship. Interface Focus. 2020;10(3):20190092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Lemons  A, Saré  RM, Beebe Smith  C. Chronic sleep deprivation in mouse pups by means of gentle handling. J Vis Exp. 2018;(140). [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Gencturk  S, Unal  G. Rodent tests of depression and anxiety: Construct validity and translational relevance. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2024;24(2):191–224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Walf  AA, Frye  CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behaviour in rodents. Nat Protoc. 2007;2(2):322–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Wright  CJ, Milosavljevic  S, Pocivavsek  A. The stress of losing sleep: Sex-specific neurobiological outcomes. Neurobiol Stress. 2023;24:100543. [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Egger  M, Smith  GD, Phillips  AN. Meta-analysis: Principles and procedures. Bmj. 1997;315(7121):1533–1537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Lewis  S, Clarke  M. Forest plots: Trying to see the wood and the trees. Bmj. 2001;322(7300):1479–1480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

fcaf309_Supplementary_Data

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its supplementary materials. Any additional data or materials can be requested from the corresponding author. All calculations were conducted using Stata 17 software, following the guidelines outlined in the Stata meta-analysis reference manual (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX). For further details, please refer to the following link: https://www.stata.com/manuals17/meta.pdf.


Articles from Brain Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES