Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2025 Aug 7;49(9):2103–2112. doi: 10.1111/acer.70132

A model of ethanol self‐administration in head‐fixed mice

Amy L Ward 1, Kion T Winston 1, Sophie Buchmaier 1, Cynara J Cooper 1, Rachel E Clarke 1,2, Michael R Martino 1, Kelsey M Vollmer 1, Jacqueline E Paniccia 1,2, Marcus S Bell 1, Elizabeth M Doncheck 1, Roger I Grant 1, Joshua Boquiren 1, Jade Baek 1, Logan M Manusky 1, Annaka M Westphal 1, Lisa M Green 1, Bayleigh E Pagoota 1, James M Otis 1,3,4,, Jennifer A Rinker 1,5,
PMCID: PMC12463755  PMID: 40776452

Abstract

Background

Significant advances in neurotechnology, such as the application of two‐photon (2P) imaging of biosensors in vivo, have enabled unparalleled longitudinal and high‐resolution access to neural circuits that coordinate behavior in rodents. Integration of these techniques would be groundbreaking for the study of alcohol use disorder (AUD). AUD is rooted in significant neural adaptations that could be functionally monitored and manipulated at the single‐cell level across the development of dependence in rodents. However, 2P imaging and related methodologies often require or are facilitated by head fixation, and a lack of head‐fixed models has hindered their integration for the study of alcohol dependence.

Methods

We developed a head‐fixed model in which animals learned to self‐administer ethanol across ~14 days. Active lever responding resulted in a tone cue and ethanol reward, whereas responding on the inactive lever resulted in neither cue nor ethanol reward. Following acquisition, animals extinguished lever pressing across a minimum of 10 days. Finally, animals were tested separately for both cue‐ and ethanol‐induced reinstatement of lever pressing.

Results

Here we show, for the first time, that in our head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration model, male and female mice reliably pressed an active, but not inactive, lever for an oral ethanol reward. Ethanol rewards positively correlated with blood ethanol concentrations at pharmacologically relevant levels. Furthermore, mice extinguished ethanol self‐administration when the ethanol reward and cue were omitted, suggesting active lever pressing was ethanol‐directed. Following extinction, presentation of the ethanol‐associated cue or priming with ethanol itself invigorated reinstatement of ethanol seeking, modeling relapse in a manner that replicates decades of work in freely moving rodent studies.

Conclusions

Overall, our head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration model will allow for incorporation of novel technologies that require or are greatly facilitated by head fixation, improving our ability to study and understand the neural adaptations and computations that underlie alcohol dependence.

Keywords: alcohol, ethanol self‐administration, head‐fixation, two‐photon imaging


Advances in neuroscience allow unparalleled access to the brain through multiphoton microscopy, high‐density electrodes, and widefield imaging; however, these techniques often require head fixation. Here, we show head‐fixed mice learn to self‐administer alcohol, extinguish lever‐press responding, and reinstate alcohol seeking to both alcohol and an associated cue, at levels comparable to freely moving animals. Our model of head‐fixed alcohol self‐administration allows for integration with complex imaging techniques, providing the unique opportunity to interrogate circuitry and cell types that drive alcohol‐seeking behaviors.

graphic file with name ACER-49-2103-g004.jpg

INTRODUCTION

Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is characterized by continuous cycles of positive reinforcement, wherein the pleasant, euphoric effects of alcohol increase alcohol consumption (Brown et al., 1980) and negative reinforcement, wherein individuals consume alcohol to avoid withdrawal or alleviate anxiety (Cooper et al., 1995; Koob & Volkow, 2010). AUD is a chronic, relapsing neuropsychological condition where excessive alcohol seeking and the inability to stop seeking despite negative consequences are hallmarks of dependence (NIAAA, 2024). Repeatedly experiencing the positive reinforcing effects of alcohol can increase the likelihood of alcohol‐seeking behaviors (Cho et al., 2019), and contextual associations, such as drug‐associated cues, environments, or the drug itself, may trigger relapse in individuals with AUD. To adequately replicate this reinforcement of seeking behaviors in preclinical models, researchers often use operant self‐administration paradigms, which reinforce alcohol (ethanol) seeking (e.g., active lever presses or nose‐pokes), the rewarding aspects of ethanol, and perpetuate a cycle that strengthens ethanol‐seeking behavior in rodent models (Gass & Olive, 2007; June & Gilpin, 2010; Samson & Czachowski, 2003). During self‐administration, animals learn an operant task that is subsequently reinforced by drug delivery, often paired with a discrete drug‐associated cue, such as a tone or a light (Haney & Spealman, 2008). Following learning, this reinforced behavior is extinguished through drug and cue omission. Once this behavior is adequately extinguished, animals can be tested for reinstatement of seeking behavior to a variety of stimuli, including cues and the drug itself, modeling relapse in humans (Epstein et al., 2006). Therefore, preclinical models can effectively parallel the complex cycle of substance use disorders through self‐administration (Koob & Le Moal, 1997).

Considering the effectiveness of using operant conditioning as a behavioral model, pairing this approach with state‐of‐the‐art techniques that enable improved interrogation of neural circuits would improve our ability to define the neurobiological mechanisms of AUD. For example, with the advent of biosensor technologies, two‐photon (2P) microscopy could be applied to self‐administration experiments, enabling longitudinal visualization of activity (e.g., through calcium or voltage sensors) and morphology (e.g., through synaptic fluorophores) in single cell‐type‐specific neurons, dendrites, and axons (Chen et al., 2013; Grienberger et al., 2022; Svoboda & Yasuda, 2006). Until recently, 2P imaging was constrained by the relatively limited behavioral procedures amenable to head‐fixation in mice, but creative adaptations have expanded the number of relevant behavioral paradigms. Specifically, recent developments have incorporated elements of operant conditioning (e.g., levers) and delivery mechanisms (e.g., sipper tubes for fluid delivery or IV catheterization for drug delivery) with sophisticated 2P microscopy, allowing for visualization of activity in single neurons from the onset of self‐administration experiments through reinstatement (Paniccia et al., 2024; Vollmer et al., 2021, 2022). While recent advances have been made, enabling some aspects of ethanol‐related behaviors to be studied in head‐fixed animals (Kalelkar et al., 2024; Timme et al., 2024), a head‐fixed model of operant ethanol self‐administration has not yet been created or experimentally validated.

Here, we present a model of ethanol self‐administration in head‐fixed mice, and we base our model design on decades of research using self‐administration in freely moving mice (Lopez & Becker, 2014; Meisch, 2001). Within this paradigm, we find that head‐fixed mice learn to press a rewarded active, but not unrewarded inactive, lever resulting in a tone cue followed by an ethanol reward. Mice rapidly discriminate between the levers and achieve physiologically relevant blood ethanol concentrations that are predicted by the number of ethanol deliveries. Moreover, when the cue and ethanol rewards are omitted during extinction, mice learn to suppress active lever pressing across days. Notably, when we reintroduce cue or ethanol rewards following extinction learning, we find that mice will significantly reinstate drug‐seeking behaviors. Collectively, these results suggest that we can effectively model ethanol self‐administration in head‐fixed mice, establishing an effective paradigm for use with high‐resolution 2P microscopy to provide extensive insight into the neural mechanisms underlying AUD.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Male and female mice from various strains [i.e., C57BL/6J (Jax Labs #000664, n = 6), or animals derived from our breeding colony: Drd2‐Cre (MMRRC_017263‐UCD), Cre+ (n = 1) and Cre− (n = 4), VMat2‐Cre (not commercially available) Cre+ (n = 2) or Cre− (n = 4), PV‐2A‐Cre (Jax Labs #012358) Cre+ (n = 2) and Cre− (n = 8), or wildtype C57BL/6J mice from our breeding colony (n = 11)] at least 8 weeks old/20 g minimum at study onset. Mice were group housed preoperatively and single‐housed postoperatively under a reversed 12:12‐h light cycle (lights off at 8:00 AM; all experiments were performed during the dark phase) with access to standard rodent chow (Teklad Diet 2918, Inotiv) and water ad libitum except during ethanol self‐administration sessions. Not all animals were exposed to every phase of experimentation, as some animals were utilized for other experiments, but data was included for unmanipulated phases; thus, the total sample size may vary. All animal experimental procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the Medical University of South Carolina and adhere to NIH guidelines for working with laboratory animals (NRC, 2011).

Head‐fixed surgeries

Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (0.8%–1.5% in oxygen; 1 L/min) and placed within a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments) for intracranial surgeries. Ophthalmic ointment (Akorn), topical anesthetic (2% Lidocaine; Akorn), analgesic (Ketorlac, 2 mg/kg, IP), and subcutaneous sterile saline (0.9% NaCl in water) treatments were given preoperatively and intraoperatively for the health and pain management of the animal. Some animals received head‐ring implantation only (see below) and were utilized for behavior only (n = 13), while some animals reported herein were utilized for other experimentation beyond the scope of this report and experienced additional surgical procedures (i.e., bilateral virus injection or fiber implantation for manipulations that occurred after the data reported were collected; n = 25). A custom‐made head‐ring (stainless steel; 5 mm ID, 11 mm OD; Clemson Engineering) was implanted onto the animal skull to allow for head fixation. Following surgeries, animals received antibiotics (Cefazolin, 200 mg/kg, sc) and recovered with access to food and water ad libitum for 14 days minimum.

Ethanol drinking habituation

Following 3 to 4 weeks of recovery from surgery (Figure 1A), animals were acclimated to ethanol (EtOH) consumption using a modified Drinking‐in‐the‐Dark (DID) paradigm (Rhodes et al., 2005, 2007). Three hours into the dark cycle (lights off at 8 AM), water bottles were replaced with volumetric sipper tubes containing 15% EtOH (v/v in reverse osmosis water, Decon Labs). Animals were permitted to drink for 2 h each day over the course of 7 days. Animals were weighed each day of home cage drinking, and EtOH consumption was normalized to body weight (grams of EtOH consumed per kilogram of body weight; g/kg).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Timeline and setup for head‐fixed self‐administration, extinction, and reinstatement. (A) Timeline for head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration, extinction, and reinstatement procedures. (B) Diagram of head‐fixed behavioral chambers. (C) Schematic of behavioral design for acquisition of ethanol self‐administration, wherein an active press results in a 1.6 s cue presentation followed by a 15% ethanol reward and an inactive press results in no cue and no ethanol reward.

Head‐fixed ethanol operant self‐administration

Acquisition

To reduce possible stress and allow for habituation of the corticosterone response caused by head‐fixation, during the 3 days following DID exposure, mice underwent head‐fixed habituation (see timeline in Figure 1A) during which they were head‐restrained in custom‐built operant chambers (Figure 1B) for 45 min with no access to the levers, tone cue, or ethanol reward [for more details, see (Vollmer et al., 2021)]. Once habituated to head‐fixation, mice were trained on a fixed‐ratio 1 (FR1) schedule of reinforcement operant conditioning paradigm. Mice were trained on an FR1 schedule of reinforcement approximately 2–3 h into the dark cycle (lights off 8 AM) to allow for optimal training conditions. Behavioral systems interfaced with an Arduino and custom software developed in Python (see Otis Lab Github Repository, https://github.com/Otis‐Lab‐MUSC/REACHER for further details) or MatLab (as described in Vollmer et al., 2021) were equipped to deliver ethanol reinforcers, record lever presses, and collect lick contacts with either the liquid or the spout. Mice were trained on stationary, custom‐built levers that were attached to 3D printed components (see Otis Lab Github Repository, https://github.com/Otis‐Lab‐MUSC/REACHER, for further details). During behavioral acquisition, the levers were placed approximately 3.5 cm in front of the restraint conical tube (Figure 1C). Mice were required to extend their forelimbs in order to reach the levers. Pressing the active lever resulted in an immediate cue presentation (8 kHz, 1.6 s), and finally an ethanol reward. Mice self‐administering ethanol received 15% EtOH droplets (~3–5 μL) in front of their mouth. Reinforced active lever pressing resulted in a 20‐s timeout period wherein continuous pressing of the active lever was recorded, but no reward or cue was presented. Pressing of the inactive lever presented neither cue nor reward. Mice underwent ethanol self‐administration for 1‐h sessions where mice were allowed to freely self‐administer ethanol for the entirety of the session.

Extinction & Reinstatement Testing

Following 14 days of self‐administration, mice learned to extinguish prior ethanol‐seeking behavior across for at least 10 days. During extinction, lever pressing no longer results in a cue or ethanol reward delivery. Following a minimum of 10 days of extinction, if animals have reached extinction criteria, which is defined as <20% of the average lever presses on the last 2 days of behavioral acquisition (Vollmer et al., 2021), they underwent either drug or cue reinstatement. For cue reinstatement, animals received a tone cue (8 kHz, 1.6 s) upon pressing the active lever but did not receive an ethanol reward. For drug reinstatement, animals were given access to a volumetric sipper tube filled with 15% ethanol for 30 min in their home cage prior to the reinstatement session. Between reinstatement sessions, mice underwent re‐extinction (<20% of the average lever presses on the last 2 days of behavioral acquisition) to reestablish low lever‐pressing rates.

Blood ethanol concentration measurements

To determine the degree of intoxication following operant self‐administration behavioral sessions, retro‐orbital blood samples were collected in heparinized capillary tubes in a subset of animals to measure blood ethanol concentrations (BECs). Blood samples were centrifuged at 10,000 g at 4°C for 10 min to separate red blood cells and plasma, and plasma samples were extracted and BECs determined using an Analox AM1 Analyzer (Analox Instruments).

Data collection and statistical analyses

The majority of pressing data during acquisition was collected using a custom MATLAB interface connected to an Arduino (Vollmer et al., 2021). A subset of pressing data (n = 8) during acquisition was collected using a new, custom‐made behavioral interface controlled by Python, also connected via an Arduino. Moreover, all licking behavior during acquisition was collected using the Python interface. GraphPad PRISM (Dotmatics, version 10) was used to conduct statistical analyses and, where appropriate, either analysis of variance (ANOVA; two‐way or three‐way) or mixed‐effects analyses or paired t‐tests were used to analyze behavioral data. The following independent variables were analyzed: lever presses (active or inactive), day (acquisition, extinction, or reinstatement), sex (male or female), and lick rate (early acquisition, late acquisition, active, or timeout lever press).

RESULTS

Home cage ethanol consumption

All mice were habituated to ethanol using a modified Drinking‐in‐the‐Dark (DID) paradigm. Animals were given access to 15% EtOH (v/v) in their home cage via volumetric sipper tubes for 7 days a week prior to head‐fixation (Figure 2A). Despite previous reports of sex‐dependent differences in ethanol consumption (Rivera‐Irizarry et al., 2023), we found that male and female mice drank similar amounts during the home cage access period through a mixed‐effects analysis (female mean 1.758 g/kg ± 0.18 vs. male mean 1.98 g/kg ± 0.15 [Figure 2A]; main effect of sex, F 1,36 = 0.88, p = 0.3526). There was a main effect of day, F 3.405,122.0 = 4.135, p = 0.0057, and Tukey's post hoc analyses showed that drinking on Day 4 decreased compared to Day 2 (p = 0.0364), but stabilized after that, and there was no sex x day interaction (F 6,215 = 1.280, p = 0.2675). Please note, one drinking value during home‐cage drinking was removed because of spillage.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration paradigm. (A) Home cage Drinking‐in‐the‐Dark (DID) ethanol (15% v/v) consumption in females (purple) and males (blue). (B) Lever‐pressing behavior across 14 days of acquisition on the active and inactive levers for males and females showing significant discrimination between the active and inactive levers, ****p < 0.0001. (C) Reinforced active lever press responding across the course of behavioral acquisition. (D) Average of active, reinforced lever pressing and inactive lever pressing across individual sessions in a subset of animals (n = 5) during early acquisition (Days 1/2). (E) Average of active, reinforced lever pressing and inactive lever pressing across individual sessions in a subset of animals (n = 5) during middle acquisition (Days 7/8). (F) Average of active, reinforced lever pressing and inactive lever pressing across individual sessions in a subset of animals (n = 5) during late acquisition (Days 13/14). (G) Correlation of blood ethanol content to number of reinforcers during behavioral acquisition. (H) Moreover, when examining lick rate of animals during behavioral acquisition, we found that animals will have increased lick frequency to a reinforced active lever press across the course of learning, ****p < 0.0001. (I) Furthermore, animals exhibit no changes in lick frequency in response to a timeout, nonreinforced lever press across the course of acquisition.

Acquisition of Ethanol Self‐Administration

Following 7 days of home cage DID exposure, mice were trained to press the active lever on an FR1 schedule of reinforcement for delivery of 15% (v/v) ethanol. Over the course of 14 days of acquisition, animals learned to reliably discriminate between the active and inactive levers (Figure 2B). A three‐way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of lever (F 1,37 = 120, p < 0.0001), but not day (F 4.282,158.4 = 0.6645, p = 0.6277) or sex (F 1,37 = 2.342, p = 0.1344), such that overall, mice pressed the active lever significantly more than the inactive lever. Additionally, there was no day × sex interaction (F 13,481 = 1.089, p = 0.3661) nor a day × sex × lever interaction (F 13,481 = 1.277, p = 0.2227), suggesting males and females performed comparably overall. However, there was a significant day x lever interaction, F 7.108,263.0 = 3.779, p = 0.0006, indicating significant discrimination between the active and inactive levers developed as acquisition went on, and a significant sex × lever interaction, F 1,37 = 5.994, p = 0.0192, suggesting males pressed the active lever slightly more than females overall. A two‐way ANOVA analyzing the number of ethanol reinforcers indicated a significant main effect of day F 13,481 = 2.1, p = 0.0092, suggesting that the number of ethanol reinforcers earned increased across acquisition (Figure 2C). There was also a main effect of sex, F 1,37 = 5.325, p = 0.0267, suggesting that overall, males earned more reinforcers than females (41.07 vs. 32.47, respectively). There was no significant day × sex interaction (p = 0.1588). Average daily active and inactive lever pressing (±SEM shown in shading) for the “early,” “middle,” and “late” sessions is shown in Figure 2D–F, respectively. Following a subset of self‐administration sessions, some animals received retro‐orbital blood sampling to measure blood ethanol concentrations (BECs). The data evinced a strong positive correlation (r = 0.957, p = 0.0027) between the number of ethanol reinforcers earned and BECs achieved, revealing that animals become reliably intoxicated with an increasing number of ethanol reinforcers during acquisition (Figure 2G).

Lick rate during Acquisition of Ethanol Self‐Administration

Lick rates were quantified in a subset of mice (n = 3–4 mice per day with at least 10 presses and 5 licks per trial) using a Python/Arduino user interface. To filter lick rates, the minimum lick duration was 1 ms and the maximum 70 ms; this filtering was designed to remove paw touches that may be longer than typical lick bouts. We conducted a two‐way ANOVA analyzing lick rates across days (3 levels: Early, Mid, Late) and time (30 bins before and 30 bins after each press, with each bin representing 250 ms or total of 15 s analyzed). A two‐way ANOVA (Δlick rate) revealed a significant effect of day (F 2,8280 = 16.696, p < 0.001), time (F 59,8280 = 6.754, p < 0.001) and a day × time interaction (F 118,8280 = 1.86, p < 0.001) for licking behavior in response to a reinforced active lever press (Figure 2H). In contrast, mice did not increase licking rates during the equivalent ethanol delivery epoch when the active lever was pressed, but no ethanol or cue was presented (i.e., during timeout presses). Furthermore, a two‐way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of day (F 2,14760 = 29.075, p < 0.001) and time (F 59,14760 = 2.539, p < 0.001) (Figure 2I) for licking behavior in response to a timeout lever press. Moreover, post hoc analyses were conducted using pairwise t‐tests with Bonferroni correction. Post hoc analyses examining the infusion epoch (between 1.6 and 3.6 s after lever press) showed significant differences in lick rate between early versus late acquisition (p < 0.001) and mid versus late acquisition (p < 0.001) in accordance with an active lever press. Further post hoc analysis indicated no significant difference in lick rate between acquisition stages in response to a timeout lever press. Overall, data reveal that mice increase licking rates upon ethanol delivery across training.

Extinction

After acquisition of ethanol self‐administration, animals underwent extinction training (n = 23; 13 males and 10 females). We have found that animals will sufficiently extinguish active lever pressing from the first day of extinction to the last day (Figure 3A). In the early days of extinction, animals will exhibit elevated lever‐pressing behavior that declines across time. A 2 × 2 RM ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of day, F 1,21 = 9.120, p = 0.0065, confirming that overall, animals significantly reduced lever‐pressing behavior from the first day to the last day of extinction (Figure 3A). Additionally, a significant main effect of sex, F 1,21 = 5.432, p = 0.0298, and a significant day x sex interaction, F 1,21 = 5.756, p = 0.0258, revealed that males pressed the active lever on the first day of extinction significantly more than females (post hoc p = 0.0017), showing that males might be more prone to extinction burst behavior. There was not a significant effect of day, F 1,21 = 2.5, p = 0.1238, on inactive lever pressing from the first to the last day of extinction. Additionally, there was not a significant effect of day × sex interaction, F 1,21 = 2.3, p = 0.1370, on inactive lever pressing from the first to the last day of extinction.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Extinction and reinstatement of operant responding for ethanol in a head‐fixed paradigm. (A) During extinction learning, responses on the active lever no longer result in delivery of the tone cue or ethanol reward (top panel); males show significantly greater responding on Day 1 of extinction compared to females (*p < 0.05), but both male and female mice significantly reduce their lever‐pressing behavior from the first to last day of extinction (**p < 0.01; bottom panel). (B) During cue‐induced reinstatement testing, active lever results in delivery of the tone cue but no ethanol reward (top panel); both males and females show significantly greater responding on the cue‐test day compared to their extinction baseline (****p < 0.0001; bottom panel). (C) During drug‐induced reinstatement testing, mice were given 30‐min access to 15% ethanol in their home cage prior to placement in the head‐fixed operant set up (top panel); both males and females show significantly greater responding on the drug‐test day compared to their extinction baseline (****p < 0.001; bottom panel).

Reinstatement tests following extinction

Lastly, we have found that head‐fixed animals will reinstate ethanol‐seeking behavior following extinction learning. Here, we show that both male and female mice will equivalently reinstate active lever‐pressing behavior to the tone cue previously paired with the ethanol reward (Figure 3B). Specifically, a 2 × 2 RM ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of day, F 1,18 = 27.90, p < 0.0001, such that active lever presses were increased after cue presentation in comparison to the previous day's extinction baseline in a sex‐independent manner (no main effect of sex, F 1,18 = 0.0541, p = 0.8187, and no day × sex interaction, F 1,18 = 0.0974, p = 0.7586). There was not a significant effect of day, F 1,18 = 1.4, p = 0.2432, on inactive pressing. Moreover, these animals will reinstate lever pressing following 30 min of home cage drinking (HCD) prior to a head‐fixed behavioral session. Using a 2 × 2 RM ANOVA we found that, similar to cue‐induced reinstatement, both male and female mice will significantly reinstate active lever‐pressing behavior in response to ethanol priming (Figure 3C) as shown by a main effect of day, F 1,16 = 22.28, p = 0.0002, but no main effect of sex, F 1,16 = 2.777, p = 0.1151, or a day × sex interaction, F 1,16 = 3.377, p = 0.0848. There was no significant effect of day, F 1,16 = 2.4, p = 0.1352, or a day × sex interaction, F 1,16 = 1.9, p = 0.1774.

DISCUSSION

Collectively, our findings indicate that mice will self‐administer ethanol in a head‐fixed operant conditioning paradigm. Moreover, we find that animals will extinguish active lever‐pressing in the absence of a tone cue and ethanol reward. Importantly, when animals are reintroduced to a tone cue or ethanol reward, animals will reinstate active lever‐pressing behavior. Therefore, we demonstrate that animals will readily acquire, extinguish, and reinstate ethanol‐seeking behaviors while head‐fixed, effectively establishing the ability to apply novel methodologies such as simultaneous 2P imaging (Clarke et al., 2024; Kalelkar et al., 2024; Vollmer et al., 2021, 2022), providing for the first time a uniquely analogous behavioral paradigm to that in freely moving mice (Lopez & Becker, 2014; Meisch, 2001). These results indicate that our head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration paradigm provides a robust and novel technique to further dissect the mechanisms underlying ethanol seeking.

Head‐fixation in drug and reward self‐administration

Head‐fixation during drug self‐administration has been a reliable model to study various drugs of abuse, including heroin and cocaine (Clarke et al., 2024; Paniccia et al., 2024; Vollmer et al., 2021). Previous studies have found that animals will effectively acquire lever pressing, extinguish, and reinstate during head‐fixed heroin and cocaine self‐administration (Vollmer et al., 2021). Notably, head‐fixed animals will learn to discriminate between active and inactive levers, indicating the acquisition of goal‐directed behavior that has been illustrated in freely moving models. In addition to drug self‐administration, the same operant self‐administration, extinction, and reinstatement phenomena have been observed in sucrose reward‐seeking models (Vollmer et al., 2021). Head‐fixation during ethanol‐seeking tasks is emerging as a novel strategy to examine circuitry during in vivo recordings (Kalelkar et al., 2024; Timme et al., 2024), providing a wealth of information on how ethanol can alter distinct neurons, circuits, and ensembles in the brain during consumption. Other models have utilized aversion‐resistance tasks during head‐fixation (Timme et al., 2024) as well as other tasks such as operant licking tasks (Kalelkar et al., 2024) to examine how animals respond to various ethanol‐seeking contexts. However, the literature on head‐fixed seeking tasks during ethanol consumption remains limited. Our data provide the first demonstration of a veritable operant‐based ethanol‐seeking task in a head‐fixed preparation, which will enable the field to better investigate neural and circuit dysfunction during volitional ethanol‐seeking and ‐taking behaviors.

Comparison to freely moving models of self administration

Moreover, our head‐fixed model of ethanol is advantageous as it parallels findings in freely moving models. Head‐fixed mice receive approximately the same number of reinforcers as freely moving models (Chu et al., 2007; Lopez & Becker, 2014) and extinguish at similar rates to freely moving mice (Tsiang & Janak, 2006). Moreover, while others have reported that females self‐administer ethanol more readily than males (Faccidomo et al., 2024), other freely moving models have shown that males and females self‐administer ethanol at similar levels (Yoon et al., 2025), mirroring our head‐fixed self‐administration findings. Collectively, this evidence shows that head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration serves as a reliable analog to freely moving models.

Integration of head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration with 2‐photon microscopy

Head‐fixation provides stability that facilitates 2P imaging in awake animals, enabling high‐resolution investigation of neuronal activity during behavior. Two‐photon microscopy has become a critical tool for studying neuropsychiatric disorders, including substance use disorders, by allowing deep tissue imaging with single‐cell specificity (Grienberger et al., 2022; Svoboda & Yasuda, 2006). Two‐photon microscopy utilizes two near‐infrared photons to excite fluorophores within a sample simultaneously, allowing for fluorescence detection despite the longer excitation photon wavelength (Svoboda & Yasuda, 2006). This excitation facilitates tissue penetration and minimizes background noise, markedly enhancing imaging resolution, clarity, and potential depth as compared with other in vivo imaging applications. Moreover, 2P microscopy reduces phototoxicity and photobleaching, which enables prolonged cellular and molecular observations in vivo (Grienberger et al., 2022).

A key advantage of 2P microscopy is it enables tracking of individual cellular elements over extended periods of time (weeks to months; Namboodiri et al., 2019, Rossi et al., 2019) providing unique insight into neuroplasticity associated with various neurobiological conditions. This temporal tracking is highly multifaceted in that it may be used to track single cells (Rossi et al., 2019); dendrites (Sadakane et al., 2015), dendritic spines (Pan & Gan, 2008; Yu & Zuo, 2014), and axons (Davalos et al., 2008). Such a longitudinal perspective will be particularly valuable for studying AUD, where dynamic changes in neuronal activity and connectivity underlie the progression from initial ethanol exposure to compulsive seeking (Radke et al., 2017; Timme et al., 2022). Combining head‐fixed ethanol self‐administration with two‐photon microscopy allows the application of single‐cell optogenetics (Piantadosi et al., 2024; Yang & Yuste, 2018), enabling functional targeting of neuronal clusters and populations that may be implicated in ethanol‐seeking behaviors. Furthermore, spatial sequencing enables the integration of two‐photon imaging datasets with RNA sequencing, providing unprecedented insight into the computational dynamics of transcriptomically unique cell populations (Xu et al., 2020). While some have demonstrated the benefits of using head‐fixed models in conjunction with two‐photon imaging and ethanol‐related tasks (Kalelkar et al., 2024), research combining 2P microscopy with ethanol‐seeking tasks remains limited. By offering a dynamic, in vivo perspective concerning the neural adaptations associated with the development of AUD, this technique could vastly improve the dissection of the complex neuronal circuits that longitudinally adapt to functionally guide dysfunctional alcohol use and seeking behaviors.

Future directions and limitations

While our head‐fixed model has shown robust and consistent results in ethanol self‐administration, there are several important considerations for adapting freely moving behaviors to head‐fixed models, though our data suggest these concerns can be largely overcome. One of the primary concerns with head‐fixed behavioral tasks is potential stress responses that may result from head restraint (Juczewski et al., 2020). This presents a valid concern as stress responses affect a variety of behaviors, including drug seeking (Mantsch & Katz, 2007). While we did not measure corticosterone responses following ethanol self‐administration, this is a future addition that could help gauge future head‐fixed behaviors. However, the longevity of habituation to the head‐fixed setup may allow rodents to become more acclimated to this restraint and prevent stress levels from influencing behavior (Schwarz et al., 2010). Others have shown that corticosterone levels decrease across days of head‐fixed training (Juczewski et al., 2020), suggesting that habituation to head fixation may be beneficial in reducing potential responses. Moreover, previous studies implementing head‐fixed drug self‐administration have not indicated changes in a behavior as a result of head restraint (Vollmer et al., 2021) and remain sensitive to stress (Vollmer et al., 2022). Nevertheless, as technology and equipment improve, there has been increasing integration of elements such as a running wheel or a treadmill into head‐fixed setups to track movement and limit potential stress from restraint. Furthermore, additions to our current head‐fixed setup could be added to track other metrics during operant tasks, such as a camera to measure aspects such as pupil diameter and whisker movement, which could be used to assess states of arousal (Ganea et al., 2020) and object recognition (Adibi, 2019).

CONCLUSION

Collectively, our results show that head‐fixed animals will rapidly and reliably learn goal‐directed behaviors to seek ethanol. Our head‐fixed paradigm indicates that animals will learn to discriminate between active and inactive levers, effectively extinguish ethanol‐seeking behavior, and subsequently reinstate to ethanol and ethanol‐paired cues. These results indicate that self‐administration protocols can be effectively applied to head‐fixed mice during 2P imaging, revealing unparalleled access to the brain during ethanol‐seeking behaviors.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

ALW, JMO, and JAR designed the experiments and wrote the manuscript. ALW, KTW, SB, CJC, REC, MSB, and MRM helped conduct the experiments. SB, REC, MSB, MRM, KMV, JEP, EMD, RIG, JB, LMM, AMW, LMG, and BEP provided intellectual support and training for self‐administration studies. All authors contributed to the article and approved the final submitted version.

FUNDING INFORMATION

This research was funded by NIAAA (R01‐AA030796 to JAR and JMO; U01‐AA029972 to JAR; Pilot Funds through the Charleston Alcohol Research Center P50‐AA010761 to JAR and JMO; F31‐AA032197 to ALW), NIDA (R01‐DA054271 to JMO), as well as NIAAA T32‐AA007474 Training Grant support for ALW and NIGMS R25‐GM113278 Postbaccalaureate Research Education Program support for KTW.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank Drs. Howard C. Becker, Jacqueline F. McGinty, and John J. Woodward for their intellectual support and advice.

Ward, A.L. , Winston, K.T. , Buchmaier, S. , Cooper, C.J. , Clarke, R.E. , Martino, M.R. et al. (2025) A model of ethanol self‐administration in head‐fixed mice. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 49, 2103–2112. Available from: 10.1111/acer.70132

Contributor Information

James M. Otis, Email: otis@musc.edu.

Jennifer A. Rinker, Email: rinker@musc.edu.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

REFERENCES

  1. Adibi, M. (2019) Whisker‐mediated touch system in rodents: from neuron to behavior. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, 40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brown, S.A. , Goldman, M.S. , Inn, A. & Anderson, L.R. (1980) Expectations of reinforcement from alcohol: their domain and relation to drinking patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 419–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chen, T.W. , Wardill, T.J. , Sun, Y. , Pulver, S.R. , Renninger, S.L. , Baohan, A. et al. (2013) Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature, 499, 295–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cho, S.B. , Su, J. , Kuo, S.I. , Bucholz, K.K. , Chan, G. , Edenberg, H.J. et al. (2019) Positive and negative reinforcement are differentially associated with alcohol consumption as a function of alcohol dependence. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 33, 58–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chu, K. , Koob, G.F. , Cole, M. , Zorrilla, E.P. & Roberts, A.J. (2007) Dependence‐induced increases in ethanol self‐administration in mice are blocked by the CRF1 receptor antagonist antalarmin and by CRF1 receptor knockout. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 86, 813–821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Clarke, R.E. , Grant, R.I. , Woods, S.N. , Pagoota, B.E. , Buchmaier, S. , Bordieanu, B. et al. (2024) Corticostriatal ensemble dynamics across heroin self‐administration to reinstatement. bioRxiv. Available from: 10.1101/2024.06.21.599790 [DOI]
  7. Cooper, M.L. , Frone, M.R. , Russell, M. & Mudar, P. (1995) Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 990–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Davalos, D. , Lee, J.K. , Smith, W.B. , Brinkman, B. , Ellisman, M.H. , Zheng, B. et al. (2008) Stable in vivo imaging of densely populated glia, axons and blood vessels in the mouse spinal cord using two‐photon microscopy. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 169, 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Epstein, D.H. , Preston, K.L. , Stewart, J. & Shaham, Y. (2006) Toward a model of drug relapse: an assessment of the validity of the reinstatement procedure. Psychopharmacology, 189, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Faccidomo, S. , Eastman, V.R. , Santanam, T.S. , Swaim, K.S. , Taylor, S.M. & Hodge, C.W. (2024) Distinct sex differences in ethanol consumption and operant self‐administration in C57BL/6J mice with uniform regulation by glutamate AMPAR activity. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 1498201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ganea, D.A. , Bexter, A. , Gunther, M. , Garderes, P.M. , Kampa, B.M. & Haiss, F. (2020) Pupillary dilations of mice performing a vibrotactile discrimination task reflect task engagement and response confidence. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gass, J.T. & Olive, M.F. (2007) Reinstatement of ethanol‐seeking behavior following intravenous self‐administration in Wistar rats. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 1441–1445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Grienberger, C. , Giovannucci, A. , Zeiger, W. & Portera‐Cailliau, C. (2022) Two‐photon calcium imaging of neuronal activity. Nature Reviews Methods Primers, 2, 67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Haney, M. & Spealman, R. (2008) Controversies in translational research: drug self‐administration. Psychopharmacology, 199, 403–419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Juczewski, K. , Koussa, J.A. , Kesner, A.J. , Lee, J.O. & Lovinger, D.M. (2020) Stress and behavioral correlates in the head‐fixed method: stress measurements, habituation dynamics, locomotion, and motor‐skill learning in mice. Scientific Reports, 10, 12245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. June, H.L. & Gilpin, N.W. (2010) Operant self‐administration models for testing the neuropharmacological basis of ethanol consumption in rats. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, Chapter 9, Unit 9.12.1‐26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kalelkar, A. , Sipe, G. , Castro, E.C.A.R. , Lorenzo, I.M. , Nguyen, M. , Linares‐Garcia, I. et al. (2024) A paradigm for ethanol consumption in head‐fixed mice during prefrontal cortical two‐photon calcium imaging. Neuropharmacology, 245, 109800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Koob, G.F. & Le Moal, M. (1997) Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science, 278, 52–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Koob, G.F. & Volkow, N.D. (2010) Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 217–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lopez, M.F. & Becker, H.C. (2014) Operant ethanol self‐administration in ethanol dependent mice. Alcohol, 48, 295–299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mantsch, J.R. & Katz, E.S. (2007) Elevation of glucocorticoids is necessary but not sufficient for the escalation of cocaine self‐administration by chronic electric footshock stress in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 367–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Meisch, R.A. (2001) Oral drug self‐administration: an overview of laboratory animal studies. Alcohol, 24, 117–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Namboodiri, V.M.K. , Otis, J.M. , Van Heeswijk, K. , Voets, E.S. , Alghorazi, R.A. , Rodriguez‐Romaguera, J. et al. (2019) Single‐cell activity tracking reveals that orbitofrontal neurons acquire and maintain a long‐term memory to guide behavioral adaptation. Nature Neuroscience, 22, 1110–1121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. National Research Council . (2011) Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals, 8th edition. Washington, DC: Institute for Laboratory Animal Research, Division on Earth and Life Studies, National Research Council of the National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
  25. NIAAA . (2024) Alcohol facts and statistics: understanding alcohol use disorder. Available from: https://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohols‐effects‐health/alcohol‐topics/alcohol‐facts‐and‐statistics
  26. Pan, F. & Gan, W.B. (2008) Two‐photon imaging of dendritic spine development in the mouse cortex. Developmental Neurobiology, 68, 771–778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Paniccia, J.E. , Vollmer, K.M. , Green, L.M. , Grant, R.I. , Winston, K.T. , Buchmaier, S. et al. (2024) Restoration of a paraventricular thalamo‐accumbal behavioral suppression circuit prevents reinstatement of heroin seeking. Neuron, 112, 772–785.e9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Piantadosi, S.C. , Zhou, Z.C. , Pizzano, C. , Pedersen, C.E. , Nguyen, T.K. , Thai, S. et al. (2024) Holographic stimulation of opposing amygdala ensembles bidirectionally modulates valence‐specific behavior via mutual inhibition. Neuron, 112, 593–610.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Radke, A.K. , Jury, N.J. , Kocharian, A. , Marcinkiewcz, C.A. , Lowery‐Gionta, E.G. , Pleil, K.E. et al. (2017) Chronic EtOH effects on putative measures of compulsive behavior in mice. Addiction Biology, 22, 423–434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Rhodes, J.S. , Best, K. , Belknap, J.K. , Finn, D.A. & Crabbe, J.C. (2005) Evaluation of a simple model of ethanol drinking to intoxication in C57BL/6J mice. Physiology & Behavior, 84, 53–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rhodes, J.S. , Ford, M.M. , Yu, C.H. , Brown, L.L. , Finn, D.A. , Garland, T., Jr. et al. (2007) Mouse inbred strain differences in ethanol drinking to intoxication. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 6, 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rivera‐Irizarry, J.K. , Zallar, L.J. , Levine, O.B. , Skelly, M.J. , Boyce, J.E. , Barney, T. et al. (2023) Sex differences in binge alcohol drinking and the behavioral consequences of protracted abstinence in C57BL/6J mice. Biology of Sex Differences, 14, 83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rossi, M.A. , Basiri, M.L. , Mchenry, J.A. , Kosyk, O. , Otis, J.M. , Van Den Munkhof, H.E. et al. (2019) Obesity remodels activity and transcriptional state of a lateral hypothalamic brake on feeding. Science, 364, 1271–1274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sadakane, O. , Watakabe, A. , Ohtsuka, M. , Takaji, M. , Sasaki, T. , Kasai, M. et al. (2015) In vivo two‐photon imaging of dendritic spines in marmoset neocortex. eNeuro, 2, ENEURO.0019‐15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Samson, H.H. & Czachowski, C.L. (2003) Behavioral measures of alcohol self‐administration and intake control: rodent models. International Review of Neurobiology, 54, 107–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Schwarz, C. , Hentschke, H. , Butovas, S. , Haiss, F. , Stuttgen, M.C. , Gerdjikov, T.V. et al. (2010) The head‐fixed behaving rat‐‐procedures and pitfalls. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 27, 131–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Svoboda, K. & Yasuda, R. (2006) Principles of two‐photon excitation microscopy and its applications to neuroscience. Neuron, 50, 823–839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Timme, N.M. , Ardinger, C.E. , Weir, S.D.C. , Zelaya‐Escobar, R. , Kruger, R. & Lapish, C.C. (2024) Non‐consummatory behavior signals predict aversion‐resistant alcohol drinking in head‐fixed mice. Neuropharmacology, 242, 109762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Timme, N.M. , Ma, B. , Linsenbardt, D. , Cornwell, E. , Galbari, T. & Lapish, C.C. (2022) Compulsive alcohol drinking in rodents is associated with altered representations of behavioral control and seeking in dorsal medial prefrontal cortex. Nature Communications, 13, 3990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tsiang, M.T. & Janak, P.H. (2006) Alcohol seeking in C57BL/6 mice induced by conditioned cues and contexts in the extinction‐reinstatement model. Alcohol, 38, 81–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Vollmer, K.M. , Doncheck, E.M. , Grant, R.I. , Winston, K.T. , Romanova, E.V. , Bowen, C.W. et al. (2021) A novel assay allowing drug self‐administration, extinction, and reinstatement testing in head‐restrained mice. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 744715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Vollmer, K.M. , Green, L.M. , Grant, R.I. , Winston, K.T. , Doncheck, E.M. , Bowen, C.W. et al. (2022) An opioid‐gated thalamoaccumbal circuit for the suppression of reward seeking in mice. Nature Communications, 13, 6865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Xu, S. , Yang, H. , Menon, V. , Lemire, A.L. , Wang, L. , Henry, F.E. et al. (2020) Behavioral state coding by molecularly defined paraventricular hypothalamic cell type ensembles. Science, 370, eabb2494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yang, W. & Yuste, R. (2018) Holographic imaging and photostimulation of neural activity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 50, 211–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Yoon, H.J. , Doyle, M.A. , Altemus, M.E. , Bethi, R. , Lago, S.H. , Winder, D.G. et al. (2025) Operant ethanol self‐administration behaviors do not predict sex differences in continuous access home cage drinking. Alcohol, 123, 87–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Yu, X. & Zuo, Y. (2014) Two‐photon in vivo imaging of dendritic spines in the mouse cortex using a thinned‐skull preparation. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 51520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from Alcohol, Clinical & Experimental Research are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES