Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 2002 Nov;78(3):271–290. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-271

The formation of linked perceptual classes.

Lanny Fields 1, Priya Matneja 1, Antonios Varelas 1, James Belanich 1, Adrienne Fitzer 1, Kim Shamoun 1
PMCID: PMC1284900  PMID: 12507004

Abstract

Multiple-exemplar training with stimuli in four domains induced two new fill-based (A1' and A2') and satellite-image-based (B1' and B2') perceptual classes. Conditional discriminations were established between the endpoints of the A1' and B1' classes as well as the A2' and B2' classes. The emergence of linked perceptual classes was evaluated by the performances occasioned by nine cross-class probes that contained fill variants as samples and satellite variants as comparisons, along with nine other cross-class probes that consisted of satellite variants as samples and fill variants as comparisons. The 18 probes were first presented serially and then concurrently. Class-consistent responding indicated the emergence of linked perceptual classes. Of the linked perceptual classes, 70% emerged during the initial serial test. An additional 20% of the linked perceptual classes emerged during the subsequently presented concurrent test block. Thus, linked perceptual classes emerged on an immediate or delayed basis. Linked perceptual classes, then, share structural and fuctional similarities with equivalence classes, generalized equivalence classes, cross-modal classes, and complex maturally occurring categories, and may clarify processes such as intersensory perception.

Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (5.4 MB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Barnes D., Keenan M. A transfer of functions through derived arbitrary and nonarbitrary stimulus relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1993 Jan;59(1):61–81. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Blough D. S. Reaction times of pigeons on a wavelength discrimination task. J Exp Anal Behav. 1978 Sep;30(2):163–167. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1978.30-163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cowley B. J., Green G., Braunling-McMorrow D. Using stimulus equivalence procedures to teach name-face matching to adults with brain injuries. J Appl Behav Anal. 1992 Summer;25(2):461–475. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1992.25-461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. DeGrandpre R. J., Bickel W. K., Higgins S. T. Emergent equivalence relations between interoceptive (drug) and exteroceptive (visual) stimuli. J Exp Anal Behav. 1992 Jul;58(1):9–18. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1992.58-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Devany J. M., Hayes S. C., Nelson R. O. Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. J Exp Anal Behav. 1986 Nov;46(3):243–257. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Fields L., Adams B. J., Newman S., Verhave T. Interactions among emergent relations during equivalence class formation. Q J Exp Psychol B. 1992 Aug;45(2):125–138. doi: 10.1080/14640749208401013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fields L., Adams B. J., Verhave T., Newman S. The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 May;53(3):345–358. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fields L., Landon-Jimenez D. V., Buffington D. M., Adams B. J. Maintained nodal-distance effects in equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995 Sep;64(2):129–145. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fields L., Reeve K. F., Adams B. J., Verhave T. Stimulus generalization and equivalence classes: a model for natural categories. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 May;55(3):305–312. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.55-305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fields L., Reeve K., Rosen D., Varelas A., Adams B. Using the simultaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: the facilitating effects of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1997 May;67(3):367–389. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1997.67-367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fields Lanny, Reeve Kenneth F., Matneja Priya, Varelas Antonios, Belanich James, Fitzer Adrienne, Shamoun Kim. The formation of a generalized categorization repertoire: effect of training with multiple domains, samples, and comparisons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):291–313. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gelman S. A. The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories. Cogn Psychol. 1988 Jan;20(1):65–95. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90025-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Haring T. G., Breen C. G., Laitinen R. E. Stimulus class formation and concept learning: establishment of within- and between-set generalization and transitive relationships via conditional discrimination procedures. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989 Jul;52(1):13–25. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.52-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hayes L. J., Thompson S., Hayes S. C. Stimulus equivalence and rule following. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989 Nov;52(3):275–291. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.52-275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Honig W. K., Stewart K. E. Pigeons can discriminate locations presented in pictures. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988 Nov;50(3):541–551. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Innis A., Lane S., Miller E., Critchfield T. Stimulus Equivalence: Effects Of A Default-response Option On Emergence Of Untrained Stimulus Relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1998 Jul;70(1):87–102. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1998.70-87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lane S. D., Clow J. K., Innis A., Critchfield T. S. Generalization of cross-modal stimulus equivalence classes: operant processes as components in human category formation. J Exp Anal Behav. 1998 Nov;70(3):267–279. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1998.70-267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Pilgrim C., Galizio M. Relations between baseline contingencies and equivalence probe performances. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 Nov;54(3):213–224. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.54-213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Reeve K. F., Fields L. Perceptual classes established with forced-choice primary generalization tests and transfer of function. J Exp Anal Behav. 2001 Jul;76(1):95–114. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Saunders R. R., Wachter J., Spradlin J. E. Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedures. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988 Jan;49(1):95–115. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.49-95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Shepard R. N. Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science. 1987 Sep 11;237(4820):1317–1323. doi: 10.1126/science.3629243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Sidman M., Kirk B., Willson-Morris M. Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. J Exp Anal Behav. 1985 Jan;43(1):21–42. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1985.43-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Spencer T. J., Chase P. N. Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 May;65(3):643–659. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Spradlin J. E., Cotter V. W., Baxley N. Establishing a conditional discrimination without direct training: a study of transfer with retarded adolescents. Am J Ment Defic. 1973 Mar;77(5):556–566. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES