Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Affect Disord. 2007 Jan 12;101(1-3):269–274. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.11.026

MOOD-WORSENING WITH HIGH-POLLEN-COUNTS AND SEASONALITY: A PRELIMINARY REPORT

Alvaro Guzman 1,2,*, Leonardo H Tonelli 1, Darryl Roberts 1,3, John W Stiller 1,2, Michael A Jackson 2, Joseph J Soriano 1, Samina Yousufi 2, Kelly J Rohan 4, Hirsh Komarow 5, Teodor T Postolache 1
PMCID: PMC1949487  NIHMSID: NIHMS26460  PMID: 17222915

Abstract

Background:

Because aeroallergens produce inflammation in the respiratory airways, and inflammation triggers depression in vulnerable individuals, we hypothesized that mood sensitivity to pollen, the most seasonal aeroallergen, will be associated with a greater seasonality of mood. Since pollen is absent during winter, we specifically predicted that mood sensitivity to tree pollen will predict non-winter SAD but not winter SAD.

Methods:

A convenience sample of African and African American college students who lived in the Washington DC metropolitan area for at least the past 3 years completed the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (SPAQ), from which the Global Seasonality Score (GSS) was calculated, a diagnosis of cumulative SAD (syndromal or subsyndromal SAD) was derived, a seasonal pattern (winter vs non-winter) identified, and self-reported mood changes during high pollen counts obtained. A Mann Whitney test was used to compare GSS between participants with versus without mood worsening during high pollen counts. The capability of mood worsening with high pollen counts, gender, ethnicity, and age to predict non-winter SAD was analyzed with logistic regressions.

Results:

GSS was greater (z=5.232, p<0.001) in those who reported mood worsening with high pollen counts. Mood sensitivity to pollen predicted non-winter SAD (p= 0.017), but not winter SAD.

Limitations:

The SPAQ is not a definitive tool to assess seasonality, and self-reported mood worsening with high pollen counts relies on recollection. No direct measures of depression scores or pollen counts were collected. The non-winter SAD concept has not been previously established.

Conclusions:

Our study, which should be considered preliminary in light of its limitations, suggests that self-reported mood-worsening with high pollen count is associated with a greater seasonality of mood, and predicts SAD of non-winter type.

Keywords: Seasonal affective disorder, seasonality, allergy, pollen

Introduction

It is now well accepted that a proportion of patients with depression decompensate during a particular time of the year. This pattern of depressive episodes associated with a particular season is termed seasonal affective disorder (SAD). The occurrence of depressive episodes during fall and winter, with remission in spring and summer represents the winter type of SAD, which is the most consistently described seasonal pattern of SAD (Rosenthal et al., 1984). Moreover, a reverse pattern with depression in summer and remission in fall and winter has also been described (Boyce & Parker, 1988; Wehr et al., 1987; Wehr & Rosenthal, 1989). Seasonal changes in environmental light drive seasonal depressive symptoms in individuals with winter SAD (Kasper et al., 1989; Rosenthal et al., 1985a; Rosenthal et al., 1987; Wehr et al., 2001), and bright light treatment is effective in reducing winter depressive symptoms in SAD (Terman et al., 1998; Eastman et al., 1998; Lewy et al., 1998; Postolache & Oren, 2005). In addition, it has been suggested, although not yet empirically tested, that high temperatures drive seasonal depressive symptoms in summer SAD (Boyce & Parker, 1988; Wehr et al., 1987; Wehr & Rosenthal, 1989).

Recently, we have examined the contribution of other seasonal environmental factors as potential triggers for seasonal changes in mood. One such factor, known to induce seasonal allergies in a large proportion of the population, is high pollen counts (Mothes et al., 2004; Traidl-Hoffmann et al., 2003). Pollen induces a strong inflammatory response of the respiratory tract in allergic individuals, and inflammatory triggers have been associated with acute worsening of mood in healthy individuals (Reichenberg et al., 2001). We hypothesize that pollen-related worsening of mood will predict a greater seasonality of mood. In fact, Bartko and Kasper (1989) reported an association between high pollen counts, mood worsening in winter, and winter SAD. However, given that high pollen concentrations are encountered during all seasons except winter, we further hypothesize that mood worsening with high pollen counts will predict a non-winter pattern of SAD. Because a stronger association between depression and allergy was previously reported in females (Timonen et al., 2002; 2003), we hypothesize that the relationship between non-winter SAD and high pollen counts will be greater in females than in males. In this preliminary study, we have used a convenience sample of college students at local universities in Washington, DC. As there was a great preponderence of African and African American students, we decided to focus our study to that population. As exposure to an allergen during early childhood may convey protection to future sensitization to the allergen (Fasce et al., 2005), we also hypothesize that mood worsening with high pollen counts will more strongly predict non-winter SAD in Africans (who are less likely to have history of exposure to the prevalent flora of the Washington, DC metropolitan area as children), than in African Americans. As an alternative hypothesis, a longer history of exposure to local pollens and urbanization could increase African Americans' exposure to pollutants that foster sensitization and inflammation, resulting in a stronger association mood worsening with high pollen counts with non-winter SAD in African Americans than in Africans (Almqvist, 2005; D'Amato et al., 2005; Lau et al., 2005;).

Methods

Sample

We analyzed data we previously collected for a larger study on seasonality in African American students (Agumadu et al., 2004). We used a convenience sample (n=845) of African American and African college students aged 17-67 years (mean = 29.7; SD = 9.39) from four academic sites in Washington, DC who had lived in the metropolitan DC area for at least 3 years prior to beginning the study. The sample included 523 females (62.7%) and 311 males (37.3%) of whom 521 (61.7%) completed the data collection instruments in their entirety. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Mental Health Services of the District of Columbia. The questionnaires were presented to student respondents as a group in a classroom setting where written informed consent was obtained.

Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (SPAQ)

The SPAQ (Kasper et al., 1989b) consists of several sections: a) demographic data, b) seasonal changes in sleep length, social activity, mood, weight, appetite, and energy level (Likert scales scored 0-4, used to calculate a Global Seasonality Score or GSS), c) the severity of the degree to which seasonal changes present a “problem” for the individual (either none present, mild, moderate, marked, severe or disabling; the problem score and the GSS are used to define the categorical diagnoses of SAD and subsyndromal SAD), d) the months of the year of feeling worse (used to define the seasonal pattern of SAD), e) sensitivity of mood to environmental variables For (e), we used only the items regarding mood sensitivity to high pollen counts and mood sensitivity to short days, converting the responses from ordinal ratings to a dichotomous variable for mood worsening (a rating of −3, −2, or −1) or not (a rating of 0 or higher). We used the criteria of Magnusson and Stefansson (1993) to define SAD and subsyndromal SAD (S-SAD) or the lack of either. For SAD, a subject had to meet the following criteria: a) GSS score of 11 or above, and b) reported a problem with seasonal mood changes, rated as at least moderate in degree. To be categorized as subsyndromal SAD, the criteria require a) a GSS of 11 or more with a problem severity rating of none or only mild or b) a GSS of 9 or 10 with a problem severity rating of at least mild. We defined the “cumulative SAD” category for those meeting criteria for either SAD or subsyndromal SAD. Based on the month of “feeling worst”, we defined a winter type (December, January, February) and a non-winter type (the rest of the year). Only 521 participants (61.7%) completed all four measures (GSS, “problem”, seasonal pattern and mood sensitivity to pollen) and were included in the statistical analysis.

Statistical Analysis

For comparing GSS between those with and without mood worsening with high pollen counts, because of the skewed distribution of the GSS (Pearson's = 0.48), we used the Mann Whitney rank sum. Stepwise logistic regressions with backwards elimination were used to test if mood worsening with high pollen counts, mood sensitivity to short days, ethnicity, gender, age, and prior length of stay in the area, or their interactions, predicted non-winter cumulative SAD and winter cumulative SAD. The criterion α, two tailed, was 0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata™ version 8.2 and Systat version 10.2.

Results

Descriptive analysis

The respondents were primarily African American (70.9%) and under the age of 35 (71.6%). The average age was 29.4 years for people reporting sensitivity to pollen and 29.5 for those who did not report sensitivity to pollen (difference non-significant). The average duration of residence in the study area was 19.3 years for those not reporting sensitivity and 20.4 years for those reporting sensitivity to pollen (non-significant) , with two-thirds of the respondents living in the area for more than 10 years. 41% met the criteria for cumulative SAD (either syndromal or subsyndromal). More than two-thirds (68.3%) of respondents reported that high pollen worsened their moods, compared with about half (55.35%) who made the same claim about short day lengths. More than one in four (26.5%) respondents met criteria for non-winter cumulative SAD compared with about one in seven (15.3%) for winter SAD.

Hypothesis-driven analyses

Respondents reporting mood worsening with high pollen counts had significantly higher GSS totals (median=9.0) compared to those with no mood worsening with high pollen counts (median=6.5) (Mann-Whitney z=5.232, p<0.001). Mood worsening with high pollen counts significantly increased the odds of meeting non-winter SAD criteria by 1.68 times (95%CI: 1.15-2.45). Mood worsening with high pollen counts also significantly predicted non-winter cumulative SAD, increasing its odds by 1.65 times (95% CI: 1.06-2.69, p=0.017, sensitivity 0.267, specificity 0.743), while its prediction of winter cumulative SAD was not significant. Our predicted effects of gender and ethnicity were not confirmed. Gender, ethnicity, age, length of stay, and their interactions had non-significant predictive effects on the non-winter type of cumulative SAD.

Discussion

This study found an association between self-reported mood worsening during periods of high pollen counts and greater dispositional seasonality. In addition, participants who endorsed mood worsening when pollen count is high were more likely to meet criteria for non-winter SAD and non-winter cumulative SAD (i.e., including subsyndromal SAD). Our findings are consistent with those of Bartko and Kasper (1989). However, our current study adds a somewhat greater degree of temporal specificity in that our analysis indicates that mood sensitivity to tree pollen specifically predicts non-winter SAD and not winter SAD. The results are also consistent with the reported association between allergy and depression (Bell et al., 1991; Hashiro and Okumura, 1998; Marshall et al., 2002; Postolache et al., 2005b; Timonen et al., 2001; Timonen et al., 2003a; Timonen et al., 2003b; Timonen et al., 2002;). The potential relevance of the association between allergy and depression is highlighted by the very high (>50%) prevalence of a positive skin reaction to allergens in the general population (Arbes et al., 2005).

Concomitant to spring peaks in tree pollen (Kosisky & Carpenter, 1997), a spring peak in completed suicides across continents, countries, and hemispheres has been consistently reported (Petridou et al., 2002; Maes et al., 1993; Chew and McCleary, 1995, among others). The precipitating factor of this spring peak in suicide is unknown. Photoperiod, changes in photoperiod, and light intensity have been proposed as possible environmental triggers (Goodwin and Jamison, 1990). However, upon careful scrutiny, the peak times of suicide occurrence (summarized in Altamura et al., 1999) do not match the peak of photoperiod (centered on the Summer Solstice in late June) or the time when photoperiodic changes are at their greatest (centered on the Spring equinox in March; Barker et al., 1994; Linkowski et al., 1992; Souetre et al., 1987; Tietjen & Kripke, 1994; Terao et al., 2002). Based on the temporal coincidence between seasonal peaks in tree pollen and peaks of suicide, we have hypothesize that seasonality of suicide could be driven by a combination of environmental triggers and vulnerability, i.e. seasonal aeroallergens and a triple vulnerability for allergy, depression, and suicide (Postolache et al., 2005b). Using a large scale epidemiological approach, we have preliminarily reported an association between windows of high tree pollen and higher suicide rates (Postolache et al., 2005a). The mechanisms that might explain the association between allergy and depression, in general, and suicide, specifically, are poorly understood. A number of potential underlying processes have been considered and are under investigation (Postolache et al. 2005b).

Limitations

Our sample included only African American and African students and, therefore, our results may not be generalizable to the entire population. However, they confirm certain results of a previous study using an ethnically diverse sample (Bartko & Kasper 1989). The prevalence of asthma and allergies is higher among African Americans, and the severity of their symptoms is greater, as they are more likely to die from an acute asthma attack than whites (Fagan et al., 2001; Gold and Acevedo-Gargia, 2005, Arbes et al 2005). No direct measures of either depression scores or pollen counts were performed. Similarly, no data on allergies was collected. SPAQ (although validated in college students) estimates seasonality, but is not a definitive tool to assess it. The concept of nonwinter SAD has not been previously established. One could also argue that our results are an expression of circular reasoning. Thus, more refined epidemiological, observational and experimental work is necessary to confirm our hypothesis. On a clinical level, there is a need for further studies that will directly and longitudinally measure depression, allergy symptoms, inflammation markers, and pollen counts. Until then, our results should be considered as preliminary.

In conclusion, our results, suggest that a non-winter pattern of SAD may be related to seasonal exposure to pollen, an environmental factor that sharply fluctuates with the seasons. This may be particularly relevant to depression in spring, when a massive peak of tree pollen overlaps with, and potentially relates to, a highly replicated peak of suicide. Identifying an aeroallergen- induced seasonal depression will require multiple levels of inquiry and could be conducive to future novel prognostic, preventative and therapeutic approaches for mood disorders.

Acknowledgments

Supported by NARSAD (Independent Investigator Award to Dr. Postolache), R21 MH075891 (PI Postolache, CoPI Tonelli) and the DC Department of Mental Health (Drs. Postolache, Stiller, Guzman and Youssufi). Dr. Tonelli was supported by NIH K12 HD43489 andR21 MH075905 (PI Tonelli, CoPI Postolache and McCarthy) and Dr. Komarow was supported by NIAID, Division of Intramural Research, Bethesda, Md.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Agumadu CO, Yousufi SM, Malik IS, Nguyen MC, Jackson MA, Soleymani K, Thrower CM, Peterman MJ, Walters GW, Niemtzoff MJ, Bartko JJ, Postolache TT. Seasonal variation in mood in African American college students in the Washington, D.C., metropolitan area. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(6):1084–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Almqvist C. High allergen exposure as a risk factor for asthma and allergic disease. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol. 2005;28(1):25–41. doi: 10.1385/CRIAI:28:1:025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Altamura C, VanGastel A, Pioli R, Mannu P, Maes M. Seasonal and circadian rhythms in suicide in Cagliari, Italy. J Affect Disord. 1999;53:77–85. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(98)00099-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arbes SJ, Jr, Gergen PJ, Elliott L, Zeldin DC. Prevalences of positive skin test responses to 10 common allergens in the US population: results from the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2005;116(2):377–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2005.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barker A, Hawton K, Fagg J, Jennison C. Seasonal and weather factors in parasuicide. Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165(3):375–80. doi: 10.1192/bjp.165.3.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartko JJ, Kasper S. Seasonal changes in mood and behavior: a cluster analytic approach. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):227–39. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90049-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bell IR, Jasnoski ML, Kagan J, King DS. Depression and allergies: survey of a nonclinical population. Psychother Psychosom. 1991;55:24–31. doi: 10.1159/000288404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyce P, Parker G. Seasonal affective disorder in the southern hemisphere. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:96–99. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.1.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chew KS, McCleary R. The spring peak in suicides: a cross-national analysis. Soc Sci Med. 1995;40(2):223–30. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)e0070-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. D'Amato G, Liccardi G, D'Amato M, Holgate S. Environmental risk factors and allergic bronchial asthma. Clin Exp Allergy. 2005;35(9):1113–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2005.02328.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Eastman CI, Young MA, Fogg LF, Liu L, Meaden PM. Bright light treatment of winter depression: a placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(10):883–9. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.10.883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fagan JK, Scheff PA, Hryhorczuk D, Ramakrishnan V, Ross M, Persky V. Prevalence of asthma and other allergic diseases in an adolescent population: association with gender and race. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2001;86(2):177–84. doi: 10.1016/S1081-1206(10)62688-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fasce L, Tosca MA, Silvestri M, Olcese R, Pistorio A, Rossi GA. “Early” cat ownership and the risk of sensitization and allergic rhinitis in Ligurian children with respiratory symptoms. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2005;94(5):561–5. doi: 10.1016/S1081-1206(10)61134-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gold DR, Acevedo-Garcia D. Immigration to the United States and acculturation as risk factors for asthma and allergy. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2005;116(1):38–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2005.04.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Goodwin FK, Jamison KR. Manic-Depressive Illness. 1 ed. Oxford University Press, Inc.; USA: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  16. Han L, Wang K, Cheng Y, Du Z, Rosenthal NE, Primeau F. Summer and winter patterns of seasonality in Chinese college students: a replication. Compr Psychiatry. 2000;41:57–62. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(00)90132-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hashiro M, Okumura M. The relationship between the psychological and immunological state in patients with atopic dermatitis. J Dermatol Sci. 1998;16:231–235. doi: 10.1016/s0923-1811(97)00074-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kasper S, Rogers SL, Yancey A, Schulz PM, Skwerer RG, Rosenthal NE. Phototherapy in individuals with and without subsyndromal seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989a;46:837–844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810090079011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kasper S, Wehr TA, Bartko JJ, Gaist PA, Rosenthal NE. Epidemiological findings of seasonal changes in mood and behavior. A telephone survey of Montgomery County, Maryland. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989b;46:823–833. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810090065010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Komarow HD, Postolache TT. Seasonal allergy and seasonal decrements in athletic performance. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24(2):e35–50. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.006. xiii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kosisky SE, Carpenter GB. Predominant tree aeroallergens of the Washington, DC area: a six year survey (1989-1994) Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 1997;78(4):381–92. doi: 10.1016/S1081-1206(10)63200-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lau S, Illi S, Platts-Mills TA, Riposo D, Nickel R, Gruber C, Niggemann B, Wahn U. Longitudinal study on the relationship between cat allergen and endotoxin exposure, sensitization, cat-specific IgG and development of asthma in childhood--report of the German Multicentre Allergy Study (MAS 90) Allergy. 2005;60(6):766–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1398-9995.2005.00781.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lewy AJ, Bauer VK, Cutler NL, Sack RL, Ahmed S, Thomas KH, Blood ML, Jackson JM. Morning vs evening light treatment of patients with winter depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(10):890–6. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.10.890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Linkowski P, Martin F, De Maertelaer V. Effect of some climatic factors on violent and non-violent suicides in Belgium. J Affect Disord. 1992;25(3):161–6. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(92)90001-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Maes M, Cosyns P, Meltzer HY, De Meyer F, Peeters D. Seasonality in violent suicide but not in nonviolent suicide or homicide. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(9):1380–5. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.9.1380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Magnusson A, Stefansson JG. Prevalence of seasonal affective disorder in Iceland. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:941–946. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240025002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Marshall PS, O'Hara C, Steinberg P. Effects of seasonal allergic rhinitis on fatigue levels and mood. Psychosom Med. 2002;64:684–689. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000021944.35402.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mothes N, Westritschnig K, Valenta R. Tree pollen allergens. Clin Allergy Immunol. 2004;18:165–184. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Petridou E, Papadopoulos FC, Frangakis CE, Skalkidou A, Trichopoulos D. A role of sunshine in the triggering of suicide. Epidemiology. 2002;13(1):106–9. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200201000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Postolache TT, Stiller JW, Herrell R, Goldstein MA, Shreeram SS, Zebrak R, Thrower CM, Volkov J, No MJ, Volkov I, Rohan KJ, Redditt J, Parmar M, Mohyuddin F, Olsen C, Moca M, Tonelli LH, Merikangas K, Komarow HD. Tree pollen peaks are associated with increased nonviolent suicide in women. Mol Psychiatry. 2005a;10:232–235. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Postolache TT, Komarow HD, Stiller JW, Tonelli LH. Allergy, Depression, and Suicide. Directions in Psychiatry. 2005b;25:59–69. [Google Scholar]
  32. Postolache TT, Oren DA. Circadian phase shifting, alerting, and antidepressant effects of bright light treatment. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24(2):381–413. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.005. xii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R, Schuld A, Kraus T, Haack M, Morag A, Pollmacher T. Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:445–452. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Carpenter CJ, Parry BL, Mendelson WB, Wehr TA. Antidepressant effects of light in seasonal affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1985a;142:163–170. doi: 10.1176/ajp.142.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, Lewy AJ, Goodwin FK, Davenport Y, Mueller PS, Newsome DA, Wehr TA. Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;41:72–80. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790120076010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, James SP, Parry B,L, Mendelson WB, Tamarkin L, Wehr TA. Seasonal affective disorder and phototherapy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1985b;453:260–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb11816.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rosenthal NE, Skwerer RG, Sack DA, Duncan CC, Jacobsen FM, Tamarkin L, Wehr TA. Biological effects of morning-plus-evening bright light treatment of seasonal affective disorder. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1987a;23:364–369. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Souetre E, Salvati E, Belugou JL, Douillet P, Braccini T, Darcourt G. Seasonality of suicides: environmental, sociological and biological covariations. J Affect Disord. 1987;13(3):215–25. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(87)90040-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Terao T, Soeda S, Yoshimura R, Nakamura J, Iwata N. Effect of latitude on suicide rates in Japan. Lancet. 2002;360(9348):1892. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11761-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Terman M, Terman JS, Ross DC. A controlled trial of timed bright light and negative air ionization for treatment of winter depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(10):875–82. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.10.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tietjen GH, Kripke DF. Suicides in California (1968-1977): absence of seasonality in Los Angeles and Sacramento counties. Psychiatry Res. 1994;53(2):161–72. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90107-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Timonen M, Hakko H, Miettunen J, Karvonen JT, Herva A, Rasanen P, Koskinen O, Zitting P. Association between atopic disorders and depression: findings from the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort study. Am J Med Genet. 2001;105:216–217. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Timonen M, Jokelainen J, Silvennoinen-Kassinen S, Herva A, Zitting P, Xu B, Peltola O, Rasanen P. Association between skin test diagnosed atopy and professionally diagnosed depression: a Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort study. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:349–355. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01364-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Timonen M, Jokelainen J, Hakko H, Silvennoinen-Kassinen S, Meyer-Rochow VB, Herva A, Rasanen P. Atopy and depression: results from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. Mol Psychiatry. 2003a;8:738–744. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Timonen M, Jokelainen J, Herva A, Zitting P, Meyer-Rochow VB, Rasanen P. Presence of atopy in first-degree relatives as a predictor of a female proband's depression: results from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2003b;111:1249–1254. doi: 10.1067/mai.2003.1546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Timonen M, Viilo K, Hakko H, Sarkioja T, Meyer-Rochow VB, Vaisanen E, Rasanen P. Is seasonality of suicides stronger in victims with hospital-treated atopic disorders? Psychiatric Res. 2004;126:167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Traidl-Hoffmann C, Kasche A, Menzel A, Jakob T, Thiel M, Ring J, Behrendt H. Impact of pollen on human health: more than allergen carriers? Int Arch Allergy Immunol. 2003;131:1–13. doi: 10.1159/000070428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wehr TA, Sack DA, Rosenthal NE. Seasonal affective disorder with summer depression and winter hypomania. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144:1602–1603. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.12.1602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wehr TA, Rosenthal NE. Seasonality and affective illness. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146(7):829–839. doi: 10.1176/ajp.146.7.829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wehr TA, Duncan WC, Jr, Sher L, Aeschbach D, Schwartz PJ, Turner EH, Postolache TT, Rosenthal NE. A circadian signal of change of season in patients with seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(12):1108–14. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES