Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Aug 12.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005 Feb;6(2):151–159. doi: 10.1038/nrn1606

Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution

The Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium*
PMCID: PMC2507884  NIHMSID: NIHMS60670  PMID: 15685220

Abstract

We believe that names have a powerful influence on the experiments we do and the way in which we think. For this reason, and in the light of new evidence about the function and evolution of the vertebrate brain, an international consortium of neuroscientists has reconsidered the traditional, 100-year-old terminology that is used to describe the avian cerebrum. Our current understanding of the avian brain —in particular the neocortex-like cognitive functions of the avian pallium — requires a new terminology that better reflects these functions and the homologies between avian and mammalian brains.


One hundred years ago, Edinger, the father of comparative neuroanatomy, formulated a unified theory of brain evolution that formed the basis of a nomenclature that has been used to define the cerebral subdivisions of all vertebrates1. This resulted in terms and associated concepts such as palaeostriatum, archistriatum, neostriatum and neocortex that are still in common use. According to this theory, the avian cerebrum is almost entirely composed of basal ganglia, the basal ganglia is involved in only instinctive behaviour, and the malleable behaviour that is thought to typify mammals exclusively requires the so-called neocortex. However, towards the end of the twentieth century, there accumulated a wealth of evidence that these viewpoints were incorrect. The avian cerebrum has a large pallial territory that performs functions similar to those of the mammalian cortex. Although the avian pallium is nuclear, and the mammalian cortex is laminar in organization, the avian pallium supports cognitive abilities similar to, and for some species more advanced than, those of many mammals. To eliminate these misconceptions, an international forum of neuroscientists (BOX 1) has, for the first time in 100 years, developed new terminology that more accurately reflects our current understanding of the avian cerebrum and its homologies with mammals. This change in terminology is part of a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution.

Box 1 Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium

Authors are ordered alphabetically in two groups: the first group, along with the first two and last two authors, are the core Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum Thinktank group; the second group are professors, postdoctoral fellows and students who also participated in the Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum. (For author affiliations see online supplementary information S1 (box).)

Erich D. Jarvis, Onur Güntürkün, Laura Bruce, András Csillag, Harvey Karten, Wayne Kuenzel, Loreta Medina, George Paxinos, David J. Perkel, Toru Shimizu, Georg Striedter, J. Martin Wild. Gregory F. Ball, Jennifer Dugas-Ford, Sarah E. Durand, Gerald E. Hough, Scott Husband, Lubica Kubikova, Diane W. Lee, Claudio V. Mello, Alice Powers, Connie Siang, Tom V. Smulders, Kazuhiro Wada, Stephanie A. White, Keiko Yamamoto, Jing Yu, Anton Reiner and Ann B. Butler.

In this article, we summarize the traditional view of telencephalic evolution before reviewing more recent findings and insights. We then present the new nomenclature that has been developed by the Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum, and discuss its implications for our understanding of vertebrate brain evolution and its associated homologies.

The classical view

The classical view of telencephalic evolution, which is still prevalent in classrooms and textbooks, began in the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries after the publication of The Origin of Species by Darwin2. Inspired by Darwin’s theory, between 1885 and 1908 Edinger formulated an influential, evolution-based model of brain organization1,3,4. Edinger and other early comparative neurobiologists combined Darwin’s concept of ‘evolution’ with the nineteenth-century version of Aristotle’s ‘scala naturae’, which resulted in the view that evolution was progressive and unilinear5 — from fish, to amphibians, to reptiles, to birds and mammals, to primates and, finally, to humans — ascending from ‘lower’ to ‘higher’ intelligence in a chronological series. They believed that the brains of extant vertebrates retained ancestral structures, and, therefore, that the origin of specific human brain subdivisions could be traced back in time by examining the brains of extant non-human vertebrates. In making such comparisons, they noted that the main divisions of the human CNS —the spinal cord, hindbrain, midbrain, thalamus, cerebellum and cerebrum or telencephalon — were present in all vertebrates (FIG. 1a). Edinger, however, noted that the internal organization of the telencephala showed the most pronounced differences between species. In mammals, the outer part of the telencephalon was found to have prominently layered grey matter (FIG. 1b, green) whereas the inner part had nuclear grey matter (FIG. 1b, purple). The inner part was located ventrally to the lateral ventricle. The outer part was more elaborate and folded in humans than in smaller mammals. In non-mammals, the outer and inner parts of the telencephala were mainly composed of nuclear grey matter, most of which was located ventrally to the lateral ventricle in reptiles and birds (FIG. 1b, purple).

Figure 1. Avian and mammalian brain relationships.

Figure 1

a | Side view of a songbird (zebra finch) and human brain to represent avian and mammalian species. In this view, the songbird cerebrum covers the thalamus; the human cerebrum covers the thalamus and midbrain. Inset (left) next to the human brain is the zebra finch brain to the same scale. Human brain image reproduced, with permission, courtesy of John W. Sundsten, Digital Anatomist Project. b | Classic view of avian and mammalian brain relationships. Although past authors had different opinions about which brain regions are pallium versus subpallium, we have coloured individual brain regions according to the meaning of the names given to those brain regions. Ac, accumbens; B, nucleus basalis; Cd, caudate nucleus; CDL, dorsal lateral corticoid area; E, ectostriatum; GP, globus pallidus (i, internal segment; e, external segment); HA, hyperstriatum accessorium; HV, hyperstriatum ventrale; IHA, interstitial hyperstriatum accessorium; L2, field L2; LPO, lobus parolfactorius; OB, olfactory bulb; Pt, putamen; TuO, olfactory tubercle. c | Modern consensus view of avian and mammalian brain relationships according to the conclusions of the Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum. Solid white lines are lamina (cell-sparse zones separating brain subdivisions). Large white areas in the human cerebrum are axon pathways called white matter. Dashed grey lines divide regions that differ by cell density or cell size; dashed white lines separate primary sensory neuron populations from adjacent regions. Abbreviations where different from b: E, entopallium; B, basorostralis; HA, hyperpallium apicale; Hp, hippocampus; IHA, interstitial hyperpallium apicale; MV, mesopallium ventrale.

On the basis of these considerations, Edinger proposed that telencephalic evolution occurred in progressive stages of increasing complexity and size, culminating with the human cerebrum. He suggested that the stages proceeded in a ventral-to-dorsal direction, with each new vertebrate group acquiring a more advanced cerebral subdivision, much as the earth’s geological strata formed over time. He proposed that, first, there was the old brain, the palaeoencephalon (also called the basal ganglia or subpallium at the telencephalic base), which controlled instinctive behaviour, followed by the addition of a new brain, the neoencephalon (also called the pallium or mantle at the top of the telecephalon), which controlled learned and intelligent behaviour4. He, Ariëns Kappers and others named the telencephalic subdivisions within each vertebrate group with the prefixes ‘palaeo’ (oldest),‘archi’ (archaic) and ‘neo’ (new) to designate the presumed relative order of evolutionary appearance of each subdivision. In Greek,‘archi’ means the oldest, the first, or the most primitive, whereas ‘palaeo’ means ancient, primitive or old, but not necessarily the oldest. Both Edinger and Ariëns Kappers misinterpreted the meaning of these prefixes and reversed them, naming structures with ‘palaeo-’ to indicate the oldest or first and ‘archi-’ to indicate old. They added to these prefixes the root word ‘striatum’ for the presumed palaeoencephalic subdivisions and ‘pallium’ or ‘cortex’ for the presumed neoencephalic subdivisions1,4,68. The term ‘striatum’ was used because a large part of the basal ganglia (palaeoencephalon) in mammals, now commonly called the caudate–putamen, has fibre bundles coursing through it that give it a striated appearance.

The classical view that became dominant was that the primordial telencephalon of fishes had a relatively small pallium and a larger subpallium, both of which were entirely devoted to olfactory information processing. The fish subpallium was named ‘palaeostriatum’ (old striatum), and was thought to be the antecedent of the human globus pallidus (FIG. 1b). Amphibians were thought to have evolved an ‘archistriatum’ (archaic striatum) above the palaeostriatum, which was proposed to be the antecedent of the human amygdala. Reptiles were thought to have evolved a ‘neostriatum’ (new striatum) above the archistriatum, which was proposed to be the antecedent of the human caudate and putamen. The palaeostriatum of reptiles was also thought to have elaborated into an older part (primitivum) and a newer part (augmentatum), both of which were considered homologous to the human globus pallidus. Following this, birds were thought to have evolved a large additional basal ganglia subdivision, the ‘hyperstriatum’ (hypertrophied striatum), which was considered to be unique to birds9.

The fish pallium was named ‘palaeocortex’, and was proposed to be the antecedent of the human olfactory cortex. Reptiles were thought to have evolved an ‘archicortex’, also thought to be olfactory and primitive, that was said to be the antecedent of the human hippocampus. Birds were thought not to have evolved any further pallial regions. By contrast, mammals were thought to have evolved the latest and greatest achievement, a ‘neocortex’, from the palaeocortex and/or archicortex6. The archicortex and/or palaeocortex, with their 2–3 cell layers, were assumed to be primitive; the neocortex, with its 6 layers, was assumed to be more recently evolved and a substrate for more sophisticated behaviour.

There were dissenting voices to the classical view1012. Some of its proponents also made partial or tentative retractions13,14. However, alternative views were not widely embraced. Instead, the classical view was codified in the important 1936 comparative neuroanatomy text by Ariëns Kappers, Huber and Crosby14 and became pervasive throughout neuroscience.

A new view of the subpallium

Substantive challenges to the classical view of the subpallium began in the 1960s and 1970s with the advent of new methods for determining both nervous system connectivity and the anatomical profiles of gene products5. These studies found that, in mammals, acetyl-cholinesterase enzymatic activity was enriched in the neostriatum15. In birds, high acetyl-cholinesterase activity was found only in the palaeostriatum augmentatum and associated lobus parolfactorius (LPO)15,16 (the LPO was considered to be part of the palaeostriatum augmentatum by Ariëns Kappers et al.14, but was named as a separate region by Karten and Hodos17). Other studies found that the mammalian neostriatum was highly enriched with dopaminergic terminals, which originated from midbrain neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta18. In birds, again, only the palaeostriatum augmentatum and LPO were enriched with dopaminergic terminals, and this input originated from neurons in the midbrain19,20. During the following decades, using new methods in double-label immunohistochemistry and tract tracing, the mammalian neostriatum was found to be enriched in two types of neuron: those containing the neuropeptide substance P (SP), which project to the internal part of the globus pallidus and substantia nigra, and those containing the neuropeptide enkephalin (ENK), which project to the external part of the globus pallidus2124. In birds, SP and ENK neurons are enriched in the palaeostriatum augmentatum (including the LPO)24,25, and, like the equivalent neurons in mammals, project to different cell types within the adjacent avian palaeostriatum primitivum. In both birds and mammals, the SP neurons seem to be involved in promoting planned movement, whereas the ENK neurons seem to have a role in inhibiting unwanted movement. Further functional studies revealed that both the mammalian neostriatum and the avian palaeostriatum augmentatum (including the LPO) participate not only in instinctive behaviour and movement, but also in motor learning26,27.

These apparent relationships between the subpallia of mammals and birds have been supported by molecular embryology studies24,2831. The developing subpallium in birds and mammals consists of two separate histogenetic zones that express different sets of transcription factors: a dorsal zone, which, in mammals, corresponds to the lateral ganglionic eminence and selectively expresses the transcription factors DLX1 and DLX2 but not NKX2.1; and a ventral zone, which, in mammals, corresponds to the medial ganglionic eminence and selectively expresses all three transcription factors. In mammals, the lateral ganglionic eminence gives rise to the dorsal striatum (neostriatum) and the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens and part of olfactory tubercle). The homologous developing territory in birds gives rise to the structures that were previously called the palaeostriatum augmentatum (including the LPO) and the olfactory tubercle. The medial ganglionic eminence in mammals gives rise to various pallidal cell groups, including the dorsal pallidum (the globus pallidus) and the ventral pallidum. The homologous developing territory in birds gives rise to the structures that were called the palaeostriatum primitivum and ventral palaeostriatum. These avian and mammalian striatal and pallidal relationships are further supported by studies of the comparative expression patterns of more than 30 other genes in adult birds and mammals3236. Similar striatal and pallidal territories have been found in the so-called palaeostriatal regions of reptiles3740. Together, these studies indicate that the avian palaeostriatum augmentatum is homologous to the mammalian neostriatum and that the avian palaeostriatum primitivum is homologous to the mammalian globus pallidus.

A new view of the pallium

With these challenges to the classical view of the subpallial relationships among birds, reptiles and mammals came challenges to the classical view of the relationships among their pallia. The mammalian pallium includes the areas known as palaeocortex, archicortex and neocortex; and has been said, more recently, to include both the claustrum and lateral parts of the amygdala28,41,42 (FIG. 1c; Holmgren11 originally proposed that the claustrum and part of the amygdala were pallial, but this view was largely ignored at the time). In birds, the finding that the structures that had been called hyperstriatum, neostriatum and archistriatum were neither striatum nor pallidum raised the question of which telencephalic sector these regions did represent. The results that were needed to answer this question also began to appear in the mid-1960s from pathway tracing16,4346 and behavioural studies4751. These studies found that the so-called avian neostriatum and hyperstriatum receive visual, auditory and somatosensory input from the thalamus, as does the mammalian neocortex. These avian brain regions also carry out the same type of sensory information processing as is performed by the mammalian neocortex. The so-called avian hyperstriatum accessorium and the archistriatum give rise to important descending projections to the premotor and motor neurons of the brainstem and spinal cord, like those of the mammalian cortico-bulbar and cortico-spinal pathways43,5254. Finally, like the mammalian neocortex, these avian brain regions carry out crucial roles in motor control and sensorimotor learning5567.

The apparent pallial relationships between these mammalian and avian brain regions were also supported by molecular embryology studies28,68,69. During development, both the avian hyperstriatum and neostriatum and the mammalian pallium express the pallium-specific transcription factors EMX1, PAX6 and TBR1. The developmental data led to uncertainties about how much of the archistriatum is pallial28,30. However, comparisons of the expression of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF ) and the glutamate receptor mGluR2 in adult birds and mammals indicated that the entire avian archistriatum, as defined in brain atlases17,70, expresses these pallium-specific mRNAs34,36. Further studies of the comparative expression patterns of other glutamate receptors in adult birds and mammals36 support these conclusions. Together, these studies indicate that the avian hyperstriatum, neostriatum, and archistriatum might be homologous to mammalian pallial regions.

This developing view was accompanied by several new proposals about one-to-one homologies between specific avian and mammalian pallial subdivisions. We will consider these in two groups — nuclear-to-layered hypotheses and nuclear-to-claustrum/amygdala nuclei hypotheses.

Nuclear-to-layered hypotheses

First proposed by Karten16,71, nuclear-to-layered hypotheses (BOX 2) propose that the similarities in connectivity between the so-called hyperstriatum, neostriatum and archistriatum of birds and the neocortex of mammals stem from a common origin of these structures — that is, they are homologous. Karten proposed that the common ancestor of birds, reptiles and mammals possessed a nuclear pallium that was transformed into a laminar pallium early in the mammalian lineage, maintaining the connectivity of the ancestral nuclear network. In this regard, he argued that the avian pallium is divided into three groups of serially connected neuron types — thalamorecipient neurons (field L2, ectostriatum and basalis), pallio-pallial neurons (neostriatum) and extratelencephalic projection neurons (archistriatum), with cell types and interconnectivity that resemble those of mammalian cortical layers IV, II–III and V–VI, respectively (BOX 2). Similar arguments were later made for the avian upper hyperstriatum (also known as the Wulst), which also has serially connected neuron types that resemble those found in the mammalian neocortex62. In this hypothesis, avian L2 neurons are homologous to layer IV neurons of mammalian primary auditory cortex, basalis neurons to layer IV of primary somatosensory cortex, ectostriatal neurons to layer IV of extrastriate visual cortex, and the interstitial hyperstriatum accessorium to layer IV of striate visual cortex. In support of this hypothesis, gene expression studies36,72,73 have shown that avian thalamorecipient nuclear fields (L2, ectostriatum, basalis and interstitial hyperstriatum accessorium) and the mammalian thalamorecipient layer IV of neocortex selectively express some of the same genes (the steroid transcription factor ROR-β and the potassium channel EAG2) and express a low level of others (the activity-dependent transcription factor ZENK and the AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid) glutamate receptor subunit GluR1). Avian extratelencephalic projection neurons (in the archistriatum, but not in the hyperstriatum accessorium) and mammalian extratelencephalic projection neurons (layer V neurons of neocortex) both show selective expression of the transcription factor ER81. So, although the avian pallium is not organized cytoarchitectonically into layers, its nuclear subdivisions bear marked similarities in connectivity and molecular profile to different layers of the mammalian neocortex.

Box 2 Working hypotheses on avian and mammalian pallial homologies

An example of a nuclear-to-layered hypothesis is shown in panel a. The connectivity of tectofugal visual pathways in avian (left) and mammalian (right) brains is shown. The hypothesis illustrated is that of Karten71. Colour-coding indicates proposed homologies between birds and mammals. An example of a nuclear-to-claustrum/amygdala nuclei hypothesis is shown in panel b. The hypothesis illustrated is that of Puelles et al.28.

I–VI, cortical layers I–VI; B, nucleus basalis; CDL, dorsal lateral corticoid area; Cl-d, claustrum, dorsal part; Cl-v, claustrum, ventral part; DP, dorsal pallium; E, ectostriatum; HA, hyperstriatum accessorium; Hp, hippocampus; HV, hyperstriatum ventrale; L2, field L2; LP, lateral pallium; LPO, lobus parolfactorius; MP, medial pallium; N, Neostriatum; OB, olfactory bulb; Pul, pulvinar nucleus; Rt, nucleus rotundus; Sc, superior colliculus; TeO, optic tectum; Tn, nucleus taenia; VP, ventral pallium.

graphic file with name nihms60670f4.jpg

Nuclear-to-claustrum/amygdala hypotheses

These hypotheses (BOX 2) provide a different interpretation of mammalian homologies with the avian ventral hyperstriatum, neostriatum and archistriatum, known collectively as the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). In an early proposal, Bruce and Neary74 proposed that the avian DVR represents an elaboration of parts of the mammalian amygdala. Subsequently, Striedter75 proposed that the avian DVR represents an elaboration of the mammalian amygdala and claustrum, and that the connectivity that the DVR shares with the neocortex evolved independently. Support for this view was based on several facts: both the avian DVR and mammalian claustrum/amygdala are nuclear in organization75; both the avian DVR and part of the mammalian amygdala have similar connections74,76,77; and both have conserved developmental expression patterns of regulatory genes that have important roles in brain regionalization and morphogenesis28,68. In the most detailed gene expression study, Puelles et al.28 proposed that the common topographic expression patterns of the transcription factors EMX1 and PAX6 in the avian hyperstriatum ventrale and in the mammalian dorsal claustrum and basolateral amygdala indicate that these structures both arose from the lateral pallium (BOX 2). They argued that the absence of EMX1 but the presence of other pallial genes in the avian neostriatum and in the mammalian ventral claustrum and lateral anterior amygdala indicate that these structures commonly arose from the ventral pallium. They further proposed that the avian archistriatum and mammalian amygdala consist of subpallial parts derived from striatal and pallidal cell groups, and, by this association, that the avian archistriatum is homologous to the mammalian amygdala, as originally proposed by Edinger1.

Both the above hypotheses have their limitations. For the nuclear-to-layered hypotheses, developmental studies have not been conducted to investigate whether the three types of serially connected neuron in birds arise from cell types similar to those that give rise to the cortical layers in mammals. Furthermore, not all gene expression patterns support one-to-one molecular relationships between avian pallial subdivisions and mammalian cortical layers78. In addition, not all findings support the nuclear-to-claustrum/amygdala hypothesis. Although initial studies28 reported that this hypothesis was supported by the lack of pallial EMX1 expression in the avian neostriatum (so-called ventral pallium) and the mammalian ventral claustrum and lateral anterior amygdala, recent fate mapping showed that the ventral claustrum contains dispersed EMX1-expressing cells and the lateral amygdala contains many EMX1-expressing cells79. The antiquity of the claustrum has also been debated. One study80 reported that monotremes (platypus and echidnas) lack a claustrum, whereas a later study81 reported that echidnas, but not platypus, have a rudimentary claustrum located more ventrally in the white matter relative to the location of the claustrum in other mammals. So, it is possible that some monotreme groups have lost the claustrum; that the echidna has independently evolved it or another deep cortical derivative; or that the claustrum is not an ancestral mammalian trait. Further investigation is required.

A new nomenclature

Despite an extensive revision of our understanding of telencephalic evolution, the common nomenclature used for the avian telencephalon has, until 2004 (REF. 82), retained all of the classical evolution- and scala naturae-based terminology. For this reason, findings in ‘birdbrains’ have been habitually misinterpreted by neuroscientists studying non-avian brains as pertaining to the basal ganglia or as largely irrelevant to mammals. To rectify this problem, an international consortium of specialists in avian, mammalian, reptilian and fish neurobiology — the Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium (BOX 1) — assembled with the goal of revising the terminology for the avian brain. Through online discussions, an Avian Brain Nomenclature Exchange web site, various meetings held over a period of 6 years and an Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum held at Duke University, North Carolina83, the group developed a new terminology that represents the current understanding of avian telencephalic organization and its homologies with mammals82 (FIG. 1c). On the basis of the evidence summarized above, we concluded that the avian telencephalon is organized into three main, developmentally distinct domains that are homologous in fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds and mammals: pallial, striatal and pallidal domains (FIG. 1c). We renamed the subdivisions within each of these domains in birds with homology-based terms or roots that allow reference to named regions in mammals, and eliminated all phylogeny-based prefixes (palaeo-, archi- and neo-) that erroneously implied the relative age of each subdivision.

The striatal and pallidal domains

We renamed the avian palaeostriatum augmentatum and LPO as the lateral and medial parts of the avian dorsal striatum, and identified a nucleus accumbens and olfactory tubercle as parts of the avian ventral striatum (FIG. 1c). We renamed the region that includes the palaeostriatum primitivum and ventral palaeostriatum as the ‘pallidum’ (FIG. 1c). Like the mammalian pallidum, the avian pallidum has a sparse distribution of cells24, giving the region its pale appearance and, therefore, its name. The dorsal region of the avian pallidum was found to be homologous to the mammalian globus pallidus and named as such, whereas the ventral part was determined to be homologous to the mammalian ventral pallidum. The dorsal pallidum, however, differs between mammals and birds. In mammals, it consists of two segments with distinct connectivity —the internal and external globus pallidus —whereas in birds, neurons with both phenotypes are intermingled25 (FIG. 1c).

The pallial domain

We concluded that the avian pallium is organized into four main subdivisions instead of three striatal subdivisions (hyperstriatum, neostriatum and archistriatum)7 and renamed them hyperpallium (hypertrophied pallium; upper part of old hyperstriatum), mesopallium (middle pallium; lower part of old hyperstriatum), nidopallium (nest pallium; old neostriatum) and arcopallium (arched pallium; most of old archistriatum) (FIG. 1c). We concluded that several neuronal populations adjacent to the arcopallium and the posterior part of what had been regarded as archistriatum are homologous to pallial and subpallial regions of the mammalian amygdala, and renamed them as members of the amygdaloid complex. Other regions that were widely recognized to be homologous among vertebrates — the hippocampus, olfactory (piriform) cortex and olfactory bulb — did not require name changes. After extensive evaluation of the various one-to-one homology hypotheses of the avian and mammalian pallia28,44,68,74,75,77,84 (BOX 2), we concluded that the evidence is not strong enough for any specific proposed homologies to be incorporated into a new pallial terminology. However, we recognize that this is an area of active research and debate, and designed the new terminology to be compatible with the adoption of any one-to-one homology hypothesis should future evidence be more convincing.

A new view of telencephalic evolution

With this new understanding of the avian telencephalic organization and its homologies with that of mammals, we can generate more informed hypotheses and conclusions about telencephalic evolution in vertebrates. It is now apparent that the organization of the true basal ganglia among birds, mammals and other vertebrates (that is, distinct nuclear striatal and pallidal domains with more dopaminergic input into the striatal domain24) is quite conserved. By contrast, the organization of the pallial domains of these groups is more varied. The avian hyperpallium has a unique organization that has so far been found only in birds69. This consists of semi-layered subdivisions, and might have evolved more recently than the mammalian six-layered cortex, as birds evolved ~50–100 million years after mammals85,86 (FIG. 2). The DVR (which, in birds, contains the mesopallium, nidopallium and arcopallium) is a nuclear, grey matter formation that is unique to birds and reptiles. The six-layered cortex is unique to mammals, and, as all the main groups of living mammals (monotremes, marsupials and placentals) have a six-layered cortex87, it was presumably inherited from their common therapsid ancestor more than 200 million years ago (FIG. 2). Furthermore, new findings indicate that mammals did not arise from reptiles, but from therapsids, and that the last common ancestor of the reptile and mammal lineages was the stem amniotes86. As all non-mammalian therapsids are now extinct (FIG. 2), it is difficult to trace from stem amniotes to mammals the evolutionary history of mammalian telencephalic organization — layered, nuclear or otherwise. Therefore, the reptilian nuclear pallial organization cannot be assumed to represent the ancestral condition for mammals.

Figure 2. Simplified modern view of vertebrate evolution.

Figure 2

The diagram begins with the fish group that contains the most recent ancestors of land vertebrates. This differs from the classic view in that instead of giving rise to reptiles, ancestral amphibians are thought to have given rise to stem amniotes. Stem amniotes then split into at least two groups: the sauropsids, which gave rise to all modern reptiles as we know them today; and the therapsids, which, through a series of now-extinct intermediate forms, evolved into mammals. Many sauropsids (reptiles) are currently living. Solid horizontal lines indicate temporal fossil evidence. Dashed lines indicate proposed ancestral links based on other types of data. MYA, million years ago Based on REFS 85,86.

Further, it is now known that evolution is not invariably progressive or linear, so there is no basis for the view that more recently evolved species or structures are more advanced. In support of this conclusion, we now know that the telencephala of fishes are not devoted mainly to olfactory function, as the olfactory area represents only a limited portion of the fish pallium88. In addition, fishes have a hippocampus (archicortex), and the main function of the hippocampus in fishes, reptiles, birds and mammals alike is not olfaction, but memory formation and spatial mapping89,90.

So, as for birds, it might be best to abandon the use of the terms archicortex, palaeocortex, archistriatum, palaeostriatum and neostriatum for mammals and other vertebrates in favour of the alternatives — hippocampus or hippocampal cortex, piriform cortex, amygdala, striatum and pallidum. However, alternative terms for ‘neocortex’, such as ‘isocortex’, have not been universally accepted. ‘Neocortex’ would be appropriate if taken to refer to the uniqueness of this cortical structure among vertebrates. However,‘neocortex’ should not be taken to mean that it is the only unique form of pallial organization, that it evolved out of a palaeo- and/or archicortex, or that it is the newest pallial organization to have evolved. In the absence of a universally accepted alternative to neocortex, for the remainder of this article we use the term ‘six-layered cortex’. Although some regions of this cortical domain have fewer than six layers, we justify our use by analogy to the term tetrapods, which refers to all taxa derived from ancestral four-footed vertebrates, including snakes and whales.

Overall, the evidence indicates that there are pallial, striatal and pallidal domains in most or all vertebrates24. Therefore, it is reasonable to propose that the telencephala of early fishes possessed all three domains, which were then inherited as a package by later vertebrates, and independently modified in them. The conserved organization of striatal and pallidal domains indicates that there might be constraints on how the basal ganglia can be organized. The diverse organizations of the pallial domains indicate that there are fewer constraints on how the pallium can be organized. This view has important implications for our understanding of neural mechanisms of cognition.

Avian cognition and brain function

On the basis of this new understanding of avian brain organization and its evolutionary relationships, we estimate that, as in mammals, the adult avian pallium comprises about 75% of the telencephalic volume (FIG. 1c; calculated from sagittal series of pigeon and zebra finch brain sections). This realization of a relatively large and well developed avian pallium that processes information in a similar manner to mammalian sensory and motor cortices sets the stage for a re-evaluation of the cognitive abilities of birds, which, since the 1950s, have been increasingly appreciated as far more complex than was originally presumed91,92. For example, pigeons can memorize up to 725 different visual patterns93, learn to categorize objects as ‘human-made’ versus ‘natural’94, discriminate cubistic and impressionistic styles of painting95, communicate using visual symbols96, rank patterns using transitive inferential logic97 and occasionally ‘lie’98,99. New Caledonian crows make tools out of leaves or novel human-made material, use them appropriately to retrieve food and are thought to pass this knowledge on to other crows through social learning100,101. Magpies develop an understanding of object constancy at an earlier relative age in their lifespan than any other organism tested and can use this skill to the same extent as humans102. Scrub-jays show episodic memory — the ability to recall events that take place at a specific time or place, which was once thought to be unique to humans103. This same species modifies its food-storing strategy according to the possibility of future stealing by other birds and, therefore, exhibits a behaviour that would qualify as theory-of-mind104. Owls have a highly sophisticated capacity for sound localization, used for nocturnal hunting, that rivals that of humans and that is developed through learning66. Parrots, hummingbirds and oscine songbirds possess the rare skill of vocal learning105. This trait is a prerequisite in humans for spoken language and, with the exceptions of cetaceans and possibly bats, is not found in any other mammal106. In addition, parrots can learn human words and use them to communicate reciprocally with humans. African grey parrots, in particular, can use human words in numerical and relational concepts107,108, abilities that were once thought to be unique to humans.

So, many birds have cognitive proficiencies that are quite sophisticated, and some birds and mammals have cognitive proficiencies that clearly exceed all other birds or mammals. As these cognitive functions are carried out by the six-layered cortex in mammals109 but by nuclear pallial areas in birds58,66,110,111, it is clear that the mammalian six-layered cortical architecture is not the only neuroarchitectural solution for the generation of complex cognitive behaviours. It is also clear that pallial–cortical folding is not required. Birds apparently cannot use cortical folding because of the nuclear organization of their telencephalon; among mammals such folding is related more to absolute brain size than to behavioural complexity112,113. The presence of specific brain subdivisions and connections are more important factors for the generation of behavioural complexity.

An example of how avian pallial and sub-pallial areas can interact to produce complex behaviour in the context of the new view of avian brain organization can be seen in the brain pathways that control learned vocal communication (FIG. 3). Most of the telencephalic auditory processing areas are in the pallium, adjacent to a smaller auditory area in the striatum (FIG. 3a). Likewise, most of the telencephalic vocal control nuclei are in the pallium, with one vocal nucleus in the striatum (FIG. 3b). The vocal nuclei that are involved in the production of learned vocalizations, including human speech in parrots111, make up a pathway that directly innervates brainstem motor neurons (FIG. 3b, black arrows). This vocal motor pathway is similar to mammalian motor corticobulbar pathways106. The vocal nuclei that are involved in the imitation of vocalizations form a pallial–basal ganglia–thalamic–pallial loop (FIG. 3b, white arrows). This vocal learning pathway is similar to mammalian cortical–basal ganglia–thalamic–cortical loops27,106,114, which are involved in motor learning, sensorimotor integration and addictive behaviours. Other avian sensory and motor systems that are used for cognitive behaviours share a common circuit organization with the auditory and vocal pathways63,64.

Figure 3. Auditory and vocal pathways of the songbird brain within the context of the new consensus view of avian brain organization.

Figure 3

Only the most prominent and/or most studied connections are indicated. a | The auditory pathway. Most of the hindbrain connectivity is extrapolated from non-songbird species. For clarity, reciprocal connections in the pallial auditory areas are not indicated. b | The vocal pathways. Black arrows show connections of the posterior vocal pathway (or vocal motor pathway), white arrows indicate the anterior vocal pathway (or pallial–basal ganglia–thalamic–pallial loop) and dashed lines show connections between the two pathways. Av, avalanche; B, basorostralis; CLM, caudal lateral mesopallium; CMM, caudal medial mesopallium; CN, cochlear nucleus; CSt, caudal striatum; DLM, dorsal lateral nucleus of the medial thalamus; DM, dorsal medial nucleus; E, entopallium; HVC (a letter-based name); L1, L2, L3, fields L1, L2 and L3; LAreaX, lateral AreaX of the striatum; LLD, lateral lemniscus, dorsal nucleus; LLI, lateral lemniscus, intermediate nucleus; LLV, lateral lemniscus, ventral nucleus; LMAN, lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium; LMO, lateral oval nucleus of the mesopallium; MLd, dorsal lateral nucleus of the mesencephalon; NCM, caudal medial nidopallium; NIf, interfacial nucleus of the nidopallium; nXIIts, nucleus XII, tracheosyringeal part; OB, olfactory bulb; Ov, ovoidalis; PAm, para-ambiguus; RA, robust nucleus of the arcopallium; RAm, retroambiguus; SO, superior olive; Uva, nucleus uvaeformis.

Conclusion

The inaccurate evolution-based terminology for the vertebrate brain that was used throughout the twentieth century became a severe impediment to the communication of scientific discoveries and the generation of new insights. Many of the tenets on which this old view of vertebrate telencephalic evolution was based have been refuted. The problems created by this view and its associated nomenclature have now been rectified for the avian brain with a new terminology that reflects the current understanding of vertebrate brain organization, homologies, evolution and function. This new understanding should facilitate a better assimilation of scientific insights into brain function through the study of birds.

Acknowledgments

This effort was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health and National Science Foundation, and by the National Science Foundation Waterman Award to E.D.J.

Footnotes

Competing interests statement

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

DATABASES

The following terms in this article are linked online to: Entrez Gene: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=gene BDNF | DLX1 | DLX2 | EAG2 | EMX1 | ENK | ER81 | GluR1 | mGluR2 | NKX2.1 | PAX6 | ROR-β | SP | TBR1 | ZENK

FURTHER INFORMATION

Avian Brain Nomenclature Exchange web site: http://avianbrain.org/

Digital Anatomist Project: http://www9.biostr.washington.edu/da.html

Access to this interactive links box is free online.

References

  • 1.Edinger L. Investigations on the Comparative Anatomy of the Brain. 1–5. Moritz Diesterweg; Frankfurt/Main: 1888–1903. (Translation from German) [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Darwin C. The Origin of Species. Murray: 1859. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Edinger L. The Anatomy of the Central Nervous System of Man and of Vertebrates in General. 5. F. A. Davis Company; Philadelphia: 1896. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Edinger L. The relations of comparative anatomy to comparative psychology. Comp Neurol Psychol. 1908;18:437–457. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Northcutt RG. Changing views of brain evolution. Brain Res Bull. 2001;55:663–674. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00560-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ariëns Kappers C. The phylogenesis of the paleo-cortex and archi-cortex compared with the evolution of the visual neo-cortex. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1909;4:161–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ariëns Kappers C. The ontogenetic development of the corpus striatum in birds and a comparison with mammals and man. Proc Kon Akad v Wetens te Amsterdam. 1922;26:135–158. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ariëns Kappers C. Three lectures on neurobiotaxis and other subjects delivered at the University of Copenhagen. Leven and Munksgaard; Copenhagen: 1928. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Edinger L, Wallenberg A, Holmes GM. Untersuchungen über die vergleichende Anatomie des Gehirns. Das Vorderhirn der Vögel Abhandlungen der Senckenbergischen naturforschenden Gesellschaft. 1903;20:343–426. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rose M. Über die cytoarchitektonische Gliederung des Vorderhirns der Vogel. J f Psychol Neurol. 1914;21(suppl 1):278–352. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Holmgren N. Points of view concerning forebrain morphology in higher vertebrates. Acta Zool Stockh. 1925;6:413–477. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kuhlenbeck H. The ontogenetic development and phylogenetic significance of the cortex telencephali in the chick. J Comp Neurol. 1938;69:273–301. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Elliot Smith G. The term ‘archipallium’ - a disclaimer. Anatomischer Anzeiger. 1910;35:429–430. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ariëns Kappers CU, Huber CG, Crosby EC. Comparative Anatomy of the Nervous System of Vertebrates, Including Man. Hafner; New York: 1936. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Parent A, Olivier A. Comparative histochemical study of the corpus striatum. J Hirnforsch. 1970;12:73–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Karten HJ. In: Comparative and Evolutionary Aspects of the Vertebrate Central Nervous System. Pertras J, editor. 1969. pp. 164–179. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Karten HJ, Hodos W. A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Pigeon (Columba livia) Johns Hopkins Univ. Press; Baltimore: 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dahlström A, Fuxe K. Evidence for the existence of monoamine-containing neurons in the central nervous system I. Demonstration of monoamines in cell bodies of brainstem neurons. Acta Physiol Scand. 1964;62:1–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Juorio AV, Vogt M. Monoamines and their metabolites in the avian brain. J Physiol. 1967;189:489–518. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Karten HJ, Dubbeldam JL. The organization and projections of the paleostriatal complex in the pigeon (Columba livia) J Comp Neurol. 1973;148:61–90. doi: 10.1002/cne.901480105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Graybiel AM. Neuropeptides in the basal ganglia. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis. 1986;64:135–161. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Smeets WJ. Comparative aspects of basal forebrain organization in vertebrates. Eur J Morphol. 1992;30:23–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Steiner H, Gerfen CR. Role of dynorphin and enkephalin in the regulation of striatal output pathways and behavior. Exp Brain Res. 1998;123:60–76. doi: 10.1007/s002210050545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reiner A, Medina L, Veenman CL. Structural and functional evolution of the basal ganglia in vertebrates. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:235–285. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jiao Y, et al. Identification of the anterior nucleus of the ansa lenticularis in birds as the homologue of the mammalian subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6998–7010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-06998.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Graybiel AM. The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophr Bull. 1997;23:459–469. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.3.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Perkel D, Farries M. Complementary ‘bottom-up’ and ‘top-down’ approaches to basal ganglia function. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:725–731. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00156-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Puelles L, et al. Pallial and subpallial derivatives in the embryonic chick and mouse telencephalon, traced by the expression of the genes Dlx-2, Emx-1, Nkx-2.1, Pax-6, and Tbr-1. J Comp Neurol. 2000;424:409–438. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000828)424:3<409::aid-cne3>3.0.co;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cobos I, Shimamura K, Rubenstein JL, Martinez S, Puelles L. Fate map of the avian anterior forebrain at the four-somite stage, based on the analysis of quail-chick chimeras. Dev Biol. 2001;239:46–67. doi: 10.1006/dbio.2001.0423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Redies C, Medina L, Puelles L. Cadherin expression by embryonic divisions and derived gray matter structures in the telencephalon of the chicken. J Comp Neurol. 2001;438:253–285. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Marín O, Rubenstein JL. A long, remarkable journey: tangential migration in the telencephalon. Nature Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:780–790. doi: 10.1038/35097509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Veenman CL. Pigeon basal ganglia: insights into the neuroanatomy underlying telencephalic sensorimotor processes in birds. Eur J Morphol. 1997;35:220–233. doi: 10.1076/ejom.35.4.220.13079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sun Z, Reiner A. Localization of dopamine D1A and D1B receptor mRNAs in the forebrain and midbrain of the domestic chick. J Chem Neuroanat. 2000;19:211–224. doi: 10.1016/s0891-0618(00)00069-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Li X-C, Jarvis E. Sensory- and motor-driven BDNF expression in a vocal communication system. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2001:538.8. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Reiner A, Meade CA, Cuthberston SL, Laverghetta A, Bottjer SW. An immunohistochemical and pathway tracing study of the striatopallidal organization of Area X in the zebra finch. J Comp Neurol. 2004;469:239–261. doi: 10.1002/cne.11012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wada K, Sakaguchi H, Jarvis ED, Hagiwara M. Differential expression of glutamate receptors in avian neural pathways for learned vocalization. J Comp Neurol. 2004;476:44–64. doi: 10.1002/cne.20201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brauth SE, Kitt CA. The paleostriatal system of Caiman crocodilus. J Comp Neurol. 1980;189:437–465. doi: 10.1002/cne.901890303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brauth SE, Reiner A, Kitt CA, Karten HJ. The substance P-containing striatotegmental path in reptiles: an immunohistochemical study. J Comp Neurol. 1983;219:305–327. doi: 10.1002/cne.902190306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Reiner A, Brauth SE, Karten HJ. Evolution of the amniote basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 1984;7:320–325. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Smeets WJAJ. In: Phylogeny and Development of Catecholamine Systems in the CNS of Vertebrates. Smeets WJAJ, editor. Cambridge Univ. Press; Cambridge, England: 1994. pp. 103–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Swanson L. What is the brain? Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:519–527. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01639-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Reblet C, et al. Neuroepithelial origin of the insular and endopiriform parts of the claustrum. Brain Res Bull. 2002;57:495–497. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00719-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zeier H, Karten HJ. The archistriatum of the pigeon: organization of afferent and efferent connections. Brain Res. 1971;31:313–326. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Karten HJ, Shimizu T. The origins of neocortex: connections and lamination as distinct events in evolution. J Cogn Neurosci. 1989;1:291–301. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Vates GE, Broome BM, Mello CV, Nottebohm F. Auditory pathways of caudal telencephalon and their relation to the song system of adult male zebra finches. J Comp Neurol. 1996;366:613–642. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960318)366:4<613::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wild JM. The avian somatosensory system: the pathway from wing to Wulst in a passerine (Chloris chloris) Brain Res. 1997;759:122–134. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00253-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Shimizu T, Hodos W. Reversal learning in pigeons: effects of selective lesions of the Wulst. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:262–272. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.2.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Mello CV, Clayton DF. Song-induced ZENK gene expression in auditory pathways of songbird brain and its relation to the song control system. J Neurosci. 1994;14:6652–6666. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-11-06652.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jarvis ED, Mello CV, Nottebohm F. Associative learning and stimulus novelty influence the song-induced expression of an immediate early gene in the canary forebrain. Learn Mem. 1995;2:62–80. doi: 10.1101/lm.2.2.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wild JM, Reinke H, Farabaugh SM. A non-thalamic pathway contributes to a whole body map in the brain of the budgerigar. Brain Res. 1997;755:137–141. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00026-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Laverghetta AV, Shimizu T. Visual discrimination in the pigeon (Columba livia): effects of selective lesions of the nucleus rotundus. Neuroreport. 1999;10:981–985. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Vicario DS. Organization of the zebra finch song control system: II. Functional organization of outputs from nucleus robustus archistriatalis. J Comp Neurol. 1991;309:486–494. doi: 10.1002/cne.903090405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wild JM. Descending projections of the songbird nucleus robustus archistriatalis. J Comp Neurol. 1993;338:225–241. doi: 10.1002/cne.903380207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wild JM, Williams MN. Rostral Wulst in passerine birds. I. Origin, course, and terminations of an avian pyramidal tract. J Comp Neurol. 2000;416:429–450. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, Leonard CM. Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J Comp Neurol. 1976;165:457–486. doi: 10.1002/cne.901650405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bottjer SW, Miesner EA, Arnold AP. Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. Science. 1984;224:901–903. doi: 10.1126/science.6719123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Shimizu T, Karten HJ. In: The Neocortex. Finlay BL, editor. Plenum; New York: 1990. pp. 75–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. J Neurosci. 1991;11:2896–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Güntürkün O. In: Neural and Behavioral Plasticity. Andrew RJ, editor. Oxford Univ. Press; Oxford: 1991. pp. 92–105. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wild JM, Karten HJ, Frost BJ. Connections of the auditory forebrain in the pigeon (Columba livia) J Comp Neurol. 1993;337:32–62. doi: 10.1002/cne.903370103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Butler AB. The evolution of the dorsal pallium in the telencephalon of amniotes: cladistic analysis and a new hypothesis. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1994;19:66–101. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)90004-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Shimizu T, Cox K, Karten HJ. Intratelencephalic projections of the visual Wulst in pigeons (Columba livia) J Comp Neurol. 1995;359:551–572. doi: 10.1002/cne.903590404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kröner S, Güntürkün O. Afferent and efferent connections of the caudolateral neostriatum in the pigeon (Columba livia): A retro- and anterograde pathway tracing study. J Comp Neurol. 1999;407:228–260. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990503)407:2<228::aid-cne6>3.0.co;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Shimizu T, Bowers AN. Visual circuits of the avian telencephalon: evolutionary implications. Behav Brain Res. 1999;98:183–191. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00083-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Brainard M, Doupe A. Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations. Nature. 2000;404:762–766. doi: 10.1038/35008083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Knudsen EI. Instructed learning in the auditory localization pathway of the barn owl. Nature. 2002;417:322–328. doi: 10.1038/417322a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Mello CV. Mapping vocal communication pathways in birds with inducible gene expression. J Comp Physiol A. 2002;188:943–959. doi: 10.1007/s00359-002-0347-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Smith-Fernandez AS, Pieau C, Repérant J, Boncinelli E, Wassef M. Expression of the Emx-1 and Dlx-1 homeobox genes define three molecularly distinct domains in the telencephalon of mouse, chick, turtle and frog embryos: implications for the evolution of telencephalic subdivisions in amniotes. Development. 1998;125:2099–2111. doi: 10.1242/dev.125.11.2099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Medina L, Reiner A. Do birds possess homologues of mammalian primary visual, somatosensory and motor cortices? Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:1–12. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01486-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kuenzel WJ, Masson M. A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Chick (Gallus domesticus) The Johns Hopkins Univ. Press; Baltimore: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Karten HJ. Homology and evolutionary origins of the ‘neocortex’. Brain Behav Evol. 1991;38:264–272. doi: 10.1159/000114393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Mello CV, Clayton DF. Differential induction of the ZENK gene in the avian forebrain and song control circuit after metrazole-induced depolarization. J Neurobiol. 1995;26:145–161. doi: 10.1002/neu.480260112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dugas-Ford J, Ragsdale C. 23rd Annual J. B Johnston Club Meeting and 15th Annual Karger Workshop 2003. Brain Behav Evol. 2003;62:168–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bruce LL, Neary TJ. The limbic system of tetrapods: a comparative analysis of cortical and amygdalar populations. Brain Behav Evol. 1995;46:224–234. doi: 10.1159/000113276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Striedter GF. The telencephalon of tetrapods in evolution. Brain Behav Evol. 1997;49:179–213. doi: 10.1159/000112991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Bruce LL, Kornblum HI, Seroogy KB. Comparison of thalamic populations in mammals and birds: expression of ErbB4 mRNA. Brain Res Bull. 2002;57:455–461. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00678-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Martínez-García F, Martínez-Marcos A, Lanuza E. The pallial amygdala of amniote vertebrates: evolution of the concept, evolution of the structure. Brain Res Bull. 2002;57:463–469. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00665-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Haesler S, et al. FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3164–3175. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4369-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Gorski JA, et al. Cortical excitatory neurons and glia, but not GABAergic neurons, are produced in the Emx1-expressing lineage. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6309–6314. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06309.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Butler AB, Molnár Z, Manger PR. Apparent absence of claustrum in monotremes: implications for forebrain evolution in amniotes. Brain Behav Evol. 2002;60:230–240. doi: 10.1159/000066698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ashwell KW, Hardman C, Paxinos G. The claustrum is not missing from all monotreme brains. Brain Behav Evol. 2004;64:223–241. doi: 10.1159/000080243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Reiner A, et al. Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 2004;473:377–414. doi: 10.1002/cne.20118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Reiner A, et al. The Avian Brain Nomenclature Forum: a new century in comparative neuroanatomy. J Comp Neurol. 2004;473:E1–E6. doi: 10.1002/cne.20119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Reiner AJ. A hypothesis as to the organization of cerebral cortex in the common amniote ancestor of modern reptiles and mammals. Novartis Found Symp. 2000;228:83–102. 102–113. doi: 10.1002/0470846631.ch7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Carroll RL. Vertebrate Paleontology and Evolution. W. H. Freeman; New York: 1988. pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Evans SE. In: Evolutionary Developmental Biology of the Cerebral Cortex. Bock GR, Cardew G, editors. John Wiley & Sons; Chichester: 2000. pp. 109–113. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Northcutt RG, Kaas JH. The emergence and evolution of mammalian neocortex. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18:373–379. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)93932-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Northcutt RG. Visual pathways in elasmobranchs: organization and phylogenetic implications. J Exp Zool Suppl. 1990;5:97–107. doi: 10.1002/jez.1402560514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Suzuki WA, Clayton NS. The hippocampus and memory: a comparative and ethological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:768–773. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00148-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Rodríguez F, et al. Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrate evolution: insights from reptiles and teleost fish. Brain Res Bull. 2002;57:499–503. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00682-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Marler P. Characteristics of some animals calls. Nature. 1955;176:6–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Thorpe WH. The learning of song patterns by birds, with special reference to the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ibis. 1958;100:535–570. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.von Fersen L, Delius JD. Long-term retention of many visual patterns by pigeons. Ethology. 1989;82:141–155. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Lubow RE. High-order concept formation in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974;21:475–483. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1974.21-475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Watanabe S, Sakamoto J, Wakita M. Pigeons’ discrimination of paintings by Monet and Picasso. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995;63:165–174. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.63-165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Lubinski D, MacCorquodale K. ‘Symbolic communication’ between two pigeons (Columba livia) without unconditioned reinforcement. J Comp Psychol. 1984;98:372–380. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.von Fersen L, Wynne CDL, Delius JD, Staddon JER. Transitive inference formation in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1992;17:334–341. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Lanza RP, Starr J, Skinner BF. ‘Lying’ in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1982;38:201–203. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1982.38-201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Munn C. Birds that cry ‘wolf’. Nature. 1986;319:143–145. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Weir AA, Chappell J, Kacelnik A. Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science. 2002;297:981. doi: 10.1126/science.1073433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Hunt GR, Gray RD. Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2003;270:867–874. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Pollok B, Prior H, Güntürkün O. Development of object-permanence in the food-storing magpie (Pica pica) J Comp Psychol. 2000;114:148–157. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.2.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Clayton NS, Dickinson A. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature. 1998;395:272–274. doi: 10.1038/26216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature. 2001;414:443–446. doi: 10.1038/35106560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Jarvis ED, et al. Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain. Nature. 2000;406:628–632. doi: 10.1038/35020570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Jarvis ED. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1016:749–777. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Pepperberg I. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Harvard Univ. Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 1999. pp. 96–167. [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Pepperberg IM, Shive HR. Simultaneous development of vocal and physical object combinations by a Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus): bottle caps, lids, and labels. J Comp Pyschol. 2001;115:376–384. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Gazzaniga MS. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Delius JD, Hollard VD. Orientation invariance of shape recognition in forebrain-lesioned pigeons. Behav Brain Res. 1987;23:251–259. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(87)90025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Lavenex PB. Lesions in the budgerigar vocal control nucleus NLc affect production, but not memory, of English words and natural vocalizations. J Comp Neurol. 2000;421:437–460. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000612)421:4<437::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Van Essen D. A tension-based theory of morphophogenesis and compact wiring in the central nervous system. Nature. 1997;385:313–318. doi: 10.1038/385313a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Striedter GF. Principles of Brain Evolution. Sinuaer Associates; Massachusetts: 2004. pp. 345–361. [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Bottjer SW, Johnson F. Circuits, hormones, and learning: vocal behavior in songbirds. J Neurobiol. 1997;33:602–618. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<602::aid-neu8>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES