Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Mar 11.
Published in final edited form as: Cortex. 2006 Jan;42(1):90–93. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70326-5

CHIMPANZEE RIGHT-HANDEDNESS: INTERNAL AND EXTERNAL VALIDITY IN THE ASSESSMENT OF HAND USE

William D Hopkins 1
PMCID: PMC2654332  NIHMSID: NIHMS96080  PMID: 16509113

Introduction

Recent studies in captive and wild monkeys and apes have documented evidence of population-level handedness (see Bradshaw and Rogers, 1993; Ward and Hopkins, 1993). Similarly, population-level limb preferences have been reported in other vertebrates (Rogers and Andrew, 2002). Despite the increasing evidence of population-level asymmetries in vertebrates, there remain many skeptics of these findings and the criticisms have been both methodological and statistical (Crow, 2004; McGrew and Marchant, 1997; Palmer, 2002, 2003). Interestingly, the nature of many of these criticisms was subject to discussion in the recent reviews of Annett’s (2002) book on handedness in this journal (see commentaries by Corballis (2004) and Elias (2004). Annett (2004) herself claimed that she was skeptical of the evidence of handedness in chimpanzees, particularly from my laboratory. In this paper, I address some of the criticisms of our work.

Internal and External Validity in Hand Use

In 1995, I reported evidence of population-level right handedness for a measure referred to as the TUBE task (Hopkins, 1995). The TUBE task assesses hand use for coordinated bimanual actions. Briefly, peanut butter is placed on the inside edges of a poly-vinyl-chloride pipe that is approximately 2.5 cm in diameter and approximately 20 to 25 cm long. The PVC is handed to the subjects and they hold the pipe with one hand and extract the peanut butter with a finger from the opposite hand. The hand of the finger used to extract the peanut butter is recorded as the dominant hand. The original study was criticized on several grounds including a) a lack of control for which hand the subjects took the TUBE with and b) we recorded each probing actions as an individual response rather than record bouts of hand use (McGrew and Marchant, 1997) and c) unequal number of observations of hand use across subjects (Palmer, 2002) and d) the results were restricted to the colony of chimpanzees housed at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (Palmer, 2002, 2003). Shown in Figure 1 are the mean handedness indices for several follow-up studies on the TUBE task. Neither controlling for which hand takes the TUBE (Hopkins et al., 2001), controlling the number of responses obtained from each chimpanzee (Hopkins and Cantalupo, 2003) nor recording bouts rather than individual responses (Hopkins et al., 2005a) altered the general conclusion of population-level right handedness for this task. Furthermore, evidence of population-level right handedness was not restricted to the Yerkes chimpanzees but evident in two additional colonies of captive chimpanzees (Hopkins et al., 2004). Lastly, consistency in hand use between colonies was not restricted to the TUBE task (see Figure 2). We have found consistent patterns of hand use between two colonies of chimpanzees for four additional measures including manual gestures, simple reaching and tool use (Hopkins et al., 2005b, 2005c).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Mean handedness index (+ s. e.) for several studies using the TUBE task in captive chimpanzees. Handedness indices are calculated following the formula [H = (#R−#L)/(#R+#L)] where R and L refer to the number of left and right hand responses (Reprinted by Hopkins et al., 2005a).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Mean handedness index (+ s. e.) for 4 behavioral measures collected in the YERKES and BASTROP chimpanzees colonies. As can be seen, HI scores were comparable across measures and colonies.

Discussion

The results of our studies indicate that captive chimpanzees show population-level right handedness. Contrary to some claims, these results are not restricted to the YERKES colony nor can these results be explained on the basis of the subject’s human rearing experience. Notwithstanding, the issue of discrepant findings between wild populations of apes compared to our studies in captive chimpanzees remains an important issue. I believe there are several possible explanations for this discrepancy.

First, in studies of captive chimpanzees, we have much greater control over positional and situational factors, which stands in strong contrast to the observation conditions in wild chimpanzees (Hopkins and Cantalupo, 2004). Having better control of these variables may allow for more robust expressions of population-level handedness in captive compared to wild chimpanzees. I believe that measuring handedness for tasks that require coordinated bimanual actions, such as the TUBE task, greatly enhance the probability of detecting population-level handedness in captive and wild chimpanzees and other great apes (see Byrne and Byrne, 1991; Corp and Byrne, 2004; Colell et al., 1995; Hopkins and Rabinowitz, 1997; Hopkins et al., 2003). Second, the behaviors measured and sensitivity of these measures differ substantially between studies in wild and captive chimpanzee. With the exception of tool use, most measures of hand use in wild chimpanzees do not elicit hand preferences at the individual level (i.e., subjects do not show lateralized preferences) (see Marchant and McGrew, 1996; McGrew and Marchant, 2001). In contrast, most structured measures used in captive studies of chimpanzee do elicit significant hand preferences at the individual level. Thus, there are differences in the sensitivity of handedness measures between settings. The exception is tool use, where strong individual preferences are elicited in both captive and wild chimpanzees (see Boesch, 1991; McGrew and Marchant, 1992; McGrew et al., 1999; Sugiyama, 1995; Sugiyama et al., 1993). Interestingly, both captive and wild chimpanzees do not show population-level handedness for tool use (see Lonsdorf and Hopkins, 2005), suggesting consistency not discrepancy in findings. Third, the effect sizes for handedness are weak to moderate in our captive subjects (Cohen’s d = .40) but we can detect this effect because of relatively large samples of subjects in our studies. In contrast, samples sizes in wild chimpanzees (and other apes) are much smaller and, arguably, too small to detect the moderate effect observed in captive populations. Lastly, all caveats aside, there is some evidence of population-level right handedness in wild chimpanzees but they have been down played in the literature (see Hopkins and Cantalupo, 2004). For example, Boesch (1991) measured handedness for reaching, grooming, wodge dipping (a form of tool use) and nut-cracking (another tool use task). If a one sample t-test is applied to the percentages right hand use for each measure observed by Boesch (1991), the chimpanzees do show population-level right handedness for grooming and wodge dipping.

In sum, I would argue that captive chimpanzees and other great apes exhibit population-level handedness. More systematic and controlled studies are needed in wild apes before any definitive statements can be made regarding the potential role of different settings on the expression of handedness. An important finding, to which the comparative method can offer some interesting insight, is the difference in distribution of handedness between apes and humans. The ratio of right- to left-handed chimpanzees is about 2 : 1 or 3 : 1 (in the case of gesture and throwing) which is lower than most reports of handedness in various human cultures (Raymond and Pontier, 2004). Whether this difference reflects the emergence of socio-cultural evolution or alterations in the genome between chimpanzees and humans remains unknown but warrants further investigation.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by NIH grants RR-00165, NS-29574, NS-36605, NS-42867 and HD-38051.

References

  1. Annett M. Left, Right, Hand, and Brain: The Right-Shift Theory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  2. Annett M. Handedness and Brain Asymmetry: The Right Shift Theory. Hove: Psychology Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  3. Annett M. Perceptions of the right shift theory. Cortex. 2004;40:143–150. [Google Scholar]
  4. Boesch C. Handedness in wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology. 1991;6:541–558. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bradshaw J, Rogers LJ. The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries, Language, Tool Use and Intellect. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  6. Byrne RW, Byrne JM. Hand preferences in the skilled gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla berengei) Cortex. 1991;27:521–536. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80003-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Colell M, Segarra MD, Sabater-pi J. Manual laterality in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in complex task. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1995;109:298–307. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Corballis MC. The Lopsided Brain: Evolution of the Generative Mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  9. Corballis MC. Taking your chances. Cortex. 2004;40:117–119. [Google Scholar]
  10. Corp N, Byrne RW. Sex difference in chimpanzee handedness. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2004;123:62–68. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Crow T. What Marian Annett can teach Noam Chomsky and could have taught Stephen Jay Gould if he’d had time to listen. Cortex. 2004;40:120–134. [Google Scholar]
  12. Elias L. Acknowledge the ambition but look elsewhere for the alternatives. Cortex. 2004;40:135–138. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hopkins WD. Hand preferences for bimanual feeding in 140 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Rearing and ontogenetic factors. Developmental Psychobiology. 1994;27:395–407. doi: 10.1002/dev.420270607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hopkins WD. Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Cross-sectional analysis. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1995;105:178–190. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hopkins WD, Bard KA, Jones A, Bales S. Chimpanzee hand preference for throwing and infant cradling: Implications for the origin of human handedness. Current Anthropology. 1993;34:786–790. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hopkins WD, Cantalupo C. Does variation in sample size explain individual differences in hand preferences of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? An empirical study and reply to Palmer (2002) American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2003;121:878–881. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hopkins WD, Cantalupo C. Individual and setting differences in the hand preferences of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): A critical analysis and some alternative explanations. Laterality. 2004;10:65–80. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hopkins WD, Cantalupo C, Freeman H, Russell J, Kachin M, Nelson E. Chimpanzees are right-handed when recording bouts of hand use. Laterality. 2005a;10:121–130. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hopkins WD, Cantalupo C, Wesley MJ, Hostetter A, Pilcher D. Grip morphology and hand use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence of a left hemisphere specialization in motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2002;131:412–423. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.131.3.412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hopkins WD, Fernandez-Carriba S, Wesley MJ, Hostetter A, Pilcher D, Poss S. The use of bouts and frequencies in the evaluation of hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): An empirical study comparing two different indices of laterality. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2001;115:294–299. doi: 10.1037//0735-7036.115.3.294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hopkins WD, Hook M, Braccini S, Schapiro S. Population-level right handedness for a coordinated bimanual task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Replication and extension in a second colony of apes. International Journal of Primatology. 2003;24:677–689. doi: 10.1023/A:1023752816951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hopkins WD, Rabinowitz DM. Manual specialization and tool-use in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effect of unimanual and bimanual strategies on hand preference. Laterality. 1997;2:267–277. doi: 10.1080/713754273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hopkins WD, Russell J, Freeman H, Buehler N, Reynolds E, Schapiro SJ. The distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Psychological Science. 2005b;16:487–493. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01561.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hopkins WD, Russell J, Freeman H, Cantalupo C, Schapiro SJ. Chimpanzee right-handedness: Throwing out the first pitch. Journal of Comparative Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.4.363. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hopkins WD, Russell J, Hook M, Braccini S, Schapiro S. Simple reaching is not so simple: Association between hand use and grip preferences in captive chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology. 2005c;26:259–277. doi: 10.1007/s10764-005-2924-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hopkins WD, Stoinski T, Lukas K, Ross S, Wesley MJ. Comparative assessment of handedness for a coordinated bimanual task in chimpanzees (Pan), gorillas (Gorilla), and orangutans (Pongo) Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2003;117:302–308. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hopkins WD, Wesley MJ, Izard MK, Hook M, Schapiro SJ. Chimpanzees are predominantly right-handed: Replication in three colonies of apes. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118:659–663. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.3.659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lonsdorf EV, Hopkins WD. Wild chimpanzees show population-level handedness for tool use. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102:12634–12638. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505806102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Marchant LF, McGrew WC. Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: Comprehensive study of spontaneous activities. Journal of Human Evolution. 1996;30:427–443. [Google Scholar]
  30. Mcgrew WC, Marchant LF. Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: Hand preference or handedness? Current Anthropology. 1992;33:114–119. [Google Scholar]
  31. McGrew WC, Marchant LF. On the other hand: Current issues in and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality of hand function in nonhuman primates. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology. 1997;40:201–232. [Google Scholar]
  32. McGrew WC, Marchant LF. Ethological study of manual laterality in the chimpanzees of the Mahale mountains, Tanzania. Behaviour. 2001;138:329–358. [Google Scholar]
  33. McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Wrangham RW, Klein H. Manual laterality in anvil use: Wild chimpanzees cracking Strychnos fruits. Laterality. 1999;4:79–87. doi: 10.1080/03069887600760101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Palmer AR. Chimpanzee right-handedness reconsidered: Evaluating the evidence with funnel plots. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2002;118:191–199. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Palmer AR. Reply to Hopkins and Cantalupo: Chimpanzee right-handedness reconsidered – sampling issues and data presentation. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2003;121:382–384. [Google Scholar]
  36. Raymond M, Pontier D. Is there geographical variation in human handedness? Laterality. 2004;9:35–51. doi: 10.1080/13576500244000274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rogers LJ, Andrew RJ. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  38. Sugiyama Y. Tool-use for catching ants by chimpanzees at Bossou and Monts Nimba, West Africa. Primates. 1995;36:193–205. [Google Scholar]
  39. Sugiyama Y, Fushimi T, Sakura O, Matsuzawa T. Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates. 1993;34:151–159. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ward JP, Hopkins WD. Primate Laterality: Current Behavioral Evidence of Primate Asymmetries. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1993. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES