Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2008 Sep 27;4(1):35–49. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn027

Amygdala activation during reading of emotional adjectives—an advantage for pleasant content

Cornelia Herbert 1, Thomas Ethofer 2,3, Silke Anders 2, Markus Junghofer 4, Dirk Wildgruber 3, Wolfgang Grodd 2, Johanna Kissler 1,
PMCID: PMC2656883  PMID: 19015080

Abstract

This event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigated brain activity elicited by emotional adjectives during silent reading without specific processing instructions. Fifteen healthy volunteers were asked to read a set of randomly presented high-arousing emotional (pleasant and unpleasant) and low-arousing neutral adjectives. Silent reading of emotional in contrast to neutral adjectives evoked enhanced activations in visual, limbic and prefrontal brain regions. In particular, reading pleasant adjectives produced a more robust activation pattern in the left amygdala and the left extrastriate visual cortex than did reading unpleasant or neutral adjectives. Moreover, extrastriate visual cortex and amygdala activity were significantly correlated during reading of pleasant adjectives. Furthermore, pleasant adjectives were better remembered than unpleasant and neutral adjectives in a surprise free recall test conducted after scanning. Thus, visual processing was biased towards pleasant words and involved the amygdala, underscoring recent theoretical views of a general role of the human amygdala in relevance detection for both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli. Results indicate preferential processing of pleasant information in healthy young adults and can be accounted for within the framework of appraisal theory.

Keywords: emotion, perception, re-entrant processing, reading, amygdala, extrastriate cortex, neuroimaging


Emotional stimuli are of particular importance for an individual and demand priority access to perception and attention (Lang et al., 1997; Öhman et al., 2001). Human lesion and neuroimaging studies suggest that the amygdala, a phylogenetically old brain structure located in the mediotemporal lobes, plays a key role in the facilitated processing of emotionally significant visual stimuli (e.g. Adolphs et al., 1999; Vuilleumier et al., 2004). Over the years many studies have shown amygdala involvement in the processing of threatening and fear-relevant stimuli, such as fearful faces or pictures of human and animal attack (see e.g. Öhman and Mineka, 2001; Adolphs, 2002; Vuilleumier, 2002). This has led to an initial conceptualization of the amygdala as a structure specialized in the detection of unpleasant and fear-relevant material, possibly even a ‘fear-module’ (Öhman and Mineka, 2001). But increased amygdala activation has subsequently also been found in response to happy faces (Williams et al., 2005) and emotionally arousing pleasant scenes and objects (see e.g. Zald, 2003 for a review) as well as for pleasantly and unpleasantly arousing vocalizations (Fecteau et al., 2007). A general role of the human amygdala beyond the processing of threat-related and fear-relevant material, extending to the processing of arousing stimuli of both valences, has therefore been discussed in the recent literature (Davis and Whalen, 2001; Sabatinelli et al., 2005; Lewis et al., 2007).

In further distinction to the view of the amygdala as especially involved in fear, or the processing of emotionally arousing stimuli, enhanced amygdala activity has sometimes been found specifically to pleasurable or rewarding pleasant stimuli (O’Doherty et al., 2002; see also, Burgdorf and Panksepp, 2006). It has also been found in response to biologically meaningful, but not inherently emotional stimuli such as human eye gaze (Bonda et al., 1996; Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Kawashima et al., 1999) and also, in response to socially and individually important stimuli (e.g. Phelps et al., 2000). Moreover, individual differences in participants’ motivational state and personality traits have been reported to modulate the magnitude of amygdala activation in response to emotionally pleasant and unpleasant stimuli. For instance, during viewing of pictures of food items, amygdala activation is higher when hungry than after food intake (LaBar et al., 2001; Morris and Dolan, 2001; Hinton et al., 2004), higher in responses to happy faces than to angry faces in highly extraverted subjects (Canli et al., 2002), and higher in response to fear-relevant stimuli in highly anxious subjects than in low-anxiety subjects (Sabatinelli et al., 2005). The latter findings are hard to reconcile with the aforementioned conceptualizations of the amygdala as a general ‘fear module’ or an ‘arousal indicator’. Thus, an even broader conceptualization of amygdala function as a more dynamic ‘evolved system for relevance detection’ has been proposed (Sander et al., 2003). According to this view, the human amygdala acts as a dynamic ‘relevance detector’. It alerts us in principle towards both hostile and pleasurable stimuli but takes into account internal milieu states as well as current environmental and individual demands (Sander et al., 2003).

The special significance of most stimuli shown to robustly elicit amygdala activation, such as emotional faces, food items or fear-relevant objects such as spiders or snakes, is at least partly innate and phylogenetically ‘prepared’ (Öhman and Mineka, 2001; Öhman, 2002). But humans as members of a social and symbolic species can also use more abstract, symbolic means to communicate emotions: written words bear no resemblance to the state or object they denote, and their emotional connotation is conveyed solely on the basis of ontogenetically learned associations. Thus, their significance might be analysed only after they are subjected to higher level semantic processing and evaluation (Vanderploeg et al., 1987; Cacioppo et al., 1993).

Dual process models of emotional processing in the brain suggest two distinct processing systems: an explicit processing system operating mainly on the basis of controlled emotional evaluation and an implicit processing system responding relatively automatically to emotionally significant stimuli (Cunningham et al., 2003; Ochsner et al., 2004; Ochsner and Gross, 2005). Current evidence indicates that the amygdala is more strongly engaged in implicit or stimulus-driven than in cognitively controlled processing of emotional stimuli (Critchley, et al., 2000; Liberzon et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2003, 2004; Winston et al., 2003; Lieberman et al., 2007). Thus, the question arises whether the amygdala is activated during visual processing of highly symbolic emotional stimuli such as words.

Several neuroimaging studies on emotional word processing report activation in dorsolateral and medial prefrontal and middle temporal brain regions to be enhanced during processing of emotional in contrast to neutral words, but fail to find amygdala activation (Beauregard et al., 1997; Crosson et al., 1999, 2002; Cato et al., 2004; Kuchinke et al., 2005). Some lesion and intracranial recording studies, on the other hand, suggest that the amygdala may amplify perception and attention to emotionally challenging words (e.g. ‘rape’, ‘bastard’), possibly via reciprocal feedback projections to the ventral visual processing stream (Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Naccache et al., 2005). In neurologically intact subjects, however, evidence in favour of this thesis is still sparse. So far, few imaging studies demonstrate amygdala activation during visual processing of emotionally arousing words. More evidence exists for the selective processing of unpleasant words (Isenberg et al., 1999; Strange et al., 2000; Tabert et al., 2001; Nakic et al., 2006; Lewis et al., 2007), although amygdala activation in response to pleasant words has also been reported (Hamann and Mao, 2002; Canli et al., 2004; Kensinger and Schacter, 2006; Lewis et al., 2007). Comparing depressed patients’ and normal controls’ brain responses to differently valenced words in a lexical decision task, Canli et al. (2004) even found stronger amygdala activation in response to pleasant than to neutral words in normal controls, but not in depressed subjects.

Two of the studies reporting amygdala activation during emotional word processing suggested a modulatory role of the amygdala on other brain regions sub-serving word perception. Comparing highly aversive (threat) words to neutral words, Isenberg et al. (1999) using Positron Emission Tomography (PET) and Tabert et al. (2001) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) found enhanced processing of unpleasant words in the visual cortex to be paralleled by enhanced amygdala activation for unpleasant in comparison to neutral words. Both authors therefore assume that the amygdala amplifies perceptual processing of threat words via direct feedback connections to the visual cortex. Tabert and colleagues (2001) provide tentative support for this suggestion by showing in nine female subjects that amygdala and occipital activity was significantly correlated during processing of unpleasant words.

The assumption that the amygdala amplifies perceptual processing of at least threat words is consistent with findings of bidirectional modulatory connections between the amygdala and extrastriate cortex, so-called ‘re-entrant processing’ loops, in non-human primates (Amaral and Price, 1984), and is supported by human lesion data on the processing of emotional faces and words (Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Vuilleumier et al., 2004).

‘Re-entrant processing’ has been favoured by many authors as a model to explain facilitated sensory processing of faces and pictures in the visual cortex (Lane et al., 1997, 1999; Morris et al., 1998; Bradley et al., 2003; Winston et al., 2003; Sabatinelli et al., 2005) and some imaging studies provide empirical support for this model by showing that activity in amygdala and extrastriate cortex is correlated or at least that the two brain structures show parallel response patterns, particularly during processing of emotional faces (e.g. Morris et al., 1998, 1999; Pessoa et al., 2002).

Extrastriate cortex regions located in the ventral and lateral parts of the inferior temporal and occipital lobes respond to word stimuli (Petersen et al., 1990; Nobre et al., 1994; Cohen et al., 2002; Jobard et al., 2006; Vigneau et al., 2005; Gaillard et al., 2006) and are sensitive to a word's lexical and semantic aspects. Therefore, as previously suggested (Tabert et al., 2001; Isenberg et al., 1999), these brain areas might represent sites of amygdala-driven re-entrant processing during word processing, affording a mechanism by which facilitated visual processing could occur for content with evolutionarily prepared as well as learned emotional significance.

But, as mentioned above, amygdala activation in response to emotional words is in itself not uncontroversial. Differences in task demands may account for some of the conflicting findings: previous hemodynamic imaging studies on emotional word processing have all used ‘active tasks’ in which subjects were explicitly asked to categorize the words according to emotional, lexical or semantic aspects. Although, attention often facilitates emotional perception (Isenberg et al., 1999; Lane et al., 1999; Vuilleumier et al., 2001; Pessoa et al., 2002; Bradley and Lang, 2007), cognitively demanding experimental tasks can mitigate stimulus-driven perceptual processing due to higher order controlled processing (Hariri et al., 2000; Ochsner et al., 2002; Phan et al., 2002 for an overview). Recent evidence even suggests that linguistic processing is especially suited to down-regulate stimulus-driven affective responses (Lieberman et al., 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008). In particular, attaching word labels to emotional stimuli reduces amygdala activation and instead increases prefrontal, particularly right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation (Lieberman et al., 2007).

Surprisingly, to date no fMRI study has investigated emotional word processing during conditions of natural reading, that is, without any instructions other than to read the words silently. Word reading is a highly over-learned automated skill. We perceive the meaning of written words without being told to attend to their content, and we cannot help but process their meaning (LaBerge and Samuels, 1974; Logan, 1988). Silent word reading is probably the most natural task to study brain activation patterns underlying enhanced stimulus-driven processing of emotional words, as it occurs spontaneously and implicitly as soon as a word is perceived. Silent word reading has recently successfully been used in investigations of category-specific divisions of the semantic system (Hauk et al., 2006, 2008). In emotion research, ‘passive’ picture viewing has been used repeatedly to measure spontaneous and naturalistic responses to emotional stimuli (e.g. Bradley et al., 2003; Sabatinelli et al., 2005; Junghofer et al., 2006), because such uninstructed processing may model processing in everyday life more closely than experimentally imposed, often highly artificial, cognitive tasks. A possible drawback of this approach is reduced control over or assessment of subjects’ cognitive activity, which may introduce more variability and ‘noise’ (i.e. activity of no interest) in the data. On the other hand, if relevant activity (e.g. amygdala) can still be identified under such conditions of implicit emotional processing, this should increase confidence in such activation occurring naturally outside the laboratory.

Finding amygdala activation during spontaneous processing of unpleasant and particularly also pleasant words would help to establish several facts about its functional role: first, amygdala activation during reading would underscore that the amygdala spontaneously responds to a broad class of emotionally relevant stimuli even when their particular relevance is conveyed by abstract symbolic stimuli.

Second, the overall activation pattern in the amygdala may inform general theories of affective processing: if activation were restricted to unpleasant stimuli, results would be in line with the view that the amygdala is specialized for detecting unpleasant stimuli. Enhanced activation during the processing of both pleasantly and unpleasantly arousing words would indicate that, at least during reading, amygdala activity is driven by the arousal value of the stimuli. Stronger amygdala activation during reading of pleasant than of neutral and unpleasant words might be more in line with the view that the amygdala acts as a ‘dynamic relevance detector’, neither specifically responding to negative valence nor exclusively driven by arousal. Instead, its response patterns might be determined by situation-specific and individual factors, as suggested by considerations from appraisal theory (Sander et al., 2005).

Third, examining the functional relationship between the amygdalae and extrastriate visual areas would provide further empirical data on the validity of the concept of re-entrant processing during emotional perception. Although, this concept is often theoretically called upon, supportive experimental data are scarce.

Extending previous findings from experimentally instructed tasks to more natural processing conditions and from negatively to positively valenced symbolic stimuli, the present study first delineates the overall pattern of cerebral activation during reading of words varying in emotional content. Then, it clarifies whether silent reading of emotionally arousing pleasant and unpleasant words induces enhanced activation in the amygdala and the ventral visual system, relative to neutral words. Primary regions of interest (ROIs) of this second, more focused analysis, therefore, comprise the left and right amygdala and the extended bilateral extrastriate cortex. Additionally, the functional relationship between these ROIs is examined by correlation analysis to examine evidence for re-entrant processes. Finally, as in previous silent reading studies (Kissler et al., 2007; Herbert et al., 2008), a surprise free recall test assesses incidental recall of the presented words to verify adequate task involvement and investigate whether reading emotional vs neutral words has a measurable and lasting differential impact on memory. Research suggests that increased amygdala activation during stimulus perception is related to superior subsequent recall (Cahill et al., 1994; Canli et al., 2000).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Fifteen healthy right-handed native speakers of German (eight males, seven females; mean age 26 years) without history of drug abuse, chronic bodily or neurological and psychiatric diseases, or medication for any of these participated in the fMRI experiment. Handedness was determined with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), and all subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision. All participants gave written informed consent prior to participation, and the study was approved by University of Tübingen Institutional Review Board. Subjects were paid 15 euros for participation.

Stimulus material

The stimulus set consisted of 102 adjectives taken from a larger corpus of words, previously collected by this research group.1 This corpus provides arousal, valence and concreteness ratings from 45 adult native speakers of German for a set of about 800 German words. Valence and arousal ratings were obtained on the Self-Assessment Manikin scale (SAM, Bradley and Lang, 1994) in analogy to the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW), a standardized list of affective norms for English words (ANEW, Bradley and Lang, 1999) and the international affective picture system (IAPS) (Lang et al., 2005). Subsets of these words have been used in previous studies of emotional word processing (Ethofer et al., 2006; Herbert et al., 2006, 2008; Kissler et al., 2007, 2008).

Thirty-four pleasant, 34 unpleasant and 34 neutral adjectives were selected. Pleasant and unpleasant adjectives were matched in emotional arousal, and both were more arousing than neutral adjectives. Mean valence differed appropriately (pleasant > neutral > unpleasant). Pleasant and unpleasant adjectives described a broad range of affective traits and states (e.g. successful, happy, in love, chilling, brutal, tortured, anxious, nervous, sick, etc.). Neutral adjectives described less arousing and salient traits and states (e.g. neutral, normal, civilian, formal, etc.). Additionally, word categories were comparable on non-emotional attributes such as concreteness, word frequency, word length, orthographic neighbourhood density and bigram frequency. Word frequency was assessed using frequency counts for written language from the CELEX database (Baayen et al., 1995). Neighbourhood density and bigram frequency were analysed with WordGen software (Duyck et al., 2004). Word categories did not differ significantly in concreteness, word length, orthographic neighbourhood density or bigram frequency. Pleasant and unpleasant adjectives had somewhat lower word frequency counts than neutral adjectives, although pleasant and unpleasant adjectives did not differ significantly in word frequency. Descriptive statistics of the word stimuli are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

shows mean valence, arousal and concreteness values, as well as mean word length, word frequency, orthographic neighbourhood size and bigram frequency counts for pleasant, neutral and unpleasant adjectives

Adjectives
Pleasant Neutral Unpleasant
Valence 6.6 (0.14)a 5.2 (0.09)b 2.5 (0.06)c
Arousal 6.0 (0.11)a 3.0 (0.06)b 6.1 (0.10)a
Concreteness 4.7 (0.16)a 4.6 (0.29)a 4.0 (0.20)a
Word length 8.5 (0.42)a 7.4 (0.36)a 8.4 (0.34)a
Word frequency 24.0 (6.2)a 95.9 (25.6)b 16.0 (4.2)a
Orthographic neighbourhood size 0.61 (0.20)a 0.79 (0.25)a 0.47 (0.16)a
Bigram frequency 27 651.3 (2853.0)a 245 61.0 (2539.7)a 31 374.5 (3004.6)a

The range and direction of the valence, emotional arousal and concreteness values are as follows: valence = 9 (extremely pleasant) to 1 (extremely unpleasant), emotional arousal = 9 (extremely arousing) to 1 (not at all arousing), concreteness = 1 (extremely concrete) to 9 extremely abstract. Same superscript alphabets (a) on numbers within each row indicate that the means are not significantly different (P > 0.05) from each other using planned comparison tests. Standard errors are in parentheses.

Experimental design

The 102 adjectives were randomly assigned to one of two sets of 51 experimental stimuli presented in two separate imaging runs of silent word reading. Each run contained 17 highly arousing pleasant, 17 highly arousing unpleasant and 17 low arousing neutral adjectives. No word occurred twice. Adjectives were presented for 1000 ms. Each was followed by a baseline consisting of an array of eight unpronounceable letter strings (xxxxx). Intertrial intervals (word offset to word onset) ranged from 7.5 s to 12.75 s in order to facilitate an event-related fMRI analysis. Stimulus presentation order was randomized within runs, and run order was counterbalanced across subjects. Each run began with a baseline trial of random letter strings presented for 5 s. Experimental runs were controlled using ‘Presentation’ software (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc. http://www.neurobs.com).

Participants were instructed to read each word silently. No reference to emotional content was made. Fifty minutes after scanning, participants were given a surprise memory test. They were asked to recall as many of the presented adjectives as they could. Valence labels (‘pleasant’, ‘unpleasant’ and ‘neutral’) were given as category cues, and subjects were asked to write down as many of the previously presented words as they could remember. Across subjects, the order of emotional category cues given at recall test was randomized.

Physiological data collection and reduction

Image acquisition

Functional and anatomical images were recorded on a 1.5 T-whole body scanner (Siemens Vision, Erlangen, Germany). T1-weighted, high-resolution (1 × 1 × 1.5 mm3 voxel size) structural brain images were obtained for each subject using a magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (192 slices, no gap, TR = 9.7 s, TE = 4 ms, α = 8°, FOV = 256 × 256 mm2). Functional images were acquired by using a T2-weighted multislice echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (28 axial slices acquired in descending direction, 4 mm thickness, 1 mm gap, TR = 3 s, TE = 39 ms, α = 90, FOV = 192 × 192 mm2, 64 × 64 matrix, 3 × 3 × 5 mm3 voxel size).

Image analysis

Imaging data were analysed with Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM99, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). The first five EPI images of each run were discarded from further analysis to exclude images preceding T1 saturation. Pre-processing of functional images included slice time correction, 3D motion correction and normalization to MNI space (Montreal Neurological Institute, Collins et al., 1994; resampled voxel size 3 × 3 × 3 mm3). Data were smoothed spatially with an isotropic Gaussian Filter of 12 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) to remove high-frequency artefacts and smoothed temporally (4 s FWHM) to permit application of random field theory for statistical inference (Worsley et al., 1996).

Statistical analysis was based on the general linear model (Friston et al., 1995). Hemodynamic responses during adjective presentation were modelled using a stick function (time-locked to stimulus onset) convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function of the SPM99 software package. Stick functions were time-locked to the onset of the stimuli, and separate regressors were used to model each condition (pleasant, unpleasant and neutral adjectives). To account for signal changes due to head movements during scanning, six regressors representing estimated head movements were added as covariates of no interest into the statistical model (Friston et al., 1996).

Group data were analysed with random-effect analyses (Holmes and Friston, 1998). For each contrast of interest (see below), individual contrast images were averaged across the two runs and entered into a second-level one-sample t-test, each analysing activity associated with reading of pleasant, unpleasant or neutral adjectives in the entire brain. Activation is reported for clusters reaching a spatial threshold of at least 20 contiguous voxels each at a significance threshold of P < 0.005 (uncorrected). These criteria correspond to what has been used in similar previous functional imaging studies on the processing of emotional words (e.g. Hamann and Mao, 2002; Cato et al., 2004).

Four different contrasts were calculated: (i) emotional vs neutral, (ii) unpleasant vs neutral, (iii) pleasant vs neutral and (iv) pleasant vs unpleasant adjectives.

ROI analysis and correlation analysis

A second, more focused analysis specifically examined the effects of emotional content on activity in visual areas and the amygdala and their functional relationship: ROIs for this analysis comprised the left and right amygdala and the bilateral extrastriate cortex, where a large extrastriate ROI included the infero-temporal gyrus (BA20), the fusiform gyrus (BA37) and the extrastriate occipital cortex (BA18 and BA 19). This ROI was chosen to be relatively large, in view of the variability concerning the localization of word-specific visual activity reported in the fMRI literature (for review, see Jobard et al., 2006). Note that larger ROIs also lead to more conservative assessment when the small volume correction (SVC) procedure is applied. Anatomical masks for volume extraction were generated on the basis of a priori anatomical criteria as defined in the automatic anatomic labelling atlas integrated in SPM99 (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Activity within these regions was statistically evaluated using the SVC procedure (Worsley et al., 1996).

The functional relationship between amygdala and visual activation during reading of emotional adjectives was tested by entering mean signal change in the voxels of peak activity in these regions into correlation analysis (Pearson's r). Mean signal change was calculated on the basis of beta values of the voxels in the amygdalae and extrastriate visual cortices that across regressors and participants showed maximal activation. In order to obtain a reasonably representative and stable estimate, activity was extracted from spheres of 6 mm around the peak voxels. As a consistency check, the analysis was repeated entering only the peak activity voxels from the amygdala and extrastriate cortex.

Memory performance

Free recall memory performance for correctly remembered pleasant, unpleasant and neutral adjectives was statistically tested with a one-way repeated-measures analysis of variance.

RESULTS

Imaging data

Emotional > neutral adjectives

Reading emotional compared to reading neutral adjectives significantly increased activity in the left middle and inferior occipital (BA 18, BA 19) cortex, the left amygdala and adjacent left parahippocampal regions. Additional clusters of activation were found in prefrontal (supplementary motor cortex) and parietal cortex (precuneus), bilaterally, as well as the cerebellum (Table 2).

Table 2.

Comparisons of overall brain activity obtained during silent reading of emotional (pleasant and unpleasant) versus neutral adjectives. The upper part of this table indicates which brain regions exhibited a significant increase in activity during reading of emotional compared to neutral adjectives. The lower part diplays the reverse contrast (neutral > emotional)

Hemisphere Brain Region Brodman area (BA) T-value Coordinates x, y, z MNI Cluster
Emotional adjectives > neutral adjectives
Temporo-occipital lobe extrastriate cortex
Left Inferior/middle occipital gyrus BA 18/19 6.31 −14, −100, −4 295
Limbic system
Left Amygdala/anterior parahippocampus 4.60 −16, −4, −24 72
Parietal lobe
Left and Right Superior parietal, precuneus BA 5/7 3.46 −8, −44, 74 21
4.00 8, −50, 72 22
Frontal lobe
Left Superior frontal gyrus, medial BA 9 3.63 −10, 54, 36 23
Left and Right SMA BA 6/8 3.47 −16, 10, 58 24
4.20 12, −6, 56 66
Cerebellum 3.92 14, −56,−18 142
Left and Right 3.54 −14, −76,−20 55

Neutral adjectives > emotional adjectives
Temporal lobe
Left and Right Superior/middle temporal gyrus BA 21/22 4.85 −60, −22, −6 128
4.90 −42, −30 4 44
50, −26, 8 185
Parietal lobe
Left and Right Supramarginal/angular gyrus BA 22 4.00 −32, −64, 36 20
3.58 44, −50, 32 32
Frontal lobe
Left and Right Inferior frontal gyrus BA 45/47 3.13 −58, 24, 10 20
3.65 58, 28, 18 44

Neutral > emotional adjectives

There was also a small and regionally distinct activity enhancement during reading of neutral vs emotional adjectives in the superior and middle temporal gyri, parts of the parietal lobe and the inferior frontal gyrus. These activities are summarized in Table 2.

Pleasant > neutral

Visual and limbic activation of the left hemisphere, encompassing the inferior and middle occipital gyrus, the inferior temporal and fusiform gyrus, and amygdala and anterior parahippocampal gyrus, were most pronounced for the contrast comparing pleasant against neutral adjectives (Table 3 and Figure 1A). Processing of pleasant compared to neutral adjectives also accounted for signal increase in bilateral inferior parietal cortex (BA 40) including the left middle cingulate (BA 23, BA 31) cortex, as well as frontal lobe and cerebellar activation.

Table 3.

Comparisons of overall brain activity obtained during silent reading of pleasant, unpleasant and neutral adjectives. Upper part: Regions showing enhanced brain activity during reading of pleasant versus neutral words. Lower part: Regions showing enhanced brain activity during reading of unpleasant versus neutral words

Hemisphere Brain Region Brodman area (BA) T-value Coordinates x, y, z MNI Cluster
Pleasant adjectives > neutral adjectives
Temporo-occipital lobe extrastriate cortex
Left Inferior/middle occipital gyrus BA 18/19 6.47 −14, −100, −6 405
Left Inferior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus BA 19/37 4.14 −42, −68, −16 69
BA 20 3.77 −36, −8, −26 43
Limbic system
Left Amygdala/anterior parahippocampus 5.52 −14, 0, −24 105
Parietal lobe
Left Middle cingulate BA 23/31 3.90 −6, −24, 44 98
Left and Right Inferior parietal gyrus BA 40 3.57 −40, −38, 44 53
3.83 24, −46, 46 64
Frontal lobe
Left Superior/middle frontal gyrus BA 8/9 4.23 −18, 12, 60 111
SMA
Cerebellum
Left and Right 4.39 −26, −54, −26 184
4.08 12, −74, −36 240

Unpleasant adjectives > neutral adjectives
Left Occipital lobe extrastriate cortex
Middle occipital gyrus BA 18 3.50 −20, −96, −4 40
Frontal lobe
Right SMA BA 6 3.68 14, −6, 56 43
cerebellum
Left 4.20 22, −84, −34 90
Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

(A) Shows brain activity elicited by emotional as compared to neutral adjectives during silent reading. Right panels provide an overview on cortical activation and left panels on amygdala activation for contrasts between neutral adjectives and emotional, pleasant or unpleasant adjectives, respectively. For visualization, functional brain activation maps are superimposed on a rendered brain incorporated in the SPM99 software package and on T1-weighted images from MRIcro software (http://www.sph.sc.edu/comd/rorden/mricro.html). Effects are displayed at a threshold of P < 0.005 uncorrected, (T-score > 3) with a spatial extend threshold of 20 contiguous voxels. For contrasts comparing unpleasant against neutral adjectives, activation is displayed at a more lenient threshold of P = 0.05 uncorrected (T-score > 1.76) to illustrate the general pattern. (B) Amygdala and extrastriate activity obtained from a ROI analysis of the left amygdala and the left extrastriate cortex. Significant correlation (r = 0.66; P < 0.001) between peak signal change in the left amygdala and the left extrastriate cortex (peak at −48 −76 −2) elicited by pleasant adjectives relative to baseline conditions.

Unpleasant > neutral

For the contrast comparing unpleasant against neutral adjectives, no clearly significant supra-threshold voxels were found within the amygdalae, although activation of the left amygdala was detectable at a very lenient significance threshold of P = 0.05 uncorrected (see also Figure 1A). There was significantly enhanced activity in the left visual brain, the right supplementary motor area (SMA) and the left cerebellum during reading of unpleasant vs neutral adjectives (Table 3), but the extent of the activation was considerably smaller than for the pleasant vs neutral comparison.

Pleasant > unpleasant

Contrasting pleasant against unpleasant adjectives corroborated significantly enhanced responses for pleasant adjectives in the left amygdala and the left extrastriate cortex (Table 4). In addition, an increase of activation for pleasant adjectives occurred in anterior parahippocampal gyrus regions adjacent to the left amygdala and in inferior parietal and parietal somatosensory cortex regions as well as in right-hemisphere temporal brain regions including areas in the superior (BA 21) and middle (BA 37) temporal gyrus and the anterior temporal pole (BA 38).

Table 4.

Comparisons of overall brain activity obtained during silent reading of pleasant, unpleasant and neutral adjectives. This table displays the brain structures that were more active during reading of pleasant compared to unpleasant adjectives

Hemisphere Brain region Brodman area (BA) T-value Coordinates x, y, z MNI Cluster
Pleasant adjectives > unpleasant adjectives
Temporo-occipital lobe extrastriate cortex
Left Inferior occipital gyrus BA 19 4.37 −46, −74, −6 73
Right Middle temporal gyrus BA 37 3.67 54, −60, 16 50
Superior temporal gyrus anterior temporal pole BA 21/28/38 5.21 64, −2, −10 32
3.54 26, 12, −24 42
Limbic system
Left Amygdala/anterior parahippocampus 3.77 −16, 2, −24 38
Parietal lobe
Left and right Superior parietal gyrus, postcentral, supramarginal BA 4/40 3.90 −38, −10, 36 69
4.10 −54, −10, 48 137
4.71 50, −26, 40 135
Left Inferior parietal gyrus, precuneus BA 5 3.82 −10, −52, 60 59

Unpleasant > pleasant

There was increased activation in the cerebellum, but no other brain region showed larger activation for unpleasant adjectives than for pleasant adjectives.

Effects of emotional content on word processing are summarized in Figure 1A.

ROI analysis and correlation analysis

Although, hypotheses had covered bilateral amydalae and extrastriate regions, results are reported only for the left hemisphere, as no corresponding right-hemisphere above-threshold activation was found for right extrastriate regions and the right amygdala, respectively (see above and Tables 2 and 3). ROI analysis and correlation analysis of the left amygdala and left extrastriate cortex showed significant effects for pleasant adjectives. Activation peaks were located in the left amygdala (peak at MNI: −20, 2, −18) and in the inferior occipito-temporal gyrus (peak at MNI: −48, −76, −2) of the left extrastriate cortex, respectively. The magnitude of activations in the left limbic and extra-striate ROIs is detailed in Table 5 (emotional > neutral; pleasant > neutral and pleasant > unpleasant). After small volume correction, no significantly activated voxels within the ROIs were found for the unpleasant > neutral and unpleasant > pleasant contrasts.

Tables 5.

T-values, MNI coordinates and corresponding Brodman areas (BA) of highest activated voxels within two pre-defined brain ROIs, the extended etxrastriate cortex and amygdala, respectively. Upper panel: ROI sub-regions showing stronger activation during reading of emotional (pleasant and unpleasant) versus neutral adjectives. Middle panel: ROI sub-regions showing stronger activation during reading of pleasant versus neutral adjectives. Bottom panel: ROI sub-regions showing stronger activation during reading of pleasant versus unpleasant adjectives.

Hemisphere Brain regions of interest Brodman area (BA) T–value Coordinates x, y, z MNI Cluster
Emotional adjectives > neutral adjectives
Extrastriate ROI
Left Occipital lobe
Extrastriate cortex BA 18/19 6.15 −18 −98 −2 198**
Inferior temporal lobe
Left Inferior temporal gyrus BA 20 3.20 −38 −10 −28 3
Fusiform gyrus BA 20 3.13 −38 −12 −26 8
Amygdalar ROI
Left Amygdala 4.14 −20 −2 −24 25**

Pleasant adjectives > neutral adjectives
Extrastriate ROI
Occipital lobe BA18/19 5.25 −20 −98 −4 311**
Left Extrastriate cortex
Inferior temporal lobe
Left Inferior temporal gyrus BA 20 4.19 −36 −8 −26 18
Fusiform gyrus BA 19 3.77 −42 −68 −16 43*
Amygdalar ROI
Left Amygdala 4.77 −20 −2 −24 20**

Pleasant adjectives > unpleasant adjectives
Extrastriate ROI Occipital lobe
Left Extrastriate cortex BA 19 4.37 −46 −74 −6 73**
Amygdalar ROI
Left Amygdala 3.25 −18 2 −22 6*

aROI-effects characterized by a significant increase (after small volume correction ∼ SVC) during reading emotional adjectives compared to reading neutral adjectives. bComparison of reading pleasant with reading neutral adjectives. cComparision of reading pleasant with reading unpleasant adjectives. Asterixes (** and *) indicate that effects are significant at cluster level corresponding to **P < 0.005 and *P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparison within small volumes (SVC) in each of the two ROIs. Because the extra-striate ROI comprised several different anatomical structures, the locations of clusters of significant activity within this ROI are further detailed in the tables. SVC was applied across all voxels of the ROIs, separately for the amygdala and the extra-striate ROI. ROI analysis did not reveal any supra-threshold activity for the contrasts unpleasant > neutral, unpleasant > pleasant, neutral > pleasant or neutral > unpleasant.

Correlation analysis was restricted to the left amygdala and left extrastriate cortex since no corresponding right-hemispheric main effects were found. Left amygdala activation was significantly correlated with perceptual processing in the left extrastriate cortex for pleasant adjectives (Pearson's r = 0.65; P < 0.01). There were no significant correlations between left amygdala and left extrastriate cortex activity for unpleasant (Pearson's r = 0.31, P > 0.2) or neutral adjectives (Pearson's r = 0.33, P > 0.2). An additional analysis, correlating for each subject only the peak voxels in the left amygdala and the left extrastriate cortex, yielded very similar results (pleasant: r = 0.58, P < 0.03; unpleasant: r = 0.23, P > 0.2; neutral: r = 0.10, P > 0.2). Results from the ROI analysis and correlation analysis are presented in Figure 1B.

Memory performance

Incidental memory performance differed depending on the emotional content of the previously read words [F(2,28) = 13.7, P < 0.01]. Post hoc tests revealed that pleasant adjectives were better remembered than unpleasant and neutral adjectives [‘Valence’: pleasant > neutral: F(1,14) = 23.6, P < 0.001; pleasant > unpleasant: F(1,14) = 12.9, P < 0.005; Figure 2].

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Memory advantage for pleasant adjectives during the surprise free recall test. Bars (±s.e.) represent numbers of correctly remembered pleasant, unpleasant and neutral adjectives. Significant differences are marked with asterisks.

DISCUSSION

The present fMRI study delineated brain structures active during silent reading of arousing pleasant and unpleasant in contrast to neutral adjectives. Beyond identifying the general brain structures more active during reading of emotional adjectives than during neutral ones, the present study was particularly interested in three questions: first, does amygdala activation occur during spontaneous processing of highly symbolic emotional stimuli such as words? Second, if so, is it restricted to one emotional valence, or does it occur in response to arousing verbal stimuli regardless of their valence? Third, can we find evidence for a functional relationship between activity in the amygdala and extrastriate visual areas? The behavioural consequences of selective processing of emotional and neutral words during reading were assessed in a free recall test after scanning.

Overall, modulation of brain activity by the words’ emotional content was identified in the visual cortex and comprised extrastriate cortex regions in the left middle and inferior occipital and temporal gyrus including the left posterior fusiform gyrus. These regions form part of the ventral visual processing stream responsible for the recognition of objects (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982; Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994), letters and words (Petersen et al., 1990; Posner and Abdullaev, 1999; Cohen et al., 2002; Gaillard et al., 2006).

Moreover, an enhancement of limbic-system activity, specifically in the left amygdala and left peri-amygdaloid regions, was observed during reading of emotional, particularly pleasant, adjectives. Peak activation in the peri-amygdaloid cortex could be assigned to the anterior parahippocampal gyrus (anterior PHG), a region which has recently been related to enhanced memory encoding of emotional salient visual stimuli (Dolcos et al., 2004). It is generally agreed that amygdala activation boosts memory encoding for emotionally arousing stimuli by adrenergic activation of the parahippocampus and hippocampus (McGaugh, 2004; for reviews see Hamann, 2001 or LaBar, 2007). Therefore, enhanced amygdala activity during stimulus perception should result in better subsequent recall. More efficient memory encoding of pleasant adjectives is supported by present behavioural data: pleasant adjectives were spontaneously better remembered than unpleasant and neutral adjectives. In the literature, the pattern of memory modulation by emotion varies: studies report superior recall of unpleasant (Ochsner, 2000) or emotionally arousing pleasant and unpleasant stimuli (e.g. Bradley et al., 1992; Hamann et al., 1999; Dolcos et al., 2005). Gruhn et al. (2005) report different recall patterns for pleasant and unpleasant emotional words, depending on whether unpleasant and pleasant stimuli are presented together in the same list or in separate lists at study. Yet other studies report better recall for pleasant than unpleasant or neutral material (Kissler and Hauswald, 2008; Koenig and Mecklinger, 2008), especially in designs that, like in the present study, use incidental encoding conditions (Kiefer et al., 2007; Herbert et al., 2008). This thesis is corroborated by a series of studies specifically addressing the role of depth of processing at encoding for recall of emotional words (Ferré, 2003). Ferré's studies suggest that, at least in memory for single words, incidental encoding conditions drive a memory advantage for pleasant items (see Ferré, 2003 for a review). As discussed in more detail below, spontaneous processing of emotional words in a silent reading task, especially when it entails amygdala activation, is likely to contribute to differential subsequent memory for pleasant, unpleasant and neutral stimuli.

Silent reading of emotional adjectives also increased activity in a number of other brain regions such as the bilateral parietal cortex, premotor and SMAs as well as in right anterior, superior (BA 38/28) and middle temporal (BA 21) brain regions. Frontal activation might signal action preparation in response to behaviourally challenging words. This has been reported both with regard to action words (Grafton et al., 1997; Hauk et al., 2004) and words with emotional connotation (Isenberg et al., 1999). According to prior studies of emotional processing, bilateral parietal and right temporal lobe activity may reflect a more detailed attentive and integrative conceptual processing of emotional visual stimuli (e.g. Lane et al., 1997, 1999; Canli et al., 2004). Again, particularly the parietal and temporal lobe structures were more strongly activated during reading of pleasant adjectives compared to neutral or unpleasant adjectives. However, in the absence of a clear a priori prediction, activation of these brain regions should be interpreted with caution.

In the visual cortex and the amygdala, which were the focus of the present study, the BOLD signal was increased during reading of adjectives with emotional, particularly pleasant content. These findings demonstrate, apparently for the first time, enhanced activation of the human amygdala during reading of emotional, particularly pleasant, words. Both visual cortex and amygdala activity were predominantly left-lateralized. This is in agreement with a stronger contribution of the language-dominant left hemisphere (Crosson et al., 1999, 2002) and the left amygdala in emotional word processing (Markowitsch, 1998; Phelps et al., 2001) and in line with neuroanatomical findings on ipsilateral connections between the amygdalae and visual cortex (Amaral et al., 2003).

Indeed, we also found evidence for a functional interplay between the left amygdala and left extrastriate regions during reading of pleasant adjectives. ‘Re-entrant processing’, according to which the amygdala amplifies perception by means of reciprocal feedback connections to the visual cortex has been suggested as a plausible explanation for findings of such bidirectional relationships between amygdala and extrastriate cortex activation during processing of emotional pictures (Bradley et al., 2003; Sabatinelli et al., 2005), fearful faces (Morris, et al., 1998, 1999; Pessoa et al., 2002) and unpleasant words (Tabert et al., 2001). The present correlation between amygdala and extrastriate activity is well in line with this thesis. These results extend findings of a functional relationship between the magnitude of amygdala and extrastriate cortex activation in the processing of emotionally salient unpleasant words (Tabert et al., 2001) to the processing of pleasant words.

Taken together, present findings indicate that, during silent reading of adjectives varying in emotional content, pleasant words in particular take advantage of a primarily left amygdala-mediated enhanced perceptual processing. They may additionally draw on right temporal lobe structures and recruit bilateral parietal attention networks. Behaviourally, pleasant words enjoy a memory advantage when recalled after scanning.

Studies have shown that both task demands and emotional arousal critically determine the amount of processing of emotional stimuli in the visual cortex (Lane et al., 1999; Bradley et al., 2003) and the degree to which the amygdala responds to pleasant and/or unpleasant stimuli (Phan et al., 2002, 2003, 2004; Sabatinelli et al., 2005). Differences in emotional arousal between pleasant and unpleasant stimuli, but also the task at hand, can affect neural responses to pleasant and unpleasant stimuli (Zald, 2003; Kuchinke et al., 2005).

In the present study, pleasant and unpleasant adjectives were matched for emotional arousal, they did not differ with respect to many linguistic visual properties, and interference from cognitive processes imposed by additional attention or categorization tasks can be excluded during silent reading. The difference in word frequency between arousing and neutral words is not likely to have accounted for the present results: if indeed less frequent words had led to larger brain activations, this should have been particularly true for unpleasant words, which had somewhat lower frequency counts than pleasant words, i.e. more brain activity would have been expected during processing of unpleasant words. But this is not observed. Moreover, activity evoked by pleasant and unpleasant words should not have differed and neither superior recall for pleasant words nor a specific correlation between amygdala and extrastriate activity is a likely consequence of differences in word frequency. Neutral words had somewhat higher and unpleasant words somewhat lower frequency counts than pleasant words. Also, a recent study explicitly addressing the effects of word emotionality and word frequency in a lexical decision task (Nakic et al., 2006) found no interaction between the main effects of word frequency and emotion.

These findings argue against both the view that the amygdala selectively responds to unpleasant material (e.g. Öhman and Mineka, 2001) and the view that stimulus arousal will determine the magnitude of the amygdala response (Davis and Whalen, 2001; Sabatinelli et al., 2005; Lewis et al., 2007). To what may this processing advantage for pleasant adjectives be attributable? Visibility and attention can bias amygdala responses to either pleasant or unpleasant stimuli: Williams and colleagues (2005) report larger amygdala responses to happy faces when faces were fully attended, whereas responses to fearful faces were enhanced when subjects’ attention was diverted away from the faces to competing stimuli. In the present, study there was no competition for spatial attention between stimuli, which may have biased neural responses in favour of pleasant contents. Also, an exposure time of 1 s per stimulus does not impose severe temporal processing constraints under which processing may be biased towards unpleasant material.

Canli et al. (2002) found amygdala responses to happy faces to increase with higher extraversion scores, suggesting that personality factors may play a role in modulating the relative magnitude of amygdala activation to aversive or pleasant stimuli. Although, we did not measure extraversion scores, in healthy people stronger cerebral responses to pleasant relative to unpleasant and neutral stimuli may arise from a general mood-congruent processing bias. Mood-congruent processing biases can account for differential responsiveness to pleasant and unpleasant stimuli and affect perception, attention, memory and overt behaviour (Deldin et al., 2001; Ferré, 2003; Fredrickson and Branigan, 2005, for review; Kuchinke et al., 2005; Kiefer et al., 2007). While increased amygdala activation in response to pleasant stimuli may be larger when positive mood is induced experimentally (Schneider et al., 1997) or in more extraverted subjects (Canli et al., 2002), similar mood-congruent ‘preferences’ for pleasant material may operate in the absence of any experimenter-induced task, mildly positive mood being the modal experience in healthy people (Diener and Diener, 1996).

Healthy people tend to view positive information as more self-relevant and self-descriptive than negative information (Deldin et al., 2001; Tagami, 2002; Lewis et al., 2007). This may implicitly bias visual processing towards pleasant adjectives, as these words describe positive emotional states or traits that may match more closely participants’ ongoing mood, expectations and intentions than adjectives describing negative traits or states. To follow up on this possibility we obtained independent ratings from 22 student subjects (11 males, 11 females) with similar biographic background and age as the participants in the present fMRI study. Subjects rated on 9-point scales, analogous to the SAM, the degree to which the words used in the present study were descriptive of personality traits or states in general and to what extent subjects thought the meaning of these words was relevant for themselves: pleasant and unpleasant adjectives were both rated as more descriptive of personal traits and states than neutral adjectives were. But only the pleasant adjectives were judged as more self-relevant than the unpleasant adjectives [pleasant–unpleasant, F(1,98) = 73.29, P < 0.001] or the neutral adjectives [pleasant–neutral, F(1,98) = 14.1, P < 0.001].

Recent results also indicate that normal subjects hold overly optimistic views about themselves and their future, the amygdala being involved in mediating this illusory optimism bias (Sharot et al., 2007). Such mood-dependent or self-concept congruent processing biases in favour of pleasant stimuli may explain why in the present study pleasant adjectives elicited more activity in the amygdala and the visual system than unpleasant or neutral adjectives. In support of the reality of the phenomenon, EEG studies investigating silent reading of and incidental recall for emotional adjectives have found a pattern similar to that of the present fMRI study (Herbert et al., 2006, 2008; Kissler et al., 2008). In these studies, a larger set of adjectives was used, and they were conducted with different subjects and in a different laboratory. Still, similar to the present study, higher incidental recall for pleasant adjectives as well as a larger late positive event-related potential in response to pleasant adjectives were observed. The late positive potential in the ERP and BOLD responses in the fMRI have recently been found to be correlated in a study investigating fMRI and electrophysiological correlates of affective picture processing in the same group of subjects (Sabatinelli et al., 2007).

Among current emotion theories, appraisal theories (e.g. Scherer, 2001; Sander et al., 2005) can account for such dynamic regulation of central nervous responses to emotional stimuli by postulating that emotional processing depends on a cascade of stimulus-evaluation checks: situational and individual relevance checks are proposed to determine whether stimuli will elicit emotional responses and what kind of response will result. This approach is well able to account for the empirical variability in responses to emotional stimuli which is becoming increasingly evident in the literature. In line with this view, the present results suggests that cerebral responses to pleasant adjectives during reading are enhanced because these stimuli were viewed as more self-relevant than either the unpleasant or the neutral adjectives.

Altogether, the present results indicate a modulatory role of the human amygdala in processing of symbolic emotional concepts during silent reading. This demonstrates that the role of the amygdala goes well beyond that of boosting visual processing for highly aversive unpleasant words (Isenberg et al., 1999; Anderson and Phelps, 2001; Tabert et al., 2001). The fact that pleasant adjectives provoke larger neural responses in the amygdala relative to unpleasant and neutral words is in line with recent theoretical views of the human amygdala as a structure for relevance detection (Sander et al., 2003). According to this view, the human amygdala alerts the organism towards a much broader class of stimuli than suggested by traditional models, including a wide range of symbolic, ontogenetically acquired representations of emotional significance. Moreover, the direction of the response may be biased by current needs, personal goals and individual preferences that converge with participants’ ongoing mood, expectations and intentions.

Acknowledgments

We thank Gregory A. Miller for helpful comments on a previous version of this article. Research was supported by the Heidelberg Academy of Sciences (Mind and Brain Programme), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the Center for Young Scientists at the University of Konstanz, Germany.

Footnotes

1

The complete list of the words used in this study (original and translation) together with valence and arousal ratings is available from the authors upon request.

REFERENCES

  1. Adolphs R. Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews. 2002;1:21–61. doi: 10.1177/1534582302001001003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Hamann S, et al. Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(10):1111–7. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00039-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amaral DG, Behniea H, Kelly JL. Topographic organization of projections from the amygdala to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 2003;118:1099–120. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)01001-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amaral DG, Price JL. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis) The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1984;230(4):465–96. doi: 10.1002/cne.902300402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature. 2001;411(6835):305–9. doi: 10.1038/35077083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baayen RH, Piepenbrock R, Gulikers L. Linguistic Data Consortium. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania; 1995. The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM) [Google Scholar]
  7. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;11(6):1891–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beauregard M, Chertkow H, Bub D, Murtha S, Dixon R, Evans A. The neural substrate for concrete, abstract, and emotional word lexica: a positron emission tomography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9(4):441–61. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonda E, Petrides M, Ostry D, Evans A. Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16(11):3737–44. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-11-03737.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bradley MM, Greenwald MK, Petry MC, Lang PJ. Remembering pictures:pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1992;18(2):379–90. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.18.2.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 1994;25(1):49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Technical report C-1. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida; 1999. Affective norms for English words (ANEW): stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of Psychophysiology. 3rd edn. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. pp. 581–607. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bradley MM, Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, Fitzsimmons JR, King W, Desai P. Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;117(2):369–80. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.2.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30(2):173–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cacioppo JT, Crites S.L., Jr, Berntson GG, Coles MG. If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed should they not affect the event-related brain potential. Psychological Science. 1993;4(2):108–12. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cahill L, Prins B, Weber M, McGaugh JL. Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature. 1994;371(6499):702–4. doi: 10.1038/371702a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Canli T, Sivers H, Thomason ME, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JD, Gotlib IH. Brain activation to emotional words in depressed vs healthy subjects. Neuroreport. 2004;15:2585–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200412030-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Canli T, Sivers H, Whitfield SL, Gotlib IH, Gabrieli JD. Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science. 2002;296(5576):2191. doi: 10.1126/science.1068749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Canli T, Zhao Z, Brewer J, Gabrieli JD, Cahill L. Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(19):RC99. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-j0004.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cato MA, Crosson B, Gokcay D, et al. Processing words with emotional connotation: an FMRI study of time course and laterality in rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16(2):167–77. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cohen L, Lehericy S, Chochon F, Lemer C, Rivaud S, Dehaene S. Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the visual word form area. Brain. 2002;125(5):1054–69. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peter TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D registration of MR volumetric data in standardized talairach space. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography. 1994;18(2):192–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, et al. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;9(2):93–105. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2&#x0003c;93::AID-HBM4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Crosson B, Cato MA, Sadek JR, et al. Semantic monitoring of words with emotional connotation during fMRI: contribution of anterior left frontal cortex. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(5):607–22. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702801394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Crosson B, Radonovich K, Sadek JR, et al. Left-hemisphere processing of emotional connotation during word generation. Neuroreport. 1999;10(12):2449–55. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199908200-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Neural components of social evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85(4):639–49. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cunningham WA, Raye CL, Johnson MK. Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16(10):1717–29. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. [Review] Molecular Psychiatry. 2001;6(1):13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Deldin PA, Keller J, Gergen JA, Miller G. Cognitive bias and emotion in neuropsychological models of depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2001;15:787–802. [Google Scholar]
  31. Diener E, Diener C. Most people are happy. Psychological Science. 1996;7:181–5. [Google Scholar]
  32. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron. 2004;42(5):855–63. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00289-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Remembering one year later: role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102. 2005;7:2626–31. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409848102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Duyck W, Desmet T, Verbeke LP, Brysbaert M. WordGen: a tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004;36(3):488–99. doi: 10.3758/bf03195595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ethofer T, Anders S, Erb M, et al. Cerebral pathways in processing of affective prosody: a dynamic causal modeling study. NeuroImage. 2006;30(2):580–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fecteau S, Belin P, Joanette Y, Armony JL. Amygdala responses to nonlinguistic emotional vocalizations. NeuroImage. 2007;36(2):480–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ferré P. Effects of level of processing on memory for affectively valenced words. Cognition and Emotion. 2003;17(6):859–80. [Google Scholar]
  38. Fredrickson BL, Branigan C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion. 2005;19(3):313–32. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak R.SJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping. 1995;2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
  40. Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RS, Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1996;35:346–55. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gaillard R, Naccache L, Pinel P, et al. Direct intracranial, FMRI, and lesion evidence for the causal role of left inferotemporal cortex in reading. Neuron. 2006;50(2):191–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Grafton ST, Fadiga L, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G. Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. NeuroImage. 1997;6(4):231–6. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gruhn D, Smith J, Baltes PB. No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(4):579–88. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hamann S. Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(9):349–400. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01707-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hamann SB, Ely TD, Grafton ST, Kilts CD. Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2(3):289–93. doi: 10.1038/6404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hamann S, Mao H. Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. Neuroreport. 2002;13(1):15–9. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hariri AR, Bookheimer SY, Mazziotta JC. Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport. 2000;11(1):43–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200001170-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hauk O, Davis MH, Ford M, Pulvermuller F, Marslen-Wilson WD. The time course of visual word-recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage. 2006;30(4):1383–1400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hauk O, Davis MH, Kherif F, Pulvermueller F. Imagery or meaning? Evidence for a semantic origin of category-specific brain activity in metabolic imaging. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;27(7):1856–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06143.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermuller F. Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron. 2004;41(2):301–7. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00838-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Herbert C, Junghofer M, Kissler J. Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology. 2008;45(3):487–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Herbert C, Kissler J, Junghofer M, Peyk P, Rockstroh B. Processing of emotional adjectives: evidence from startle EMG and ERPs. Psychophysiology. 2006;43(2):197–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hinton EC, Parkinson JA, Holland AJ, Arana FS, Roberts AC, Owen AM. Neural contributions to the motivational control of appetite in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;20(5):1411–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03589.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Holmes AP, Friston KJ. Generalisability, random effects and population inference. NeuroImage. 1998;7:754. [Google Scholar]
  55. Isenberg N, Silbersweig D, Engelien A, et al. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96. Vol. 18. 1999. Linguistic threat activates the human amygdala; pp. 10456–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jobard G, Crivello F, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading: a metaanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2006;20(2):693–712. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00343-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Junghofer M, Sabatinelli D, Bradley MM, Schupp HT, Elbert TR, Lang PJ. Fleeting images: rapid affect discrimination in the visual cortex. Neuroreport. 2006;17(2):225–9. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000198437.59883.bb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kawashima R, Sugiura M, Kato T, et al. The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring. A PET study. Brain. 1999;122(Pt 4):779–83. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.4.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. Processing emotional pictures and words: effects of valence and arousal. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2006;6(2):110–26. doi: 10.3758/cabn.6.2.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kiefer M, Schuch S, Schenck W, Fiedler K. Mood states modulate activity in semantic brain areas during emotional word encoding. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(7):1516–1530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kissler J, Hauswald A. Neuromagnetic activity during recognition of emotional pictures. Brain Topography. 2008;20(4):192–204. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0044-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kissler J, Herbert C, Peyk P, Junghofer M. Buzzwords: early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science. 2007;18(6):475–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01924.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kissler J, Herbert C, Winkler I, Junghofer M. Biological Psychology. 2008. Emotion and attention in visual word processing – an ERP study. [Epub ahead of print; 14 March 2008] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Koenig S, Mecklinger A. Electrophysiological correlates of encoding and retrieving emotional events. Emotion. 2008;8(2):162–73. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kuchinke L, Jacobs AM, Grubich C, Vo ML, Conrad M, Herrmann M. Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2005;28(4):1022–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. LaBar KS. Beyond fear emotional memory mechanisms in the human brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16(4):173–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00498.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. LaBar KS, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Kim YH, Nobre AC, Mesulam MM. Hunger selectively modulates corticolimbic activation to food stimuli in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115(2):493–500. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.2.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. LaBerge D, Samuels SJ. Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology. 1974;6:293–323. [Google Scholar]
  69. Lane RD, Chua PM, Dolan RJ. Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(9):989–97. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00017-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Bradley MM, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(11):1437–44. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00070-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Lang PJ, Simons RF, Balaban M. Attention and Emotion: Sensory and Motivational Processes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1997. Motivated attention: affect, activation, and action; pp. 97–135. [Google Scholar]
  72. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida; 2005. International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. [Google Scholar]
  73. Lewis PA, Critchley HD, Rotshtein P, Dolan RJ. Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(3):742–8. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Fig LM, Decker LR, Koeppe RA, Minoshima S. Limbic activation and psychophysiologic responses to aversive visual stimuli. Interaction with cognitive task. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23(5):508–16. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI, Crockett MJ, Tom SM, Pfeifer JH, Way BM. Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science. 2007;18(5):421–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Logan G. Towards an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review. 1988;95:492–527. [Google Scholar]
  77. Markowitsch HJ. Differential contribution of right and left amygdala to affective information processing. Behavioural Neurology. 1998;11:233–44. doi: 10.1155/1999/180434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2004;27:1–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Morris JS, Dolan RJ. Involvement of human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in hunger-enhanced memory for food stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(14):5304–10. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-05304.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Morris JS, Friston KJ, Buchel C, et al. A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain. 1998;121(1):47–57. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan R. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating ‘unseen’ fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1999;96:1680–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Naccache L, Gaillard R, Adam C, et al. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 21. Vol. 102. 2005. A direct intracranial record of emotions evoked by subliminal words; pp. 7713–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Nakic M, Smith BW, Busis S, Vythilingam M, Blair RJ. The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: the role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2006;31(4):1752–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Nobre AC, Allison T, McCarthy G. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature. 1994;372(6503):260–3. doi: 10.1038/372260a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Ochsner KN. Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology General. 2000;129:242–261. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.2.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14(8):1215–29. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(5):242–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, et al. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage. 2004;23(2):483–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. O’Doherty JP, Deichmann R, Critchley HD, Dolan RJ. Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron. 2002;33(5):815–26. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00603-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Öhman A. Automaticity and the amygdala: nonconscious responses to emotional faces. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11(2):62–6. [Google Scholar]
  91. Öhman A, Flykt A, Esteves F. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2001;130:466–78. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Öhman A, Mineka S. Fears, phobias, and prepardness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review. 2001;108(3):483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 17. Vol. 99. 2002. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. pp. 11458–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME. Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science. 1990;249(4972):1041–4. doi: 10.1126/science.2396097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Phan KL, Taylor SF, Welsh RC, et al. Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53(3):211–5. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01485-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Phan KL, Taylor SF, Welsh RC, Ho SH, Britton JC, Liberzon I. Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;21(2):768–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–48. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Phelps EA, O’Connor KJ, Cunningham WA, et al. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12(5):729–38. doi: 10.1162/089892900562552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Phelps EA, O’Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M. Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4(4):437–41. doi: 10.1038/86110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Posner MI, Abdullaev YG. Neuroanatomy, circuitry and plasticity of word reading. Neuroreport. 1999;10(3):12–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Sabatinelli D, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Lang PJ. Parallel amygdala and inferotemporal activation reflect emotional intensity and fear relevance. NeuroImage. 2005;24(4):1265–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, Keil A, Bradley MM. Emotional perception: correlation of functional MRI and event-related potentials. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(5):1085–91. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Sander D, Grafman J, Zalla T. The human amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2003;14(4):303–36. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2003.14.4.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Sander D, Grandjean D, Scherer KR. A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks. 2005;18(4):317–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Scherer KR. Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In: Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 92–120. [Google Scholar]
  107. Schneider F, Grodd W, Weiss U, et al. Functional MRI reveals left amygdala activation during emotion. Psychiatry Research. 1997;76:75–82. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(97)00063-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sharot T, Riccardi AM, Raio CM, Phelps EA. Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature. 2007;450(7166):102–5. doi: 10.1038/nature06280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Strange BA, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. Brain mechanisms for detecting perceptual, semantic, and emotional deviance. NeuroImage. 2000;12(4):425–33. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Tabert MH, Borod JC, Tang CY, et al. Differential amygdala activation during emotional decision and recognition memory tasks using unpleasant words: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(6):556–73. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Tabibnia G, Lieberman MD, Craske MG. The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images. Emotion. 2008;8(3):307–17. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Tagami K. Negative bias on self-referent processing in depression: focused on mood congruent effects. Shinrigaku Kenkyu. 2002;73(5):412–8. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.73.412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1994;4(2):157–65. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90066-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW, editors. Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1982. pp. 549–86. [Google Scholar]
  116. Vanderploeg R, Brown WS, Marsh JT. Judgements of emotions in words and faces: ERP correlates. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1987;5:193–205. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(87)90006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Vigneau M, Jobard G, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Word and non-word reading: what role for the visual word form area? NeuroImage. 2005;27:694–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Vuilleumier P. Facial expression and selective attention. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2002;15(3):291–300. [Google Scholar]
  119. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2001;30(3):829–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00328-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(11):1271–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Williams MA, McGlone F, Abbott DF, Mattingley JB. Differential amygdala responses to happy and fearful facial expressions depend on selective attention. NeuroImage. 2005;24(2):417–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Winston JS, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Common and distinct neural responses during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions. NeuroImage. 2003;20(1):84–97. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00303-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping. 1996;4:458–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zald DH. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research. 2003;41:88–123. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00248-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES