Abstract
Stimulant drugs, including d-amphetamine, cocaine and methylphenidate, increase cigarette smoking in controlled human laboratory experiments. Although the mechanism(s) underlying this effect are unknown, it is possible that stimulants may enhance directly the abuse-related effects of nicotine. In the present study, we characterized the behavioral pharmacological interactions between methylphenidate and nicotine in the intravenous self-administration, drug discrimination and locomotor cross-sensitization procedures. Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats were trained to respond for intravenous nicotine (0.01 or 0.03 mg/kg/infusion) or sucrose, and the acute effects of methylphenidate (1.25-10 mg/kg) were determined; in addition, separate groups of rats were treated with methylphenidate (2.5 mg/kg) or saline prior to 12 consecutive nicotine (0.03 mg/kg/infusion) self-administration sessions. Next, the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine (0.03-0.3 mg/kg) and methylphenidate (1.25-10 mg/kg), alone and in combination with a low nicotine dose (0.056 mg/kg), were tested in nicotine-trained rats. Finally, the locomotor effect of repeated methylphenidate (2.5 mg/kg) was tested in rats previously treated with nicotine (0.2-0.8 mg/kg). Results indicated that acute methylphenidate increased the rate of nicotine self-administration at doses that reduced sucrose-maintained responding; furthermore, tolerance to this effect was not apparent following repeated methylphenidate. Methylphenidate, while not substituting for nicotine alone, dose-dependently enhanced the discriminative stimulus effect of a low nicotine dose. In addition, repeated nicotine exposure promoted the development of locomotor sensitization to methylphenidate. Taken together with recent clinical findings, these results suggest that methylphenidate may enhance the abuse-related behavioral effects of nicotine, perhaps increasing vulnerability to tobacco dependence.
Keywords: Drug Discrimination, Locomotor Activity, Methylphenidate, Nicotine, Self-administration, Sensitization
INTRODUCTION
Converging lines of evidence indicate that the use of stimulant drugs, including d-amphetamine and cocaine, is associated with tobacco cigarette smoking. Cigarette smoking is positively correlated with recent cocaine use among college students (Schorling et al, 1994), and drug abusers testing positive for recent cocaine use have elevated urinary levels of the nicotine metabolite cotinine (Roll et al, 1997). Results from controlled human laboratory experiments also indicate that acute administration of stimulant drugs can directly increase rates of spontaneous cigarette smoking. Relative to placebo, acute administration of oral d-amphetamine or bupropion, or intranasal administration of cocaine, has been shown to dose-dependently increase cigarette smoking (Cousins et al, 2001; Henningfield and Griffiths, 1981; Roll et al, 1997). In addition, d-amphetamine has been shown to increase choice of cigarettes over money (Tidey et al, 2000) and can increase progressive-ratio (PR) breakpoints maintained by cigarettes (Sigmon et al, 2003). Taken together, these results suggest that stimulant drugs can promote cigarette smoking in the natural environment, as well as in controlled laboratory settings, perhaps by augmenting the reinforcing efficacy of nicotine. However, since cigarette smoking involves pharmacologic and non-pharmacologic factors in addition to nicotine (Bardo et al, 1999; Fowler et al, 2003; Rose and Levin, 1991), other interpretations cannot be ruled out.
Although the exact mechanism(s) underlying stimulant-induced increases in cigarette smoking have yet to be elucidated, evidence from preclinical animal studies suggests that the interactive effect of nicotine and stimulants on mesolimbic dopamine function may be involved. In a microdialysis study using rats, co-administration of cocaine with nicotine was shown to produce an additive increase in extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens relative to administration of nicotine alone (Gerasimov et al, 2000). Similarly, behavioral studies have shown that administration of indirect dopamine agonists, such as monoamine oxidase inhibitors (which prevent intracellular metabolism of dopamine) or bupropion (a dopamine reuptake inhibitor), increase nicotine self-administration in rats (Guillem et al, 2005, 2006; Rauhut et al, 2003). Thus, it is possible that stimulant drugs increase nicotine self-administration, at least in part, by augmenting nicotine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens.
Similar to the effects observed with cocaine and amphetamine, a recent report by Rush et al (2005) found that methylphenidate also increased cigarette smoking in humans tested in a controlled laboratory setting. In that study, methylphenidate (5-40 mg) dose-dependently increased both the total number cigarettes smoked, as well as positive subjective ratings of cigarette smoking relative to placebo. Specificity of the methylphenidate-induced facilitation of cigarette smoking was also evident, as methylphenidate dose-dependently decreased food consumption (Rush et al, 2005). Based on these findings, the current study sought to determine if there is a direct pharmacologic interaction between methylphenidate and nicotine using rat models of drug abuse. First, we assessed the dose effect of acute methylphenidate pretreatment on nicotine self-administration and sucrose-maintained responding. Second, we determined the effect of repeated methylphenidate pretreatment on nicotine self-administration. Third, we examined the discriminative stimulus effects of methylphenidate, both alone and in combination with a low nicotine dose, in rats trained to discriminate nicotine from saline. Finally, we examined the effect of repeated nicotine on subsequent methylphenidate-induced hyperactivity.
METHODS AND MATERIALS
Subjects
Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan Industries, Inc., Indianapolis, IN, USA), initially weighing 250-275 g, were used in all experiments. Rats used for operant conditioning experiments were housed individually in standard plastic cages, and rats used for the locomotor activity experiment were housed 2 per cage. For the self-administration and food-maintained responding experiments, rats were allocated 18-20 g of food per day (the daily food allotment for a given rat was held constant for the duration of each experiment). For the drug discrimination experiment, rats were provided sufficient food to maintain body weight at ~350 g. For the locomotor activity experiment, rats were provided ad lib access to food. All rats had constant access to water in the home cage. Rats were housed in a temperature- and humidity-controlled colony set to a 14:10 light/dark cycle (lights on at 0600 hr) and all experimental procedures were conducted during the light phase. Experimental protocols were in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (1996) and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Kentucky.
Apparatus
The drug self-administration, sucrose-maintained responding and drug discrimination experiments were conducted in operant conditioning chambers (ENV-008, MED Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). Each chamber was housed in a sound-attenuating enclosure (ENV-018M, Med Associates), and was connected to a personal computer via an interface (SG-502, MED Associates). A 5 × 4.2 cm opening that allowed access to a recessed food tray was located on the front panel of the operant conditioning chamber. Two retractable metal response levers were mounted next to the food tray (one on each side) 7.3 cm above a metal-grid floor. A 28 V, 3-cm diameter, white cue light was centered 6 cm above each response lever. For the drug self-administration experiments, an infusion pump (PHM-100, Med-Associates) delivered drug infusions via a silastic tube attached to a swivel mounted on the outside of the back wall.
Locomotor activity was monitored with an automated Digiscan animal activity monitoring system (AccuScan Instruments, Columbus, OH, USA). The system consisted of 12 clear Plexiglass chambers (42 × 42 × 30 cm) made opaque by attaching sheets of white plastic to each outer surface. Each chamber incorporated a horizontal 16 × 16 grid of photo beam sensors spaced 2.5 cm apart and 7.0 cm above the chamber floor. The activity monitors were interfaced to a personal computer operating Digipro System software (v. 1.40, AccuScan Instruments). Horizontal activity was measured as photo beam interruptions and expressed as distance traveled (cm).
Nicotine Self-Administration
For nicotine self-administration, rats were first trained briefly to lever-press for sucrose reinforcement (45 mg BioServ pellets) by pressing an active lever (lever designations were counter-balanced across rats) in the two-lever operant conditioning chamber under a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of reinforcement in 15-min daily sessions. Over the next 6 sessions, the response requirement was incremented to an FR5. Training was considered complete following 2 sessions of responding under the FR5 schedule.
Following completion of the initial pre-training period, rats underwent intravenous catheterization surgery. Briefly, rats were anesthetized with injections of ketamine (60 mg/kg, ip.) and xylazine (8 mg/kg, ip.). The right jugular vein was then isolated, and one end of a silastic catheter was inserted. The other end of the catheter exited the skin and was secured to the skull by an acrylic head mount. During experimental sessions, a silastic leash protected by flexible metal tubing was used to attach the catheter to the drug infusion pump.
Nicotine self-administration commenced following a 5-day surgical recovery period, and was based on the general method of Corrigall and Coen (1989), with some modifications. Rats were reintroduced to the operant conditioning chamber during daily 60-min sessions. Responses on the active lever (FR1) were recorded and resulted in an infusion of nicotine (0.01 or 0.03 mg/kg/infusion, delivered in a volume of 100 μl over 5.9 sec); responses on the inactive lever were recorded but had no scheduled consequence. Completion of the FR requirement resulted in simultaneous activation of the infusion pump and the cue lights, which signaled a 20-sec time-out (TO) period during which responding on either lever had no programmed consequence. Over the next 6 sessions, the FR1 schedule was gradually increased to a terminal FR5 20-sec TO schedule. Rats were trained on the FR5 schedule until responding stabilized for 3 consecutive sessions according to the following criteria: (1) a minimum of 10 infusions per session, (2) less than 20% variability in the number of infusions earned, and (3) a minimum 2:1 active:inactive lever response ratio.
To assess the dose effect of methylphenidate on nicotine self-administration, separate groups of rats were pretreated with methylphenidate (0, 1.25, 2.5, 5 or 10 mg/kg, sc; random order within subjects) 10 min prior to a session in which the unit dose of nicotine was either 0.01 mg/kg/infusion (n=7) or 0.03 mg/kg/infusion (n=7). At least two maintenance sessions (no pretreatment) intervened between each pretreatment session in order to maintain stable nicotine self-administration behavior. To assess the effect of repeated methylphenidate on nicotine self-administration (0.03 mg/kg/infusion), separate groups of rats were pretreated 10 min prior to each of 12 consecutive sessions with methylphenidate (2.5 mg/kg, sc; n=8) or saline (n=6). Following drug pretreatment sessions, both groups of rats were pretreated with saline for 3 additional nicotine self-administration sessions.
Sucrose-Maintained Responding
In order to use procedures similar to the nicotine self-administration experiments, a separate group of rats (n=8) was trained to lever press for sucrose reinforcement during daily 60-min sessions under a FR5 20-sec TO and was allocated the same amount of daily food as the nicotine self-administration rats; however, these rats did not undergo surgery. In addition, since sucrose reinforcement maintains higher response rates than nicotine, these rats were given 25 sucrose pellets in the home cage 15 min prior to each experimental session in an attempt to reduce response rates by partial satiation. Rats were monitored to ensure that all pellets were consumed prior to each session. Although insertion of a long TO is sometimes used to decrease food reinforcement rate (Paterson et al, 2003), we did not use this procedure because it can impose an artificial ceiling effect which would not be sensitive to potential increases in the rate of responding. Training continued until stable responding was obtained, defined as: (1) a minimum of 10 pellets earned per session, (2) less than 20% variability in the number of pellets earned, and (3) a minimum 2:1 active:inactive lever response ratio. The effect of methylphenidate (0, 1.25, 2.5, 5 or 10 mg/kg, sc; random order within subjects) given 10 min prior to the sessions was then determined. At least two maintenance sessions (no pretreatment) intervened between each pretreatment session in order to maintain stable sucrose-maintained behavior.
Nicotine Drug Discrimination
Rats (n=6) were trained initially to lever press for sucrose reinforcement under an FR1 schedule. The FR requirement for reinforcement was subsequently increased over several sessions to a terminal FR 10. In order to enhance acquisition of the nicotine-saline discrimination, only one lever (the saline-appropriate lever; counter-balanced across rats) was presented during these initial sessions. Once rats responded for 2 sessions under the FR10 schedule, nicotine discrimination training began. In this phase, nicotine (0.3 mg/kg, sc) or saline was administered prior to each session. Ten minutes after nicotine or saline was administered, rats were placed in the operant conditioning chamber, and the cue lights were illuminated to signal the beginning of the session and they remained illuminated for the duration of the session. When nicotine was administered, the nicotine-appropriate lever was presented and the saline-appropriate lever was removed. When saline was administered, the saline-appropriate lever was presented and the nicotine-appropriate lever was removed. For half of the rats, the left lever was designated the nicotine-appropriate lever and the right lever was designated the saline lever; the reverse was true for the remaining rats. Nicotine and saline were administered according to a double-alternation sequence (i.e., NNSSNN or SSNNSS, counterbalanced across rats) for 8 consecutive sessions. Then, for the remainder of the study, both levers were presented each day, and responding on the injection-appropriate lever was reinforced according to the FR10 schedule; responses on the incorrect lever were recorded but had no programmed consequence. Training sessions were 15 min in duration. Training continued until the following criteria were met on 8 consecutive sessions: (1) ≥85% of the total session responses occurred on the injection-appropriate lever and (2) first FR10 was completed on the injection-appropriate lever. Once these criteria were met, the test phase was initiated.
During the test phase, sessions were 3 min in length; on these sessions, completion of an FR10 on either lever was reinforced with sucrose. Each test session was separated by at least two 15-min training sessions in which saline and the nicotine training dose were administered once each (random order). Test sessions were conducted only if baseline performance during the intervening training sessions remained stable (i.e., ≥85% of the total session responses occurred on the injection-appropriate lever and the first FR10 was completed on the injection-appropriate lever). Two dependent measures were collected during test sessions: (1) percentage of total responses occurring on the nicotine-appropriate lever (calculated as the number of responses on the nicotine-appropriate lever divided by the total number of responses on either lever) and (2) rate of responding (calculated as the total number of responses on either lever divided by 180 sec). During test sessions, a dose-effect curve for nicotine (0.03, 0.056, 0.1, 0.17 and 0.3 mg/kg, sc; random order within subjects) was determined first, followed by determination of dose-effect curves for methylphenidate (1.25, 2.5, 5 and 10 mg/kg, sc; random order within subjects) administered alone or in combination with a sub-threshold nicotine dose (0.056 mg/kg). Each drug dose that was tested alone was administered 10 min prior to the test session; in the drug combination experiment, methylphenidate (1.25, 2.5, 5 and 10 mg/kg, sc; random order within subjects) was administered 15 min prior to the session and nicotine (0.056 mg/kg) was administered 10 min prior to the start of the session.
Locomotor Effect of Methylphenidate in Nicotine-Sensitized Rats
Rats were placed individually for 60 min in the locomotor activity apparatus for a single habituation day. Each rat was then assigned randomly to receive repeated injections of a single dose of nicotine (0.2, 0.4 or 0.8 mg/kg, sc) or saline. Nicotine or saline injections were administered for 10 consecutive days (Days 1-10), beginning on the day following the habituation session. After each injection, rats were placed immediately in the activity monitors for 60 min. Following the 10-day nicotine treatment period, rats remained in their home cages for a 14-day (Days 11-24) drug-free period. Rats from each nicotine dose treatment group were then assigned randomly to receive challenge injections of methylphenidate (2.5 mg/kg) or saline immediately prior to placement in the activity monitors for 60 min for three consecutive sessions (Days 25-27). The dependent measure was distance traveled (cm).
Drugs
S(-)-nicotine ditartrate (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) and methylphenidate HCl (Mallinckrodt, St. Louis, MO) were prepared in 0.9% NaCl (saline). Dilute NaOH was added to the nicotine solution until a pH of 7.4 was attained. The nicotine doses are expressed as the base weight and the methylphenidate doses are expressed as the salt weight.
Statistical Analyses
A one-way repeated measures ANOVA (with dose as the within-subjects factor) was used to evaluate dose-effect curves in the acute nicotine self-administration, sucrose-maintained responding and drug discrimination experiments. In cases where the dose effect attained statistical significance, Dunnett's post-hoc tests were conducted to compare each dose to the corresponding saline control. Specifically, the dose effects of methylphenidate in rats self-administering either 0.01 mg/kg/infusion or 0.03 mg/kg/infusion unit doses of nicotine were compared to the effect of saline pretreatment within the same groups. In the repeated nicotine self-administration and locomotor activity experiments, data were analyzed with mixed-factor ANOVA, with pretreatment dose serving as a between-subjects factor and session serving as a within-subjects factor. When data from experiments designed to test a priori hypotheses were analyzed, one-tailed tests were used. Where appropriate, the Newman-Keuls post hoc test was used to make multiple comparisons; in those instances, a conservative alpha level of p≤.01 used as the criteria for statistical significance to control for Type I errors. In all other cases, p≤.05 determined significance. In drug discrimination, the nicotine dose estimated to produce 50% nicotine-appropriate responding (i.e., the ED50 with 95% confidence intervals, expressed in mg/kg) was calculated using nonlinear regression of individual data points.
RESULTS
Dose Effect of Methylphenidate on Nicotine Self-Administration and Sucrose-Maintained Responding
Figure 1 illustrates the dose effect of methylphenidate on nicotine self-administration using two different unit doses (left panel) and on sucrose-maintained responding (right panel). Baseline responding for nicotine was higher in rats earning a unit dose of 0.01 mg/kg/infusion than in rats earning a unit dose of 0.03 mg/kg/infusion. At the 0.03 mg/kg/infusion unit dose, ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of methylphenidate dose (F4,25 = 2.90, p<.05). Post hoc tests indicated that 2.5 and 5 mg/kg of methylphenidate significantly increased the number of nicotine infusions earned relative to saline pretreatment. At the 0.01 mg/kg/infusion unit dose, ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of methylphenidate dose (F4,25 = 2.55, p<.05). Post hoc tests indicated that 1.25 mg/kg of methylphenidate significantly increased the number of nicotine infusions earned relative to saline pretreatment. For sucrose-maintained responding, ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of methylphenidate dose (F4,28 = 3.41, p<.05). Post hoc tests indicated that pretreatment with 1.25, 5 and 10 mg/kg of methylphenidate significantly decreased the number of pellets earned relative to saline pretreatment.
Effect of Repeated Methylphenidate on Nicotine Self-Administration
Figure 2 illustrates the effect of repeated pretreatment with methylphenidate (2.5 mg/kg) on nicotine self-administration (0.03 mg/kg/infusion). A mixed-factor, repeated measures ANOVA across all sessions (3 baseline, 12 pretreatment, 3 saline alone) revealed a significant pretreatment × day interaction (F17,204 = 1.969; p<.05), indicating that pretreatment with methylphenidate, but not saline, increased nicotine self-administration. The effect of repeated methylphenidate was subsequently assessed by conducting both within-subject and between-subject analyses. Post-hoc within-subject comparisons indicated that methylphenidate-treated rats self-administered significantly more nicotine infusions on Pretreatment Days 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9 and 12, as well as on the first saline pretreatment day (Day 13), relative to their baseline levels. Post-hoc within-subject comparisons of saline-treated rats revealed a significant decrease in the number of nicotine infusions earned on Pretreatment Day 12, relative to their baseline levels. Post-hoc between-group comparisons also indicated that methylphenidate-treated rats self-administered significantly more nicotine infusions than saline-treated rats on Pretreatment Days 1, 2, 3, 7, 8, 11 and 12, as well as on the first saline pretreatment day (Day 13).
In order to examine whether the methylphenidate-induced increase in nicotine self-administration was due to a general increase in lever-pressing, a mixed-factor, repeated measures ANOVA was also conducted on the inactive lever response data (results not shown). This analysis revealed no significant main effects or interactions, indicating that the methylphenidate-induced increase in responding was specific to the active lever.
Discriminative Stimulus Effects of Methylphenidate in Nicotine-Trained Rats
Figure 3 illustrates the dose effects of nicotine (left panels) and methylphenidate, alone or in combination with nicotine (right panels), on responding in rats trained to discriminate nicotine from saline. In substitution testing, nicotine (0.03-0.3 mg/kg) produced dose-dependent and full generalization, with an ED50 of 0.08 (0.05-0.12) mg/kg. The main effect of nicotine dose attained statistical significance (F5,35 = 21.88, p<.001), with post-hoc tests indicating that 0.1, 0.17 and 0.3 mg/kg of nicotine elicited significantly greater nicotine-appropriate responding than saline control values; response rates were not disrupted significantly by any nicotine dose. Conversely, methylphenidate did not elicit nicotine-appropriate responding at any dose when administered alone. However, when methylphenidate was co-administered 5 min prior to a sub-threshold dose of nicotine that did not elicit nicotine-appropriate responding by itself (0.056 mg/kg), a significant dose-dependent increase in responding on the nicotine-appropriate lever was obtained (F4,29 = 5.50, p<.01). Post-hoc tests indicated that, relative to saline, significant increases in nicotine-appropriate responding were obtained following administration of 5 and 10 mg/kg of methylphenidate in combination with 0.056 mg/kg of nicotine. Co-administration of nicotine with the 5 and 10 mg/kg doses of methylphenidate also produced significantly greater levels of nicotine-appropriate responding relative to these doses alone. Response rates were not disrupted significantly by any methylphenidate dose, whether administered alone or in combination with nicotine.
Effect of Repeated Nicotine on the Development of Methylphenidate Locomotor Sensitization
Figure 4 illustrates the dose effect of repeated nicotine on locomotor activity. Across the repeated treatment phase, nicotine produced progressive increases in locomotor activity, an effect indicative of sensitization. A mixed-factor, repeated measures ANOVA of these data revealed a significant nicotine dose × day interaction (F27,441 = 11.07, p<.001). On Day 1, 0.8 mg/kg of nicotine produced significant hypoactivity relative to saline. Across treatment days, 0.4 mg/kg of nicotine produced a significant increase in activity by Day 3 and all nicotine doses elicited a significant increase in locomotor activity on Days 5-10 relative to saline.
Figure 5 illustrates the dose effect of repeated nicotine on subsequent challenge injections of either saline (upper panel) or 2.5 mg/kg of methylphenidate (lower panel). A mixed-factor ANOVA (using prior nicotine treatment and challenge drug as between-subjects factors and challenge day as the within-subjects factor) revealed a significant challenge drug × day interaction (F2,100 = 9.3, p<.001). Subsequent analysis of the saline challenge data indicated that prior nicotine treatment did not alter activity in response to saline on any challenge day. Analysis of the methylphenidate challenge data indicated that prior nicotine treatment also did not alter significantly the initial response to methylphenidate on Day 1. However, by Day 3, ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of prior nicotine treatment (F3,25 = 4.02, p<.05), indicating that exposure to nicotine promoted the induction of locomotor sensitization to methylphenidate. Post-hoc Dunnett's tests indicated that rats treated previously with 0.8 mg/kg of nicotine exhibited significantly greater activity following methylphenidate relative to rats treated previously with saline (p<.01). To explore this latter finding further, the time course effect of methylphenidate on Challenge Days 1 and 3 in the rats treated previously with saline or nicotine (0.8 mg/kg) were compared (Figure 6); note that the these data points reflect the time course effect of the total activity data of rats treated previously with saline and 0.8 mg/kg of nicotine during Challenge Days 1 and 3 presented in the lower portion of Figure 5. A mixed-factor ANOVA revealed a significant nicotine treatment × time interval × challenge day interaction, F11,132 = 2.03, p<.05. Post-hoc analysis indicated that, whereas nicotine-treated rats were significantly more active following methylphenidate at only the 35-min time point relative to saline-treated rats on Day 1 (Figure 6, left panel), nicotine-treated rats were significantly more active at the 10- and 45-60-min time points on Day 3 (Figure 6, right panel). Thus, although the data from Day 1 provided little evidence of cross-sensitization to the initial methylphenidate challenge, prior exposure to 0.8 mg/kg of nicotine promoted the induction of sensitization following repeated methylphenidate injections.
DISCUSSION
The results of the present preclinical investigation demonstrate that methylphenidate enhances the abuse-related behavioral effects of nicotine in rats as assessed in the intravenous self-administration, drug discrimination and locomotor cross-sensitization procedures. Acute pretreatment with methylphenidate (1.25-10 mg/kg) increased nicotine self-administration, although this effect differed depending on the nicotine unit dose available. In rats self-administering a low unit dose of nicotine (0.01 mg/kg/infusion), an increase in the number of infusions earned was obtained following pretreatment with 1.25 mg/kg of methylphenidate, but not following pretreatment with higher methylphenidate doses (2.5-10 mg/kg). In rats self-administering a higher unit dose of nicotine (0.03 mg/kg/infusion), an increase in the number of infusions earned was obtained only following 2.5 and 5 mg/kg of methylphenidate. These results indicate that as the unit dose of nicotine is increased, a greater pretreatment dose of methylphenidate is needed to enhance responding. However, since there were no differences in the effect of pretreatment with 10 mg/kg of methylphenidate between rats self-administering either nicotine unit dose, it is possible that high doses of stimulants can alter nicotine self-administration nonspecifically. While the precise reason(s) for this biphasic pattern of results is unknown, Rauhut et al (2003) found similar biphasic dose effects of acute bupropion or methamphetamine pretreatment on nicotine self-administration in rats. Prada and Goldberg (1985) also reported that caffeine pretreatment increased nicotine self-administration in squirrel monkeys at low doses (3-10 mg/kg), while higher doses (60-100 mg/kg) decreased response rates. It is possible that the descending limb of the biphasic dose effect curve for stimulant drugs on nicotine self-administration is due to nonspecific behavioral impairment or to the induction of stereotypic behavior that is incompatible with lever pressing. Thus, the nonspecific effect of 10 mg/kg of methylphenidate may have masked the change in responding that normally occurs when the unit dose of nicotine is altered, thereby eliminating the difference between the two nicotine self-administration groups.
Interestingly, the dose-dependent increase in nicotine self-administration following acute methylphenidate did not appear to undergo tolerance, as repeated administration of 2.5 mg/kg of methylphenidate continued to increase self-administration of nicotine (0.03 mg/kg/infusion) across 12 consecutive sessions. Nicotine self-administration returned to control levels across a subsequent 3-day period of saline administration, indicating that the effect of methylphenidate was reversible. The lack of tolerance to repeated methylphenidate is comparable to studies demonstrating that tolerance does not develop to the effects of repeated administration of bupropion on nicotine self-administration. Specifically, Shoaib et al (2003) demonstrated that repeated administration of 30 mg/kg of bupropion continued to increase nicotine self-administration across a 28-day treatment period and Rauhut et al (2005) reported that the acute decrease in nicotine self-administration produced by administration of 70 mg/kg of bupropion did not undergo tolerance across a 14-day treatment period.
Although these results indicate that low doses of methylphenidate can specifically increase nicotine self-administration under an FR schedule of reinforcement, the exact mechanisms underlying this effect are not known. One potential interpretation is that methylphenidate attenuated the reinforcing effect of nicotine, thus leading to an increase in nicotine intake in an attempt to surmount the attenuation in reinforcing effectiveness (Yokel, 1987). However, given that prior work has demonstrated that selective nicotinic acetylcholine receptor antagonists such as mecamylamine and dihydro-β-erythriodine (DHβE) generally fail to increase nicotine self-administration (Corrigall and Coen, 1989; Watkins et al, 1999; but see Fattore et al (2002) for evidence of a mecamylamine-induced increase in nicotine self-administration), it seems unlikely that methylphenidate attenuated the reinforcing effect of nicotine. Although an FR schedule was used in the present study, assessing the effects of methylphenidate pretreatment on nicotine self-administration under a PR schedule could help to clarify the mechanisms underlying the rate-increasing effect of methylphenidate on nicotine self-administration observed using an FR schedule (Arnold and Roberts, 1997; Richardson and Roberts, 1996). Alternatively, since nicotine infusions were signaled by illumination of cue lights, methylphenidate may have augmented the response to the visual stimuli known to play an important role in nicotine self-administration behavior in rats (Caggiula et al, 2001, 2002; Palmatier et al, 2006). Consistent with this notion, Files et al (1989) reported that methylphenidate (5-20 mg/kg) increased response rates during extinction (i.e., when food reinforcement was no longer available) if responding resulted in presentation of conditioned stimuli paired previously with food delivery, but not when responding occurred without the presentation of food-paired stimuli. Likewise, methylphenidate has been shown to selectively increase response rates maintained by a water-paired conditioned stimulus, while decreasing response rates in the absence of a water-paired stimulus (Robbins, 1978). Regardless of the interpretation, however, given that methylphenidate selectively increased responding for nicotine, but not sucrose, it appears that methylphenidate can differentially alter the incentive motivation for a drug and nondrug reinforcer. This conclusion parallels the recent clinical work of Rush et al (2005) showing that oral methylphenidate increases tobacco smoking in humans tested in a controlled laboratory environment, while decreasing food consumption.
To our knowledge, this is the first report to examine the discriminative stimulus effect of methylphenidate in nicotine-trained rats. Although nicotine produced dose-dependent substitution, methylphenidate did not elicit appreciable levels of nicotine-appropriate responding at any dose. However, when co-administered with a sub-threshold dose of nicotine (0.056 mg/kg), methylphenidate produced a dose-dependent increase in nicotine-appropriate responding, although the maximal effect of methylphenidate did not meet criteria used traditionally for declaring full substitution (i.e., ≥80% nicotine-appropriate responding). This augmentation in the effect of a low nicotine dose by methylphenidate is similar to results reported by Gasior et al (2002), who examined the discriminative stimulus effects of caffeine in nicotine-trained rats. In that report, rats were trained to discriminate nicotine (0.4 mg/kg) from saline, and dose-effect curves were generated for nicotine and caffeine, both alone and in combination with a sub-threshold dose of nicotine (0.05 mg/kg). Similar to the present results, caffeine administered alone did not elicit nicotine-appropriate responding at any dose; however, when combined with a low dose of nicotine, caffeine dose-dependently generalized to the nicotine cue (Gasior et al, 2002). The fact that methylphenidate did not substitute for nicotine when given alone contrasts with other reports showing that the discriminative stimulus effects of other classical stimulant drugs overlap, at least partially, with the nicotine cue. Bardo et al (1997) reported that nicotine substituted partially for d-amphetamine, and Desai et al (1999; 2003) reported that nicotine substituted fully for cocaine; in addition, both amphetamine and cocaine substitute partially for the nicotine cue (Mansbach et al, 1998). Regarding bupropion, some reports have found that bupropion substitutes partially (Desai et al, 2003) or fully (Wiley et al, 2001; Young and Glennon, 2002) for nicotine in generalization tests; however, a range of bupropion pretreatment doses failed to shift the nicotine dose-response curve (Shoaib et al, 2003), and mecamylamine pretreatment blocks the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine, but not bupropion (Wiley et al, 2001). Thus, while the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine and stimulant drugs, including methylphenidate, appear to overlap to some extent, there is sufficient evidence to conclude that the cues produced by these drugs are dissociable.
Consistent with other reports (Dwoskin et al, 1999; LeSage et al, 2006), the results of the locomotor activity experiment revealed that nicotine initially induced hypoactivity, followed by hyperactivity across repeated injections. More important, although there was little evidence indicative of cross-sensitization to the initial methylphenidate challenge following nicotine exposure, nicotine pre-exposure promoted the induction of locomotor sensitization following three repeated methylphenidate injections. The failure to find cross-sensitization between repeated nicotine and acute methylphenidate contrasts with evidence of cross-sensitization between nicotine and other stimulant drugs. Nicotine pre-exposure enhances the subsequent locomotor stimulant effect of acute amphetamine (Birrell and Balfour, 1998), bupropion (Wilkinson et al, 2006), cocaine (Collins and Izenwasser, 2004) and methamphetamine (Kuribara, 1999). While the explanation for these dissociable effects among stimulants is not known, the nicotine-induced enhancement in locomotor sensitization with repeated methylphenidate indicates at least a partial overlap in the mechanisms underlying both nicotine and methylphenidate sensitization.
Although the present study does not address the neuropharmacological mechanisms underlying the interactive behavioral effects of methylphenidate and nicotine, it is possible that the mesolimbic dopamine pathway, a brain circuit implicated in drug reward (Bardo, 1998; Leshner and Koob, 1999; Wise, 1998), is involved. Nicotine increases dopaminergic neurotransmission by activation of high-affinity β2 subunit-containing nicotinic cholinergic receptors localized on dopamine cell bodies in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), as well as by altering the tone of GABA and glutamate inputs to the VTA (Mansvelder et al, 2002, 2003); the net effect is an increase in extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc; Pontieri et al, 1996). In contrast, methylphenidate, a dopamine and norepinephrine transport inhibitor, does not release dopamine directly, but rather prevents dopamine clearance in the NAcc and other corticolimbic structures, thus yielding a net increase in extracellular dopamine in those terminal fields (Grace, 2001). As these mechanisms would suggest, it has been shown that co-administration of methylphenidate augments nicotine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine content in the NAcc (Gerasimov et al, 2000), suggesting that the interactive effects of these drugs on mesolimbic dopamine transmission underlies the behavioral interactions noted in the present study. Regarding self-administration specifically, blockade of central dopamine receptors reduces nicotine self-administration (Corrigall and Coen, 1991; Corrigall et al, 1992), whereas administration of monoamine oxidase inhibitors or the dopamine reuptake inhibitor bupropion increases nicotine self-administration (Guillem et al, 2005, 2006; Rauhut et al, 2003). One caveat to this interpretation is that methylphenidate and bupropion are relatively nonselective inhibitors of the norepinephrine and dopamine transporters (Dwoskin et al, 2006; Han and Gu, 2006). While this suggests that both dopaminergic and noradrenergic mechanisms may be involved, a role for norepinephrine seems unlikely given that reboxetine, a selective norepinephrine uptake inhibitor, does not increase nicotine self-administration in rats (Rauhut et al, 2002). Thus, it appears that an enhanced response to nicotine-evoked dopamine release in reward-relevant limbic terminal fields following methylphenidate administration may underlie the increase in nicotine self-administration.
Similarly, locomotor sensitization may also reflect alterations in dopamine transmission, as an enhanced dopamine response following repeated administration of psychostimulant drugs, including nicotine, is linked with the expression of behavioral sensitization (Benwell and Balfour, 1992; Pierce and Kalivas, 1997; Vezina, 2004). Interestingly, nicotine can augment dopamine transporter function (Middleton et al, 2004) and density (Harrod et al, 2004), suggesting that enduring nicotine-induced alterations in dopamine transporter function and/or density may play a role in the enhanced effect of repeated methylphenidate following nicotine treatment.
In drug discrimination, methylphenidate pretreatment may have also enhanced the discriminative stimulus effect of a low nicotine dose by enhancing the dopaminergic effect of nicotine. Caffeine, which augments extracellular dopamine levels, has been reported to potentiate the discriminative stimulus effect of a low nicotine dose (Gasior et al, 2002), suggesting that a similar mechanism may be implicated in the present drug discrimination results. However, evidence of a primary role for dopamine in mediating the nicotine cue is lacking, as selective dopamine antagonists generally fail to alter nicotine discrimination or attenuate nicotine discrimination only at doses that suppress response rates (Brioni et al, 1994; Corrigall and Coen, 1994; Le Foll et al, 2005). Since methylphenidate also elevates extracellular levels of acetylcholine in addition to dopamine (Tzavara et al, 2006), it may be that methylphenidate enhances nicotine's discriminative stimulus effects via a primarily non-dopaminergic mechanism.
From a clinical perspective, it is interesting to note that methylphenidate (Ritalin®) is the most commonly prescribed pharmacotherapy for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a behavioral disorder characterized by excessive levels of activity, impulsivity and inattention (Arnsten, 2006; Solanto et al, 2001; Volkow et al, 2005). Methylphenidate is an effective treatment for the behavioral and cognitive deficits associated with ADHD when administered orally at therapeutic doses (Greenhill, 2001; Volkow and Swanson, 2003; Biederman et al, 2004). Thus, one potential caveat of the present study is that methylphenidate was administered via subcutaneous injections, which may have produced a more rapid onset of action than orally-administered methylphenidate as used in clinical settings. A study by Gerasimov et al (2000) demonstrated that orally-administered methylphenidate in rats is less potent in stimulating locomotor activity and increasing DA levels in the nucleus accumbens relative to the same methylphenidate doses administered via intraperitoneal injection; whether the same holds true for subcutaneous injections has yet to be determined. Another potential caveat is that the dose range of methylphenidate used in the present study included doses higher than those used clinically. Kuczenski and Segal (2005) advocated for the use of lower doses of methylphenidate (i.e., ≤3 mg/kg) in preclinical studies in order to approximate clinically-relevant plasma levels of methylphenidate. While some of the methylphenidate doses used in the present study (i.e., 5 and 10 mg/kg) likely exceeded clinical doses, the lower doses evaluated (i.e., 1.25 and 2.5 mg/kg) are thought to fall within the therapeutic dose range. Importantly, these lower doses produced the greatest increase in nicotine self-administration, suggesting that the present results have clinical relevance.
In addition to the present results showing that methylphenidate alters the behavioral effects of nicotine in adult rats, developmental exposure to methylphenidate has been shown to alter the response to cocaine in adult rats. Administration of methylphenidate to periadolescent rats produces changes in subsequent cocaine self-administration (Brandon et al, 2001), cocaine-conditioned place preference (Andersen et al, 2002; Carlezon et al, 2003), and cocaine-induced hyperactivity (Torres-Reveron and Dow-Edwards, 2005) in adulthood. While the present findings demonstrate that methylphenidate can increase nicotine self-administration in adult rats, there is evidence that the behavioral effects of methylphenidate can differ between adolescent and adult rats (Andersen, 2005; Torres-Reveron and Dow-Edwards, 2005; Wooters et al, 2006). Since methylphenidate is commonly administered to children and adolescents in clinical settings, an important avenue for future research will be to determine the effects of developmental exposure to low doses of oral methylphenidate on subsequent acquisition of nicotine self-administration.
Individuals diagnosed with ADHD smoke cigarettes at a higher rate than the general population (Barkley et al, 1990; Kollins et al, 2005; Rohde et al, 2004). Although the exact reason(s) for this relationship have yet to be determined, one potential explanation is that tobacco smoking represents a form of self-medication in the ADHD population. Nicotine has comparable therapeutic effects to methylphenidate in adolescents (Potter and Newhouse, 2004) and adults (Connors et al, 1996; Levin et al, 2001) diagnosed with ADHD, raising the possibility that tobacco smoking in the ADHD population is maintained by the positive behavioral effects of nicotine. Alternatively, given the widespread exposure to methylphenidate in ADHD-diagnosed individuals, there has been interest in determining whether methylphenidate exposure increases the risk for smoking. In one prospective longitudinal study, Lambert and Hartshough (1998) reported that 93% of adult smokers diagnosed with ADHD during childhood who were treated with stimulants were daily smokers, compared to only 80% of ADHD-diagnosed smokers who were not treated previously with stimulant drugs. Moreover, methylphenidate has been shown to increase tobacco cigarette smoking in a controlled human laboratory experiment (Rush et al, 2005). The present preclinical results extend this previous work by providing evidence of a direct potentiating interaction between the abuse-related behavioral effects of methylphenidate and nicotine. Taken together, these results suggest that caution may be warranted when prescribing methylphenidate to cigarette smokers with ADHD.
Acknowledgments
DISCLOSURE We gratefully acknowledge the technical assistance provided by Joshua Cutshall, Laura Fenton and Emily Geary. We also thank Dr. Drake Morgan for helpful suggestions regarding the drug discrimination training procedure. During preparation of the manuscript, T.E. Wooters was supported by NIDA Predoctoral Training Grant T32 DA007304 and N.M. Neugebauer was supported by NIDA Predoctoral Training Grant T32 DA016176. The authors do not have any conflicts of interest (financial or otherwise) that might bias this work. Funding was provided by NIH grant DA 017548.
REFERENCES
- Andersen SL. Stimulants and the developing brain. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2005;26:237–43. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, Arvanitogiannis A, Pliakas AM, LeBlanc C, Carlezon WA., Jr Altered responsiveness to cocaine in rats exposed to methylphenidate during development. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:13–4. doi: 10.1038/nn777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold JM, Roberts DC. A critique of fixed and progressive ratio schedules used to examine the neural substrates of drug reinforcement. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;57:441–7. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00445-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnsten AF. Stimulants: therapeutic actions in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2376–83. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT. Neuropharmacological mechanisms of drug reward: beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 1998;12:37–67. doi: 10.1615/critrevneurobiol.v12.i1-2.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT, Bevins RA, Klebaur JE, Crooks PA, Dwoskin LP. (-)-Nornicotine partially substitutes for (+)-amphetamine in a drug discrimination paradigm in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;58:1083–7. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00303-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bardo MT, Green TA, Crooks PA, Dwoskin LP. Nornicotine is self-administered intravenously by rats. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:290–6. doi: 10.1007/s002130051119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley RA, Fischer M, Edelbrock CS, Smallish L. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;29:546–57. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199007000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benwell ME, Balfour DJ. The effects of acute and repeated nicotine treatment on nucleus accumbens dopamine and locomotor activity. Br J Pharmacol. 1992;105:849–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1992.tb09067.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biederman J, Spencer T, Wilens T. Evidence-based pharmacotherapy for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004;7:77–97. doi: 10.1017/S1461145703003973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birrell CE, Balfour DJ. The influence of nicotine pretreatment on mesoaccumbens dopamine overflow and locomotor responses to D-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140:142–9. doi: 10.1007/s002130050751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandon CL, Marinelli M, Baker LK, White FJ. Enhanced reactivity and vulnerability to cocaine following methylphenidate treatment in adolescent rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:651–61. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00281-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brioni JD, Kim DJ, O'Neill AB, Williams JE, Decker MW. Clozapine attenuates the discriminative stimulus properties of (-)-nicotine. Brain Res. 1994;643:1–9. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90002-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caggiula AR, Donny EC, White AR, Chaudhri N, Booth S, Gharib MA, Hoffman A, Perkins KA, Sved AF. Cue dependency of nicotine self-administration and smoking. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;70:515–30. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00676-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caggiula AR, Donny EC, White AR, Chaudhri N, Booth S, Gharib MA, Hoffman A, Perkins KA, Sved AF. Environmental stimuli promote the acquisition of nicotine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2002;163:230–7. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1156-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlezon WA, Jr, Mague SD, Andersen SL. Enduring behavioral effects of early exposure to methylphenidate in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:1330–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins SL, Izenwasser S. Chronic nicotine differentially alters cocaine-induced locomotor activity in adolescent vs. adult male and female rats. Neuropharmacology. 2004;46:349–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2003.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connors CK, Levin ED, Sparrow E, Hinton SC, Erhardt D, Meck WH, et al. Nicotine and attention in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1996;32:67–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigall WA, Coen KM. Nicotine maintains robust self-administration in rats on a limited-access schedule. Psychopharmacology. 1989;99:473–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00589894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigall WA, Coen KM. Selective dopamine antagonists reduce nicotine self-administration. Psychopharmacology. 1991;104:171–176. doi: 10.1007/BF02244174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigall WA, Franklin KB, Coen KM, Clarke PB. The mesolimbic dopaminergic system is implicated in the reinforcing effects of nicotine. Psychopharmacology. 1992;107:285–9. doi: 10.1007/BF02245149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigall WA, Coen KM. Dopamine mechanisms play at best a small role in the nicotine discriminative stimulus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;48:817–20. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90353-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cousins MS, Stamat HM, de Wit H. Acute doses of d-amphetamine and bupropion increase cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology. 2001;157:243–53. doi: 10.1007/s002130100802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desai RI, Barber DJ, Terry P. Asymmetric generalization between the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine and cocaine. Behav Pharmacol. 1999;10:647–56. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199911000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desai RI, Barber DJ, Terry P. Dopaminergic and cholinergic involvement in the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine and cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2003;167:335–43. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1426-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dwoskin LP, Crooks PA, Teng L, Green TA, Bardo MT. Acute and chronic effects of nornicotine on locomotor activity in rats: altered response to nicotine. Psychopharmacology. 1999;145:442–51. doi: 10.1007/s002130051079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dwoskin LP, Rauhut AS, King-Pospisil KA, Bardo MT. Review of the pharmacology and clinical profile of bupropion, an antidepressant and tobacco use cessation agent. CNS Drug Rev. 2006;12:178–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-3458.2006.00178.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fattore L, Cossu G, Martellotta MC, Fratta W. Baclofen antagonizes intravenous self-administration of nicotine in mice and rats. Alcohol Alcohol. 2002;5:495–8. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/37.5.495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Files FJ, Branch MN, Clody D. Effects of methylphenidate on responding under extinction in the presence and absence of conditioned reinforcement. Behav Pharmacol. 1989;1:113–121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler JS, Logan J, Wang GJ, Volkow ND. Monoamine oxidase and cigarette smoking. Neurotoxicology. 2003;24:75–82. doi: 10.1016/s0161-813x(02)00109-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasior M, Jaszyna M, Munzar P, Witkin JM, Goldberg SR. Caffeine potentiates the discriminative-stimulus effects of nicotine in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2002;162:385–95. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1113-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerasimov MR, Franceschi M, Volkow ND, Rice O, Schiffer WK, Dewey SL. Synergistic interactions between nicotine and cocaine or methylphenidate depend on the dose of dopamine transporter inhibitor. Synapse. 2000;38:432–7. doi: 10.1002/1098-2396(20001215)38:4<432::AID-SYN8>3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerasimov MR, Franceschi M, Volkow ND, Gifford A, Gatley SJ, Marsteller D, Molina PE, Dewey SL. Comparison between intraperitoneal and oral methylphenidate administration: a microdialysis and locomotor study. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;295:51–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace AA. Psychostimulant actions on dopamine and limbic system function: relevance to the pathophysiology and treatment of ADHD. In: Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX, editors. Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience. University Press; Oxford: 2001. pp. 134–157. [Google Scholar]
- Greenhill LL. Clinical effects of stimulant medication in ADHD. In: Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX, editors. Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience. University Press; Oxford: 2001. pp. 31–72. [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, Vouillac C, Azar MR, Parsons LH, Koob GF, Cador M, Stinus L. Monoamine oxidase inhibition dramatically increases the motivation to self-administer nicotine in rats. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8593–600. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2139-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, Vouillac C, Azar MR, Parsons LH, Koob GF, Cador M, Stinus L. Monoamine oxidase A rather than monoamine oxidase B inhibition increases nicotine reinforcement in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:3532–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05217.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han DD, Gu HH. Comparison of the monoamine transporters from human and mouse in their sensitivities to psychostimulant drugs. BMC Pharmacol. 2006;6:6. doi: 10.1186/1471-2210-6-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrod SB, Mactutus CF, Bennett K, Hasselrot U, Wu G, Welch M, Booze RM. Sex differences and repeated intravenous nicotine: behavioral sensitization and dopamine receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;78:581–92. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henningfield JE, Griffiths RR. Cigarette smoking and subjective response: effects of d-amphetamine. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1981;30:497–505. doi: 10.1038/clpt.1981.194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kollins SH, McClernon FJ, Fuemmeler BF. Association between smoking and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a population-based sample of young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:1142–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuczenski R, Segal DS. Stimulant actions in rodents: implications for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder treatment and potential stimulant abuse. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1391–1396. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuribara H. Does nicotine modify the psychotoxic effect of methamphetamine? Assessment in terms of locomotor sensitization in mice. J Toxicol Sci. 1999;24:55–62. doi: 10.2131/jts.24.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert NM, Hartsough CS. Prospective study of tobacco smoking and substance dependencies among samples of ADHD and non-ADHD participants. J Learn Disabil. 1998;31:533–44. doi: 10.1177/002221949803100603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Foll B, Sokoloff P, Stark H, Goldberg SR. Dopamine D3 receptor ligands block nicotine-induced conditioned place preferences through a mechanism that does not involve discriminative-stimulus or antidepressant-like effects. Neuropsychopharm. 2005;30:720–30. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeSage MG, Gustaf E, Dufek MB, Pentel PR. Effects of maternal intravenous nicotine administration on locomotor behavior in pre-weanling rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;85:575–83. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leshner AI, Koob GF. Drugs of abuse and the brain. Proc Assoc Am Physicians. 1999;111:99–108. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1381.1999.09218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin ED, Connors CK, Silva D, Canu W, March J. Effects of chronic nicotine and methylphenidate in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2001;9:83–90. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.9.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansbach RS, Rovetti CC, Freedland CS. The role of monoamine neurotransmitter systems in the nicotine discriminative stimulus. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;52:125–34. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00085-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansvelder HD, De Rover M, McGehee DS, Brussaard AB. Cholinergic modulation of dopaminergic reward areas: upstream and downstream targets of nicotine addiction. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;480:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2003.08.099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansvelder HD, McGehee DS. Cellular and synaptic mechanisms of nicotine addiction. J Neurobiol. 2002;53:606–17. doi: 10.1002/neu.10148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton LS, Cass WA, Dwoskin LP. Nicotinic receptor modulation of dopamine transporter function in rat striatum and medial prefrontal cortex. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2004;308:367–77. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.055335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmatier MI, Evans-Martin FF, Hoffman A, Caggiula AR, Chaudhri N, Donny EC, Liu X, Booth S, Gharib M, Craven L, Sved AF. Dissociating the primary reinforcing and reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine using a rat self-administration paradigm with concurrently available drug and environmental reinforcers. Psychopharmacology. 2006;184:391–400. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0183-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paterson NE, Semenova S, Gasparini F, Markou A. The mGluR5 antagonist MPEP decreased nicotine self-administration in rats and mice. Psychopharmacology. 2003;167:257–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1432-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce RC, Kalivas PW. A circuitry model of the expression of behavioral sensitization to amphetamine-like psychostimulants. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1997;25:192–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00021-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pontieri FE, Tanda G, Orzi F, Di Chiara G. Effects of nicotine on the nucleus accumbens and similarity to those of addictive drugs. Nature. 1996;382:255–7. doi: 10.1038/382255a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potter AS, Newhouse PA. Effects of acute nicotine administration on behavioral inhibition in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology. 2004;176:182–94. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1874-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prada JA, Goldberg SR. Effects of caffeine or nicotine pretreatments on nicotine self-administration by the squirrel monkey. Pharmacologist. 1985;27:226. [Google Scholar]
- Rauhut AS, Mullins SN, Dwoskin LP, Bardo MT. Reboxetine: attenuation of nicotine self-administration in rats. J Pharm Exp Ther. 2002;303:664–72. doi: 10.1124/jpet.303.2.664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauhut AS, Neugebauer N, Dwoskin LP, Bardo MT. Effect of bupropion on nicotine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169:1–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1450-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauhut AS, Dwoskin LP, Bardo MT. Tolerance does not develop to the decrease in nicotine self-administration produced by repeated bupropion administration. Nicotine Tob Res. 2005;7:901–7. doi: 10.1080/14622200500381384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson NR, Roberts DC. Progressive ratio schedules in drug self-administration studies in rats: a method to evaluate reinforcing efficacy. J Neurosci Methods. 1996;66:1–11. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(95)00153-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW. The acquisition of responding with conditioned reinforcement: effects of pipradrol, methylphenidate, d-amphetamine and nomifensine. Psychopharmacology. 1978;58:79–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00426794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde P, Kahler CW, Lewinsohn PM, Brown RA. Psychiatric disorders, familial factors, and cigarette smoking: III. Associations with cessation by young adulthood among daily smokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6:509–22. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001723663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose JE, Levin ED. Inter-relationships between conditioned and primary reinforcement in the maintenance of cigarette smoking. Br J Addict. 1991;86:605–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01816.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roll JM, Higgins ST, Tidey J. Cocaine use can increase cigarette smoking: evidence from laboratory and naturalistic settings. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5:263–8. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.5.3.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rush CR, Higgins ST, Vansickel AR, Stoops WW, Lile JA, Glaser PE. Methylphenidate increases cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology. 2005;181:781–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0021-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schorling JB, Gutgesell M, Klas P, Smith D, Keller A. Tobacco, alcohol and other drug use among college students. J Subst Abuse. 1994;6:105–115. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(94)90143-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shoaib M, Sidhpura N, Shafait S. Investigating the actions of bupropion on dependence-related effects of nicotine in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2003;165:405–12. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1277-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sigmon SC, Tidey JW, Badger GJ, Higgins ST. Acute effects of D-amphetamine on progressive-ratio performance maintained by cigarette smoking and money. Psychopharmacology. 2003;167:393–402. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1416-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solanto MV, Arnsten AFT, Castellanos FX. Stimulant Drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience. University Press; Oxford: 2001. pp. 134–157. [Google Scholar]
- Tidey JW, O'Neill SC, Higgins ST. D-amphetamine increases choice of cigarette smoking over monetary reinforcement. Psychopharmacology. 2000;153:85–92. doi: 10.1007/s002130000600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres-Reveron A, Dow-Edwards DL. Repeated administration of methylphenidate in young, adolescent, and mature rats affects the response to cocaine later in adulthood. Psychopharmacology. 2005;181:38–47. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2221-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzavara ET, Bymaster FP, Overshiner CD, Davis RJ, Perry KW, Wolff M, McKinzie DL, Witkin JM, Nomikos GG. Procholinergic and memory enhancing properties of the selective norepinephrine uptake inhibitor atomoxetine. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11:187–95. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vezina P. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27:827–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Swanson JM. Variables that affect the clinical use and abuse of methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1909–18. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Ding YS. Imaging the effects of methylphenidate on brain dopamine: new model on its therapeutic actions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1410–5. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins SS, Epping-Jordan MP, Koob GF, Markou A. Blockade of nicotine self-administration with nicotinic antagonists in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;62:743–51. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(98)00226-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiley JL, Lavecchia KL, Martin BR, Damaj MI. Nicotine-like discriminative stimulus effects of bupropion in rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;10:129–35. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.10.2.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson JL, Palmatier MI, Bevins RA. Preexposure to nicotine alters the subsequent locomotor stimulant effects of bupropion in rats. Nicotine Tob Res. 2006;8:141–6. doi: 10.1080/14622200500484642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;51:13–22. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wooters TE, Dwoskin LP, Bardo MT. Age and sex differences in the locomotor effect of repeated methylphenidate in rats classified as high or low novelty responders. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:18–27. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0445-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokel RA. Intravenous self-administration: response rates, the effects of pharmacological challenges, and drug preferences. In: Bozarth MA, editor. Methods of Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1987. pp. 1–33. [Google Scholar]
- Young R, Glennon RA. Nicotine and bupropion share a similar discriminative stimulus effect. Eur J Pharmacol. 2002;443:113–8. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)01554-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]