Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jul 15.
Published in final edited form as: Neuroreport. 2009 Jul 15;20(11):984–989. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832d0a67

Functional neuroimaging of mentalizing during the trust game in social anxiety disorder

Chandra Sehkar Sripada a, Mike Angstadt a, Sarah Banks c, Pradeep J Nathan d, Israel Liberzon a,b, K Luan Phan a,b
PMCID: PMC2746411  NIHMSID: NIHMS121762  PMID: 19521264

Abstract

Individuals with generalized social anxiety disorder tend to make overly negative and distorted predictions about social events, which enhance perceptions of threat and contribute to excessive anxiety in social situations. Here, we coupled functional magnetic resonance imaging and a multiround economic exchange game (‘trust game’) to probe mentalizing, the social-cognitive ability to attribute mental states to others. Relative to interactions with a computer, those with human partners (‘mentalizing’) elicited less activation of medial prefrontal cortex in generalized social anxiety patients compared with matched healthy control participants. Diminished medial prefrontal cortex function may play a role in the social-cognitive pathophysiology of social anxiety.

Keywords: anxiety, functional magnetic resonance imaging, mentalizing, social cognition, trust

Introduction

Generalized social anxiety disorder is a highly prevalent and disabling psychiatric illness that is characterized by excessive fear of public scrutiny and negative evaluation across a variety of social situations [1,2]. The underlying cause of this exaggerated social fear is unknown, but could partly be because of deficits in social cognition, which manifest as a tendency toward inaccurate and distorted interpretations of the beliefs and intentions of others during interpersonal interactions [3]. Identification of a neural mechanism that explains these social-cognitive deficits in generalized social anxiety disorder (hereafter referred to as ‘social anxiety’) remains elusive.

So far, evidence for brain-based dysfunction in social anxiety has focused on exaggerated reactivity of the amygdala in response to facial signals that convey criticism and/or negative feedback (angry, contemptuous/disgusted, and/or fearful faces) (for meta-analysis, see Ref. [4]). However, the usefulness of static face stimuli in elucidating social-cognitive deficits in social anxiety is likely to be limited, as these stimuli primarily engage perception of emotional signals, typically do not engage prefrontal cortex, and do not reflect real-world social interactions that are inherently dynamic and interactive.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of social cognition have recently used a novel approach called ‘social neuroeconomics’ [5,6], which uses economic game theory to dynamically model fundamental aspects of social interaction such as cooperation, trust, and social signaling [7-11]. Collectively, these studies have begun to characterize the neural correlates of social interactions, including processes that encode the motives and intentions of social partners to guide appropriate behaviors, referred to as ‘theory of mind’ or ‘mentalizing’ [12,13]. The medial prefrontal cortex has been implicated as a key brain region that implements mentalizing during the social interactions by these studies [12-16], including those studies that have specifically used economic exchange games [7,10].

As a prototypical interactive economic exchange game, the ‘trust game’ serves as a potent probe of mentalizing abilities because it sets up the need to make inferences about the mental state of others [7], including partners’ beliefs, desires, intentions, and certain dispositions (e.g. trustworthiness, reliability, approachability, etc.). Such information is crucial for predicting partners’ responses; as recently noted by Frith and Frith [14], ‘It is important for us to be able to read the minds of others because it is their mental states that determine their actions’. Therefore, functional neuroimaging of the trust game [17] may shed light on a novel behavioral and neural mechanism that explains the tendency of individuals with social anxiety to misinterpret partner motives, which may contribute to their reticence to enter into or continue social interactions. In this study, we used a version of the trust game previously coupled with fMRI [7,11] to probe the neural correlates of mentalizing in social anxiety. Given that individuals with social anxiety tend to form inaccurate impressions about others [18], we predicted that relative to healthy controls, prefrontal areas such as the medial prefrontal cortex would exhibit less engagement during mentalizing in individuals with social anxiety.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-six patients with generalized social anxiety disorder, based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition) criteria as confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition) with additional probes from the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale and the Social Phobic Situations Interview, and 26 healthy control individuals (hereafter referred to as ‘controls’) participated. None of the social anxiety patients had a current depressive episode or recent substance abuse/dependence (within 6 months of study entry), or a lifetime history of bipolar disorder, psychotic disorder, any autism/pervasive developmental disorder, or mental retardation. All participants were right-handed and free of current or past major medical or neurologic illness, as confirmed by a medical exam by a physician. All of the participants were free of psychoactive medications at the time of the study, and none of the participants tested positive on a urine toxicology screen or alcohol breathalyzer on the day of scanning. Demographic and clinical characteristics are reported in Table 1. All participants provided written informed consent for this study, as approved by the local institutional review board.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

Patients (nλ=λ26) Controls (nλ=λ26) P valuea
λAge (years, mean±SD) 32.7±8.1 28.0±8.2 NS
Sex (n, male/female) 13/12 9/16 NS
Education (years, mean±SD) 15.6±1.6 15.7±1.4 NS
Social anxiety severityb (mean±SD) 77.3±18.35 15.6±11.9 1.52e-14
Trait anxiety levelc (mean±SD) 48.8±9.64 29.6±5.3 9.53e-9
Depression severityd (mean±SD) 10.3±6.23 1.9±2.6 1.24e-6

NS, nonsignificant.

a

Between-group comparisons by t-test or χ2, as appropriate.

b

Severity of social anxiety as measured by the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale.

c

Trait anxiety level as measured by the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.

d

Depression severity as measured by the Beck Depression Inventory.

Social-cognitive task

The fMRI task involved an event-related design. Participants were instructed that they are assigned to be ‘Decision Maker 1’ (DM1) in a ‘decision-making’ game. They were also told that they would be playing with other people (HUMAN partners) who had previously participated in the same game as ‘Decision Maker 2’ (DM2) and whose responses were previously recorded and now serve as DM2 ‘reactions’ to their (DM1’s) decisions. Participants were further instructed to imagine as if they were playing the game with DM2 partners in real-time. As DM1, the participant was informed of the task in the following way (Fig. 1): (i) for each trial, you are given 20 monetary units (to be converted into actual money after the end of the experiment); (ii) for each trial, you must make a decision to keep, and thus, equally divide the 20 units between yourself and your partner (KEEP) or to invest the 20 units (INVEST); (iii) if you choose to KEEP, then the actual outcome of this trial is complete (e.g. you will receive 10 units, and your partner will receive 10 units); (iv) if you choose to INVEST, then the amount doubles to 40 monetary units, and the actual outcome of the trial is to be decided by DM2, who can decide to split the money equally with you (e.g. you will receive 20 units, and DM2 will receive 20 units) or to keep the entire amount (e.g. you will receive 0 units, and DM2 will receive 40 units) (SPLIT). Participants were told that they would play with three types of DM2 players who were classified based on their previously recorded actions as: (i) type 1: ‘tended to split the money more than 50% of the time’; (ii) type 2: ‘tended to split the money about 50% of the time’; and (iii) type 3: ‘tended to split the money less than 50% of the time.’ They were also told that they were playing with a computer partner (COMPUTER) who ‘split the money 50% of the time.’ Unbeknownst to the participants, the frequency to ‘split’ (e.g. reciprocate) an investment was actually fixed at the following frequencies: (i) type 1λ=λ75%; (ii) type 2λ=λ50%; (iii) type 3λ=λ25%. To maximize economic gain, participants had to decode and predict DM2’s actions (e.g. operationalized as the likelihood that DM2 would reciprocate), and thus engage mentalizing.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic representation of the trust game. (a) Choice options and potential outcomes for Decision Maker 1 (DM1). (b) Exemplar trial structure with probe during decision phase and four potential outcomes based on participant (DM1) choice.

At the start of the trial (Fig. 1), participants viewed three different face photographs representing each HUMAN DM2 type but the ‘face’ was obscured by a colored oval (purple, yellow, brown) so as to convey a sense of anonymity and to obviate neural responses to facial features, or they viewed an image of a computer screen (COMPUTER). The DM2 image appeared for 4λs during which the participants were instructed to make their choice (KEEP or INVEST) by button press. In real-time and based on the participant’s own decision/choice, feedback was provided immediately in the form of a DM2 image reappearing for 2λs along with information about the participant’s choice, and the DM2’s actual (in instances of INVEST) or hypothetical (in instances of KEEP) response. Each trial was separated by an intertrial interval (blank grey-scale screen with fixation cross-hair), jittered from 0 to 12λs. There were a total of 80 trials equally representing the four DM2 types, which were semirandomly presented and distributed evenly across four fMRI runs. After the experimental session was complete, participants were paid according to the actual outcomes during the fMRI trust game.

Behavioral analysis

We analyzed the data pertaining to the choice [frequency (i.e. % of trials) INVEST] the participants made during fMRI scanning to confirm the manipulations of DM2 split frequencies would affect participants’ behavior as intended; in contrast, it was expected that over time, participants would accurately associate a DM2 type with the corresponding split frequency and modify their INVEST versus SPLIT choice accordingly. We conducted a repeated-measures analysis of variance using group (patients, controls) as the between-subject factor and the four types of DM2 as the within-subject factor; significant main effects and interactions were clarified by t-tests.

Neuroimaging

All scanning was performed with BOLD-sensitive whole-brain fMRI on a 3.0 Tesla GE Signa System (General Electric; Milwaukee, Wisconsin, USA) using a standard radiofrequency coil and associated software (LX 8.3, Neuro-optimized gradients). Whole-brain functional scans were acquired using a T2*-weighted reverse spiral sequence (echo timeλ=λ25λms, repetition timeλ=λ2000λms, 64 × 64 matrix, flip angleλ=λ77°, field of viewλ=λ24λcm, 30 contiguous 5λmm axial slices per volume, aligned with the anterior commissure posterior commissure line). A high-resolution T1 scan (3D-MPRAGE; repetition timeλ=λ25λms; min echo time; field of viewλ=λ24λcm; slice thicknessλ=λ1.5λmm) was also acquired.

Neuroimaging analysis

Data from all 52 participants met criteria for high quality and scan stability with minimum motion correction (<1λmm displacement in any one direction) and were subsequently included in the data analyses. The first four volumes from each run were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. Preprocessing steps, implemented using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM2; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) were as follows: (i) spatial realignment; (ii) normalization to the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) template through the use of nonlinear warping algorithm; (iii) spatial smoothing through the use of a Gaussian 8λmm full-width-half-maximum kernel. Statistical analyses were performed at the individual and group level using the general linear model and Gaussian random fields theory as implemented in SPM2 for event-related fMRI analyses [19] with covariates representing each DM2 type ‘event’ (three HUMAN DM2s, one COMPUTER DM2). In the first-level analysis, covariates were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function, using the temporal derivative to account for intersubject variability in BOLD signal time to peak. In the second-level analysis, we conducted a random-effects comparison of the linear contrast between HUMAN and COMPUTER trials within and between the two groups (patients, controls). Statistical maps were first created using a threshold of Puncorrected less than 0.005 with a cluster volume of at least 120λmm3, consistent with earlier studies of mentalizing/impression formation [10,20] and the trust game with fictive partners [7,11]. As this was the first study of this kind, we set a liberal significance threshold to maximize sensitivity for group differences in the whole-brain search; to obviate bias, we report all activations surviving this threshold. These statistical maps were superimposed on normalized high-resolution T1-weighted images and their locations interpreted by using known neuroanatomical landmarks with the brain atlas by Tzourio-Mazoyer and colleagues [21].

Results

Behavioral results

The behavioral results confirmed that the reciprocity manipulation (of DM2’s split frequency) as implemented in this modified multiround trust game affected participants’ behavior, and did so similarly in both control and social anxiety groups. There was a significant main effect of DM2 partner type on frequency (% of trials) of INVEST decisions showing that participants in both groups chose to INVEST rather than KEEP more frequently when playing against DM2 types who exhibited a higher likelihood of reciprocation [main effect of DM2 type: F(3,150)λ=λ55.71, P<0.001]; subsequent t-tests revealed that percentage of INVEST trials had the following pattern (type 1>type 2λ=λCOMPUTER>type 3; all P<0.05) (Fig. 2). However, the main effect of group and group × DM2 type interactions were not significant (all P>0.50) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Bar graph showing percent of trials (mean±SEM) in which participants made a decision to invest across the four different partner types (three HUMAN partners, one COMPUTER partner) for social anxiety and healthy control groups. *P<0.05.

Neuroimaging results

In control participants, the contrast of HUMAN DM2s (relative to COMPUTER DM2) activated a diverse network including dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, right inferior frontal gyrus, calcarine cortex, inferior and middle occipital gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, putamen (ventral striatum), and postcentral gyrus (Table 2). In social anxiety participants, a similar, though less distributed, set of regions was activated including calcarine cortex, inferior occipital gyrus, fusiform gyrus, and hippocampus (Table 2). Notably, there was an absence of engagement of medial prefrontal cortex in social anxiety patients (Fig. 3). The difference in medial prefrontal cortex activation to HUMAN (>COMPUTER) partners was confirmed by between-group comparisons (controls>patients) (Fig. 3). To ensure that this group difference was not driven by differential activation to COMPUTER partners between the control and patient groups, we compared activations to the COMPUTER partner (>fixation) across the two groups, and observed no group differences in medial prefrontal cortex.

Table 2.

Activation foci in response to HUMAN (>COMPUTER) partners within each group and differences between groups

Group and brain region Cluster size (mm3) MNI coordinates (x,y,z) Z-score
λControls
νCalcarine 10λ600 0, −80, 0 5.62
νInferior occipital gyrus 2632 −46, −80, −6 5.47
νInferior occipital gyrus 1464 46, −80, −4 4.90
νInferior frontal gyrus 176 50, 26, 28 3.91
νMiddle temporal gyrus 408 48, −70, 8 3.83
νPutamen 120 −14, 10, −8 3.50
νPostcentral gyrus 160 −64, −2, 20 3.28
νMiddle occipital gyrus 232 −24, −86, 12 3.15
νSuperior medial frontal gyrus 120 8, 52, 24 2.91
Patients
νCalcarine 8376 0, −82, −2 5.20
νInferior occipital gyrus 2000 48, −76, −8 4.39
νFusiform gyrus 472 42, −48, −24 4.16
νInferior occipital gyrus 984 −46, −78, −4 4.09
νHippocampus 128 −16, −14, −12 2.92
Controls>patients
νPostcentral gyrus 920 52, −14, 24 3.54
νMiddle occipital gyrus 216 −24, −86, 12 3.43
νSuperior medial frontal gyrus 200 10, 50, 22 3.12
νCuneus 144 −12, −84, 28 3.08
νInferior frontal gyrus 128 48, 6, 16 2.87
νInferior frontal gyrus 144 52, 14, 22 2.78
Patients>controls
νSupplementary motor area 312 −8, 18, 74 3.56
νSupramarginal gyrus 128 −62, −52, 32 3.41
νMiddle frontal gyrus 120 44, 44, 32 2.91

MNI, Montreal Neurologic Institute.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Group differences in brain response during mentalizing. Statistical t-maps (P<0.005) rendered on a canonical brain image and bar graph of extracted BOLD response showing less activation to HUMAN (>COMPUTER) partners in social anxiety patients compared with healthy controls in medial prefrontal cortex. *P<0.05. a.u., arbitrary units.

To further clarify the group differences noted above, we extracted parameter estimates [β weights (units) of the amplitude of the BOLD fMRI response] from each participant for the main contrast (HUMAN vs. COMPUTER) from a 10λmm sphere surrounding the voxel of peak activation in the group difference map within the medial prefrontal cortex [MNI coordinates: (10,22,50)] using the Plot Function in SPM2. Extracted BOLD response from these regions showed that the control group activated medial prefrontal cortex, whereas the patient group exhibited ‘deactivations’ in this area (mean β±SEM: medial prefrontal cortex, control: 0.128±0.046; patient: −0.084±0.053; tλ=λ3.02; Pλ=λ0.004, two-tailed) (Fig. 3).

Discussion

To begin to test a brain-based model of social-cognitive dysfunction in generalized social anxiety, this study probed the neural correlates of mentalizing during the trust game in patients with social anxiety and healthy controls. The online invest decisions made by participants during this multiround trust game confirmed our intended experimental manipulation involving three different person partner types, with differing motives/intentions as operationalized as their likelihood of reciprocation (‘SPLIT’). At the behavioral level, we did not observe group differences in INVEST frequency across the three different HUMAN partner types. At the brain level, the groups did differ in their response to HUMAN partners versus a COMPUTER partner. Unlike healthy controls, individuals with social anxiety failed to activate the medial prefrontal cortex, a key region engaged by mentalizing and impression formation of other people. These findings add to our understanding of current neurobiological models of social anxiety that implicate a social threat-sensitive amygdala and expand the model to include the medial prefrontal cortex in the context of mentalizing and social cognition. More generally, these results show that social neuroeconomic approaches that use tasks derived from game theory can be used to better reflect the dynamic nature of social interactions, and can illuminate the neural basis of social impairments observed in a number of mental illnesses (e.g. autism, schizophrenia, personality disorders) [22,23].

Our finding of activation in medial prefrontal cortex in control participants during interactions with HUMAN (>COMPUTER) partners is consistent with earlier studies that reliably activated medial prefrontal cortex in neuroeconomic exchange games similar to the one used in this study, in which participants interact with the same social partners over time and use information about partners’ prior actions to learn about their motives, intentions, and reputations [7,10]. The dorsal regions of the medial wall prefrontal cortex is increasingly understood to be a central node in the brain’s mentalizing network with processing in this region uniquely specialized for storing and reasoning about desires, intentions, and (potentially false) beliefs of others [12,13,17]. Consistent with this view, several fMRI studies that attempt to selectively engage ‘pure’ mental state attributions without engaging processing of faces (similar to our approach here), eye gaze or bodily motion have specifically found activation in medial prefrontal cortex as opposed to other regions in the brain’s mentalizing network [7,24]. Furthermore, many studies find that medial prefrontal cortex is preferentially engaged during conditions that require inferences about stable underlying character traits and behavioral dispositions compared with control conditions in which temporary intentions or goals must be inferred [16,20,25].

Conclusion

This is the first study to use game theory to model the neural activity associated with mentalizing in a simulated social exchange in participants with social anxiety. We found that compared with healthy controls, individuals with social anxiety exhibit impaired responses in medial prefrontal cortex, a region increasingly implicated by social neuroscience as part of the brain’s social-cognitive network, and particularly critical to mentalizing and forming impressions about others.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Kevin McCabe, John Cacioppo, and Dan Fitzgerald for discussions on the design of the trust game task used in this study, and Rose McCarron for participant recruitment and study coordination.

This study was supported, in part, by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grant MH076198 (to K.L.P.) and by the Brain Research Foundation (to K.L.P.). C.S.S. was supported by a NIMH Mental Health Research Education Grant R25MH063742 (to I.L.). This study was conducted at the University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, USA.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest: none of the authors report any biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest relevant to the subject matter of this study.

References

  • 1.Stein MB, Stein DJ. Social anxiety disorder. Lancet. 2008;371:1115–1125. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60488-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Schneier FR. Clinical practice. Social anxiety disorder. N Eng J Med. 2006;355:1029–1036. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp060145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hirsch CR, Clark DM. Information-processing bias in social phobia. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:799–825. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1476–1488. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fehr E, Camerer CF. Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:419–427. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Loewenstein G, Rick S, Cohen JD. Neuroeconomics. Ann Rev Psychol. 2008;59:647–672. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McCabe K, Houser D, Ryan L, Smith V, Trouard T. A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proc Natl Acad Sci U A. 2001;98:11832–11835. doi: 10.1073/pnas.211415698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.King-Casas B, Tomlin D, Anen C, Camerer CF, Quartz SR, Montague PR. Getting to know you: reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science (New York, NY) 2005;308:78–83. doi: 10.1126/science.1108062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science (New York, NY) 2003;300:1755–1758. doi: 10.1126/science.1082976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1694–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Delgado MR, Frank RH, Phelps EA. Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1611–1618. doi: 10.1038/nn1575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mitchell JP. Mentalizing and Marr: an information processing approach to the study of social cognition. Brain Res. 2006;1079:66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:77–83. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Frith CD, Frith U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron. 2006;50:531–534. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;M:M–M. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Krueger F, Grafman J, McCabe K. Neural correlates of economic game playing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond. 2008;363:3859–3874. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stopa L, Clark DM. Social phobia and interpretation of social events. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38:273–283. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00043-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes A, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses. NeuroImage. 1998;7:30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mitchell JP, Neil Macrae C, Banaji MR. Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;26:251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.King-Casas B, Sharp C, Lomax-Bream L, Lohrenz T, Fonagy P, Montague PR. The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science. 2008;321:806–810. doi: 10.1126/science.1156902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Seres I, Unoka Z, Keri S. The broken trust and cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Neuroreport. 2009;20:388–392. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328324eb4d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gallagher HL, Jack AI, Roepstorff A, Frith CD. Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game. Neuroimage. 2002;16:814–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mitchell JP, Heatherton TF, Macrae CN. Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:15238–15243. doi: 10.1073/pnas.232395699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES