Abstract
Error detection and correction are essential components of motor skill learning. These processes have been well characterized in cognitive psychology using electroencephalography (EEG) to record an event-related potential (ERP) called error-related negativity (ERN). However, it is unclear whether this ERP component is sensitive to the magnitude of the error made in a sensorimotor adaptation task. In the present study, we tested the function of error-related activity in a visuomotor adaptation task. To examine whether error size is reflected in the ERP, two groups of participants adapted manual aiming movements to either a small (30°) or large (45°) rotation of the visual feedback display. Each participant's trials were sorted into large and small error trials using a median split to examine potential error magnitude waveform differences. We also examined these trial types at the early and late stages of adaptation. There were no group differences for the behavioral or neural measures; however, waveforms from large error trials were significantly different from small error trials. The waveforms also changed as a function of practice as early adaptation waveforms were larger than late adaptation waveforms. The observed ERP component reflected differences in error magnitude with the amount of activity corresponding to the size of the error. Movement monitoring potentials likely affected the frequency and time course of the waveform so that it did not resemble the typical ERN; however, error-related activity was still distinguishable. The present findings are discussed in terms of current theories of the ERN as well as skill acquisition.
INTRODUCTION
During skill acquisition, participants make much larger errors in the first few minutes of practice than even 20 min later with this change in performance commonly described through the power law of practice (cf. Newell and Rosenbloom 1981). Performance improves in a more incremental fashion on entering the middle stage of learning (Fitts and Posner 1967) and even more slowly later in learning (Fitts and Posner 1967; Karni and Sagi 1993; Karni et al. 1998; Puttemans et al. 2005). This division has also been described as a transition from a conscious mode, where strategies are utilized to enhance inexperienced performance, to an unconscious mode, where performance proceeds in an automatic fashion after learning has occurred [cf. control-based learning theory (COBALT)] (Willingham 1998). However, the separation of learning into distinct stages has been somewhat arbitrary in the literature, primarily reflecting the time course of the experiment at hand (Adams 1971; Fitts and Posner 1967; Karni et al. 1998; Puttemans et al. 2005; Schmidt 1976; Seidler et al. 2006; Willingham 1998). Focusing on the early and late stages has provided a useful framework for examining the sensorimotor adaptation process, given that the middle stage of learning is thought to be contaminated by components engaged during the early and late time points (Fitts and Posner 1967; Willingham 1998).
For over 100 years, visuomotor adaptation has been used as a model task to study how the CNS adapts to changes in the mapping between visual and motor representations (cf. Bock 1992; Seidler et al. 2006; von Helmholtz 1962; Welch et al. 1974). These studies typically involve reaching or throwing movements toward targets under normal and distorted visual feedback, with the largest off-target errors observed during the initial exposure to the altered mapping environment. Accuracy improves as the CNS adapts to the discrepant mapping (cf. Cunningham and Welch 1994), with adaptation scaling to the size of a previous error (Scheidt et al. 2001; Wolpert and Ghahramani 2000). Similarly, reinforcement learning theory proposes that the extent of learning is proportional to the magnitude of the predicted error (Barto 1994, 2003b; Sutton and Barto 1988). However, Fine and Thoroughman (2006, 2007) have demonstrated that, under certain task parameters, corrective actions are not related to error size.
Neuroimaging studies of sensorimotor adaptation have implicated several brain structures, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), in the adaptation process (Anguera et al. 2007; Graydon et al. 2005; Miall et al. 2001; Seidler et al. 2006). However, it is not clear what role this structure plays during sensorimotor adaptation. It has been suggested that the ACC serves as an active monitoring system that reacts to the presence of conflict and/or performance errors in cognitive tasks (Botvinick et al. 1999; Carter et al. 1998; Gehring and Knight 2000; Gehring et al. 1993; Yeung et al. 2004). In particular, the ACC is believed to be the origin of a medial frontal negative event-related potential (ERP) component called the error-related negativity (ERN) (Debener et al. 2005; Holroyd et al. 2004; Miltner et al. 1997). This component peaks shortly following an incorrect response in speeded response tasks (Falkenstein et al. 1990; Gehring et al. 1993) and following the presentation of error feedback in reinforcement learning tasks (Badgaiyan and Posner 1998; Holroyd and Coles 2002; Miltner et al. 1997).
ERN studies have been very useful in providing insight into the cognitive and neural mechanisms involved in performance monitoring as well as the adjustment of flexible, goal-directed behavior. A recent computational model suggested that larger ERN amplitudes early in learning should be associated with a greater amount of overall learning as a function of avoiding maladaptive responses (Frank et al. 2005). This relates very well to the reinforcement learning theories of motor learning as they also suggest a correlation between the amount of learning and the predicted error between the actual and intended movement (Barto 1994, 1997, 2003; Sutton and Barto 1988; Wolpert and Ghahramani 2000). In fact, the reinforcement learning theory of the ERN (Holroyd and Coles 2002) suggests that ERN amplitude is magnified when participants are first learning a stimulus-response mapping, and this amplitude diminishes as the appropriate mapping is learned. It is not clear, however, whether the ERN scales with the size of motor errors in a similar fashion.
The ERN has been primarily studied using basic reaction time tasks [e.g., the Eriksen Flanker task (Eriksen and Eriksen 1971)] in which errors are binary in nature (present or absent). This is in direct contrast to errors in motor learning tasks (e.g., reaching or aiming movements, force production) where errors can extend over time, range in magnitude, and reflect mistakes in goal attainment and/or response execution. There has been a recent surge of studies examining performance monitoring via the ERN during motor tasks (de Bruijn et al. 2003; Contreras-Vidal and Kerick 2004; Krigolson and Holroyd 2006, 2007a,b); however, these studies have failed to characterize how this ERP component relates to error size.
These ERN studies of motor tasks have consistently shown a fronto-central negative-polarity potential associated with goal attainment errors (“high-level” errors) (as defined by Krigolson and Holroyd 2006). This activity resulted when participants incorrectly responded with the wrong hand or amount of force (cf. de Bruijn et al. 2003), failed to reach a designated target (Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a; Krigolson et al. 2008), or failed to avoid a barrier in a motor tracking task (Krigolson and Holroyd 2006, 2007b). These types of errors are binary in nature (“correct or incorrect”) with corrections not possible following a response. However, corrections are possible for response execution errors (“low-level” errors) (as defined by Krigolson and Holroyd 2006); for example, correcting an initially erroneous trajectory to reach a desired target (Contreras-Vidal and Kerick 2004; Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a; Krigolson et al. 2008) or avoid an oncoming barrier (Krigolson and Holroyd 2006, 2007b). For these types of errors, Krigolson and Holroyd (2006) observed parietal activity without medial-frontal/ERN activity. In light of these findings, Krigolson and Holroyd (2006) proposed a hierarchical error processing theory in which the frontal error system evaluates only high-level errors, whereas the posterior error system responds only to low-level errors. Such results suggest that the ACC may only be providing a binary (present or absent) high-level error signal rather than processing detailed information about the size of motor errors (see also Diedrichsen et al. 2005). Testing this hypothesis is critical to understanding the role that this neural process plays in motor learning and adaptation.
In the present study, we determined whether motor errors made during visuomotor adaptation elicit an ERP component the activity of which corresponds to the size of the motor error. Two groups of individuals performed a visuomotor adaptation task using a joystick, with each group exposed to a differing amount of feedback rotation. This design permitted us to test whether sensorimotor adaptation under more perturbed conditions would be associated with a greater amount of activity. We also examined this activity after separating trials into large and small error magnitudes relative to median performance for each participant at early and late adaptation. These approaches allowed us to evaluate whether errors of differing sizes would be reflected in the ERP. Thus we were able to test two mutually exclusive hypotheses: the error signal simply provides a binary alert that an error has occurred, without regard to the size of the error or the error signal is relative to the size of the error made.
METHODS
Participants
Nineteen women and 21 men between the ages of 18 and 27 yr [mean: 22.8 ± 3.5 yr] participated in the study. All participants were recruited from the University of Michigan student population and were paid for their participation. Each signed an IRB-approved informed consent form and a health history questionnaire prior to their participation. All participants were strongly right-handed (0.88 ± 0.06), as assessed by using the 20-item version of the Edinburgh Inventory (Oldfield 1971). They were randomly assigned to one of two perturbation groups: a 30° feedback rotation group (20 participants; 10 men) and a 45° feedback rotation group (20 participants; 11 men). A between-group design was used to examine potential ERP differences for perturbations of different magnitudes as previous work using these rotations has induced group differences (cf. Seidler et al. 2006). We avoided using a within-subject design as exposing participants to multiple perturbations has been shown to contaminate the adaptation experience with participants using their previous experience to progress through the adaptation period at a faster rate (Seidler 2004).
Apparatus and procedure
The stimuli were presented on a 15-in NEC (Tokyo, Japan) Multisync computer monitor at a viewing distance of 60 cm, which was used to provide real-time movement feedback. Participants moved a dual potentiometer joystick that was fixed to a table with Velcro, placed within arm's reach. They were instructed to hold the joystick with their thumb and index finger (see Fig. 1).
The participants were instructed to use the joystick to position a cursor in a target circle on the screen. Targets (0.8 cm in diameter) appeared for 4 s in one of four locations: 4.8 cm to the right, left, above, or below the centrally located home position (0.8 cm in diameter). Participants were asked to move the cursor into the target circle as quickly (the velocity of the cursor was participant controlled) and accurately as possible and maintain the cursor within the circle until the target disappeared. On target disappearance, they were told to release the spring-loaded joystick handle, so that it would recenter for each subsequent trial. The next trial began 1 s later, resulting in an inter-trial interval (from 1 target presentation to the next) of 5 s.
The 30 and 45° rotation perturbations indicate a clockwise rotation of the cursor position about the central start location (cf. Cunningham and Welch 1994). When a participant in the 30° group moved toward a target at the top of the screen in the “12 o' clock” direction, they observed the cursor moving instead toward “1 o' clock” (see Fig. 1). Thus to reach a target in the 30° rotation condition, a participant would need to aim 30° counterclockwise of the target, or toward “11 o' clock.” Participants were not informed in advance as to whether the upcoming block was an adaptation or control block nor were they provided with any information about the applied rotations.
Participants performed 14 blocks (B) of the task (28 trials per block) with their right hand (see Table 1). Blocks 2–3 were considered to be the baseline (pre-test) stage for two reasons: the real-time feedback of the participants' movements was not altered during this period, and omitting the first block ensures that there was no task novelty effect present in the baseline measure. Blocks 4–5 were considered to be the early stage as these blocks encompassed the initial exposure to the visuomotor perturbation, and blocks 13–14 were the late stage. Both early and late stages were limited to having an equivalent number of blocks and trials to the baseline condition.
TABLE 1.
Block Number | Rotated Feedback | Condition |
---|---|---|
B1 | NO | Practice |
B2 | NO | Baseline |
B3 | NO | Baseline |
B4 | 30°/45° | Early adaptation |
B5 | 30°/45° | Early adaptation |
B6 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B7 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B8 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B9 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B10 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B11 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B12 | 30°/45° | Adaptation |
B13 | 30°/45° | Late adaptation |
B14 | 30°/45° | Late adaptation |
B, block; 30°/45°, adaptation blocks with corresponding rotated feedback.
Electrophysiological acquisition parameters
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 37 scalp electrode sites with sintered Ag/AgCl electrodes embedded in a nylon mesh cap (Easycap, Falk Minow Services). The electrode locations consisted of Fp1, Fp2, AFz, F7, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, F8, FT7, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, FT8, T7, C3, C1, Cz, C2, C4, T8, TP7, CP3, CPz, CP4, TP8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, Oz, and O2 (American Electroencephalographic Society 1991). EEG data were recorded with a left mastoid reference. An average mastoid reference was derived off-line using right mastoid data. The electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from Ag/AgCl electrodes above and below the right eye, the naison, and external to the outer canthus of each eye. A ground electrode was placed on the forehead. Impedances were kept <10 kΩ. The EEG and EOG were amplified by SYNAMPS amplifiers (Neuroscan), recorded from 0.01 to 100 Hz (half-amplitude cutoff), and the data were digitized at 1,000 Hz. To time-lock the initiation of each trial to the EEG recordings, a flashing oval (3 cm in diameter) was activated for 5 ms on the appearance of each target circle with this flash captured by a photocell on the right side of the screen (see Fig. 1). The photocell channel data were acquired by the Neuroscan system in parallel with the EEG data to ensure that signal synchronization was achieved. To prevent this flashing from distracting the participants, the right side of the screen where the photocell was located was shielded from their view.
Behavioral data processing
The x and y coordinates from the joystick were recorded at a rate of 100 Hz. We analyzed the joystick data off-line using custom Labview 6.1 software (National Instruments) to track behavioral changes with learning. We first filtered the data with a dual low-pass Butterworth digital filter (cf. Winter 1990), using a cutoff frequency of 10 Hz. The resultant joystick path was calculated by computing the square root of the sum of the squared x and y coordinate data at each time point. The tangential velocity profile was then calculated through differentiation. Movement onset and offset were computed through the application of Teasdale et al.'s (1993) optimal algorithm to the velocity profile for each movement. We used initial endpoint error (IEE), the distance from the end of the initial ballistic movement position to the target, to assess the extent of adaptation. This measure is calculated by finding the first time point of acceleration following the deceleration of the initial ballistic movement, which would reflect the phase of the movement associated with the initiation of the first corrective submovement (see Fig. 2 for illustration). We also calculated the corrective submovement distance (CSMD) traveled to assess whether there was a correlation between the IEE and corresponding corrective movement. The following temporal measures were also analyzed: reaction time (RT), primary submovement duration (PSD), and corrective duration time (CDT).
We used a group × block × trial mixed model ANOVA to compare performance between the two groups. Significant interactions were followed up with simple contrasts. The Huynh-Feldt epsilon (Huynh and Feldt 1970) was evaluated to determine whether the repeated-measures data met the assumption of sphericity (Σ > 0.75). The trials for each stage were then pooled together, creating a block pair for each period (e.g., blocks 2 and 3 = block pair 1). In doing this, we were able to utilize a greater number of trials (56 trials per stage) in the EEG waveform averages; ≥46 trials went into each mean bin for the ERP analysis for each subject. Trials within each block pair for each participant were sorted using a median split of IEE values, resulting in large and small error trials.
EEG data reduction
After recording, the EEG data were digitally low-pass filtered with a half-amplitude cutoff point at 50 Hz (24 dB/octave roll-off) and reduced to a sampling rate of 200 Hz. Data were corrected for eye movement artifacts using the method designed by Gratton et al. (1983). Statistical analyses used data filtered at the 50-Hz cutoff, referenced to the average of the mastoid electrodes. EEG data were averaged off-line for epochs of 1,000 ms, using the photoreceptor flash time stamp as a starting point each trial. Each epoch was then time-locked to each participant's movement onset based on their RT and the time point when an initial corrective submovement began. For the RT time-locked waveforms, the baseline chosen was 200 ms prior to movement onset,1 while the IEE used a baseline 200 ms prior to the initiation of a corrective submovement. We quantified the ERP waveforms recorded using mean amplitude measures in three time windows: 200–400, 400–600, and 600–800 ms. While the 600- to 800-ms time window inclusion is much later than most studies examining response monitoring effects, the time course of error correction for a VMA task would typically require this long duration to ensure capturing a complete corrective submovement(s). Large and small error trials determined via the median split for each participant at each stage were then compared within each defined time window. We focused our statistical ERP analyses on activity recorded at the FCz electrode as previous ERN research has shown the greatest effect at this electrode. The data shown in the figures were generated using a 16-Hz low-pass Butterworth filter (Matlab 7.1; Mathworks, Natick, MA).
RESULTS
ERP data from two participants (1 in each group) were lost due to technical problems and were excluded from the analyses. Thus the behavioral results are presented for twenty participants in each group, while the ERP results are presented for nineteen participants in each group (10 males for each group).
Behavioral
INITIAL ENDPOINT ERROR AND CORRECTIVE SUBMOVEMENT DISTANCE.
Sample spatial trajectories for a single participant (under 30° rotation) are depicted in Fig. 2 at the early stages of adaptation (A) and the final adaptation block pair (B). Adaptation to the rotated feedback is seen in the less distorted trajectories employed in reaching the targets in B versus those in A.
Figure 3 illustrates the performance for each group by block for IEE. Note that an IEE score of ≤4 mm (the radius of the target circle) indicates that the initial ballistic movement ended within the target circle; thus a corrective submovement was not required. However, participants still made corrections to movements that were inside the target circle to have the final endpoint as close to the center as possible. This design allowed for a comparison of across-error magnitudes without categorizing each trial in a binary fashion as “correct” versus “incorrect.” A repeated-measures ANOVA (RM ANOVA) with block (14) and trial (28) resulted in significant main effects for block [F(13,494) = 31.31, P < 0.0001, partial eta squared (ηp2) = 0.45], so follow-up tests were conducted. There was no difference between groups for the baseline block pair [B2–B3; F(1,38) = 0.51, P > 0.40, ηp2 = 0.013], the early adaptation block pair [B4–B5; F(1,38) = 2.30, P > 0.10, ηp2 = 0.06], or the late adaptation block pair [B13–B14; F(1,38) = 1.58, P = 0.22, ηp2 = 0.04]. There were no group differences for any of the subsequent analyses that were performed; therefore we omitted the group variable from our analysis. The rotated feedback impaired accuracy of movements, as observed through performance differences between the baseline and early adaptation block pair [F(1,39) = 134.70, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.78]. Participants adapted to the rotated feedback as evidenced by the difference between the early block pair versus the late block pair [F(1,39) = 108.82, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.74]. The pooled late block pair was significantly different from the baseline block pair [F(1,39) = 13.33, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.26], which reflects that performance did not reach baseline levels by the end of practice.
Each group showed impaired movement accuracy when the rotation was first introduced during the early stage, followed by improvements in accuracy following practice at the late stage. As there were no differences in error magnitude between the two groups at these stages, we pooled their data and sorted it into large and small error trials based on a median split of the data for each block pair. Following the differentiation of each participant's trials using the median split into large and small error trials for all blocks, we observed a significant within group difference for IEE between large and small trial errors for each group [30°: F(2,119) = 42.34, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.54; 45°: F(2,119) = 39.27, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.51]. Consistent with previous between group analyses, there was no difference between groups for a group × block × error type ANOVA [F(1,38) = 0.54, P > 0.80, ηp2 < 0.01; see Fig. 4]. There was a significant correlation between IEE and the corresponding CSMD traveled at each period (baseline: r = 0.39, early: r = 0.29, late: r = 0.48, r > 0.27 = P < 0.05).
Temporal measures
The temporal performance variables (RT, PSD, and CDT) are presented in Table 2. At the early adaptation stage, both groups were equivalent for RT [F(1,38) = 0.05, P > 0.80, ηp2 < 0.01] and PSD [F(1,38) = 2.41, P > 0.10, ηp2 = 0.06]; however, there was a group difference for CDT [F(1,38) = 7.09, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.16] with the 45° group taking more time correcting their initial movement. Initial exposure to the rotated feedback impaired both the accuracy of movements as well as the duration of the movement made, as the participants' RT [F(1,38) = 18.97, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.33], PSD [F(1,38) = 36.06, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.49], and CDT [F(1,38) = 14.85, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.28] were longer for the early block pair compared with baseline. There was no group difference here for RT [F(1,38) = 0.63, P > 0.40, ηp2 = 0.02] or PSD [F(1,38) = 0.48, P > = 0.45, ηp2 < 0.01], but there was for CDT [F(1,38) = 5.66, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.13] as the 45° group took more time in implementing their corrections. The participants improved with practice as evidenced by the temporal improvements between the early and late block pair for RT [F(1,38) = 15.25, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.29], PSD [F(1,38) = 14.91, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.28], and CDT [F(1,38) = 15.29, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.29]. Again, a group difference was only apparent for CDT [F(1,38) = 8.12, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.18].
TABLE 2.
Reaction Time, ms | Primary Submovement Duration, ms | Corrective Submovement Duration, ms | |
---|---|---|---|
Baseline | 286 ± 56 | 439 ± 72 | 339 ± 139 |
Early | 388 ± 124 | 479 ± 73 | 442 ± 125 |
Late | 345 ± 94 | 518 ± 100 | 348 ± 113 |
Values are means ± SD.
There was no group difference across the late stage as participants took an equivalent amount of time for RT [F(1,38) = 1.16, P > 0.25, ηp2 = 0.03], PSD [F(1,38) = 0.14, P > 0.70, ηp2 < 0.01], and CDT [F(1,38) = 1.91, P > 0.15, ηp2 = 0.05]. The pooled late stage (B13–B14) showed a significant difference from the baseline blocks for PSD [F(1,38) = 81.38, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.68] but not for RT [F(1,38) = 3.06, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08] or CDT [F(1,38) = 0.35, P > 0.50, ηp2 < 0.01]. Again, there was no group difference between these stages for RT [F(1,38) = 0.15, P > 0.70, ηp2 < 0.01], PSD [F(1,38) = 0.03, P > 0.80, ηp2 < 0.01], or CDT [F(1,38) = 2.72, P > 0.10, ηp2 = 0.07].
Electrophysiological data
We chose to perform all ERP statistical analyses on activity recorded at the FCz electrode because this experiment was motivated by studies of the ERN, which is typically found at the 10–20 location FCz. We did not observe any differences between the two groups for any of the ERP comparisons that follow; therefore we omitted that variable from our analysis. A 2 (error magnitude, large vs. small) × 3 (stage: baseline, early, late) × 3 (time window: 200–400, 400–600, 600–800 ms) RM ANOVA of ERPs time-locked to movement onset failed to find a main effect of error magnitude [F (1,37) = 0.04, P > 0.80, ηp2 < 0.01] or stage [F(2,74) = 0.37, P > 0.60, ηp2 = 0.01] but did show a main effect of time window [F(2,74) = 56.70, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.61] and a time window × stage interaction [F(4,148) = 13.00, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.26]. Visual inspection of these data highlight these results as large and small error waveforms pooled across all adaptation periods did not differ (Fig. 4; see Table 3 for mean ERP amplitudes for each stage). These findings did not change when a prestimulus baseline (200-ms time window) was used instead (see Supplementary Materials,2 Fig. S1). Therefore we continued our analyses examining the time-locked ERP recordings at the onset of the corrective submovement.
TABLE 3.
Large Errors | Small Errors | |
---|---|---|
RT time-locked | ||
Baseline | ||
200–400ms | −5.99 ± 0.75 | −5.65 ± 0.72 |
400–600ms | −9.24 ± 0.75 | −9.55 ± 0.77 |
600–800ms | −7.94 ± 0.68 | −7.71 ± 0.81 |
Early | ||
200–400ms | −3.63 ± 0.90 | −3.57 ± 0.80 |
400–600ms | −8.54 ± 0.88 | −8.58 ± 0.79 |
600–800ms | −9.26 ± 0.86 | −10.42 ± 0.83 |
Late | ||
200–400ms | −5.48 ± 0.71 | −5.93 ± 0.68 |
400–600ms | −9.25 ± 0.87 | −9.16 ± 0.83 |
600–800ms | −8.89 ± 0.93 | −8.23 ± 0.86 |
IEE time-locked | ||
Baseline | ||
200–400ms | −3.89 ± 0.68 | −2.99 ± 0.79 |
400–600ms | −2.36 ± 0.69 | −1.03 ± 0.88 |
600–800ms | −0.25 ± 0.61 | 1.58 ± 0.86 |
Early | ||
200–400ms | −6.40 ± 0.61 | −5.62 ± 0.74 |
400–600ms | −7.01 ± 0.82 | −5.54 ± 0.96 |
600–800ms | −5.79 ± 0.88 | −4.25 ± 0.94 |
Late | ||
200–400ms | −3.51 ± 0.59 | −1.75 ± 0.68 |
400–600ms | −2.35 ± 0.80 | 0.32 ± 0.85 |
600–800ms | −0.88 ± 0.81 | 2.49 ± 0.82 |
Values are means±SE. RT, reaction time; ERP, event-related potential; IEE, initial endpoint error.
We were interested in determining whether the rotated feedback led to increased ERN amplitude by comparing the early adaptation stage relative to baseline, how ERN amplitude changed as adaptation progressed from the early to late stage, and whether, regardless of stage, errors of large magnitude produced greater ERN activity than small errors. A 2 (error magnitude, large vs. small) × 3 (stage: baseline, early, late) × 3 (time window: 200–400, 400–600, 600–800 ms) RM ANOVA resulted in a main effect of time window [F(2,74) = 47.65, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.56], a main effect of stage [F(2,74) = 28.08, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.43], a main effect of error magnitude [F(1,37) = 18.07 P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.33], a time window × stage interaction [F(4,148) = 17.20, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.32], and a time window × error magnitude interaction [F(2,74) = 11.78, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.24]. High and low error waveforms pooled across all stages were significantly different when time-locked to the initiation of the corrective submovement (Fig. 5; see Table 3 for mean ERP amplitudes for each learning period). In the following text we explore these main effects and interactions using simple effects tests in the context of the issues described in the preceding text.
Overall error magnitude
The ANOVA comparing the pre test, early, and late stages showed a main effect of error magnitude[F(1,37) = 18.07, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.33] but no interaction between stage and error magnitude [F(2,74) = 1.57, P > 0.20, ηp2 = 0.04; see Fig. 5]. The waveforms suggest that the difference between conditions grows throughout the epoch, which was confirmed by a time window by error magnitude interaction [F(2,74) = 11.78, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.24].
Early versus baseline
The waveforms in Fig. 6 suggest that the perturbation caused an increase in the ERP amplitude, seen as a difference in the ERP amplitude in the early adaptation versus baseline comparison. A repeated-measures ANOVA involving only the early and baseline waveforms showed a greater negativity in the early adaptation waveforms than in the baseline period, [F (1,37) = 52.39, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.59] and a time window × stage interaction [F(2,74) = 24.65, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.40]. Inspection of the waveforms suggests that the early versus baseline difference grows larger in the later time windows, and indeed a simple effects test showed that the difference was larger in the late time window than in the early one [F(1,37) = 34.43, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.48]. Topographic distribution of activity selective to large and small error magnitudes for early adaptation between 0 and 600 ms is shown in Fig. 7, illustrating a broad negativity encompassing the FCz electrode.
Progression of adaptation
The waveforms in Fig. 6 suggest that the negativity in the early stage was greater in amplitude than in the late stage. A repeated-measures ANOVA involving only the early and late waveforms confirmed a larger negativity in early than in late waveforms [F(1,37) = 40.75, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.52]. An interaction was also present between time window and stage [F(2,74) = 19.40, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.34], reflecting a shift toward positive polarity late in the epoch. There was also an error magnitude × time window interaction [F(2,74) = 7.56, P < 0.005, ηp2 = 0.17]. Mean ERP amplitudes for each block pair across the entire adaptation period showed a significant linear fit [F(1,37) = 31.59, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.46]. A separate ANOVA comparing baseline and late waveforms failed to find a significant difference [F(1,37) = 0.517, P > 0.40, ηp2 = 0.01]. A partial correlation controlling for the CSMD between IEE and mean ERP amplitude time-locked to the initiation of the corrective submovement was not significant at any stage (baseline: r = 0.15, early: r = 0.05, late: r = −0.13; r critical for P < 0.05, r > 0.32).
DISCUSSION
In the present study, we observed ERP waveforms associated with motor errors. While waveforms time-locked to movement onset did not distinguish between large and small errors, those time-locked to the onset of a corrective submovement did. There was a significant difference between these correction time-locked waveforms when all stages were collapsed, as well as distinctive waveforms at specific stages of the adaptation process. These findings support the idea that this signal is sensitive to differences in error magnitude and is modulated as a function of practice.
Large versus small errors: effects of error size
When all stages and time windows were collapsed, there was a significant difference between large and small error IEE time-locked waveforms. These results agree with reinforcement learning theories suggesting that the magnitude of the difference between the predicted and actual outcome is reflected by a corresponding amount of neural activity in the medial-frontal cortex (Barto 1994; Frank et al. 2005; Holroyd and Coles 2002). In addition, these findings are inconsistent with the proposed idea of neural activation being insensitive to error magnitude (Fine and Thoroughman 2006, 2007), although we cannot rule out the possibility of this being a condition-specific effect. Krigolson and Holroyd (2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008) have demonstrated that while motor execution errors (low-level) do not elicit medial frontal activation, errors in goal attainment (high-level) do. The present findings demonstrate, however, that this is not the case. That is, we observed medial-frontal activity for low-level errors (correctable deviant trajectories). All errors in the present study were low level with a trial duration (4,000 ms) that provided participants more than enough time to initiate a movement, make corrections, and reach the target circle. With this methodological difference, comparing large errors in the present study to high-level errors found by Krigolson and Holroyd (2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008) cannot be tested here. Similarly, this trial duration prevented the comparison with correct versus high-level endpoint-locked waveforms (cf. Krigolson and Holroyd 2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008), as participants had accurately reached the target circle at this time. However, the present findings can be compared with their low-level errors as they are of the same nature (correctable trajectories) and show a similar CDT [471 ms (Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a) vs. 479 ms here].
There are other methodological differences which potentially explain the discrepancies between our findings and those of Krigolson and Holroyd (2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008). In the present study, we specifically examined how error-related waveforms scaled with the size of errors, whereas Krigolson and Holroyd (2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008) utilized the binary measure of reaching a target (Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a; Krigolson et al. 2008) or erroneously hitting a barrier during a tracking task (Krigolson and Holroyd 2006). However, we believe that both high- and low-level errors are present at each stage in our study, as discrepant trajectories led to difficulty in reaching the target. We suggest that the distinct, slow moving negative waveforms observed here reflect the ongoing evaluation and correction of the erroneous trajectory during the corrective submovement(s) following the initial ballistic movement.
Another key difference from the present study is that Krigolson and Holroyd (2006, 2007a) and Krigolson et al. (2008) did not allow participants to make corrective actions (they had no control over the cursor for part of the trial) on 50% of the trials in which they made a low-level trajectory error, forcing a high-level goal attainment error. Although participants did not report awareness of a loss of control, their overall strategy and corresponding neural activity may have changed for the task given the presence/absence of corrective submovements. It is common for participants to make one or more of these submovements during manual aiming tasks (see Fig. 2B) (also see Meyer et al. 1988). If, as our data suggest, medial-frontal activation does not appear to be solely tied to high-level errors, one would expect similar, if not greater, negativity during correctable trials as at least some of the submovements are imprecise (Meyer et al. 1988). In fact, Krigolson and Holroyd (2006) reported greater negativity for the correctable ERP compared with the uncorrectable condition. Furthermore, Krigolson and Holroyd (2007a) required the use of a difference waveform to isolate the ERN. It is not clear, however, that the peak in the difference waveform was caused by the ERN; the individual condition waveforms suggest that a positive-polarity peak on correct trials is a likely source of the apparent negative peak in the difference waveform.
Practice effects
There were significant changes in performance with practice with participants reducing errors from the early to late adaptation stage, and early adaptation waveforms being larger than baseline or late learning waveforms. This suggests that more error monitoring may have been occurring when performance was perturbed, and this monitoring led to more pronounced differences between these stages as time passed. Several fMRI studies have demonstrated differential involvement of the ACC depending on the stage of learning one is at (Bush et al. 1998; Jenkins et al. 1994; Raichle et al. 1994; Toni and Passingham 1999). In these studies, the ACC showed significant activation during the earliest stages of the task with a diminishing contribution as learning progressed. This pattern of activation at the ACC has also been observed with a visuomotor adaptation paradigm in an fMRI (Anguera et al. 2007) and ERP study (Contreras-Vidal and Kerick 2004).
Holroyd and Coles (2002; p. 694) suggested that the change in activity at the ACC as a function of practice reflects the “first indication that the consequences of an action are worse than expected.” These authors propose that activation originating from the ACC acts as a training signal that serves to adjust control of the motor system in subsequent trials. In the present study, this interpretation is supported by the waveform differences between large and small error magnitudes at early adaptation as a larger error would require a more robust training signal to adjust control in an effective manner. The present findings suggest that the signal does not reflect error size relative to the stage of adaptation but instead reflects the relative size of the error. While the present findings do show scaling across two points (large and small), a more precise understanding of this scaling would be facilitated by the inclusion of “medium-”sized errors into the analysis. We did not pursue this approach in the current study as it would have lead to an insufficient number of trials for each trial type (∼18 per block pair) for ERP averaging.
One could argue that the observed ERP varies with the magnitude of the movement to be initiated and not with the error committed, especially considering the observed influence of the MMP. This interpretation cannot be completely overlooked given the correlation between IEE and the corrective submovement distance at each stage. However, the partial correlation controlling for CSMD between IEE and ERP amplitude at each stage was not significant, suggesting that the ERP amplitude is not reflecting a greater distance to be traveled for a given error. This finding further supports the role of error monitoring processes in the observed ERP.
ERN and error processing
As the present study was motivated by the use of ERN to distinguish between error types during a visuomotor adaptation task, we will consider our results in the context of the three best known theories of response ERN production: error detection theory, reinforcement learning theory, and conflict monitoring theory. The error-detection theory proposes that the ERN reflects a process that compares motor system output (via an efferent copy of the movement command) to the best estimate of the correct response at the time of the ERN occurrence (Falkenstein et al. 1991a; Gehring et al. 1993). The ERN could then reflect the output of the comparison process (Coles et al. 2001; Falkenstein 1990), the comparison process itself (Falkenstein et al. 2000; Vidal et al. 2000), or a process that uses the error signal to prevent or correct the error, possibly through a strategic adjustment (Gehring et al. 1993; Holroyd and Coles 2002). The reinforcement-learning theory of the ERN (Holroyd and Coles 2002) suggests that ERN amplitude reflects a generic error processing system that incorporates both error-related feedback as well as an efference copy of the given motor command. This theory suggests that ERN amplitude is determined by a signal generated by the basal ganglia, which is then carried by the midbrain dopamine system to the ACC. Finally, the response-conflict monitoring theory proposes ERN amplitude reflects monitoring for conflict between incompatible responses (Botvinick et al. 2001; Yeung et al. 2004). Under this theory, the function of the ERN is to relay this information to neural regions involved in cognitive control, like the prefrontal cortex (Botvinick et al. 2001; Yeung et al. 2004).
In agreement with the error detection theory, greater negativity was associated with a greater disparity between the best estimate of a correct movement and the compared efferent copy. Similarly, larger performance errors (or larger prediction errors between the expected and actual outcome) corresponded with increased negativity, which would agree with reinforcement-learning theory. This would also be the case for the response conflict theory, as the competition between the initial response and the error correction would presumably lead to greater negativity for large error trials. Thus the present findings are consistent with each theory but do not clearly differentiate between them.
Interestingly, the medial frontal negativity recorded from a visuomotor adaptation ERP study attributed this activity to the acquisition and modification of an internal model of the visuomotor representation to the perturbed environment (Contreras-Vidal and Kerick 2004). Other ERN studies have also credited the medial frontal activity to the comparison of predicted and intended responses (Rodrguez-Fornells et al. 2002; Swick and Turken 2002), which is consistent with the role of feedback for internal model theory. We propose that the frontal–central negativity in the present study serves a performance monitoring function during the motor adaptation process (cf. Botvinick et al. 2001; Carter et al. 1998; Holroyd and Coles 2002). However, further research is warranted to isolate the role of the ERN and its potential involvement in the modification of the internal model of the task.
Do the waveforms reflect ERN activity?
Although the locus of the negativity in the present study cannot be completely attributed to the ACC, we have previously observed ACC activation in an fMRI study of this same task (Anguera et al. 2007). Likewise, the observed scalp distribution is similar to that of ERN, which previous studies have linked to the ACC (Carter et al. 1998; Herrmann et al. 2004; Miltner et al. 1997). While the present findings fit with each of the response-based theories of the ERN, they are also in line with a related component, the feedback-related negativity (FRN). FRN occurs following a feedback stimulus long after a given response (Badgaiyan and Posner 1998; Gehring and Willoughby 2002; Holroyd and Coles 2002; Miltner et al. 1997; Nieuwenhuis et al. 2004a) with greater amplitude following negative, unexpected feedback (Cohen et al. 2007; Hajcak et al. 2007; Holroyd et al. 2003; Potts et al. 2006).
The present findings would also agree with the FRN interpretation, as the unexpected change in mapping would correspond with greater negativity, while at late learning this change was more expected. However, recent studies suggest that this component covaries with outcome in a binary fashion (good or bad) without any gradation in outcome value (Hajcak et al. 2006; Holroyd et al. 2004, 2006; Nieuwenhuis et al. 2004b; Yeung and Sanfey 2004; but see Goyer et al. 2008). This is in contrast to the present findings, suggesting that this component may not play as prominent a role as the “classic” response-based ERN in this study. Nevertheless, although there is some similarity between the activity observed in the present study and the ERN, additional work is needed to determine whether they represent the same component or are instead two distinct ERP components.
One might question whether the pronounced, slow moving negative waveforms observed in the present study might be better explained by another component, as the shape and time course of the present waveforms do not resemble those of the ERN observed in more typical speeded button-press tasks. One possibility would be movement-monitoring potentials (MMPs) (Chiang et al. 2004; Grunewald-Zuberbier and Grunewald 1978; Slobounov et al. 1998, 2000, 2002), which are slow-moving negative potentials associated with preparation and execution of an action. The time course of this potential spans several seconds, with a rising negativity ∼1,500 ms before movement onset, and a much larger ERP amplitude than the ERN that can persist for well over 1,000 ms (cf. Chiang et al. 2004; Slobounov et al. 2000, 2002).
This component is readily observed during a force generation task (Slobounov et al. 2002) with a higher rate of force production corresponding with increased negativity and an EEG time series comparable to those observed in the present study. de Bruijn et al. (2003) also observed a force production negativity after response onset in differing conditions, which led to their conducting separate analyses for the high and low force ranges to separate out the potential contributions from a MMP. In doing so, they suggest that error monitoring processes can be dissociated from the MMP despite the potential interference of MMP activity. Like de Bruijn et al. (2003), we suggest that the negativity observed in the present study reflects not only a component of the MMP but also encompasses error-related ERP activity. Other ERP studies support this assertion, as a slowly rising negativity like the one observed here was associated with high-level errors from an incorrect response execution made during a force production task (de Bruijn et al. 2003), a continuous tracking task (Krigolson and Holroyd 2006, 2007b), and a computerized aiming task (Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a; Krigolson et al. 2008).
We hypothesize that the prolonged waveforms in the present study represent the on-line modification of the erroneous movement. The signal observed here remains on for an extended period of time, potentially providing for continuous modulation of the action as it is completed. Given the relatively lengthy duration of a trial in this study, it makes sense for the system to be in a state of readiness for a longer time to initiate a remedial action at any point between the initiation of the movement and its completion. These findings also suggest that the MMPs observed in other studies might serve a performance monitoring function. However, it should be noted that the MMP is typically isolated during “self-paced” initiation tasks, unlike the present task, suggesting that further investigation of the role of MMP warrants further investigation.
Conclusions
We found a differential ERP effect for errors of different magnitude (averaged across stage of adaptation). There was a significant difference between early adaptation waveforms versus baseline or late adaptation waveforms with early adaptation large and small error waveforms being distinct from their corresponding baseline and late adaptation waveforms. We propose that the medial frontal neural processes involved in sensorimotor adaptation interpret differences in error magnitude in a relative fashion with the amount of activity corresponding to the size of the error. While the waveforms observed are atypical compared with most ERN studies, it is plausible that they represent ERN-related activity involved in the sensorimotor adaptation process.
GRANTS
This work was supported by National Institute of Aging Grant AG-024106 to R. D. Seidler.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank S. Crafton, C. Papachristou, C. Russell, A. Savine, and C. Tcruz for assistance with data collection and analyses.
Present address of J. A. Anguera: Dept. of Neurology and Keck Center for Integrative Neurosciences, University of California, San Francisco, CA.
Footnotes
We also examined the effect of using the 200 ms prior to stimulus onset for the RT time-locked waveforms as a baseline and observed no difference in the results (see the Supplementary Materials).
The online version of this article contains supplemental data.
REFERENCES
- Adams 1971.Adams J. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Mot Behav 3: 111–149, 1971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anguera et al. 2007.Anguera J, Russell C, Noll D, Seidler R. Neural correlates associated with intermanual transfer of sensorimotor adaptation. Brain Res 1185: 136–151, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badgaiyan and Posner 1998.Badgaiyan R, Posner M. Mapping the cingulate cortex in response selection and monitoring. Neuroimage 7: 255–260, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barto 1994.Barto A. Reinforcement learning in motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4: 888–893, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barto 2003a.Barto A. Reinforcement learning In: Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks (2nd ed.), edited by Arbib MA.Cambridge: MIT Press, 2003a, p. 963–968.
- Barto 2003b.Barto A. Reinforcement learning in motor control. In: Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks (2nd ed.), edited by Arbib MA.Cambridge: MIT Press, 2003b, p. 968–972.
- Bock 1992.Bock O. Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance, evidence for direction-independent gain control. Behav Brain Res 51: 41–50, 1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick et al. 2001.Botvinick M, Braver T, Barch D, Carter C, Cohen J. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108: 624–652, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown and Braver 2005.Brown J, Braver T. Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior ingulate cortex. Science 307: 1118–1121, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush et al. 2000.Bush G, Luu P, Posner M. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 215–222, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush et al. 1998.Bush G, Whalen P, Rosen B, Jenike M, McInerney S, Rauch S. The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging–validation study with functional MRI. Hum Brain Mapp 6: 270–282, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter et al. 1998.Carter C, Braver T, Barch D, Botvinick M, Noll D, Cohen J. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280: 747–749, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiang et al. 2004.Chiang H, Slobounov S, Ray W. Practice-related modulations of force enslaving and cortical activity as revealed by EEG. Clin Neurophysiol 115: 2013–2024, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen et al. 2007.Cohen MX, Elger CE, Ranganath C. Reward expectation modulates feedbackrelated negativity and eeg spectra. Neuroimage 35: 968–978, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coles et al. 2001.Coles MGH, Scheffers MK, Holroyd CB. Why is there an ERN/NE on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error processing. Biol Psych 56: 173–189, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contreras-Vidal and Kerick 2004.Contreras-Vidal J, Kerick S. Independent component analysis of dynamic brain responses during visuomotor adaptation. Neuroimage 21: 936–945, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham and Welch 1994.Cunningham HA, Welch RB. Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20: 987–999, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruijn et al. 2003.de Bruijn E, Hulstijn W, Meulenbroek R, Van Galen G. Action monitoring in motor control: ERPs following selection and execution errors in a force production task. Psychophysiology 40: 786–795, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debener et al. 2005.Debener S, Ullsperger M, Siegel M, Fiehler K, von Cramon DY, Engel AK. Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring. J Neurosci 25: 11730–11737, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene et al. 1994.Dehaene S, Posner M, Tucker D. Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 5: 303–305, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Delorme and Makeig 2004.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics. J Neurosci Methods 134: 9–21, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen et al. 2005.Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R. Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci 25: 9919–9931, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eriksen and Eriksen 1974.Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percep Psychophys 16: 143–149, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Falkenstein et al. 1990.Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L. Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In: Psychophysiological Brain Research, edited by Bmnia CH, Gaillard AW, Kok A. Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press, 1990, p. 192–195.
- Falkenstein et al. 1991.Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L. Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late erp components. 2. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78: 447–455, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falkenstein et al. 2000.Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Christ S, Hohnsbein J. Erp components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial. Biol Psychol 51: 87–107, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fine and Thoroughman 2006.Fine M, Thoroughman K. Motor adaptation to single force pulses: sensitive to direction but insensitive to within-movement pulse placement and magnitude. J Neurophysiol 96: 710–720, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fine and Thoroughman 2007.Fine M, Thoroughman K. Trial-by-trial transformation of error into sensorimotor adaptation changes with environmental dynamics. J Neurophysiol 98: 1392–404, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitts and Posner 1967.Fitts PM, Posner MI. Learning and Skilled Performance in Human Performance. Belmont CA: Brock-Cole, 1967.
- Frank et al. 2005.Frank M, Woroch B, Curran T. Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases. Neuron 47: 495–501, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gehring et al. 1993.Gehring W, Goss B, Coles M, Meyer D, Donchin E. A neural system for error-detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4: 385–390, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Gehring and Knight 2000.Gehring W, Knight R. Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nat Neurosci 3: 516–520, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gehring and Willoughby 2002.Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 29: 2279–2282, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghilardi et al. 2000.Ghilardi M, Ghez C, Dhawan V, Moeller J, Mentis M, Nakamura T, Antonini A, Eidelberg D. Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning. Brain Res 871: 127–145, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goyer et al. 2008.Goyer JP, Woldorff MG, Huettel SA. Rapid electrophysiological brain responses are influenced by both valence and magnitude of monetary rewards. J Cogn Neurosci 20: 2058–2069, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton et al. 1983.Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E. A new method for the off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55: 468–484, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graydon et al. 2005.Graydon F, Friston K, Thomas C, Brooks V, Menon R. Learning-related fMRI activation associated with a rotational visuo-motor transformation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22: 373–383, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grunewald-zuberbier et al. 1978.Grunewald-zuberbier E, Grunewald G, Jung R. Slow potentials of human precentral and parietal cortex during goal-directed movements (zielbewegungspotentiale). J Physiol 284: P181–P182, 1978. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajcak et al. 2006.Hajcak G, Moser JS, Holroyd CB, Simons RF. The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biol Psychol 71: 148–154, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajcak et al. 2007.Hajcak G, Moser JS, Holroyd CB, Simons RF. It's worse than you thought: the feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks. Psychophys 44: 905–912, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann et al. 2004.Herrmann M, Römmler J, Ehlis A, Heidrich A, Fallgatter A. Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 20: 294–299, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd and Coles 2002.Holroyd CB, Coles M. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109: 679–709, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd et al. 2006.Holroyd CB, Hajcak G, Larsen JT. The good, the bad and the neutral: Electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli. Brain Res 1105: 93–101, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd and Krigolson 2007.Holroyd CB, Krigolson O. Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. Psychophysiology 44: 913–917, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd et al. 2003.Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Cohen JD. Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. Neuroreport 14: 2481–2484, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd et al. 2004.Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Nystrom L, Mars R, Coles M, Cohen J. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals. Nat Neurosci 7: 497–498, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huynh and Feldt 1970.Huynh H, Feldt LS. Conditions under which the mean square ratios in repeated measures designs have exact F-distributions. J Am Stat Assoc 65: 1582–1589, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Imamizu et al. 2000.Imamizu H, Miyauchi S, Tamada T, Sasaki Y, Takino R, Pütz B, Yoshioka T, Kawato M. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature 403: 192–195, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue et al. 2000.Inoue K, Kawashima R, Satoh K, Kinomura S, Sugiura M, Goto R, Ito M, Fukuda H. A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. Neuroimage 11: 505–516, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins et al. 1994.Jenkins I, Brooks D, Nixon P, Frackowiak R, Passingham R. Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 14: 3775–3790, 1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kagerer et al. 1997.Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE. Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp Brain Res 115: 557–561, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karni et al. 1998.Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams M, Turner R, Ungerleider L. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 861–868, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karni and Sagi 1993.Karni A, Sagi D. The time course of learning a visual skill. Nature 365: 250–252, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawato 1999.Kawato M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9: 718–727, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawato and Wolpert 1998.Kawato M, Wolpert D. Internal models for motor control. Novartis Found Symp 218: 291–304; discussion 304–297, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitazawa et al. 1998.Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Yin P. Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements. Nature 392: 494–497, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakauer et al. 2004.Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Mentis M, Barnes A, Veytsman M, Eidelberg D, Ghez C. Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. J Neurophysiol 91: 924–933, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krigolson and Holroyd 2006.Krigolson O, Holroyd C. Evidence for hierarchical error processing in the human brain. Neuroscience 137: 13–17, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krigolson and Holroyd 2007a.Krigolson O, Holroyd C. Hierarchical error processing: different errors, different systems. Brain Res 1155: 70–80, 2007a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krigolson and Holroyd 2007b.Krigolson O, Holroyd C. Predictive information and error processing: the role of medial-frontal cortex during motor control. Psychophysiology 44: 586–595, 2007b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krigolson et al. 2008.Krigolson OE, Holroyd CB, Van Gyn G, Heath M. Electroencephalographic correlates of target and outcome errors. Exp Brain Res 8190: 401–411, 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs et al. 1998.Krebs H, Brashers-Krug T, Rauch S, Savage C, Hogan N, Rubin R, Fischman AJ, Alpert NM. Robot-aided functional imaging: application to a motor learning study. Hum Brain Mapp 6: 59–72, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maschke et al. 2004.Maschke M, Gomez C, Ebner T, Konczak J. Hereditary cerebellar ataxia progressively impairs force adaptation during goal-directed arm movements. J Neurophysiol 91: 230–238, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer et al. 1988.Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JE. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95: 340–370, 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall et al. 2001.Miall RC, Reckess GZ, Imamizu H. The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 4: 638–644, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miltner et al. 1997.Miltner WHR, Braun CH, Coles MGH. Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J Cogn Neurosci 9: 788–798, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morton and Bastian 2006.Morton S, Bastian A. Cerebellar contributions to locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walking. J Neurosci 26: 9107–9116, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newell and Rosenbloom 1981.Newell A, Rosenbloom PS. Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In: Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition, edited by Anderson JR. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1981, p. 1–55.
- Nieuwenhuis et al. 2004a.Nieuwenhuis S, Holroyd C, Mol N, Coles M. Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28: 441–448, 2004a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis et al. 2004b.Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Holroyd CB, Schurger A, Cohen JD. Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. Cereb Cortex 14: 741–747, 2004b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield 1971.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113, 1971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polich 2007.Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118: 2128–2148, 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potts et al. 2006.Potts GF, Martin LE, Burton P, Montague PR. When things are better or worse than expected: the medial frontal cortex and the allocation of processing resources. J Cogn Neurosci 18: 1112–1119, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puttemans et al. 2005.Puttemans V, Wenderoth N, Swinnen S. Changes in brain activation during the acquisition of a multifrequency bimanual coordination task: from the cognitive stage to advanced levels of automaticity. J Neurosci 25: 4270–4278, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle et al. 1994.Raichle M, Fiez J, Videen T, MacLeod A, Pardo J, Fox P, Petersen S. Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cereb Cortex 4: 8–26, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof et al. 2004.Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306: 443–447, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez-Fornells et al. 2002.Rodriguez-Fornells A, Kurzbuch A, Münte T. Time course of error detection and correction in humans: neurophysiological evidence. J Neurosci 22: 9990–9996, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheidt et al. 2001.Scheidt R, Dingwell J, Mussa-Ivaldi F. Learning to move amid uncertainty. J Neurophysiol 86: 971–985, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt 1976.Schmidt RA. The schema as a solution to some persistent problems in motor-learning theory. In: Motor Control: Issues and Trends, edited by Stelmach GE. New York: Academic, 1976, p. 41–65.
- Seidler 2004.Seidler RD. Multiple motor learning experiences enhance motor adaptability. J Cogn Neurol 16: 65–73, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidler et al. 2006.Seidler R, Noll D, Chintalapati P. Bilateral basal ganglia activation associated with sensorimotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 175: 544–555, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr and Holcomb 1997.Shadmehr R, Holcomb H. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science 277: 821–825, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shima and Tanji 1998.Shima K, Tanji J. Role for cingulate motor area cells in voluntary movement selection based on reward. Science 282: 1335–1338, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slobounov et al. 2000.Slobounov S, Fukada K, Simon R, Rearick M, Ray W. Neurophysiological and behavioral indices of time pressure on visuomotor task performance. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 9: 287–298, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slobounov et al. 2002.Slobounov S, Johnston J, Chiang H, Ray W. Movement-related potentials are force or end-effector dependent: evidence from a multifinger experiment. Clin Neurophysiol 113: 1125–35, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slobounov and Ray 1998.Slobounov S, Ray W. Movement-related potentials with reference to isometric force output in discrete and repetitive tasks. Exp Brain Res 123: 461–473, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith et al. 2000.Smith M, Brandt J, Shadmehr R. Motor disorder in Huntington's disease begins as a dysfunction in error feedback control. Nature 403: 544–549, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith and Shadmehr 2005.Smith M, Shadmehr R. Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington's disease but not cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol 93: 2809–2821, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer et al. 1990.Sommer W, Matt J, Leuthold H. Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 16: 902–915, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton and Barto 1998.Sutton RS, Barto AG. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1998.
- Swick and Turken 2002.Swick D, Turken A. Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 16354–16359, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teasdale et al. 1993.Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M, Young D, Proteau L. Determining movement onsets from temporal series. J Mot Behav 25: 97–106, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toni and Passingham 1999.Toni I, Passingham R. Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor associations: a PET study. Exp Brain Res 127: 19–32, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidal et al. 2000.Vidal F, Hasbroucq T, Grapperon J, Bonnet M. Is the “error negativity” specific to errors? Biol Psych 51: 109–128, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Helmholtz 1962.von Helmholtz H. A Treatise on Physiological Optics, translated by Southall JP. New York: Dover, 1962. (Original work published 1866).
- Welch et al. 1974.Welch RB, Choe CS, Heinrich DR. Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. J. Exp Psychol 103: 700–705, 1974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willingham 1998.Willingham D. A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 105: 558–584, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winter 1990.Winter D. Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement. (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1990.
- Wolpert and Ghahramani 2000.Wolpert D, Ghahramani Z. Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci 3, Suppl: 1212–1217, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert and Kawato 1998.Wolpert D, Kawato M. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw 11: 1317–1329, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung et al. 2004.Yeung N, Cohen J, Botvinick M. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 111: 931–959, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung and Sanfey 2004.Yeung N, Sanfey AG. Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J Neurosci 24: 6258–6264, 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.