Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2009 Dec 18;5(12):e1000609. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000609

Power-Law Scaling in the Brain Surface Electric Potential

Kai J Miller 1,*, Larry B Sorensen 1, Jeffrey G Ojemann 2, Marcel den Nijs 1
Editor: Olaf Sporns3
PMCID: PMC2787015  PMID: 20019800

Abstract

Recent studies have identified broadband phenomena in the electric potentials produced by the brain. We report the finding of power-law scaling in these signals using subdural electrocorticographic recordings from the surface of human cortex. The power spectral density (PSD) of the electric potential has the power-law form Inline graphic from 80 to 500 Hz. This scaling index, Inline graphic, is conserved across subjects, area in the cortex, and local neural activity levels. The shape of the PSD does not change with increases in local cortical activity, but the amplitude, Inline graphic, increases. We observe a “knee” in the spectra at Inline graphic, implying the existence of a characteristic time scale Inline graphic. Below Inline graphic, we explore two-power-law forms of the PSD, and demonstrate that there are activity-related fluctuations in the amplitude of a power-law process lying beneath the Inline graphic rhythms. Finally, we illustrate through simulation how, small-scale, simplified neuronal models could lead to these power-law observations. This suggests a new paradigm of non-oscillatory “asynchronous,” scale-free, changes in cortical potentials, corresponding to changes in mean population-averaged firing rate, to complement the prevalent “synchronous” rhythm-based paradigm.

Author Summary

For a very long time, the measurement of the large scale potentials produced by the brain from outside of the head, using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography, and from inside the head, using electrocorticography, has fixated on changes in specific rhythms and frequency ranges. This fixation presupposes physiologic changes where neuronal populations synchronously oscillate at specific timescales. Here, we demonstrate that there are phenomena which obey a broadband, power-law form extending across the entire frequency domain, with no special timescale. It is shown that, with local brain activity, there is an increase in power across all frequencies, and the power-law shape is conserved. Furthermore, we illustrate through simple simulation how fluctuations in this phenomenon may be linked to increases and decreases in “noise-like” patterns of activity in neuronal populations. Although power-laws have been postulated to exist in background electrical brain activity, the view that local activity can be captured by fluctuations in a broadband power-law in the power spectrum of electric potential timeseries represents a fundamentally new way of thinking about changes in the electric potential produced by the brain, and provides insight into what types of neuronal processes might produce these potentials.

Introduction

Neuronal electrical activity may be measured at many scales, from individual ion channels [1] to the largest scale measurement of electroencephalographic (EEG) potentials entirely outside the head [2]. Synaptic current produces a change in the local electric field, and it is believed that large scale field potentials reveal primarily the aggregate synaptic activity from large neuronal populations [3],[4]. Our particular experiments measure these potentials at the brain surface, using arrays of platinum electrocorticographic (ECoG) electrodes (Figure 1). Interaction properties between synapses, when averaged across the entire ensemble, may be revealed by the potential auto-correlation function:

graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e008.jpg (1)

which is an average over the entire time interval of the recording. The Fourier Transform of Inline graphic is the power spectral density (PSD), and reveals to what degree the potential at one point in time is correlated with the potential at a later point in time. Because of this, characteristic phenomena in the cortical potential PSD have interpretable implications for the interaction properties between elements within neuronal populations.

Figure 1. Experimental setting.

Figure 1

(A) Cortical surface potential measurement: The electrode array locations are shown on a template brain for subject 1 (black, temporal, positions calculated from x-ray [69]) and subject 2 (white, fronto-parietal). Potentials of all 32 electrodes are measured simultaneously with respect to a scalp reference and ground, before they are pair-wise re-referenced to obtain 52 electrode pair channels. (B) Spectral correction and fitting: The raw power spectral density (PSD) of an electrode channel pair (green) is corrected first for amplifier frequency dependent attenuation (roll-off shown in the blue trace, the corrected PSD is shown gray), and then for amplifier noise floor (denoted by the horizontal black line, with corrected PSD in black). The corrected PSD was then fit linearly on these log-log axes (red line shows fit here), and the slope of this fit determines the exponent of an associated power-law form (here Inline graphic). The sharp line noise spikes at 60 Hz and its harmonics were excluded in our fitting analysis (and are thus omitted in Figures 24).

For example, since Adolf Beck first described in the 1890s how simple behavioral change produced widespread amplitude changes in rhythmic properties of the electric potential timeseries [5], findings of peaked phenomena in the PSD have pointed to oscillatory activity that is synchronized across the neuronal population [6][12], and have been linked to known large-scale brain phenomena like cortical-subcortical feedback loops which change during behavior [8], [13][16]. Simple behaviors produce robust change in the oscillations: Opening the eyes decreases the occipital Inline graphic-rhythm (8–12Hz) amplitude [17],[18], and movement decreases the lateral frontoparietal Inline graphic and Inline graphic (18–25Hz) rhythm amplitudes [19][23].

Other studies have attributed band-specific processes in the so-called “high-Inline graphic” range (60–150Hz) to local cortical processing [24],[25], with specific timescales linked to the particular choice of frequency range. In lateral brain regions, we observed a lack of distinct peaks in the cortical potential PSD beyond Inline graphicHz, and hypothesized the existence of broadband changes across all frequencies which were obscured at low frequencies by covariant fluctuations in the Inline graphic/Inline graphic/Inline graphic rhythms [22],[26]. While previous studies had hypothesized that background power-laws existed in the PSD [27][30], we hypothesized the existence of behaviorally-associated changes in a power-law process of the form Inline graphic, and named attempts to capture it the “Inline graphic-band/index,” at the higher frequencies where it is most plainly observed [22],[26],[31],[32]. Our early studies, sampled at 1kHz, had PSDs that truncated above Inline graphic250Hz. Although we observed structure in these PSDs, and hypothesized the existence of a power-law, we needed data with a higher sampling rate to establish it firmly. The purpose of this study was to sample higher, at 10 kHz and determine, as accurately as possible, whether there is indeed such a power-law in the human cortical potential power spectrum, and how it might change with cortical activity. Results from this higher sampling rate (4 subjects) might then allow us to return, informed, to the large group of lower sampling rate data (16 subjects), and re-examine it with knowledge of how it must behave at higher frequencies. Here, we identify and characterize a scale-free process in the ECoG potential PSD, revealed by a power-law. The existence of such a power-law process points to phenomena with no special timescale, where the neuronal population beneath is not synchronously oscillating. We demonstrate through very basic simulation how such spectra might arise from simple processes, and how observed broadband power-law changes in the PSD might simply reflect a change in population mean firing rate.

Results

We measured the surface potential between pairs of surface electrocorticographic electrodes separated by one centimeter from each other on the lateral brain surface of 20 human subjects. From these potentials, we calculated power spectral densities (PSDs) averaged over several minutes of data. As detailed in the methodology section below, each PSD was examined for the presence and character of a power-law form. The strongest empiric finding from this study was the robust fit of a power-law form Inline graphic, with Inline graphic, for frequencies above 80Hz. We performed a stringent fitting protocol in the frequency range 80HzInline graphic580Hz of the averaged electrode pair PSDs of 4 subjects, and found extremely tight fits to the form Inline graphic and Inline graphic in each case. This was obtained by fitting 10 kHz sampled data from subjects 1–4, during a simple fixation task. Figure 2A–D shows the PSD, averaged over electrode pairs. The inserts illustrate the robust quality of the power-law form, where the jitter of data around the fit is more than one decade down from the signal. The exponent Inline graphic and the parameters Inline graphic and Inline graphic in the form Inline graphic were estimated via a set of log-log least-squares linear fits of the power spectral density, with a range-shrinking scheme to ensure that local fits within the fitting range produced the same fit as the global form. The fitting range chosen for each was a lower bound of 80 Hz (in order to stay above a “knee” at 75Hz) and the highest frequency where spectra could be resolved from the noise floor in each subject (579, 530, 534, 559 Hz for subjects 1–4), excluding harmonics of 60 Hz. The combined spectra fit values of Inline graphic were 3.97, 3.94, 3.97, and 4.02 for subjects 1–4, respectively. The error estimates (of order Inline graphic0.1 or less for each subject) were based on robustness against range shrinking as well as the deviations of the best fit with respect to the actual data across the entire frequency range (see insets in Figure 2A–D).

Figure 2. The power-law in the cortical spectrum, above 80Hz.

Figure 2

(A) The averaged PSD for all electrode-pair channels from subject 1 (black trace, with 60Hz harmonics omitted; gray line shows PSD prior to noise floor subtraction) on log-log axes, with the red line showing the best linear fit from 80Hz until the PSD hits the noise floor at Inline graphic = 579Hz. The blown-up inset illustrates the quality of the fit in the higher frequencies at higher resolution. (B)–(D) As in (A), but for subjects 2, 3, and 4, respectively. The fitting ranges were from 80Hz to 530, 534, 559 Hz for subjects 2–4, respectively. (E) Histogram of the fit exponents of individual electrode pair channels, stacked for subjects 1–4, between 80 and 400Hz.

To test for universality, we also performed the same type of fits to each individual electrode pair spectrum for subjects 1–4, from 80 Hz to 400 Hz. The histogram of these individual fits is shown in Figure 2E. The mean of the individual fits was Inline graphic = 4.01, with SD = 0.13 (N = 151), in strong agreement with the averaged spectra. Individual electrode pair channel PSDs, fit between 80HzInline graphic400Hz, produced the same result as the average spectrum, without systematic variation by brain area. This power-law scaling extends over four decades in power and, because Inline graphic is large, over one decade in frequency. The noise around this straight line is minimal, and is robust against range shrinking (that is, the fit exponent is unchanged if a smaller frequency range interval, within the total range, is chosen for fitting). This quantitative level of power-law scaling is rarely seen in experimental data [33].

Preliminary calculations showed a clear crossover (“knee”) in the PSD at Inline graphic75Hz, below which the PSD takes a different form (see Figures 2 and 3). A natural question to ask is whether there is a different power-law form at lower frequencies, with exponent Inline graphic. Previous power-law estimates in the cortical potential focused on this lower frequency range [34][37], and most naively fit scale-free exponents directly to spectra known to contain scale-dependent phenomena (oscillatory brain rhythms peaked at specific frequencies). We wished to avoid the confounding influence of these rhythms, but, unfortunately, the Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms were strongly pronounced in most cortical channel pairs of the data recorded at 10kHz (subjects 1–4, clearly visible in Figure 2). They obscured whatever asynchronous (scale-free, non-peaked) phenomena might be present underneath at frequencies below Inline graphic, and there were simply not enough channels without them to be meaningfully examined in the 10kHz sampled data. Therefore, we returned to our large set of initial 1kHz sampled data, and circumvented these rhythms with two approaches: the first was simple avoidance, by selection of channels where the rhythms were absent during the fixation task; the second was to use data from an experimental setting (finger movement) which caused the rhythms and the underlying broad-band change to vary differently, and the rhythms could be removed.

Figure 3. A power-law fit at lower frequency.

Figure 3

The data sampled at 1kHz was first fit naively for a low-frequency power law below 80Hz. It was then fit iteratively to identify the corner frequency, Inline graphic, and low-frequency exponent, Inline graphic, on a case-by-case basis. (A) A single PSD, from subject 19, showing the raw spectrum (blue) with the form Inline graphic below 80Hz, with a transition to Inline graphic above. After adjusting for a freely-fit Lorentzian form factor Inline graphic, it follows Inline graphic (i.e. Inline graphic and Inline graphic). (B) The histogram for naïve fits yielded Inline graphic = 2.46Inline graphic0.32 (meanInline graphicSD, N = 91, fit range 15–80Hz). Fits for a more complex form (equation 2, fit from 15–195Hz), yielded exponent Inline graphic = 2.01Inline graphic0.18 (in (C)), and corner frequency Inline graphic = 77HzInline graphic14Hz (in (D)).

In order to evaluate what the exponent in such a lower power-law would be (below 80Hz), we first naively fit the resting spectra of a large ensemble of fixation data (subjects 5–20) sampled at 1kHz. Only channel pairs which lacked Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms, and for which the noise floor was relatively small compared with the power, were selected and fit. This naïve fit of a low frequency power law yielded values of Inline graphic = 2.46Inline graphic0.32 (meanInline graphicSD, N = 91) (Figure 3). We then modified the power spectra of this 1kHz fixation data, dividing by the product of 2 Lorentzian-like form factors:

graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e062.jpg (2)

based upon the knee observed in the spectra of the 10 kHz data, subject to the constraint that Inline graphic (because the 10kHz fit implied that this must be the case for large Inline graphic), and calculated the values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic for which the modified spectra had a slope closest to zero, on the frequency interval 15–195Hz. These post-modification fits yielded Inline graphic = 2.01Inline graphic0.18 (Inline graphicSD, N = 91), and Inline graphic was 77HzInline graphic14Hz, as shown in Figure 3. It should be noted that this likely represents a true range, where the “knee” at Inline graphic may vary by location and individual.

The change in the shape of the PSD Inline graphic during different levels of neuronal population activity reveals different dynamics within the population. A shift in the exponent, Inline graphic, would suggest a change in the correlation between neurons, whereas a shift in the coefficient, Inline graphic, would suggest an overall increase or decrease in population activity. In a recent manuscript [38], we demonstrated how motor-behavior-related variation in the Inline graphic and Inline graphic bands allow them to be removed from the measured PSD in primary motor cortex. We repeated the same process as that on pair-wise re-referenced electrode channels and, removed the oscillatory (peaked) phenomena from the PSDs. Activity-related changes in individual channel pairs were examined by dividing the active, movement, spectra (“Inline graphic”) by the inactive, rest, spectra (“Inline graphic”), element-wise (Inline graphic). Calculating the Inline graphic vs. Inline graphic slope removes the common shape (including the effect of Inline graphic), and reveals whether there is a shift in the slope, Inline graphic, during activity. This shift in exponent, when fit from 25–195Hz, was insignificant: 0.03Inline graphic0.09, (Inline graphicSD, N = 25, Inline graphic, by paired t-test; subjects 16–20, 5 electrode pairs from motor cortex each). Because there was no significant shift in the exponent, then active/inactive power ratio Inline graphic (between the amplitudes Inline graphic, Figure 4) was obtained for each channel simply by averaging Inline graphic across frequencies in each channel. The geometric mean of these ratios was Inline graphic with a variation (standard deviation) of order 0.31 (maximum 2.47, minimum 1.29, N = 25, Inline graphic).

Figure 4. The conserved shape of the power spectrum with brain activity.

Figure 4

The average shift in power spectral density in electrodes after removal of the Inline graphic/Inline graphic peaks in the hand cortical area during finger movement, for subjects 16–20; 5 electrode-pair channels each (25 total). (A) This shift demonstrates that, in motor cortex, movement (red) increases the overall power while preserving the shape of the rest (blue) spectrum. Inline graphic power-law fits (grey) are consistent with Inline graphic. (B) Channel locations (interpolated between the two paired electrodes) across all subjects, projected to the left-hand side. (C) Remaining spectra after dividing out a Lorentzian, Inline graphic to illustrate consistency with the two-power-laws form with Inline graphic. The residual lines are consistently parallel, and therefore, the exponent shift with activity in individual channels is negligible (shift in exponent  = 0.03Inline graphic0.09, Inline graphicSD, N = 25, Inline graphic, by paired t-test). The shift up is an overall factor of 1.76 with a variation (standard deviation) of order 0.31 (maximum 2.47, minimum 1.29, N = 25, Inline graphic).

A simple simulation using Poisson-distributed pre-synaptic action potentials to a single neuron beneath one of our electrodes (Inline graphic neurons beneath each [39]) reproduced the spectra that we see, at different levels of cortical activity with simulated rates of 15, 30, and 60 Inline graphic (Figure 5; “Inline graphic” = action potential). Using a modified leaky integrate-and-fire model (without a firing component), consisting of an exponentially decaying post-synaptic current, temporal integration, and passive current efflux to estimate the time-dependence of a dendritic current-dipole field, a spectrum with the power-law form Inline graphic emerged. A Inline graphic factor, contributed by exponentially decaying synaptic current, directly follows previous work by Bedard et al [27]. The value of Inline graphic for high frequencies (80–500Hz) in this simulated data was 4.0 at all three levels of cortical activity (linear fit on log-log axes). The ratio of the coefficient, Inline graphic, was 4.03 for 60 vs. 15 Inline graphic, and 1.96 for 30 vs. 15 Inline graphic. Because the PSD of the superposition of many such uncorrelated model neurons will have the same Inline graphic shape as one, the model in a single neuron will generalize to an entire neuronal population.

Figure 5. How these power-law phenomena in the cortical potentials might be generated.

Figure 5

(A)–(B) Beneath each one of our 2.3 mm diameter electrodes, there are of order Inline graphic neurons, and each of these has Inline graphic synapses where it receives input [39]. Currents into and out of each of these Inline graphic synapses, and gradual ohmic current through the dendritic membrane as the post-synaptic charge bolus diffuses toward the soma, produce transient dendritic dipoles that are thought to be the source of the macroscale potentials, Inline graphic, that we measure with ECoG. (cross-sectional stain from Ramon y Cajal) (C) In our simple simulation, a single neuron receives 6000 presynaptic inputs. Each of these inputs delivers a temporally Poisson-distributed series of action potentials (with each arrival time as a delta function). Each of these action potentials induces a post-synaptic current shape which has an extremely fast rise and an exponential decay. The input from all synapses is combined, and charge accumulates over time and is lost, ohmically, through the dendritic membrane. We approximate the time dependence of the current dipole that produces our potentials as the time dependence of this ohmic membrane current (bottom trace in C). The equations to the right of the schematic indicate how each element of this heuristic illustration would contribute to the experimentally-fit analytic structure of the PSD. (D) The PSD of this signal has a knee at 70Hz, with power-law of the form Inline graphic beyond. The change in the spectra with increasing mean spike rate of synaptic input strongly resembles the change observed experimentally over motor cortex during activity (Figure 4).

Discussion

Although traditional EEG studies have been limited to measurements of frequencies below 100Hz, the timing of fundamental neuronal processes suggests that information content should be present at much higher frequencies. Propagation time of a spike along an axon, synaptic neurotransmitter diffusion time, or the recursion time of reciprocally coupled neurons are all near or below 10 ms [39][42]. Synchronizations and correlations associated with these should exist at least up to 1kHz.

The human brain is arguably the most complex and largest network available to observe scale free behavior in a natural setting, and at the 5mm2 scale of our electrodes, we have observed robust power-law scaling. Power-laws represent scale free behavior – the finding of which typically evokes discussion of scale free networks, complexity, avalanches, and self-organized criticality (SOC) [43][45], and if our measured value of Inline graphic were distinct from an integer, we might have discussed SOC at the cortical surface. SOC is a process where very complex global phenomena arise in a population of interacting elements due to very simple properties of the individual element, and simple rules that dictate how pairs of elements interact with one another [43]. The global complexity is generally not immediately apparent from the simple properties and rules. A popular example of this is the emergence of earthquakes of different sizes and the frequencies on which they occur [46], which have a power-law relationship. The form of this earthquake distribution that is observed in nature can also be derived from a simple model where blocks of matter are held together by springs, but slip against one another with friction [47],[48] – global complexity emerges from basic interaction, and can be characterized by a power law. One might hypothesize that the interaction between neurons in a population, producing sophisticated computation, would exhibit SOC, and be revealed by a power law. Our experimental finding of power law scaling in the brain surface electric potential does not suggest SOC. Non-integer exponents in power-law relationships imply self-organized criticality in the population of constituent elements, but multiple-of-two integers, such as our finding of Inline graphic, point towards simple, noise-like, analytic functions, i.e. to a non-singular, non-fractal, non-complexity explanation of the shape of the power spectrum (such as a diffusive process or filtered noise). Perhaps SOC behavior (if it exists) is only expressed in the PSD in more subtle ways, within the Inline graphic uncertainty, or at finer spatial scales, in the cortical surface potential. While not present in this study, evidence for complex neural correlations (and different exponents) may emerge in different experimental settings, such as power-fluctuations in the Inline graphic-rhythm [49], in the magnitude of spatially-correlated cascades of activity in the LFP [50], or the gain of neuronal firing in response to cyclical driving potentials [51].

In order to examine the PSD structure at lower frequencies, the 91 channels from subjects 5–20 without Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms were first blindly fit linearly on log-log axes up to the knee in the spectrum, from 15–80Hz, producing an estimate of a low-frequency power-law with exponent Inline graphic. Based upon the knee at Inline graphic, and the blind fit in Figure 3a, we examined a more complex parameterized form of the PSD which accounts for the knee, could be fit across a larger range (15–195Hz), and goes to Inline graphic for large Inline graphic (i.e. of the form of equation 2). Note that the discovered factor of Inline graphic might represent a form factor Inline graphic where the lower boundary of our fitting range is above Inline graphic, so a lower knee with a flattening of the spectrum is not appreciated. There may, in fact, be a lower “flattening” of the spectrum, below 20Hz, that is masked by the Inline graphic and (ubiquitous) Inline graphic/Inline graphic rhythms (see, for example, plots in [27],[28]). As described by Sigeti and Horsthemke, these types of “2+2” spectra can emerge from noise-like processes which have two simple correlation times [52]. There are many such combinations of two simple known neuronal processes, such as temporal integration in dendrites or soma, exponentially decaying membrane currents, low-pass RC filtering by tissue, or local network connectivity which, when modeled, will produce precisely this form (one such is illustrated in Figure 5).

In a previous paper [22], we hypothesized that observed high frequency changes called “high-Inline graphic[53] or Inline graphic [31] were reflective of broad-band, power-law shifts, and were obscured by the Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms at lower frequencies in motor cortex. Indeed, the intersection of these two phenomena, “Inline graphic”, was subsequently shown to lie at Inline graphic (meanInline graphicSD) (range 32–57 Hz) during hand motor movement [26]. When we made this hypothesis, it was uncertain whether this shift might reflect a change in the exponent, Inline graphic, or the coefficient, Inline graphic, of a power-law of the form Inline graphic. In a more recent manuscript, we performed a decomposition technique which removed the Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms [38], revealing broadband increase beneath. Figure 4 shows that when this method is applied, and the residual broadband spectra are modified with a Lorentzian form, both active and inactive spectra are approximately linear on a log-log plot with slope −2. In other words, they can be reasonably described by a power-law with exponent Inline graphic. When individual channel pairs were examined independently for active and inactive spectra, there was no difference in fit exponent: the shape of the PSD was unchanged, but the overall amplitude was. This implies that, at the spatial scale of our electrodes, after spatially and temporally averaging, the structure and complexity of the large-scale neural networks do not change during computation, but the overall amount of activity does. The active/inactive power ratio between the amplitudes (Inline graphic1.76) provides a sense of the dynamic range of this network in the behaving brain as it shifts between ‘idling’ and ‘computing’ regimes.

An important caveat to these findings is that the PSDs which we fit were averaged over long periods of time (minutes of fixation or seconds of movement/rest). If the same is done over very small windows, there are deviations from the form of the averaged PSD. It is within these small windows that computations take place, and the “instantaneous PSD” will not have the power-law shape at all times. Without reoccurring synchronized oscillatory processes, however, it averages to the power-law shape over time.

To gain intuition about what may produce these signals, we performed a simple simulation from the perspective of a single neuron beneath one of our electrodes (Inline graphic neurons immediately underneath each electrode [39]), and take into account only three factors: Poisson-distributed input action potentials, exponentially decaying post-synaptic currents, and ohmic current in the dendrite, produced a time-dependent signal with a PSD of the same shape that we measure, and with the same change during increase in activity. While our particular choice of model was one of many potential models, we believe that any simulation of the ECoG PSD should rely on very simple factors, ubiquitous in the cortical neuronal population, because the effect must be conserved after averaging across Inline graphic neurons. Although this simulation was largely oblivious both to the details of dendritic and overall neuronal processing (between neurons, etc), and to many factors which must influence the creation of dendritic current dipoles, it does exhibit two things that we would like to stress. The first is that the knee we observe in the spectra likely corresponds to the timescale of a very simple process, like post-synaptic potential current of particular timescale, which occurs throughout the cortical surface. The second is that changes in the amplitude, Inline graphic, of the power-law reflect changes in Poisson-distributed (after coarse graining) input action potentials beneath each of the electrodes. Indeed, we have recently shown that the capture of this broad-band, here demonstrated to obey a power-law, reveals local cortical activity with high temporal precision [38]. The values that the simulation obtains (factor of 2/4 increase in Inline graphic with a doubling/quadrupling of the action potential rate) suggest that the difference we observe experimentally during finger movement might represent roughly a doubling of mean input action potential rate for the population of neurons.

Collectively, these findings have important implications for understanding the electric potential at the cortical surface, with the necessary caveat that the effects seen reflect an average of Inline graphic neurons. Because of the connection between the autocorrelation in the potential and the PSD (equation 1), we can try to connect the form we observe in the PSD to correlation in the physiologic processes which produce it. At this coarse level, there is a special frequency at roughly Inline graphic75Hz, and this must be accounted for. This may be due to an exponentially decaying temporal correlation of 2–4ms from post-synaptic current, tissue low-pass filter, protein dissociation, or some other. Perhaps this timescale corresponds to a recurrent process of 11–16ms such as characteristic reciprocal connectivity in local neuronal circuits, or to the “conduction time” of single neurons – how much time it takes for a coordinated pre-synaptic super-threshold pulse to produce an action potential at the axon hillock. If there is a lower “knee” below our fitting range, and masked by the theta rhythm, that implies a second timescale with physiologic importance of its own which must be accounted for. Each of these must also correspond to a factor of Inline graphic above the associated characteristic frequency. Our simple simulation follows that of Bedard et. al. [27] for the first factor, with exponentially decaying post-synaptic current accounting for Inline graphic and one factor of Inline graphic, and charge accumulation in the dendrite producing the second factor of Inline graphic, with the dendritic current leakage producing a second native frequency well below any fitting range we examined. Fluctuations in firing rate produce overall increases and decreases in the PSD, without a change in the frequency dependence of the PSD.

Our experimental results, in contrast, differ significantly from the Bedard et. al. paper [27]. They reported a Inline graphic to Inline graphic transition in their PSD measurements. As we do, they attribute a factor of Inline graphic to Poisson-distributed spikes and the shape of the post-synaptic current. They attribute the remaining Inline graphic to passive tissue filtering, which has since been contradicted experimentally by Logothetis et. al. [54]. To the eye, the PSD from the Bedard et. al, study appears as if it may have been better fit by an Inline graphic to Inline graphic shape like the one found in this study. By extension of their logic to our finding, our power-of-two structure may point away from the presence of Inline graphic tissue attenuation.

Activity-related narrow-band PSD increases, correlating with fMRI [55], have been demonstrated in the “high-Inline graphic” (40–100Hz) frequency range of the LFP [56][60] and the MEG [61],[62]. In each case, these are peaked phenomena in the PSD, reflecting a coherent, oscillatory process, which increases with activity, and is specific to visual cortex. This is a very different phenomenon from the power-law increase that we demonstrate here. In fact, Siegel and Konig, in 2003, explicitly distinguished between a peaked, lower Inline graphic, phenomena at 44–53 Hz, and a different, broad-spectral increase, beginning at 45 Hz extending well beyond 100Hz to the upper limit of their recording from cat visual cortex [57]. Henrie and Shapley, as well as Liu and Newsome, made the same distinction, with similar effect, in visual areas of the non-human primate [58],[60]. Extracted broadband changes across the entire human ECoG spectrum, after removing the low-frequency rhythms, were recently demonstrated to capture the timing of individual finger movements with very high fidelity, and explicitly better than band-filtered high-frequency changes [38]. Even more recently, broadband LFP changes were demonstrated to correlate more highly with mean firing rate than any particular frequency band in single unit recordings from human cortex [63]. We suggest that what these manuscripts identify as broadband change, distinct from Inline graphic oscillations, and what others have called “high-Inline graphic” when referring to broad spectral increases [24],[25],[42],[53], are primarily shifts in the noise-like process identified here, captured at frequencies above the range of band-limited oscillations. This power law process likely reflects the mean input spike rate to neuronal populations, without a preferred timescale. True Inline graphic-oscillation, however, is likely due to population synchronization by fast-spiking inhibitory interneurons [64],[65], reflected by peaked elements in the PSD, and possibly specific to visual cortex (note that none of the data in this manuscript was recorded from occipital visual areas).

When one is sitting on the seashore, it is possible to hear individual waves breaking, first on the left, and then on the right, correlated by their relation to shape of the shore. As one walks away, however, the correlation between individual waves is lost because many are heard at once, from progressively larger stretches of the beach. The combination of our empirical and modeling findings point to a similar picture, where the internal correlations between neuronal events are lost by averaging over large spatial areas, but the changes that we measure do inform us about the overall number of events taking place in the population.

We would like to propose that the popular “high-Inline graphic” range, where it has been postulated that synchronous, rhythmic, action potential activity produces changes, is often a reflection of changes in asynchronous activity instead, and revealed by this power-law process. This shift in thinking, to noise-like non-oscillatory changes, is a fundamentally new addition to the way people think about changes in the cortical potential spectrum. Whereas changes in characteristic brain rhythms are thought to reflect synchronized populations that coherently oscillate across large cortical regions, power-law scaling likely reflects asynchronous, averaged, input to the local neural population.

Materials and Methods

In addition this section, there is an extensive methodological supplement (Text S1), addressing each aspect of the analysis in further detail, with illustration, for the more involved reader.

Ethics statement

All patients participated in a purely voluntary manner, after providing informed consent, under a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Washington

Experimental setting

Twenty epileptic human subjects had subdural electrode arrays placed on the brain surface of the lateral frontal, temporal, and parietal cortical areas for the localization of seizure foci. These arrays were composed of circular platinum electrodes with 2.3mm diameter exposed, at 1 cm inter-electrode distance (center-to-center), embedded in silastic. Electrodes lying on top of vasculature, near seizure foci, or with aberrantly high noise floors were excluded from the study. All kept data were recorded away from seizure times. Potentials at each electrode were recorded at 10 kHz (subjects 1–4) or 1 kHz (subjects 5–20). The first type of experiment, fixation, was performed (subjects 1 to 20) by the subjects fixating with their eyes open on an “X”, on the wall 3m away, for several minutes. The second type of experiment (subjects 16 to 20) consisted of simple repeated finger movement (visually cued).

Spectral calculation

The brain surface potentials from the array were first re-referenced in terms of neighboring differential pair channels (bipolar re-referencing for all nearest neighbors), which significantly reduced the overall noise in the signal.

Fixation spectra

The potential time series were broken-up into 1s epochs, overlapping by 0.5s. Each was Hann-windowed, Fourier transformed. The average over epochs was the power spectral density (PSD).

Movement spectra

For 5 hand region channels per subject, one second epochs flanking time of maximum flexion of each finger movement (dataglove 5dt, Irvine, CA)), and random one second epochs during times of no movement were identified, Hann-windowed, Fourier transformed, and absolute squared. A principal component method was used to remove the Inline graphic&Inline graphic rhythms [38], and the resulting spectra were averaged for movement and rest separately.

Correction for amplifier artifact

We empirically determined the amplitude attenuation function of the amplifiers independently using an external function generator. For the 10kHz data, a “reasonable range” of empirical amplifier noise floors was determined experimentally by measuring the potential across an equivalent conformation of resistors. Because there was a range of potential floor values for each electrode, depending upon the day, temperature, room, etc, the specific noise floor subtracted from each calculated spectra was determined within the empiric range using a recursive, self-consistent method.

Spectral fitting of the 10kHz fixation data

For the 10kHz sampled, fixation task data, the value of the exponent Inline graphic in the power-law relation Inline graphic was obtained by fitting a straight line to the experimentally measured PSD, Inline graphic, on Inline graphic vs. Inline graphic axes, after correcting for amplifier imposed artifacts (Figures 15). An infamous mistake in this procedure is to apply global least squares fit, and leave it at that. On a log-log plot, that assigns too much weight to the highest density of datapoints, at high frequency, where the low power and high relative influence of the noise floor make the data noisiest. In reality, a fit should be stable throughout the fitting range, and we employed a technique which is robust against range shrinking to a sub-range within the total fitting range. We determined local fits for the exponent Inline graphic by performing least-squares linear fits to the power spectrum (on log frequency by log Power axes) to obtain local slopes over varying frequency intervals, Inline graphic (harmonics of 60Hz were explicitly excluded). The most appropriate value of Inline graphic globally is the one that is most stable across many values of Inline graphic and Inline graphic, for a given value of the noise floor, Inline graphic. Because variation in the noise floor exists and confounds the quantitative analysis, the appropriate value in a given channel pair for a given experiment is a self-consistent, recursive, 3 parameter fit to the form Inline graphic, over the entire frequency range Inline graphic, treating Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic as free fitting parameters (Inline graphic is constrained within the empiric range of experimentally measured noise floors). In each iteration, Inline graphic is determined as the average exponent from a distribution of fit exponents, each calculated from a different sub-interval Inline graphic. Lower fit values ranged from Inline graphic and higher fits ranged from Inline graphic. The smallest value of Inline graphic, 80Hz, was chosen so that it would be sufficiently above an apparent “knee” in the PSD at Inline graphic. The highest value of Inline graphic was dictated by the noise floor (beyond which the amplitude of the signal was far below the amplitude of the noise).

One-kHz fixation data and lower frequency fit

After rejecting electrode pair channels which had notable Inline graphic and Inline graphic peaks in the PSD or a high noise floor (leaving 91 channels), the 1kHz fixation data from subjects 5–20 was fit to a power law form below Inline graphic. They were corrected for frequency-dependent amplifier attenuation, but not for noise floor, since the contribution of the noise floor to the spectra was not pronounced in the fit range chosen, or the channel was rejected if an excessive noise floor was observed.

First, a naïve linear least-squares fit was performed between 15–80Hz on the plot of the PSD the on Inline graphic vs. Inline graphic axes for each electrode-pair channel independently. We then divided the PSD through at each frequency by the form of equation 2, and, based upon the fit of 10kHz data, set the constraint Inline graphic (i.e. for Inline graphic, the form goes to Inline graphic, so Inline graphic). We then iteratively fit Inline graphic and Inline graphic, until both converged on stable values. Each iteration consisted of two steps. The PSD is first multiplied by a factor of Inline graphic, and the residual is fit on a log-log plot between 15–195Hz to determine a new value of Inline graphic. Then, the PSD is divided by the full form of equation 2, for all values of Inline graphic 15–195Hz in 0.25Hz increments, and the residual is fit on a log-log plot; the value of Inline graphic for which the slope of the fit is closest to zero is chosen as the new Inline graphic. These two steps were iterated until both Inline graphic and Inline graphic converged to stable values.

Spectral changes in active cortex

A finger movement task was used first as a tool to first remove synchronous rhythms from the PSD and then examine changes in the PSD during increases in brain activity. A method developed in a recent manuscript characterized how differing covariance between frequencies during different tasks allows underlying motifs to be isolated from the PSD [38]; we removed the motifs corresponding to the low frequency Inline graphic/Inline graphic rhythms during a finger movement task. Because this was shown to remove most, but not all of those rhythms, we avoided the center frequency of the beta rhythm, and performed fits to the data above 25Hz; without this method, we would not be able to address shifts in the power law process below Inline graphic60Hz, where the beta rhythm causes an intersection between the movement and rest spectra [26]. The residual movement PSD Inline graphic was then divided by the rest PSD Inline graphic at each frequency. A least-squares linear fit was then performed on the plot of Inline graphic vs. Inline graphic. The slope of this fit for each channel pair reveals whether there was a shift in the exponent, Inline graphic, of the power-law shape spectrum Inline graphic in “active” cortex. Because the shift in slope was found to be zero, then the frequency-averaged ratio Inline graphic reveals the relative overall shift in the coefficient, Inline graphic. The significance of this shift in Inline graphic was estimated with a t-test of the distribution of Inline graphic vs. zero (across electrode pair channels).

Simulation

While there are many potential models that are mathematically consistent with the form of the spectrum that we found experimentally, we performed a simplified simulation of only one such model. We model one pyramidal neuron with 6000 synapses, and only 3 simple processes to produce time dependence: Poisson-distribution arrival times of pre-synaptic action potentials (Inline graphic, timeseries denoted Inline graphic below); stereotyped, transient, exponentially decaying post-synaptic current, with Inline graphic (consistent with experiment [40], Inline graphic below), and each synapse has random peak current on the interval from −1 to 1 (arbitrary units, Inline graphic below). These are summated, and integrated over time (representing accumulated charge [66]). The leakage current of this charge through the dendritic membrane, produced by a transmembrane potential, is what we simulate as the time-dependence of the dendritic dipole (with time constant Inline graphic [67]).

graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e234.jpg (3)
graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e235.jpg (4)

Where we denote a series of delta functions reflecting the spike arrival times at synapse Inline graphic as Inline graphic, the shape of the post-synaptic response as Inline graphic (total length Inline graphic), a random number on the interval from −1 to 1, as Inline graphic, the decay timescale for dendritic current efflux as Inline graphic, and the convolution operation Inline graphic (and Inline graphic is zero padded at the edges). Inline graphic is the simulated time dependence of our surface potential measurements, from which we calculate our simulated spectrum.

As noted by Sigeti and Horsthemke [52], “2+2” spectra, such as the one we have observed from cortical surface spectra, can result from linear systems described by:

graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e245.jpg (5)
graphic file with name pcbi.1000609.e246.jpg (6)

Where Inline graphic is a Gaussian white noise variable, and the resulting power spectrum of Inline graphic, Inline graphic, has characteristic corner frequencies at Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Note that equation 6 is the same as equation 4, but that we explicitly construct Inline graphic with a convolution that allows us to connect the expression to known physiology in an intuitive way (but Inline graphic and Inline graphic ultimately do have the same properties, where the decay timescale of our Inline graphic is Inline graphic).

Our representation is intended to make the connection between the simulation and simple small-scale physiology more intuitive. The timing of this ohmic transmembrane current produced by accumulated charge gradient across the dendritic membrane is modeled as the time dependence producing the macroscale PSD. 6000 synaptic inputs, with input firing rates of 15, 30, and 60 Inline graphic were simulated for 2 minutes of 10kHz data [39]. Recent in-vivo simultaneous transmembrane and local field potential recordings have demonstrated a strong correlation between these two [68], suggesting that models like this, based upon a relationship between post-synaptic potentials and field potential, may provide useful insight.

Supporting Information

Text S1

Supplemental Detailed Methodology

(9.64 MB PDF)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Eberhard Fetz, Rajesh Rao, and Adrienne Fairhall for extensive discussion, and Nathan Crone, Nancy Kopell, Joshua Jacobs and the reviewers/editors at PLoS-CB for critical reading of the manuscript. We appreciate the time and dedication of the patients and staff at Harborview Hospital in Seattle, WA.

Footnotes

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

This research was supported by the National Science Foundation (0622252 & 0130705). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Hille B. Ionic channels of excitable membranes. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Incorporated; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Niedermeyer E, da Silva FL. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Magnetoencephalography—theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Reviews of Modern Physics. 1993;65:413–497. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Nunez PL, Cutillo BA. Neocortical dynamics and human EEG rhythms. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. p. xii, 708. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Beck A. Mapping of localization in brain and cortex by means of electric phenomena. Polska Akademica Umiejetnosci. 1891;2:186–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97:1867–1872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Steriade M. Cellular substrates of brain rhythms. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins. 1993:27–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nunez PL, Wingeier BM, Silberstein RB. Spatial-temporal structures of human alpha rhythms: Theory, microcurrent sources, multiscale measurements, and global binding of local networks. Human Brain Mapping. 2001;13:125–164. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Murthy VN, Fetz EE. Synchronization of neurons during local field potential oscillations in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;76:3968–3982. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural Synchrony in Brain Disorders: Relevance for Cognitive Dysfunctions and Pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jefferys JGR, Traub RD, Whittington MA. Neuronal networks for induced ‘40 Hz’rhythms. Trends in Neurosciences. 1996;19:202–208. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)10023-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Destexhe A, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ. Thalamic and thalamocortical mechanisms underlying 3 Hz spike-and-wave discharges. Progress in Brain Research. 1999;121:289–310. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)63080-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jackson A, Spinks RL, Freeman TCB, Wolpert DM, Lemon RN. Rhythm generation in monkey motor cortex explored using pyramidal tract stimulation. Journal of Physiology. 2002;541:685–699. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.015099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lopes da Silva FH, Hoeks A, Smits H, Zetterberg LH. Model of brain rhythmic activity. Biological Cybernetics. 1974;15:27–37. doi: 10.1007/BF00270757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Andersen P, Andersson SA. Physiological Basis of the Alpha Rhythm. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Feige B, Scheffler K, Esposito F, Di Salle F, Hennig J, et al. Cortical and subcortical correlates of electroencephalographic alpha rhythm modulation. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;93:2864–2872. doi: 10.1152/jn.00721.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Aoki F, Fetz EE, Shupe L, Lettich E, Ojemann GA. Increased gamma-range activity in human sensorimotor cortex during performance of visuomotor tasks. Clinical Neurophysiology. 1999;110:524–537. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(98)00064-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Crone NE, Miglioretti DL, Gordon B, Sieracki JM, Wilson MT, et al. Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis. I. Alpha and beta event-related desynchronization. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 12):2271–2299. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.12.2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gastaut H. Electrocorticographic study of the reactivity of rolandic rhythm. Review Neurologique (Paris) 1952;87:176–182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Miller KJ, Leuthardt EC, Schalk G, Rao RP, Anderson NR, et al. Spectral changes in cortical surface potentials during motor movement. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:2424–2432. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3886-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pfurtscheller G. Event-Related Desynchronization (ERD) and Event Related Synchronization (ERS); In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F, editors. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins; 1999. pp. 958–967. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Brovelli A, Lachaux JP, Kahane P, Boussaoud D. High gamma frequency oscillatory activity dissociates attention from intention in the human premotor cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;28:154–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, et al. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science. 2006;313:1626–1628. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Miller KJ, Shenoy P, den Nijs M, Sorensen LB, Rao RN, et al. Beyond the gamma band: the role of high-frequency features in movement classification. IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering. 2008;55:1634. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.918569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bedard C, Kroeger H, Destexhe A. Does the 1/f frequency scaling of brain signals reflect self-organized critical states? Physical Review Letters. 2006;97:118102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.97.118102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Freeman WJ, Rogers LJ, Holmes MD, Silbergeld DL. Spatial spectral analysis of human electrocorticograms including the alpha and gamma bands. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2000;95:111–121. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(99)00160-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Miller KD, Troyer TW. Neural noise can explain expansive, power-law nonlinearities in neural response functions. Journal of neurophysiology. 2002;87:653–659. doi: 10.1152/jn.00425.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Pritchard WS. The brain in fractal time: 1/f-like power spectrum scaling of the human electroencephalogram. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;66:119–129. doi: 10.3109/00207459208999796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Miller KJ, denNijs M, Shenoy P, Miller JW, Rao RP, et al. Real-time functional brain mapping using electrocorticography. Neuroimage. 2007;37:504–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Miller KJ, Sorensen LB, Ojemann JG, den Nijs M. ECoG observations of power-law scaling in the human cortex. arXiv. . 2007:0712.0846. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman MEJ. Power-law distributions in empirical data. arXiv. . 2007:706. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Nikouline VV, Palva JM, Ilmoniemi RJ. Long-Range Temporal Correlations and Scaling Behavior in Human Brain Oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:1370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-04-01370.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pereda E, Gamundi A, Rial R, González J. Non-linear behaviour of human EEG: fractal exponent versus correlation dimension in awake and sleep stages. Neuroscience Letters. 1998;250:91–94. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00435-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.de Arcangelis L, Perrone-Capano C, Herrmann HJ. Self-Organized Criticality Model for Brain Plasticity. Physical Review Letters. 2006;96:28107. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.96.028107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Freeman WJ, Holmes MD, Burke BC, Vanhatalo S. Spatial spectra of scalp EEG and EMG from awake humans. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2003;114:1053–1068. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00045-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Miller KJ, Zanos S, Fetz EE, den Nijs M, Ojemann JG. Decoupling the Cortical Power Spectrum Reveals Real-Time Representation of Individual Finger Movements in Humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3132. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5506-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Shepherd GM. The synaptic organization of the brain. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. xiv, 719. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sabatini BL, Regehr WG. Timing of neurotransmission at fast synapses in the mammalian brain. Nature. 1996;384:170–172. doi: 10.1038/384170a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Koch C. Biophysics of Computation: Information Processing in Single Neurons (Computational Neuroscience Series) New York, , NY, , USA: Oxford University Press, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ray S, Crone NE, Niebur E, Franaszczuk PJ, Hsiao SS. Neural Correlates of High-Gamma Oscillations (60–200 Hz) in Macaque Local Field Potentials and Their Potential Implications in Electrocorticography. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:11526. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2848-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bak P, Tang C, Wiesenfeld K. Self-organized criticality. Physical Review A. 1988;38:364–374. doi: 10.1103/physreva.38.364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bak P, Tang C, Wiesenfeld K. Self-organized criticality: An explanation of the 1/f noise. Physical Review Letters. 1987;59:381–384. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.59.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tang C, Bak P. Critical Exponents and Scaling Relations for Self-Organized Critical Phenomena. Physical Review Letters. 1988;60:2347–2350. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.60.2347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Christensen K, Danon L, Scanlon T, Bak P. Unified scaling law for earthquakes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2002;99:2509–2513. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012581099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Paczuski M, Boettcher S. Universality in sandpiles, interface depinning, and earthquake models. Physical Review Letters. 1996;77:111–114. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.77.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.de Sousa Vieira M. Self-organized criticality in a deterministic mechanical model. Physical Review A. 1992;46:6288–6293. doi: 10.1103/physreva.46.6288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Freyer F, Aquino K, Robinson PA, Ritter P, Breakspear M. Bistability and Non-Gaussian Fluctuations in Spontaneous Cortical Activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8512. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0754-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Beggs JM, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:11167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-11167.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lundstrom BN, Higgs MH, Spain WJ, Fairhall AL. Fractional differentiation by neocortical pyramidal neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:1335. doi: 10.1038/nn.2212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Sigeti D, Horsthemke W. High-frequency power spectra for systems subject to noise. Physical Review A. 1987;35:2276–2282. doi: 10.1103/physreva.35.2276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Crone NE, Miglioretti DL, Gordon B, Lesser RP. Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis. II. Event-related synchronization in the gamma band. Brain. 1998;121(Pt 12):2301–2315. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.12.2301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Logothetis NK, Kayser C, Oeltermann A. In vivo measurement of cortical impedance spectrum in monkeys: implications for signal propagation. Neuron. 2007;55:809–823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature. 2001;412:150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Taylor K, Mandon S, Freiwald WA, Kreiter AK. Coherent oscillatory activity in monkey area v4 predicts successful allocation of attention. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:1424–1437. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Siegel M, Konig P. A functional gamma-band defined by stimulus-dependent synchronization in area 18 of awake behaving cats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:4251–4260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04251.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Henrie JA, Shapley R. LFP power spectra in V1 cortex: the graded effect of stimulus contrast. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:479–490. doi: 10.1152/jn.00919.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Belitski A, Gretton A, Magri C, Murayama Y, Montemurro MA, et al. Low-frequency local field potentials and spikes in primary visual cortex convey independent visual information. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:5696. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0009-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Liu J, Newsome WT. Local field potential in cortical area MT: stimulus tuning and behavioral correlations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:7779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5052-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Hoogenboom N, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Parkes LM, Fries P. Localizing human visual gamma-band activity in frequency, time and space. Neuroimage. 2006;29:764–773. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Siegel M, Donner TH, Oostenveld R, Fries P, Engel AK. High-frequency activity in human visual cortex is modulated by visual motion strength. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:732. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Manning JR, Jacobs J, Fried I, Kahana MJ. Broadband shifts in LFP power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:13613–13620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2041-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:45–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Fries P, Nikoli D, Singer W. The gamma cycle. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:309–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Connor JA, Stevens CF. Inward and delayed outward membrane currents in isolated neural somata under voltage clamp. The Journal of Physiology. 1971;213:1–19. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1971.sp009364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Koch C, Rapp M, Segev I. A brief history of time (constants). Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6:93. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Okun M, Lampl I. Intracellular recordings in awake rodent unveil the relation between local field potential and neuronal firing. Chicago, , Il, , USA: Society for Neuroscience; 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Miller KJ, Makeig S, Hebb AO, Rao RP, denNijs M, et al. Cortical electrode localization from X-rays and simple mapping for electrocorticographic research: The “Location on Cortex” (LOC) package for MATLAB. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007;162:303–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Text S1

Supplemental Detailed Methodology

(9.64 MB PDF)


Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES