Abstract
Rod and cone photoreceptors detect light and relay this information through a multisynaptic pathway to the brain via retinal ganglion cells (RGCs)1. These retinal outputs support not only pattern vision, but also non-image forming (NIF) functions, which include circadian photoentrainment and pupillary light reflex (PLR). In mammals, NIF functions are mediated by rods, cones and the melanopsin-containing intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs)2, 3. Rod/cone photoreceptors and ipRGCs are complementary in signalling light intensity for NIF functions4–12. The ipRGCs, in addition to being directly photosensitive, also receive synaptic input from rod/cone networks13, 14. To determine how the ipRGCs relay rod/cone light information for both image and non-image forming functions, we genetically ablated ipRGCs in mice. Here we show that animals lacking ipRGCs retain pattern vision, but have deficits in both PLR and circadian photoentrainment that are more extensive than those observed in melanopsin knockouts8, 10, 11. The defects in PLR and photoentrainment resemble those observed in animals that lack phototransduction in all three photoreceptor classes6. These results indicate that light signals for irradiance detection are dissociated from pattern vision at the retinal ganglion cell level, and animals that cannot detect light for NIF functions are still capable of image formation.
The retinal ganglion cells that express melanopsin (rendering them intrinsically photosensitive) send monosynaptic projections to the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) and the intergeniculate leaflet (IGL), responsible for circadian photoentrainment, and the olivary pretectal nucleus (OPN), responsible for PLR15. It has been reported that RGCs that do not contain melanopsin innervate the same NIF brain centres15–19. In mice genetically engineered to lack melanopsin protein, the RGCs that would normally express this opsin still project to the NIF centres, but these cells are no longer intrinsically photosensitive. In these animals, PLR and circadian light responses are reduced but not absent, indicating that rods/cones are capable of light detection for NIF functions8–11. The rod/cone signals for NIF functions are undoubtedly relayed to the brain by RGCs, but it is unclear whether ipRGCs, conventional RGCs, or both are responsible (Fig. 1a).
To eliminate ipRGCs, we introduced an attenuated diphtheria toxin A subunit (aDTA)20 into the mouse melanopsin gene locus (Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). Using antibody staining in retinas from animals expressing aDTA (Opn4aDTA/+), we found 3.1±1.5% of melanopsin cells remained in the Opn4aDTA/+ animals (Fig. 1b). X-gal staining in animals with the aDTA gene in one melanopsin allele and tau-LacZ in the other (Opn4aDTA/tau-LacZ)3 revealed that 17.2±1.0% of melanopsin cells remained in the retinas of these animals at 6 months of age (Fig. 1c). Consistent with the elimination of ipRGCs, we observed fewer fibre terminals in the SCN, IGL and OPN of Opn4aDTA/tau-LacZ compared to Opn4tau-LacZ/+ animals (Fig. 2a–c). The degree of ipRGC ablation in the heterozygous animals increased with age (Supplementary Fig. 2a–c).
To more completely ablate ipRGCs, we generated animals homozygous for aDTA (Opn4aDTA/aDTA). The morphology and thickness of the retina in the Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals was not different from wild types (Fig. 1d). Injection of fluorescently conjugated cholera toxin in the eye, which labels all ganglion cell fibres from the retina, showed that few fibres innervated the SCN and IGL in the Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals (Fig. 2d, e). Furthermore, comparison between age-matched Opn4aDTA/+ and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice verified that the extent of ipRGC ablation was greater in homozygous animals (Supplementary Fig. 2d). We also observed that target innervation by other RGCs was unaffected both in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus, which is important for image formation, and in the rostral core of the OPN15 (Fig. 2e, f).
To assess whether image-forming functions are affected in animals expressing aDTA, we measured electroretinograms, optokinetic nystagmus responses (OKN), visual acuity and the ability of the animals to detect a visual cue. We found that the electroretinograms and OKN were normal in animals that lack ipRGCs (Supplementary Fig. 3a, b). Based on optomotor responses, the acuity of Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice was slightly decreased compared to wild-type (Fig. 1e) and Opn4tau-LacZ/tau-LacZ mice (Supplementary Fig. 3d). This effect is most likely due to enlarged pupil diameters in the Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals (see Fig. 3c and Supplementary Fig. 3c). We also determined that these animals could use a visual cue to locate a flag-marked platform in a water maze paradigm (Fig. 1e). These results demonstrate that image formation is functional despite the elimination of ipRGCs in Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals. Therefore, we could determine the relative contribution of ipRGCs to PLR and circadian photoentrainment in the context of normal image formation.
Pupil constriction regulates the amount of light entering the eye and thus pupil diameter is negatively correlated with light intensity. At high light intensity, the iris decreases the area of the pupil by 95% (full constriction) compared to dark-adapted conditions (fully dilated). At low light intensity in which the pupil constricts 50% or less, rod/cone input is the main signal8, 12 (Supplementary Table 1). On the other hand, the intrinsic photosensitivity through the melanopsin protein in ipRGCs is necessary for full pupil constriction8, 12 (Supplementary Table 1). We found that the PLR was absent in all Opn4aDTA/+ mice at a light intensity that constricts the pupil in wild-type animals to ~50% (Fig. 3a, b). This result suggests that although the melanopsin protein is not required for PLR at low light intensity, rod/cone input still requires the ipRGCs. We propose that ipRGCs are able to code light intensities that are below the detection level of the melanopsin photopigment by integrating rod/cone signals and relaying this information to the OPN for pupil constriction.
At high light intensity, 6 of 9 Opn4aDTA/+ mice constricted their pupils to 95%, similar to wild-type animals (Fig. 3a, b). Full pupil constriction in these 6 animals indicates that the intrinsic photosensitivity persists in the remaining ipRGCs in Opn4aDTA/+ mice and that only ~17% of ipRGCs are sufficient to drive full pupil constriction. The remaining 3 animals had pupil constriction defects at high light intensity (59±14% compared to 96±1% in wild type; Fig. 3b). Interestingly, when the Opn4aDTA/+ animals were placed under 24-hour light/dark cycles, animals that had full pupil constriction photoentrained (Supplementary Fig. 4a), while animals with defective pupil constriction had photoentrainment defects (Supplementary Fig. 4b).
The aDTA homozygotes have greater ablation of ipRGCs, and consequently all 12 Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals had defective PLR (42±6%) at high light intensity (Fig. 3c, d). It is noteworthy to mention that similar pupil constriction (~40%) to that observed in Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice is achieved by both wild-type and melanopsin knockout animals to a light stimulation that is ~1000-fold less in intensity8. This reduction in pupil sensitivity was observed in an intensity response curve in Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice at constrictions that are 55% or higher in the wild types (Supplementary Fig. 5). Together, these data show that the rod/cone-dependent pupil constriction is reliant on signalling through ipRGCs at all light intensities (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Fig. 5). The residual pupil constriction observed in the homozygous animals may reveal a possible role for the non-melanopsin RGCs in pupil constriction. Alternatively, this residual pupil constriction could originate from rod/cone input through a few remaining ipRGCs in the Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice.
To assess the contribution of ipRGCs to circadian photoentrainment, we analyzed the wheel-running activity of wild-type (n=11) and Opn4aDTA/aDTA (n=12) animals using 24-hour light dark cycles. Under constant dark conditions (DD) both wild-type and Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals had a functional circadian oscillator with period lengths of 23.3±0.1 and 23.8±0.1, respectively (Fig. 4a). Similar to Math5−/− mice with severe optic nerve hypoplasia, Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals had significantly longer periods compared to wild types (Supplementary Table 2) confirming the loss of the fibres projecting to the SCN21. A light stimulus (1500 lux) administered for 15 minutes at CT16 delayed the phase onset of the activity of the wild types by 1.66±0.23 hours while it did not affect the phase onset of activity in any Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals (−0.06±0.09 hours) (Fig. 4a). When the light/dark cycle was advanced or delayed by 6 hours, the activity of all wild-type animals synchronized with the shifted cycle (Fig. 4b and Supplementary Table 2). Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals segregated into two groups with distinct responses. The first group of animals (8 of 12) free ran completely under the shifted light/dark cycle (Fig. 4b and Supplementary Fig. 6b–f) similar to animals that lack all functional photoreceptors in the retina6, indicating that they were completely blind to the light shift. The second group of animals (4 of 12) was weakly light responsive, but not photoentrained, since they did not display a stable phase relation to the light/dark cycle (Supplementary Fig. 7). Given that these 4 animals demonstrated very weak light responsiveness in the light/dark cycle, but were not phase-shifted by the 15 minute light pulse, we tested whether constant light (LL) conditions would lengthen the circadian period of Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice. Under LL, 7 of 11 wild-type animals had periods longer than 24 hours10, while the other 4 animals became completely arrhythmic (Supplementary Fig. 6a). In contrast, both groups of Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals had periods shorter than 24 hours (Fig. 4c, Supplementary Fig. 6b–f, 7 and Supplementary Table 2). Previously, it had been demonstrated that animals lacking only the melanopsin protein are similar to wild-type controls in adjusting to a 24-hour light/dark cycle and have only minor defects in both their period lengthening under constant light and phase-delaying pulses of light10, 11. The severe defects in circadian photoentrainment observed in animals lacking ipRGCs demonstrate that these cells are required not only for intrinsically signalling light information through the melanopsin protein, but also for conveying rod/cone light information to the SCN. Although several reports indicated that RGCs that do not express melanopsin innervate the SCN17–19, 22, our results reveal that the contribution of rod/cone signalling through these RGCs for photoentrainment is negligible.
The acute effects of light on activity (also known as masking) can be studied by utilizing an ultradian 7-hour light/dark (3.5:3.5 LD) cycle that disrupts the oscillator6. Under the ultradian paradigm, wild types confined their activity mostly to the dark portion of the ultradian cycle (84±4%). In contrast, the Opn4aDTA/aDTA animals were active nearly randomly across an ultradian day irrespective of light (64±3%; Fig. 4d). This result demonstrates that melanopsin-containing cells are essential for a direct light-driven physiological response that is independent of the circadian oscillator.
In summary, we have shown that the loss of ipRGCs does not influence image formation and therefore involvement of this ganglion cell class in classical vision is only modulatory13, 23. In contrast, irradiance-dependent NIF functions are substantially impaired in the absence of ipRGCs. Given that the ipRGCs constitute less than 2% of the total RGC population, it is striking that light information for circadian photoentrainment and PLR is predominantly conveyed through these cells. Moreover, the ability to form pattern vision does not impact photoentrainment.
Methods Summary
Animals
All experiments were conducted in accordance with NIH guidelines and approved institutional animal care and use committees of the Universities involved.
Behavioural analyses
We used behavioural tests that measure the integrity of the outer retina (electroretinograms)23, eye tracking functions (optokinetic nystagmus)24, visual acuity (optomotor)25, object identification (water maze)26, pupil constriction (PLR)8, 12, the period of the circadian oscillator (wheel running activity)10, 11, the adjustment of the circadian clock to different light stimulations (circadian photoentrainment, phase shifting, and constant light) 10, 11 and direct light effects on activity (masking)9.
Methods
aDTA mice
Using the homologous arms that we used previously3, we targeted the aDTA gene to the melanopsin locus. The targeting construct contained the diphtheria toxin A subunit and the neomycin resistance genes. The construct was flanked by a 4.4kb 5′ of the ATG site of the mouse melanopsin gene, and a 1.6kb fragment containing 654bp of exon9 plus 946bp 3′UTR. Following electroporation of the linearized construct into 129.1 mouse strain embryonic stem (ES) cells and drug selection (400μg/ml of G418), one positive ES clone was injected into C57BL/6 blastocysts. Chimeric animals were mated to C57BL/6 mice to produce heterozygous animals.
Immunostaining
Whole retinas from Opn4aDTA/+ and wild-type animals, fixed in 70% ethanol, were immunostained using the c-terminus melanopsin antibody (1:500 dilution). Fluorescently-conjugated secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:1000 dilution), Molecular Probes, OR) was applied for 1–3hrs.
X-gal staining
Animals anaesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of Avertin (20ml/kg) were intracardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde, and brains and eyes isolated. Eye-cups or brain sections (50micron) were first incubated in Buffer B (100mM phosphate buffer at pH7.4, 2mM MgCl2, 0.01% sodium deoxycholate, 0.02% IGEPAL) then stained in Buffer B plus 5mM potassium ferricyanide, 5mM potassium ferrocyanide, and 1mg/ml X-gal for 3 days.
Cholera toxin injections in the eye
Mice were anesthetized with Ketamine (80mg/kg)/Xylazine (8mg/kg). Eyes were injected intravitreally with 2μL of cholera toxin B subunit conjugated with Alexa Fluor 488 or 555 (Invitrogen, CA). Three days after injection, brains were isolated and sectioned.
Electroretinograms
Animals maintained under 12:12 light/dark cycles for 3 days were used to collect data within the light phase (after 50mins dark adaptation). The ERG setup was similar to that we used previously23. Mydriatics (tropicamide 1% and phenylephrine 2.5%) and hypromellose solution (0.3%) were used to dilate the pupil and retain corneal moisture in anesthetized mice. A signal conditioner (Model 1902 Mark III, Cambridge Electronic Design, Ltd., CED, UK) differentially amplified (×3000) and filtered (band-pass filter cut-off 0.5 to 200Hz) the signal before it was digitized (Model 1401, CED) and recorded (sampling rate 10kHz) via the Signal 2.15 Software (CED, UK).
White light (10ms in duration) was provided by a xenon arc source (Cairn Research, Ltd., UK). Neutral density filters (Edmund Optics, UK) were used (unattenuated intensity, 320 μW/cm2 at the cornea). For scotopic recordings, flashes were administered in the dark and testing began with the dimmest stimulus. Depending on the intensity, stimuli were presented at a rate of 0.5 to 0.2Hz and 6–20 repetitions were collected and averaged. Photopic ERGs were recorded by presentation of an unattenuated light (20 stimuli presented at a rate of 1Hz) against blue filtered (Grass blue filter, Astro-Med, RI) rod-saturating background light (160μW/cm2).
OKN
A mouse stabilized with a head post was placed into an acrylic holder in a 12′ inch diameter drum. Computer generated stimuli were projected down onto the drum walls. Black and white stripes 4 degrees in width were rotated at five degrees per second. Mice eye movements were captured with an infrared video system. The fast saccade components were counted per 30-second intervals using an algorithm, which evaluated the high velocity eye movements that followed a slow velocity movement in the opposite direction (ISCAN, MA).
Visual Acuity
A virtual cylinder “OptoMotry” (Cerebral Mechanics, Lethbridge, Canada) was used to determine visual acuity by measuring the image-tracking reflex of mice25. A sine wave grating was projected on the screen rotating in a virtual cylinder. The animal was assessed for a tracking response upon stimulation for approximately 5 seconds. All acuity thresholds were determined using the staircase method with 100% contrast.
Morris Water Maze
In order to assess the ability of mice to detect a visual cue, we trained the animals to find a platform marked by a 10cm tall, high contrast visual cue under bright light (500lux) in an 85cm pool. On day one, mice were trained with 4 trials 15 minutes apart. On the following day, latency to find the island was recorded first with and second without the cue.
PLR
All animals were dark adapted for at least one hour and the eye of each animal receiving the photic stimulus was treated with 0.1% atropine prior to the start of recording. Measurements were restricted to the middle of the subjective day (CT 4–8). One eye of each mouse was digitally captured at a frequency of 1 image per second for 63 seconds using a CCD camera. The light stimuli (xenon arc light source) consisted of a 60 second pulse at an intensity of 3.8mW/cm2 or 1.8μW/cm2 of white light. For the Opn4aDTA/aDTA experiments, the eye receiving the photic stimulus was treated with 1% atropine. While one eye received light stimulation from a 470nm LED light source (E27-B24, Super Bright LEDs, MO; 161μW/cm2), a digital camcorder (Sony DCRHC96, Japan) was used to record from the other eye (30 seconds) at 30frames/second under a 940nm light (LDP LLC, NJ). The digital video recoding was deconstructed to individual frames using Blaze MediaPro Software (Mystik Media, NC). The percent pupil constriction was calculated as the percent of pupil area at 30 seconds after the initiation of the stimulus (steady state) relative to the dilated pupil size.
Wheel Running Activity
Mice were placed in cages with a 4.5-inch running wheel and their activity was monitored with VitalView software (Mini Mitter, OR). Period was calculated using ClockLab (Actimetrics, IL). For phase shifting experiments, each animal was exposed to a light pulse (1500lux; CT16) for 15min. After 41 days of constant dark, mice were then re-entrained to 12:12 light/dark cycles for 19 days. Animals were then exposed to two jet-lag paradigms: 16 days of a six-hour advance followed by 32 days of a six-hour delay. During the last two weeks of this treatment the animals were tested for PLR. Animals were then exposed to constant light for three weeks followed by an ultradian 3.5:3.5 light/dark cycles.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Jennifer Mackes and Gwendolyn Harrison for help in genotyping the animals, Drs. Rejji Kuruvilla, Mark Van Doren, Beverly Wendland, Marnie Halpern, Michael Caterina, Chih-Ying Su, Jonathan Bradley and lab members in the Biology Department at the Johns Hopkins University for scientific discussions and comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (to S.H. and K.-W.Y.), the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (to R.J.L.) and the David and Lucile Packard and Alfred P. Sloan Foundations (to S.H.).
Footnotes
Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions. The authors declare no competing financial interests. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to S.H. (shattar@jhu.edu) or R.J.L. (robert.lucas@manchester.ac.uk).
Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at www.nature.com/nature.
Author Contributions A.D.G. and S.H. wrote the paper. J.L.E., R.J.L., D.M.B. and T.C.B. gave helpful comments on the manuscript. A.D.G., J.L.E., and C.M.A. in S.H.’s laboratory carried out all the behavioural studies on the aDTA homozygous animals, as well as the X-gal staining of the Opn4aDTA/tau-lacZ and Opn4aDTA/+ animals, the morphology of the retina, the cholera toxin injections, the water maze and the optomotor studies. D.M.B. helped in analyzing the brains of the Opn4aDTA/tau-lacZ and the cholera toxin-injected animals. G.S.L. and A.R.B. in R.J.L.’s laboratory carried out all the behavioural studies on the aDTA heterozygous animals, and the ERG studies. T.C.B. provided the construct and suggestions for the aDTA targeting strategy. H.C. carried out the OKN recordings. H.-W.L. in K.-W.Y.’s laboratory carried out the melanopsin immunostaining on aDTA heterozygous mice. Animals were first conceived in K.-W.Y.’s laboratory and produced by S.H. and S. Haq to the chimeric stage. Germ-line transmission was obtained independently in the laboratories of S.H. (with help from H.Z.) and K.-W.Y. All other authors helped in the planning, technical support and discussions of experiments.
References
- 1.Hubel DH. Eye, brain, and vision (Scientific American Library) W.H. Freeman; New York: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295:1070–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1067262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, Berson DM, Yau KW. Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science. 2002;295:1065–70. doi: 10.1126/science.1069609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Freedman MS, et al. Regulation of mammalian circadian behavior by non-rod, non-cone, ocular photoreceptors. Science. 1999;284:502–4. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5413.502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Czeisler CA, et al. Suppression of melatonin secretion in some blind patients by exposure to bright light. N Engl J Med. 1995;332:6–11. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199501053320102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hattar S, et al. Melanopsin and rod-cone photoreceptive systems account for all major accessory visual functions in mice. Nature. 2003;424:75–81. doi: 10.1038/nature01761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Panda S, et al. Melanopsin is required for non-image-forming photic responses in blind mice. Science. 2003;301:525–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1086179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lucas RJ, et al. Diminished pupillary light reflex at high irradiances in melanopsin-knockout mice. Science. 2003;299:245–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1077293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mrosovsky N, Hattar S. Impaired masking responses to light in melanopsin-knockout mice. Chronobiol Int. 2003;20:989–99. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120026043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Panda S, et al. Melanopsin (Opn4) requirement for normal light-induced circadian phase shifting. Science. 2002;298:2213–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1076848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ruby NF, et al. Role of melanopsin in circadian responses to light. Science. 2002;298:2211–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1076701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lucas RJ, Douglas RH, Foster RG. Characterization of an ocular photopigment capable of driving pupillary constriction in mice. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:621–6. doi: 10.1038/88443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dacey DM, et al. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature. 2005;433:749–54. doi: 10.1038/nature03387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wong KY, Dunn FA, Graham DM, Berson DM. Synaptic influences on rat ganglion-cell photoreceptors. J Physiol. 2007 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.133751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hattar S, et al. Central projections of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. J Comp Neurol. 2006;497:326–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.20970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gooley JJ, Lu J, Fischer D, Saper CB. A broad role for melanopsin in nonvisual photoreception. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7093–106. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-18-07093.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hannibal J, Fahrenkrug J. Target areas innervated by PACAP-immunoreactive retinal ganglion cells. Cell Tissue Res. 2004;316:99–113. doi: 10.1007/s00441-004-0858-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Morin LP, Blanchard JH, Provencio I. Retinal ganglion cell projections to the hamster suprachiasmatic nucleus, intergeniculate leaflet, and visual midbrain: bifurcation and melanopsin immunoreactivity. J Comp Neurol. 2003;465:401–16. doi: 10.1002/cne.10881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sollars PJ, et al. Melanopsin and non-melanopsin expressing retinal ganglion cells innervate the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus. Vis Neurosci. 2003;20:601–10. doi: 10.1017/s0952523803206027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Maxwell F, Maxwell IH, Glode LM. Cloning, sequence determination, and expression in transfected cells of the coding sequence for the tox 176 attenuated diphtheria toxin A chain. Mol Cell Biol. 1987;7:1576–9. doi: 10.1128/mcb.7.4.1576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wee R, Castrucci AM, Provencio I, Gan L, Van Gelder RN. Loss of photic entrainment and altered free-running circadian rhythms in math5−/− mice. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10427–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-23-10427.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gooley JJ, Lu J, Chou TC, Scammell TE, Saper CB. Melanopsin in cells of origin of the retinohypothalamic tract. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1165. doi: 10.1038/nn768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Barnard AR, Hattar S, Hankins MW, Lucas RJ. Melanopsin regulates visual processing in the mouse retina. Curr Biol. 2006;16:389–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.12.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Iwakabe H, Katsuura G, Ishibashi C, Nakanishi S. Impairment of papillary responses and optokinetic nystagmus in the mGluR6-deficient mouse. Neuropharmacology. 1997;36:135–43. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(96)00167-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Prusky GT, Alam NM, Beekman S, Douglas RM. Rapid quantification of adult and developing mouse spatial vision using a virtual optomotor system. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:4611–6. doi: 10.1167/iovs.04-0541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:848–58. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.