Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cognition. 2010 May 23;116(2):283–288. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.001

A multidimensional scaling analysis of own- and cross-race face spaces

Megan H Papesh 1, Stephen D Goldinger 1
PMCID: PMC2900505  NIHMSID: NIHMS204880  PMID: 20501337

Abstract

We examined predictions derived from Valentine’s (1991) Multidimensional Space (MDS) framework for own- and other-race face processing. A set of 20 computerized faces was generated from a single prototype. Each face was saved as Black and White, changing only skin tone, such that structurally identical faces were represented in both race categories. Participants made speeded “same-different” judgments to all possible combinations of faces, from which we generated psychological spaces, with “different” RTs as the measure of similarity. Consistent with the MDS framework, all faces were pseudo-normally distributed around the (unseen) prototype. The distribution of faces was consistent with Valentine’s (1991) predictions: Despite their physical identity to the White faces, Black faces had lower mean inter-object distances in psychological space. Other-race faces are more densely clustered in psychological space, which could underlie well-known recognition deficits.


In general, people are expert face processors, capable of recognizing hundreds of faces, even decades after their last exposure (Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittlinger, 1975). This expertise, however, does not always extend to faces belonging to members of races other than one’s own. Other-race effects1 (OREs) have been observed in recognition memory (Meissner & Brigham, 2001), speeded classification (Levin, 1996), stereotypical memory distortions (Eberhardt, Dasgupta, & Banaszynski, 2003), and many other measures. To date, theorists have proposed varied explanations for OREs, citing levels of inter-racial contact (Ng & Lindsay, 1994), perceptual expertise (Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991), overlearning (Goldstein & Chance, 1980), and the treatment of race as a basic visual feature (Levin, 2000).

One prominent theory has been Valentine’s (1991) Multidimensional Space (MDS) framework, which was proposed to explain effects of distinctiveness, caricaturing, inversion, and race in face perception. According to this framework, faces are represented as points in an n-dimensional Euclidean space, wherein each dimension represents some physiognomic aspect of faces useful for discrimination. The framework assumes that faces are normally distributed in this space around the central tendency, with information derived from a lifetime of experience. Because people have more experience with own-race faces, Valentine predicted that the learned dimensions would mainly reflect the most useful features for differentiating among own race faces. These dimensions may prove less efficient for other-race faces. Therefore, the MDS model predicts that the dispersion of faces in psychological space will be different for own- and other-race faces. Specifically, own-race faces should disperse relatively sparsely, reflecting precise appreciation of the details used in perception and memory. Other-race faces should cluster more tightly, reflecting their confusability along dimensions better suited for own-race discrimination.

Although the MDS framework is very influential, the idiosyncratic variations in natural faces have made it difficult to empirically scrutinize. Faces are a class of homogenous stimuli, which vary continuously along many underlying (and unknown) dimensions (Byatt & Rhodes, 1998). Thus, faces may appear clustered in psychological space because of race, but other freely-varying factors can also affect MDS solutions. The functional utility of Valentine’s model is that, although dimensions are not specified a priori (but see Catz et al., in press), they are exploited in perceptual decisions. Thus, in a given task, responses can lead to the creation of a visualizable space from which relevant dimensions can be inferred. In the current study, we applied this MDS logic, while controlling the potential confounds that usually arise in cross-race face studies. Specifically, we used established multidimensional scaling techniques (Kruskal & Wish, 1978) with synthesized faces, controlling all structural factors of the faces, and using a speeded “same-different” task that minimizes response strategies.

Valentine’s (1991) MDS framework supports two non-mutually exclusive models, a norm-based coding (NBC) model, and an exemplar-only, absolute-coding (ABC) model. As discussed by Byatt and Rhodes (1998), the primary assumption of NBC is that faces are represented in terms of deviation from a norm (typically the own-race prototype), such that all that is stored per face is a vector distance from the prototype. Conversely, in ABC, each face is encoded according to its absolute value per dimension, and is represented by a single point. The key prediction regarding unequal, race-based dispersion of faces is common to both models, and was the prediction tested here. Note, however, that distinct predictions have been derived and tested for each model (Byatt & Rhodes, 1998).

Prior support for Valentine’s (1991) MDS framework has come from both empirical research and computational modeling. Experimental evidence has predominantly come attempts to multidimensionally scale faces, allowing examination of the resultant psychological spaces. Johnston, Milne, Williams, and Hoise (1997) examined the basic that more typical faces should cluster toward the center of psychological space, with more distinctive faces located along the periphery. Using distinctiveness ratings and a similarity judgment task, Johnston et al. (1977) found that the average distance between the origin of the space and typical faces was smaller than the average distance between the origin and distinctive faces, supporting the theoretically-predicted architecture of psychological space. Byatt and Rhodes (2004) examined the race-based predictions of the MDS model. They had White participants rate the similarity of pairs of White and Chinese faces (using real photographs), which produced an MDS space with a denser cluster of other-race faces, relative to own-race faces. Furthermore, the spatial locations of faces were valid predictors of future identification performance, such that more proximal faces were more difficult to identify.

Computational analyses have also assessed the spread of faces in psychological space, and have again found differences in face dispersion, based on race. Caldara and Abdi (2006) trained two neural networks as either “pure Caucasians” or “pure Asians” (i.e., each model was trained with only one race, without exposure to the other). After training, each network was presented with faces from the untrained race. From the models, Caldara and Abdi derived the Euclidean distances and cosine values for each set of faces, both within its own (same-race) network and within the other-race network. In general, the psychological spaces supported Valentine’s predictions: Faces in the same-race space were represented more diffusely, with greater pairwise distances between the faces, relative to the same faces represented within the other-race space.

Although Byatt and Rhodes (2004) and Caldara and Abdi (2006) demonstrated results consistent with the predictions of Valentine’s (1991) MDS framework, their approaches did not allow alternatives to be ruled out. Byatt and Rhodes derived spaces using similarity judgments from only one race of participants; Caldara and Abdi trained neural networks in an extreme and unrealistic manner. In the present study, we evaluated the prediction from Valentine (1991) by deriving psychological spaces from participants’ speeded same-different ratings to well-specified computer-generated faces. Using these faces, we could test only one race of participants, but still cover the full experimental design. Specifically, we manipulated the perceived race of each face, such that participants rated each face, both when it was an own- and other-race face. Because Valentine’s framework is based on the hypothetical “face space” in memory, we used multidimensional scaling procedures. We used the same-different procedure because response times (RTs) in this task provide an indirect measure of psychological similarity (i.e., people are faster to respond “different” when two faces are less similar), while minimizing strategic analysis by participants (Sergent & Takane, 1987). Examining RTs allowed us to generate a psychological space that is intuitive (distance in space is negatively correlated with RT) and free of response bias.

Method

Participants

Seventy-three Arizona State University undergraduates participated in exchange for partial course credit. Sixty-eight percent of the participants self-reported as White, 3% as Black, and 29% as “other.” All participants had normal or corrected vision.

Stimuli

FaceGen Modeller software (Singular Inversions, Inc., 2004) was used to create a racially ambiguous male face, 2 a prototype from which we created a set of 20 new faces. One of our goals was to examine the psychological space, knowing that the structure can be logically predicted from the design of the materials (i.e., as a validation procedure). Faces were generated by systematically distorting the prototype face with the “genetic randomness” tool available in the software. When genetic randomness is set to a low value (e.g., 20%), the new face bears a sibling-like resemblance to the original. As the parameter increases, faces look more like cousins, and eventually unrelated. As shown in Figure 1, two faces were created at each of the randomness levels in five-step increments from 10% – 55% (the two faces per level were never identical).3 One face per level was set to a Black skin tone; the other was set to a White skin tone. A second set of faces was created by reversing the assignments of skin tone, such that each face represented a Black man and a White man, in counterbalanced fashion across participants. This allowed the same stimuli to served as both own- and other-race faces, greatly increasing experimental control. The 61 shape parameters provided by FaceGen were compared for each Black-White pair, confirming that the faces were structurally identical.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Two sets of faces generated from a single, racially-ambiguous prototype. The percentages listed down the center represent percentages of distortion from the original image. Faces connected by lines (bottom) are structurally identical and only differ in racial coloring.

Procedure

Participants performed two blocks of speeded same-different judgments, separated by a go/no-go task (Levin, 2000). During each same-different block, participants made judgments to all 192 face pairs in a set (presentation order of the sets was counterbalanced and pair presentation was random), with 40 trials containing identical faces. Each face was 400 × 400 pixels, and pairs were presented side-by-side, approximately 5 cm apart. Participants quickly pressed buttons labeled “same” and “different,” and no feedback was given. Trials were separated by a 1-second ITI. During the go/no-go task, participants completed two blocks, each containing 60 trials. In each block, participants saw individual faces for a maximum of 2000 ms and were asked to push the ‘1’ key, classifying by race. In one block, they responded only to Black faces and, in the other, only to White faces (block order was counterbalanced). Feedback was provided, in the form of a red ‘X’ and a 3000-ms penalty, following incorrect responses. E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, 2006) was used to conduct the experiments on 17” CRT monitors (which minimize visual distortion across screen positions).

Results

We briefly consider the raw same-different RTs, followed by the go/no-go results. We then focus on the critical MDS analyses.

Go/No-Go RTs

We replicated the pattern from Levin (2000), finding faster responses to Black faces (232.7 ms) than to White faces (246.4 ms), t(70) = −2.66, p < .05. Although these data were not used for MDS analyses, we included this task to verify that our stimuli reproduced a well-documented phenomenon. According to Levin (2000), people are faster to verify Black faces because they impart the basic feature “+Black.” Although our data cannot speak to this interpretation, our results suggest that race differences were perceived as intended.

Same-Different RTs

Within-race (White-White and Black-Black) judgment RTs were analyzed in a paired-samples t-test. Same-different judgments were significantly slower for pairs of Black faces (1662 ms) than for pairs of White faces (1544 ms), t(72) = 2.11, p = .03. This pattern was consistent for all within-race comparisons; RTs were unaffected by the degree of relative distortion between the faces, F(7, 504) = 1.79, p = .08.

Scaling Solutions

MDS solutions were based on similarity estimates, derived from correct RTs in “different” trials, such that RTs positively correlated with psychological similarity. The scaling analyses were completed in two steps, using functions in SPSS (Giguère, 2006). We conducted our analyses in several stages: We first derived PROXSCAL solutions for each of the matrices derived separately (i.e., the first and second blocks of same-different trials). We derived both two- and three-dimensional solutions for each matrix; these were equivalent in explained variances, with each approximately R2 = 75%. Given this finding, and their greater visual interpretability, we limited all analyses to two-dimensional solutions.

Our next step was to create within-race similarity matrices, for all possible White-White and Black-Black pairs. These matrices were also analyzed using PROXSCAL, leading to the separate two-dimensional solutions shown in Figure 2. As shown, both solutions were well-formed, with a general trend for smaller distortions to occur toward the center of the spaces. Thus, although the prototype was never presented to participants, the faces appear to organize around it in a distortion-specific manner. This suggests that people appreciated fine differences among the stimuli, whether the faces were Black or White. It is important to note two aspects of the results in Figure 2: First, although both spaces are well-formed, the space for White faces (left panel) has a more defined shape. Second, although both solutions occupy most of their depicted spaces, MDS solutions do not provide a clear depiction of absolute distance. That is, each solution is illustrated in an optimal manner (the program “zooms in”), which means that one solution is potentially “bigger” in true psychological space than the other. For this reason, our final step was to combine matrices, deriving an overall solution for all possible pairs, including the same faces when perceived as Black and White.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Separately derived two-dimensional spaces for the White (left panel) and Black (right panel) faces. The axes represent the primary and secondary dimensions, shown in normalized units.

To create a combined solution, we used the separate matrices (from blocks 1 and 2), and applied PREFSCAL (multidimensional unfolding) to create a combined common space, using normalized distances (Busing, Groenen, & Heiser, 2005). This analysis seeks the best solution for all stimuli, by adjusting positions of each item in space, in the usual gradient-descent process. We conducted the analysis with 10,000 different starting configurations and random seeds, with highly stable results. The derived common space (Figure 3) explained 85% of the variance, with normalized stress = .003. The primary (horizontal) dimension was clearly race, as light and dark-skinned faces grouped to separate halves of the space. Despite the physical similarity of certain faces (e.g., light and dark versions of a 40% morph), there were essentially no mixed-race clusters. Within the race groups, faces were again roughly arranged by levels of distortion: Similar distortion levels were often close “neighbors.” Of key interest, when all faces are analyzed together, the best-fitting interpretation of the data shows a spread among the White faces, and a general compression among the Black faces, suggesting that absolute psychological distances differed across depicted races.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Combined, two-dimensional solution for all faces. The axes represent the primary and secondary dimensions, shown in arbitrary units.

Distance Analyses

To test Valentine’s (1991) MDS hypothesis, we analyzed the inter-face distances, as estimated by PREFSCAL. Distances were pooled within each set to obtain averages for all possible light-light pairs, and all dark-dark pairs; these averages were tested in a within-subjects, repeated-measures ANOVA. As suggested by Figure 3, the average distance among light-skinned faces (210 arbitrary units) was significantly greater than the distance among the dark-skinned faces (165 units), F(1, 80) = 186.3, p < .0001, η2 p = .85.

Discussion

In the present study, we empirically constructed a version of Valentine’s (1991) theoretical model using physically identical White and Black faces. The distance estimates obtained from the multidimensional unfolding analysis provide strong support for the race-based dispersion assumption of Valentine’s framework. Valentine (1991) suggested that other-race faces should be located more distant from the central tendency of their shared psychological space, and that they should be more tightly clustered. Our results corroborated the second prediction, but not the first (likely owing to the fact that all faces were derived from the same prototype). Although faces from different races possess similar absolute levels of physiognomic variability (Goldstein, 1979), the present approach ensured that all faces were equivalent. Despite the structural identity among our Black and White sets, inter-face distances among the Black faces were substantially smaller than distances among the White faces, suggesting that perceived race strongly modulates the dispersion of faces in psychological space.

Several previous approaches have been used to investigate the hypothetical, n-dimensional space underlying face processing. Connectionist models, for instance, have been constructed using principal-components analysis, with simulations run to test predictions of the MDS framework (Burton et al., 1999; 2001). Other methods involve participants’ direct similarity ratings (Lee, Byatt, & Rhodes, 2000) or judgments about different facial features (Rhodes, 1988). The present study used an indirect, same-different task to construct a psychological space, ultimately supporting the predictions from Valentine (1991). Our results also indirectly support predictions from Levin (2000) and Hugenberg and Sacco (2008). These authors suggest that perceivers first categorize faces by race, seeking individuating information for other-race faces only when motivated. We found evidence for this in our same-different and go/no-go RTs, as participants were fast to classify Black faces (go/no-go), but slow to individuate them (same-different). Considered together with the finding that psychological space “shrinks” for other-race faces, these results suggest that other-race faces are encoded or stored less completely than own-race faces.

Because the own- and other-race faces in this study were physically identical, differing only in color, participants could not use different feature sets for their judgments. As such, our results somewhat contradict Valentine’s (1991) explanation for differences in the spatial organization of faces. Valentine suggested that the feature dimensions used to store other-race faces may be inappropriate, reflecting greater experience extracting features from own-race faces. Our results suggest that the same features were used in processing all faces, but with unequal salience, depending upon perceived race. In fact, we suggest that other-race faces are more tightly clustered in psychological space because of increased reliance on features. Whereas own-race faces receive enhanced configural processing (Rhodes, Hayward, & Winkler, 2006), other-race face processing may rely more heavily on featural processing, yielding “equivalency clusters” in psychological space. Our future research will focus on identifying the psychological spaces for own- and other-race faces using more naturally-varied stimuli, relating such information to the well-known ORE in recognition memory.

Acknowledgments

Support was provided by the National Institutes of Health Grant R01-DC04535-10 to S.D. Goldinger. We thank Peter Killeen for comments on a previous version of this article.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

Please note that the term race, as typically used, is inconsistent with the literal meaning of the term (i.e., there is one human race). Although the term ethnicity would be more accurate, we maintain the more familiar terminology in this article.

2

In a pilot experiment (N = 83), we identified two faces, one that was labeled “Black” approximately 75% of the time and one that was labeled “White” approximately 75% of the time. We morphed these faces together to create a racially ambiguous male, shown at the top of Figure 1.

3

Because participants were generally unable to distinguish between faces at the 10% distortion level, these faces were dropped from later analysis.

References

  1. Bahrick HP, Bahrick PO, Wittlinger RP. Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General. 1975;104:54–75. [Google Scholar]
  2. Burton AM, Bruce V, Hancock PJB. From pixels to people: A model of familiar face recognition. Cognitive Science. 1999;23:1–31. [Google Scholar]
  3. Burton AM, Miller P, Bruce V, Hancock PJB. Human and automatic face recognition: A comparison across image formats. Vision Research. 2001;41:3185–3195. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00186-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Busing F, Groenen P, Heiser W. Avoiding degeneracy in multidimensional unfolding by penalizing on the coefficient of variation. Psychometrika. 2005;70:71–98. [Google Scholar]
  5. Byatt G, Rhodes G. Recognition of own-race and other-race caricatures: Implications for models of face recognition. Vision Research. 1998;38:2455–2468. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00469-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Byatt G, Rhodes G. Identification of own-race and other-race faces: Implications for the representation of race in face space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2004;11:735–741. doi: 10.3758/bf03196628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caldara R, Abdi H. Simulating the 'other-race' effect with autoassociative neural networks: Further evidence in favor of the face-space model. Perception. 2006;35:659–670. doi: 10.1068/p5360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Catz O, Kampf M, Nachson I, Babkoff H. From theory to implementation: Building a multidimensional space for face recognition. Acta Psychologica. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.010. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Eberhardt JL, Dasgupta N, Banaszynski TL. Believing is seeing: The effects of racial labels and implicit beliefs on face perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003;29:360–370. doi: 10.1177/0146167202250215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Giguère G. Collecting and analyzing data in multidimensional scaling experiments: A guide for psychologists using SPSS. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology. 2006;2:26–37. [Google Scholar]
  11. Goldstein AG, Chance JE. Memory for faces and schema theory. The Journal of Psychology. 1980;105:47–59. [Google Scholar]
  12. Hugenberg K, Sacco DF. Social categorization and stereotyping: How social categorization biases person perception and face memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2:1052–1072. [Google Scholar]
  13. Johnston RA, Milne AB, Williams C, Hoise J. Do distinctive faces come from outer space? An investigation of the status of a multidimensional face-space. Visual Cognition. 1997;4:59–67. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kruskal JB, Wish M. Multidimensional Scaling. London: Sage Publications; 1978. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Application in the Social Sciences, 07-011. [Google Scholar]
  15. Lee K, Byatt G, Rhodes G. Caricature effects, distinctiveness, and identification: Testing the face space framework. Psychological Science. 2000;11:379–385. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Levin DT. Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 1996;22:1364–1382. [Google Scholar]
  17. Levin DT. Race as a visual feature: Using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2000;129:559–574. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.4.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lindsay DS, Jack PC, Jr, Christian MA. Other-race face perception. Journal of Applied Psycbology. 1991;76:587–589. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.76.4.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Meissner CA, Brigham JC. Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 2001;7:3–35. [Google Scholar]
  20. Ng W, Lindsay RCL. Cross-race facial recognition: Failure of the contact hypothesis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1994;25:217–232. [Google Scholar]
  21. Psychology Software Tools. E-Prime. Pittsburgh: PA: Author; 2006. (Version 1.2). [Computer software] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rhodes G. Looking at faces: First-order and second-order features as determinants of facial appearance. Perception. 1988;17:43–63. doi: 10.1068/p170043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rhodes G, Hayward WG, Winkler C. Expert face coding: Configural and component coding of own-race and other-race faces. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2006;13:499–505. doi: 10.3758/bf03193876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Sergent J, Takane Y. Structures in two-choice reaction-time data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1987;13:300–315. [Google Scholar]
  25. Singular Inversions, Inc. FaceGenModeller. 2004 (Version 3.1.4) http://www.FaceGen.com.
  26. Valentine T. A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1991;43A:161–204. doi: 10.1080/14640749108400966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES