Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2010 Oct 21;68(2-13):309–320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.001

Genetic Advances in the Study of Speech and Language Disorders

DF Newbury 1,, AP Monaco 1
PMCID: PMC2977079  PMID: 20955937

Summary

Developmental speech and language disorders cover a wide range of childhood conditions with overlapping but heterogeneous phenotypes and underlying etiologies. This characteristic heterogeneity hinders accurate diagnosis, can complicate treatment strategies, and causes difficulties in the identification of causal factors. Nonetheless, over the last decade, genetic variants have been identified that may predispose certain individuals to different aspects of speech and language difficulties. In this review, we summarize advances in the genetic investigation of stuttering, speech-sound disorder (SSD), specific language impairment (SLI), and developmental verbal dyspraxia (DVD). We discuss how the identification and study of specific genes and pathways, including FOXP2, CNTNAP2, ATP2C2, CMIP, and lysosomal enzymes, may advance our understanding of the etiology of speech and language disorders and enable us to better understand the relationships between the different forms of impairment across the spectrum.

Main Text

Speech and Language Disorders

Developmental disorders of communication represent one of the most common reasons for pediatric referrals (Harel et al., 1996) and account for a large proportion of statements of educational need (Law et al., 2000). This clinical category includes many individuals in whom speech and language problems are symptomatic of a more global developmental condition, such as autistic spectrum disorder, learning disability, or hearing impairment. For others, the speech and language deficit occurs in an otherwise normal developmental trajectory and has no obvious cause (Bishop, 1994). Such primary speech and language disorders are currently classified into five distinct categories: expressive language disorder, mixed receptive-expressive language disorder, phonological disorder, stuttering, and communication disorder not-otherwise-specified (DSM-IV). Disorders of speech involve an impairment in the production of fluent and comprehensible speech and include stuttering, in which the fluency of speech is disrupted; phonological disorder (including speech-sound disorder [SSD]), in which the problem lies in the production and proper use of speech sounds; and developmental verbal dyspraxia, in which there is impairment in the coordination and motor control of the speech organs. Disorders of language are perhaps less perceptible but no less profound. They may involve problems with the correct formation of words (morphology) or sentences (syntax), the derivation of meaning (semantics), or the use of linguistic context (pragmatics) and may affect expressive and/or receptive language as well as nonverbal language (e.g., reading and writing—developmental dyslexia). The term specific language impairment (SLI) is often used as an umbrella term for expressive language disorder, mixed receptive-expressive language disorder, and sometimes phonological disorder.

Although many of these speech and language disorders are differentiated at a clinical level, they are highly comorbid with each other. Many children do not fall neatly into a single diagnostic cluster, and others may change between categories as their language develops. Approximately 15% of children with persistent speech disorders also have a language disorder and approximately 5% of children with SLI also experience speech difficulties (Shriberg et al., 1999). Over 60% of stutterers present with concurrent speech and language disorders, the most common of which is articulation disorder (Blood et al., 2003). Furthermore, although the diagnostic criteria for SLI necessitates the absence of explanatory medical conditions, studies have found that affected individuals are at an increased risk of associated developmental delays, cognitive impairment, social problems, literacy deficits, and behavioral difficulties (Conti-Ramsden and Botting, 1999; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2001; Law et al., 2000; Wadman et al., 2008). The exact relationship between speech and language deficits and these other developmental problems remains a matter of debate, and the etiological basis of these overlaps are unclear.

It is well documented that genetic factors contribute to susceptibility to speech and language impairments. Speech and language deficits are heritable and show strong familial aggregation (e.g., Barry et al., 2007; Clark et al., 2007; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2007; Lewis et al., 2007). Moreover, twin studies report an increase in monozygotic twin concordance rates over that of dizygotic twins, suggesting that much of this aggregation can be attributed to genetic influences (Bishop, 2002; Felsenfeld et al., 2000; Hayiou-Thomas, 2008). Nonetheless, it is generally thought that the genetic mechanisms underlying susceptibility to speech and language disorders are multifactorial in nature, involving complex interactions between several common genetic variants and environmental factors. Despite this complexity, researchers have recently begun to identify genetic factors that may play a role in the etiology of speech and language disorders (Kang et al., 2010; Newbury et al., 2009; Vernes et al., 2008). It is hoped that the identification of contributory genetic risk factors will allow the elucidation of biological pathways and neurological mechanisms that contribute to speech and language acquisition processes and play a critical role in the etiology of speech and language disorders. In turn, the investigation of identified genetic factors may help untangle the complex relationships between speech and language disorders and related developmental conditions.

In this review, we provide an overview of recent developments in the genetic study of spoken developmental speech and language disorders. We do not describe the study of developmental dyslexia as excellent reviews of this area have recently been published (Scerri and Schulte-Körne, 2010).

Methods for Identifying Contributory Genetic Variants

For some genetic disorders, it is possible to select candidate genes on the basis of their function alone. However, for speech and language disorders, in which the underlying biological mechanism is unclear, the identification of susceptibility genes usually starts with an unbiased screening approach. This step allows the identification of a candidate region and thus acts to reduce the number of possible contributory genes to a manageable size prior to a more in-depth investigation. These screening approaches usually take the form of genome-wide linkage or association studies (Elston and Anne Spence, 2006).

In a linkage study, one investigates families with members affected by the disorder under study. By genotyping polymorphic genetic markers spread across the genome, it is possible to generate measures of genetic identity between sibling pairs. Linkage studies look for regions of the human genome in which there is a correlation between the level of genetic identity and the level of phenotypic similarity for any given sib pair. Perhaps the most simple example of linkage analysis is provided by the study of single-gene disorders in large families. In such circumstances, one is able to identify specific chromosome segments that cosegregate with disease status between affected relative pairs. Nonetheless, as discussed below, the study of such pedigrees in complex disorders is the exception rather than the rule. Instead, the majority of linkage studies for complex disorders investigate large numbers of smaller nuclear families and compare genetic identity between sibling pairs within family units. Such analyses can be performed under the assumption of a defined genetic model (parametric linkage analysis) or by using a model-free method (nonparametric linkage analysis). Linkage analyses provide a robust and powerful method of detecting contributing genetic factors but afford a low level of resolution. Thus, although linkage studies can be completed with relatively small sample sizes, they often lead to the identification of large genetic intervals, which necessitate follow-up higher-resolution (targeted association) investigations to allow the identification of a specific candidate gene. Linkage studies are usually completed at a genome-wide level (i.e., across all chromosomes—i.e., a genome-wide linkage analysis or GWLA) but may target specific chromosome regions (targeted linkage) if the researcher has an a priori reason to do so.

Because linkage studies simply investigate the level of genetic sharing (joint inheritance), they do not allow the identification of specific contributory genetic variants. For this crucial step, an association study is required. In contrast to linkage, an association study is based upon the principle that contributory genetic variants will be more common in affected (cases) than unaffected (controls) individuals. In the simplest example, if a given genetic variant was necessary and sufficient for the onset of the disorder then we would expect this variant to be present in 100% of cases but never in controls. In complex disorders, the effect is not expected to be so stark but the rationale still holds. Association studies provide a better resolution of genetic information than linkage studies but are not as powerful and necessitate the genotyping of a high number of genetic markers in extremely large samples. These factors bring their own complications in terms of the logistics of data generation and multiple testing issues. Nonetheless, it is now possible to routinely generate genotypes for 2.5 million genetic variants (single-nucleotide polymorphisms [SNPs]) in thousands of case and control individuals enabling a so-called genome-wide association (GWA) study. The fact that association models were developed for case-control cohorts means that they often do not provide a convenient method for the fine mapping of linkage peaks in family-based samples. However, it is possible to apply such methods to genetic data collected from families by using parents or unaffected siblings as controls (Lunetta et al., 2000) or by applying quasi-association extensions (Abecasis et al., 2000). Such methods provide an important solution to fine mapping and the investigation of quantitative traits and may offer increased power to detect etiological variants.

It should be noted that although association studies are theoretically capable of identifying specific causal variants with relatively low effect sizes, in practice the genetic variants identified by association studies are not necessarily functional. Instead, they tend to be proxies that mark the approximate position of the etiologic genetic variant within a small segment of DNA. Thus, while association studies provide a strong starting point for functional studies, even high-density SNP association screens will require follow-up investigations to enable proof of causality (McCarthy and Hirschhorn, 2008).

Linkage and association investigations of speech and language disorders followed on from similar investigations of the related disorder developmental dyslexia in the 1990s (Scerri and Schulte-Körne, 2010). Over the last decade, researchers of speech-sound disorder have applied targeted linkage studies, while investigators of SLI and stuttering have performed genome-wide linkage studies and subsequent targeted association studies. In addition, a rare and specific form of verbal dyspraxia has been attributed to mutations of a specific gene known as FOXP2. A summary of loci that have been implicated in these various speech and language disorders, as well as particular genes that have been identified, is provided in Table 1.

Table 1.

Summary of Loci Implicated in Speech and Language Disorders

Chromosome Region Gene Disorder Gene Identification Method Identification References Replication Notes
1p NA Speech-sound disorder Targeted linkage Miscimarra et al. (2007) Region also linked to dyslexia
1q NA Stuttering GWLA Riaz et al. (2005)
2q NA Stuttering GWLA Suresh et al. (2006) Replicated by Wittke-Thompson et al. (2007)
3 NA Speech-sound disorder Targeted linkage Stein et al. (2004) Region also linked to dyslexia
3p14 FOXP1 Developmental delay with speech and language impairment Translocation mapping and chromosome abnormality screen Pariani et al. (2009); Carr et al. (2010); Horn et al. (2010); Vernes et al. (2009) Replicated across several individuals with variable phenotypes
3q NA Stuttering GWLA Wittke-Thompson et al. (2007) Linkage to 3q also found by Raza et al. (2010)
5q NA Stuttering GWLA Riaz et al. (2005) Replicated by Wittke-Thompson et al. (2007)
6p NA Speech-sound disorder Targeted linkage Smith et al. (2005) Region also linked to dyslexia
7q31 FOXP2 Verbal dyspraxia GWLA with subsequent translocation mapping in an unrelated affected individual Fisher et al. (1998); Lai et al. (2001) Disrupted in a small no. of individuals with verbal dyspraxia. Not associated with SLI or autism.
7q NA Stuttering GWLA Riaz et al. (2005) Replicated by Suresh et al. (2006) in male individuals only
7q36 CNTNAP2 SLI Targeted association of candidate gene Vernes et al. (2008) Also associated with a range of other neurodevelopmental disorders. Association with SLI yet to be replicated.
9p NA Stuttering GWLA Suresh et al. (2006)
12q23 GNPTAB Stuttering GWLA and subsequent targeted candidate gene sequencing Kang et al. (2010); Riaz et al. (2005) Linkage replicated by Suresh et al. (2006) when conditioning on chr 7q stuttering locus
13 NA SLI GWLA Bartlett et al. (2002) Replicated by Bartlett et al. (2004)
13q NA Stuttering GWLA Wittke-Thompson et al. (2007) Overlaps with Bartlett linkage to SLI
15p NA Stuttering GWLA Suresh et al. (2006)
15q NA Stuttering GWLA Wittke-Thompson et al. (2007)
15q NA Speech-sound disorder Targeted linkage Smith et al. (2005) Region also linked to dyslexia. Replicated by Stein et al. (2006)
16p13 GNPTG Stuttering Targeted candidate gene sequencing Kang et al. (2010)
16p13 NAGPA Stuttering Targeted candidate gene sequencing Kang et al. (2010)
16q24 ATP2C2 SLI GWLA and subsequent targeted association Newbury et al. (2009); SLIC (2002) Linkage replicated by Falcaro et al. (2008); Monaco (2007); SLIC (2004). Association with SLI yet to be replicated.
ATP2C2 associated with ADHD (Lesch et al., 2008)
16q24 CMIP SLI GWLA and subsequent targeted association Newbury et al. (2009); SLIC (2002) Linkage replicated by Falcaro et al. (2008); Monaco (2007); SLIC (2004). Association with SLI yet to be replicated.
18p NA Stuttering GWLA Shugart et al. (2004)
19q13 NA SLI GWLA SLIC (2002) Linkage replicated by Falcaro et al. (2008); Monaco (2007); SLIC (2004).
21p NA Stuttering GWLA Suresh et al. (2006) Linkage in female individuals only

GWLA, genome-wide linkage analysis; SLI, specific language impairment.

FOXP2

The first gene to be implicated in a speech and language disorder was identified by the investigation of a large family affected by a distinctive form of speech impairment known as verbal dyspraxia. Verbal dyspraxia is characterized by difficulties in the control of orofacial muscles leading to a deficit in the production of fluent speech. In addition to their speech problems, affected members of this family also had expressive and receptive language deficits and, in some cases, written language problems and nonverbal cognitive impairment (Watkins et al., 2002). The remarkable thing about the speech and language impairments observed in this family was that the pattern of inheritance indicated that they may be caused by a mutation in a single gene (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1995). Genome-wide linkage analysis identified linkage to chromosome 7q (Fisher et al., 1998) and fine mapping of the locus indeed identified a mutation in the FOXP2 gene (OMIM 605317) that was present in all affected family members but not in unaffected individuals (Lai et al., 2001). The relevance of FOXP2 mutations to other cases of verbal dyspraxia was supported by the identification of an unrelated child with a similar form of speech impairment who was found to have a chromosome rearrangement that disrupted the FOXP2 gene (Lai et al., 2001). These data have since been confirmed by several independent FOXP2 screening studies that have identified additional individuals with disruptions of this gene, all of whom have syndromes that feature verbal dyspraxia (Feuk et al., 2006; Lai et al., 2000; Lennon et al., 2007; MacDermot et al., 2005; Pariani et al., 2009; Shriberg et al., 2006; Tomblin et al., 2009; Zeesman et al., 2006). The FOXP2 gene encodes a winged helix/forkhead DNA-binding protein from the FOX family. This protein acts as a transcriptional repressor and has four alternative isoforms (Bruce and Margolis, 2002; Lai et al., 2001; Schroeder and Myers, 2008). The FOXP2 gene shows a widespread pattern of expression across the majority of tissues and developmental time points. Nonetheless, within each tissue its expression appears to be tightly regulated in a complex pattern of expression with a high degree of conservation across species (Ferland et al., 2003; Lu et al., 2002; Schroeder and Myers, 2008; Shu et al., 2001, 2007).

The identification of FOXP2 opened up a whole field of research encompassing a wide range of disciplines including neuroimaging, animal models (primarily mouse and songbird), molecular studies of gene function and expression, and population and evolutionary studies (reviewed in Fisher, 2006; Fisher and Scharff, 2009). Although the exact role of FOXP2 in the cause of verbal dyspraxia has yet to be elucidated, it is clear that this gene is of particular importance in the development of brain regions responsible for fine motor control (motor cortex, striatum, and cerebellum) and that its disruption has exceptionally severe consequences for the development of speech. Many detailed reviews of FOXP2 have been previously published and we refer the reader to these for a more in-depth discussion of this gene (Fisher, 2006; Fisher and Scharff, 2009).

Despite its obvious importance in severe and rare forms of verbal dyspraxia, it seems increasingly unlikely that FOXP2 represents a general risk factor for genetically complex forms of language impairment (Meaburn et al., 2002; Newbury et al., 2002; O'Brien et al., 2003), nor is it expected to play a role in the related disorder autism (Gauthier et al., 2003; Laroche et al., 2008; Li et al., 2005; Marui et al., 2005; Wassink et al., 2002). However, the identification of this gene heralded a new era for the genetic study of speech and language as it allowed the identification of some of the biological pathways and mechanisms important for speech and language acquisition processes. Since FOXP2 is a transcription factor, it regulates the expression of other genes, some of which may be expected to be involved in more common forms of speech and language deficits. Gene-targeting screens indicate that the FOXP2 protein is responsible for the downregulation of between 300 and 400 neural genes, many of which functionally represent good candidate genes for speech and language disorders (Spiteri et al., 2007; Vernes et al., 2007).

CNTNAP2

The list of genes regulated by FOXP2 includes a gene on chromosome 7 known as CNTNAP2 (OMIM 604569) (Spiteri et al., 2007; Vernes et al., 2007). Recent evidence indicates that this gene may play a role in susceptibility to genetically complex forms of language impairment (Vernes et al., 2008). This study found that nine common genetic variants, involving changes at single base pairs (single-nucleotide polymorphisms) across the CNTNAP2 sequence, were significantly correlated (minP = 5.0 × 10−5) with reduced performance across a number of linguistic measures (expressive and receptive language and phonological short-term memory) in a cohort of language-impaired families (Vernes et al., 2008). The mechanism by which these SNPs might alter CNTNAP2 function has yet to be elucidated, and these findings have yet to be replicated in additional language-impaired samples. However, alternative variations across this gene (including common variation and rare disruptions or mutations) have also been implicated in a range of neurodevelopmental disorders including autism (Alarcón et al., 2008; Arking et al., 2008; Bakkaloglu et al., 2008; Jackman et al., 2009; Poot et al., 2010; Rossi et al., 2008), Gilles de Tourette syndrome (Belloso et al., 2007; Verkerk et al., 2003), schizophrenia (Friedman et al., 2008; O'Dushlaine et al., 2010), epilepsy (Friedman et al., 2008; Mefford et al., 2010; Strauss et al., 2006), ADHD (Elia et al., 2010), and learning disability (Zweier et al., 2009). Furthermore, CNTNAP2 variation has also been associated with the normal personality trait “openness to experience” (Terracciano et al., 2010).

CNTNAP2 encodes a neurexin protein that is responsible for the localization of potassium channels in developing neurons and plays an important role in the facilitation of axonal-glial interactions (Poliak et al., 2003; Zweier et al., 2009). Brain expression studies indicate that while this gene is evenly expressed across the rodent brain, it shows a specific pattern of expression in the song control nuclei of male songbirds (Panaitof et al., 2010) and is enriched in the frontal cortex of humans (Abrahams et al., 2007). Structural MRI studies of population cohorts found that individuals who carry two copies of the genetic “risk” variants previously associated with autistic disorder have significantly reduced volumes of gray and white matter across several brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex, fusiform gyri, occipital cortices, and cerebellum, which have previously been shown to be important in autistic disorder (Tan et al., 2010).

Thus current data suggest that CNTNAP2 plays a fundamental role in neuronal development and that perturbations of its function may contribute to susceptibility to a diverse range of neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders as well as normal variations in brain function (Corvin, 2010).

FOXP1

On the basis of FOXP2 data, researchers have suggested that other forkhead binding genes represent good candidates for involvement in speech and language impairments. The human FOX gene family consists of over 40 members classified into 19 subfamilies (designated FOXA to FOXS) according to specific motifs within the DNA binding domain (Hannenhalli and Kaestner, 2009). The FOXP subfamily includes four genes (FOXP1-4) with diverse functions. The proteins encoded by these genes are found to bind to each other to form active heterodimer DNA binding molecules (Li et al., 2004, 2007). In particular, it has been suggested that FOXP1 and FOXP2 may have a particularly close relationship with overlapping functions that allow them to work in a cooperative manner during tissue development (Shu et al., 2007). In 2009, Pariani et al. described a patient with a deletion of chromosome 3 that disrupted the FOXP1 gene and deleted three other transcripts (EIF4E3, GPR27, and PROK2). The primary clinical presentation was described as blepharophimosis (drooping eyelids) and arthrogryposis (contractures of hands and feet). However, the child also presented with developmental delays and speech and language deficits, which the authors hypothesized might be caused by the disruption of FOXP1 (Pariani et al., 2009). A subsequent sequencing screen of the FOXP1 coding sequence in 49 probands with a clinical diagnosis of developmental verbal dyspraxia described a nonsynonymous coding change that was observed in a single proband (Vernes et al., 2009). However, a similar change was also documented in an unaffected control individual (Vernes et al., 2009). More recently, a study described a patient with a Chiari I malformation (cerebellar tonsil abnormality) and epileptiform discharges with periods of motor arrest (Carr et al., 2010). This child was initially referred because of concerns over speech delay and was found to have a deletion involving only the FOXP1 gene (Carr et al., 2010). Although the clinical presentation of this patient differed considerably from that observed by Pariani et al., the authors suggest that the blepharophimosis and arthrogryposis in the former case may be caused by the deletion of additional transcripts. They go on to surmise that the FOXP1 disruptions are likely to account for the similarities in patient phenotype, namely deficits in motor development and speech delays. This hypothesis has recently gained support from a large-scale study for chromosome abnormalities in 1523 individuals with learning disability (Horn et al., 2010). This investigation identified deletions of the FOXP1 gene in three unrelated patients (two males, one female) with moderate learning disabilities, global developmental delays, and severe speech and language disorders. MRI and electroencephalography of the patients did not reveal any gross structural brain abnormalities, and a similar chromosome deletion was again observed in a control individual who was not reported to have learning difficulties.

The above studies illustrate the difficulties of correlating genetic variation with specific phenotypic features. Even in cases where a sequence disruption can be definitively demonstrated across a number of individuals, the proof of causality is not always straightforward. While it is true that all of the above cases had severe speech and language difficulties, these impairments often formed part of a more global developmental delay that was not necessarily consistent across individuals. In addition, control individuals with no reported developmental problems were also found to carry disruptions of the FOXP1 sequence. Furthermore, FOXP1 has also been associated with the skin disorder vitiligo (Jin et al., 2010), host-response to hepatitis vaccines (Davila et al., 2010), and the likelihood of cancer survival (Fox et al., 2004; Jais et al., 2008). The diversity of these data are typical and by no means insinuate that these particular results must be spurious. Indeed, similar results have been described by the CNTNAP2 and FOXP2 studies described above—it is important to remember that no gene product acts in isolation. The function of genes may vary across different developmental time points and in different tissues and are modulated by a large number of stochastic variables that differ between individuals. Thus, it is unlikely that any genetic investigation will ever be able to draw a simple relationship between a given gene and a specific outcome. Furthermore, it is entirely possible that a transcription factor, such as FOXP1, which has the potential to intersect many biological pathways, may play a role in many seemingly unrelated disorders. Thus, given the recent convergence of evidence, it is likely that FOXP1 is involved in biological processes that are particularly important in the development of speech and language.

The function of the FOXP1 protein in the brain remains unclear, but recent studies suggest that it may play a role in motor neuron diversification, through its interactions with Hox proteins (Dasen et al., 2008; Rousso et al., 2008); in neuronal migration, by gating Reelin signaling pathways (Palmesino et al., 2010); and in neuronal differentiation, via regulation of the Pitx3 protein (Konstantoulas et al., 2010).

Thus the accumulation of recent data suggests that, like its partner FOXP2, FOXP1 may also be involved in the determination of neural circuitry important for the development of speech and language.

Speech-Sound Disorder

Aside from FOXP2, its downstream targets, and its binding partners, researchers have reasoned that candidate genes or genetic regions implicated in dyslexia may also represent good candidates for speech and language disorders. This is particularly true for speech-sound disorder (SSD), which is characterized by the substitution or emission of speech sounds leading to intelligible speech. Speech-sound errors are a common feature of the language acquisition process but in children with SSD persist beyond the appropriate developmental time point. For example, it is estimated that while approximately 16% of 3-year-old children use inappropriate speech-sound formations, these errors only persist in 4% of children at 6 years of age (Shriberg et al., 1999). Speech-sound disorder is believed to be a specific problem with the development of phonological awareness (i.e., the conceptualization of speech-sound units) and, as such, of all the speech and language disorders is proposed to share the greatest etiological overlap with developmental dyslexia. In support of this theory, the presence of early speech problems has been shown to represent a significant risk factor for later literacy impairments, especially when the speech problems are accompanied by additional language-related deficits (Peterson et al., 2009; Raitano et al., 2004; Sices et al., 2007). These similarities have led researchers of SSD to focus entirely upon specific genetic regions that have previously been identified by linkage studies of dyslexia, namely chromosomes 1p, 3, 6p, and 15q (Scerri and Schulte-Körne, 2010), and in some cases, this approach has proved successful. Significant levels of linkage (minP = 0.00002) have been described between chromosome 3 (DYX5, OMIM 606896) and phonological memory and phonological decoding traits in SSD families (Stein et al., 2004). Suggestive levels of linkage have been reported on chromosomes 1 (DYX8, OMIM 608995) (minP = 0.0009, Miscimarra et al., 2007) 6 (DYX2, OMIM 600202) (minP = 0.0006, Smith et al., 2005), and 15 (DYX1, OMIM 127700) (minP = 0.004, Smith et al., 2005). An additional study found linkage to a binary affection status of SSD and quantitative measures of oral motor control, articulation, and phonological short-term memory in a region that flanked the DYX1 locus (Stein et al., 2006, 15 Mb distal). This region has previously been implicated in autism (AUTS4, OMIM 608636) and is commonly deleted in Prader-Willi Syndrome (Cook et al., 1998). It is possible that this result may represent variations in the precise positions of peaks of linkage, which may be caused by differences in genetic structure, or it may be that there are two loci on chromosome 15 that contribute to SSD etiology, one of which overlaps with the autism locus and a second of which coincides with DYX1.

Recent targeted association studies in dyslexic families have led to the identification of specific candidate genes within the dyslexia linkage loci mentioned above (reviewed in Scerri and Schulte-Körne, 2010). Intriguingly, many of these genes have functions in neuronal migration, a critical step in cortex development (reviewed by Gabel et al., 2010). These data complement the findings of classical postmortem neuroanatomical studies of dyslexic individuals (Galaburda et al., 1985) and suggest that this process may play a role in the etiology of reading disorders. The direct evaluation of these specific risk variants in SSD populations would categorically provide an answer to the question regarding etiological overlaps between these two disorders.

Specific Language Impairment

In contrast to SSD, investigations of SLI have applied a genome-wide linkage approach to the identification of candidate genes or regions. SLI is diagnosed as an unexpected disorder in the acquisition of language despite adequate intelligence and opportunity and in the absence of any explanatory medical conditions (e.g., autism or hearing impairment) (Law et al., 2000). By definition, SLI is a very heterogeneous disorder that includes disorders of speech that have no obvious motoric etiology and incorporates deficits in other areas of expressive language (e.g., grammar, syntax, and semantics) as well as impairments in receptive linguistic abilities.

Two whole-genome linkage screens have been performed identifying three primary sites of linkage to SLI. These are on chromosome 13 (SLI3, OMIM 607134) (Bartlett et al., 2002), 16 (SLI1, OMIM 606711) (SLIC, 2002), and 19 (SLI2, OMIM 606712) (SLIC, 2002). These studies both involved relatively small numbers of affected families and yielded borderline significant p values. Nonetheless, of all the linkage data presented in this review, these three loci probably represent the strongest and most developed results as they have all been confirmed by subsequent replication and fine-mapping studies (Bartlett et al., 2004; Falcaro et al., 2008; Monaco, 2007; SLIC, 2004). The SLI1 region on chromosome 16 has been subject to the most in-depth investigation, and a targeted association study recently enabled the identification of specific genetic variants that may cause linkage to this locus (Newbury et al., 2009). Significant association was identified to two distinct clusters of common genetic variants located within the ATP2C2 gene (OMIM 613082, minP = 2 × 10−5) and the CMIP gene (OMIM 610112, minP = 6 × 10−7). Variation in both of these genes was predominantly associated with performance on a task of phonological short-term memory, supporting the importance of memory processes in language acquisition (Newbury et al., 2009). Regression modeling indicated that ATP2C2 and CMIP exerted an independent effect upon phonological memory ability, supporting a role for both regions in language ability. Within this targeted association study, a marginally significant, similar trend of association was observed to ATP2C2 (minP = 0.006) in a sample of low-language performers selected from a population cohort. Thus, although this is perhaps the most thoroughly investigated locus described in this review, these results still require external replication, and a functional variant or mechanism has yet to be identified. Little is known regarding the functions of the CMIP and ATP2C2 proteins in the brain but both represent reasonable candidates. ATP2C2 encodes a calcium ATPase responsible for the regulation of cellular calcium and manganese levels. Calcium is an important intracellular messenger and is involved in many neuronal processes including short-term memory (Dash et al., 2007). Manganese ions are toxic to neuronal cells at high concentrations (Perl and Olanow, 2007), and manganese deficiency is linked with epilepsy (Carl et al., 1986). CMIP was identified through a screen to identify large proteins expressed in the brain (Nagase et al., 2000) and encodes an adaptor protein thought to form part of the cytoskeleton (Grimbert et al., 2003). Cytoskeletal remodeling plays a key role in synaptic formation and neuronal migration (Heng et al., 2010). The CMIP protein has been shown to interact with FilaminA (Grimbert et al., 2004), mutation of which is associated with the neuronal migration disorder X-linked periventricular heterotopia (OMIM 300017). Other studies have found that it also interacts with the RelA (a subunit of the transcription factor NF-kappaB) (Kamal et al., 2009) and PI3 kinase proteins (Kamal et al., 2010), indicating that, as described for the FOX proteins above, CMIP may function across multiple biological pathways. Neither CMIP nor ATP2C2 was identified as a FOXP2 target (Spiteri et al., 2007; Vernes et al., 2007), and no obvious relationships can be formulated between the two genes or with that of the CNTNAP2 gene, indicating that the follow up of these data may implicate new pathways and processes that are important for the development of language. Interestingly, variants within ATP2C2 have been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD, Lesch et al., 2008) indicating that the pleiotropic effects described for other language loci may also extend to this region.

In addition to the genome-wide approaches described above, one group has specifically investigated dyslexia-linked loci and the FOXP2 region of chromosome 7 in language-impaired families (Rice et al., 2009). This study found a suggestive level of linkage to variants across all regions investigated on chromosomes 1 (DYX8), 3 (DYX5), 6 (DYX2), 7 (FOXP2), and 15 (DYX1), with various measures of linguistic ability. Association analyses of a denser panel of variants across FOXP2 and KIAA0319 revealed a marginal level of association (minP = 0.01) between KIAA0319 and measures of speech, language, and reading and between FOXP2 and reading, language, and articulation (Rice et al., 2009), indicating that the behavioral effects of these genes may extend across related phenotypes. This study further supports the etiological relationships between dyslexia and speech and language disorders, as suggested by the SSD investigations described above.

Stuttering

The potential of founder and consanguineous pedigrees has recently been demonstrated by the identification of genetic pathways that may be important in the etiology of stuttering. Stuttering is defined as a disorder of speech fluency characterized by interruptions in speech flow and involuntary repetition or elongation of syllables (Prasse and Kikano, 2008). This disorder is relatively distinct from SSD and SLI and is persistent in approximately 20% of cases, particularly males (Prasse and Kikano, 2008). The majority of molecular genetic studies of stuttering have focused upon large consanguineous pedigrees in which stuttering appears to be familial and persistent. Four genome-wide linkage and one genome-wide association study have been performed for persistent familial stuttering. These investigations have yielded suggestive linkage to chromosomes 2, 3, 5, 7, 9, 12, 15, 18, and 21 and indicate a strong but complex sex effect in the relationships between risk variants (Raza et al., 2010; Riaz et al., 2005; Shugart et al., 2004; Suresh et al., 2006; Wittke-Thompson et al., 2007). Nonetheless, these studies yielded only moderate evidence of linkage or association to any one region, and the overlaps between the results were sparse. The most consistent and significant region implicated is on chromosome 12q (STUT2, OMIM 609261) (Riaz et al., 2005; Suresh et al., 2006). Sequencing of 45 genes across STUT2 revealed four coding mutations in the GNPTAB gene that were present in significantly more affected family members than unaffected individuals (Kang et al., 2010) The GNPTAB gene (OMIM 607840) encodes subunits of the N-acetylglucosamine-1-phosphotransferase enzyme, which plays a role in lysosomal targeting processes. An additional subunit of this enzyme is encoded by the GNPTG gene (OMIM 607838) on chromosome 16p and a downstream enzyme, N-acetylglucosamine-1-phosphodiester alpha-N-acetylglucosaminidase is encoded by the NAGPA gene on chromosome 16p (OMIM 607985). Sequencing of these related transcripts revealed additional coding mutations that were not observed in control individuals (Kang et al., 2010). Across the GNPTAB, GNPTG, and NAGPA genes, mutations were observed in 3.2% of the case chromosomes and 0.5% of the control chromosome screened (Kang et al., 2010). This study therefore implicates an additional biological mechanism in speech disorders—that of the lysosomal enzyme pathway. Mutations in the GNPTAB and GNPTG genes have previously been described to cause the metabolic disorders mucolipidosis types II and III, respectively (OMIM 252500 and 252605) (Paik et al., 2005; Raas-Rothschild et al., 2000). These disorders are not characterized by stuttering but instead by skeletal abnormalities, restricted joint movement, and disorders of the heart, liver, and spleen. In some cases (particularly type II), these features are accompanied by developmental delay and speech problems but not typically stuttering. However, none of the mutations identified by Kang et al. were identical to those that had previously been implicated in mucolipidosis. Furthermore, since mucolipidosis is a recessive disorder, affected individuals must carry two copies of a mutation. In the stuttering cohort, all but two individuals carried one normal copy of the GNPTAB and GNPTG gene. This may therefore account for the less severe phenotype described in this sample. This intriguing study has yet to be replicated or extended and there are many interesting questions that can be asked regarding the mechanism of susceptibility to stuttering in these individuals.

Conclusions

In summary, the last decade has seen an explosion in our understanding of the genetic basis of speech and language disorders. The identification of FOXP2 precipitated a whole field of research that continues to advance our understanding of the foundations of speech and language. It is likely that in the future, the investigation of the pathways that involve and intersect with FOXP2 will identify many more candidate genes and mechanisms underlying speech and language disorders. The investigation of SSD has indicated that this disorder may share some genetic basis with developmental dyslexia. The study of SLI has enabled the identification of two candidate genes on chromosome 16 (ATP2C2 and CMIP), and stuttering research has identified the lysosomal enzyme pathway (GNPTAB, GNPTG, and NAGPA) as another candidate mechanism.

Although the recent progress in this field is promising, it should be noted that, in comparison to other developmental disorders with a genetic contribution, speech and language disorders are relatively understudied. Many of the studies described above involved relatively small samples and have yet to be replicated in independent cohorts. As genetic technologies develop, the generation of larger data sets becomes progressively easier, allowing the identification of genetic variants with smaller effect sizes. Nonetheless, the application of these technologies demands the existence of large sample sets with consistent ascertainment and assessment standards. Even then, as we have seen above, and as evidenced by many other GWA studies, the interpretation of findings is far from straightforward. GWA studies in other complex disorders have enabled a revolution in candidate gene identification, but it is estimated that these loci still only account for a small proportion of the observed familial clustering. The source of this so-called “missing heritability” is still a matter of debate but has been suggested to arise from the presence of common genetic variation with minimal effect sizes, rare variants, structural deviations, and genetic modifications and interactions that are not well characterized by currently available GWA technology or algorithms (Manolio et al., 2009).

Thanks to advances in technology, however, alternative methodologies are now available to complement the GWA. It is now feasible to directly sequence entire genomes for increasingly large sample sizes, and this will be important in the identification of rare mutations (Cirulli and Goldstein, 2010). It is also now recognized that successful GWAs rely upon adequate sample sizes and precise phenotyping, and to achieve this, meta-analyses may be required. Improved sample sizes and the investigation of alternative populations both afford increased power to detect variants with smaller effect sizes and enable the investigation of gene-gene interaction effects (Manolio et al., 2009). In addition, the genome-wide investigation of epigenetic effects may identify alternative sources of genetic variation that are not readily captured by the GWA approach (Maunakea et al., 2010). It is envisaged that these advances will facilitate the identification of greater numbers of risk variants, and this is reflected in the current trend away from the identification of “disease genes” toward an understanding of the biological pathways that underlie specific pathologies. Given this appetite for ever-larger data sets, it is perhaps ironic that the gene that has provided us with the most information regarding speech and language disorders was identified by the investigation of a single family with a rare and specific form of speech impairment. The FOXP2 story demonstrates the continuing validity of the genetic approach and exemplifies how such data can be used to complement and inform larger genome-wide and biological pathway investigations.

Through this review, we hope not only to have summarized the findings of genetic investigations of speech and language disorders but also to have indicated the challenges faced during the genetic investigation of such disorders. As genetic throughput expands, the true challenge is not the generation of data but the interpretation of the findings and the proof of causality. It will be interesting to see whether the recent findings pertaining to speech and language disorders can be replicated and how these avenues of investigation will eventually combine with those generated from larger high-throughput studies. Ultimately, it is hoped that the research summarized in this paper will allow a better understanding of the causes of speech and language disorders and the complex relationships between these impairments, thereby facilitating better diagnostic and treatment schedules for affected individuals.

Acknowledgments

A.P.M. is funded by a Wellcome Trust programme grant (grant no. 076566), and the research of the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics is funded generally by a Wellcome Trust core award grant (grant no. 075491).

References

  1. Abecasis G.R., Cardon L.R., Cookson W.O. A general test of association for quantitative traits in nuclear families. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2000;66:279–292. doi: 10.1086/302698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abrahams B.S., Tentler D., Perederiy J.V., Oldham M.C., Coppola G., Geschwind D.H. Genome-wide analyses of human perisylvian cerebral cortical patterning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:17849–17854. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706128104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alarcón M., Abrahams B.S., Stone J.L., Duvall J.A., Perederiy J.V., Bomar J.M., Sebat J., Wigler M., Martin C.L., Ledbetter D.H. Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2008;82:150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arking D.E., Cutler D.J., Brune C.W., Teslovich T.M., West K., Ikeda M., Rea A., Guy M., Lin S., Cook E.H., Chakravarti A. A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2008;82:160–164. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bakkaloglu B., O'Roak B.J., Louvi A., Gupta A.R., Abelson J.F., Morgan T.M., Chawarska K., Klin A., Ercan-Sencicek A.G., Stillman A.A. Molecular cytogenetic analysis and resequencing of contactin associated protein-like 2 in autism spectrum disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2008;82:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barry J.G., Yasin I., Bishop D.V. Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:66–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00232.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartlett C.W., Flax J.F., Logue M.W., Vieland V.J., Bassett A.S., Tallal P., Brzustowicz L.M. A major susceptibility locus for specific language impairment is located on 13q21. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2002;71:45–55. doi: 10.1086/341095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bartlett C.W., Flax J.F., Logue M.W., Smith B.J., Vieland V.J., Tallal P., Brzustowicz L.M. Examination of potential overlap in autism and language loci on chromosomes 2, 7, and 13 in two independent samples ascertained for specific language impairment. Hum. Hered. 2004;57:10–20. doi: 10.1159/000077385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belloso J.M., Bache I., Guitart M., Caballin M.R., Halgren C., Kirchhoff M., Ropers H.H., Tommerup N., Tümer Z. Disruption of the CNTNAP2 gene in a t(7;15) translocation family without symptoms of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;15:711–713. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bishop D.V. Is specific language impairment a valid diagnostic category? Genetic and psycholinguistic evidence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1994;346:105–111. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1994.0134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bishop D.V. The role of genes in the etiology of specific language impairment. J. Commun. Disord. 2002;35:311–328. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(02)00087-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blood G.W., Ridenour V.J., Qualls C.D., Hammer C.S. Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. J. Commun. Disord. 2003;36:427–448. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00023-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bruce H.A., Margolis R.L. FOXP2: Novel exons, splice variants, and CAG repeat length stability. Hum. Genet. 2002;111:136–144. doi: 10.1007/s00439-002-0768-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carl G.F., Keen C.L., Gallagher B.B., Clegg M.S., Littleton W.H., Flannery D.B., Hurley L.S. Association of low blood manganese concentrations with epilepsy. Neurology. 1986;36:1584–1587. doi: 10.1212/wnl.36.12.1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carr C.W., Moreno-De-Luca D., Parker C., Zimmerman H.H., Ledbetter N., Martin C.L., Dobyns W.B., Abdul-Rahman O.A. Chiari I malformation, delayed gross motor skills, severe speech delay, and epileptiform discharges in a child with FOXP1 haploinsufficiency. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2010 doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2010.96. in press. Published online June 23, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cirulli E.T., Goldstein D.B. Uncovering the roles of rare variants in common disease through whole-genome sequencing. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2010;11:415–425. doi: 10.1038/nrg2779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark A., O'Hare A., Watson J., Cohen W., Cowie H., Elton R., Nasir J., Seckl J. Severe receptive language disorder in childhood—familial aspects and long-term outcomes: Results from a Scottish study. Arch. Dis. Child. 2007;92:614–619. doi: 10.1136/adc.2006.101758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Conti-Ramsden G., Botting N. Characteristics of children attending language units in England: A national study of 7-year-olds. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 1999;34:359–366. doi: 10.1080/136828299247333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Conti-Ramsden G., Botting N., Simkin Z., Knox E. Follow-up of children attending infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2001;36:207–219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conti-Ramsden G., Falcaro M., Simkin Z., Pickles A. Familial loading in specific language impairment: Patterns of differences across proband characteristics, gender and relative type. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6:216–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cook E.H., Jr., Courchesne R.Y., Cox N.J., Lord C., Gonen D., Guter S.J., Lincoln A., Nix K., Haas R., Leventhal B.L., Courchesne E. Linkage-disequilibrium mapping of autistic disorder, with 15q11-13 markers. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 1998;62:1077–1083. doi: 10.1086/301832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Corvin A.P. Neuronal cell adhesion genes: Key players in risk for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and other neurodevelopmental brain disorders? Cell. Adh. Migr. 2010;4:509–512. doi: 10.4161/cam.4.4.12460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dasen J.S., De Camilli A., Wang B., Tucker P.W., Jessell T.M. Hox repertoires for motor neuron diversity and connectivity gated by a single accessory factor, FoxP1. Cell. 2008;134:304–316. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dash P.K., Moore A.N., Kobori N., Runyan J.D. Molecular activity underlying working memory. Learn. Mem. 2007;14:554–563. doi: 10.1101/lm.558707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Davila S., Froeling F.E., Tan A., Bonnard C., Boland G.J., Snippe H., Hibberd M.L., Seielstad M. New genetic associations detected in a host response study to hepatitis B vaccine. Genes Immun. 2010;11:232–238. doi: 10.1038/gene.2010.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Elia J., Gai X., Xie H.M., Perin J.C., Geiger E., Glessner J.T., D'arcy M., deBerardinis R., Frackelton E., Kim C. Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Mol. Psychiatry. 2010;15:637–646. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Elston R.C., Anne Spence M. Advances in statistical human genetics over the last 25 years. Stat. Med. 2006;25:3049–3080. doi: 10.1002/sim.2650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Falcaro M., Pickles A., Newbury D.F., Addis L., Banfield E., Fisher S.E., Monaco A.P., Simkin Z., Conti-Ramsden G., SLI Consortium Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:393–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00364.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Felsenfeld S., Kirk K.M., Zhu G., Statham D.J., Neale M.C., Martin N.G. A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behav. Genet. 2000;30:359–366. doi: 10.1023/a:1002765620208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ferland R.J., Cherry T.J., Preware P.O., Morrisey E.E., Walsh C.A. Characterization of Foxp2 and Foxp1 mRNA and protein in the developing and mature brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2003;460:266–279. doi: 10.1002/cne.10654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Feuk L., Kalervo A., Lipsanen-Nyman M., Skaug J., Nakabayashi K., Finucane B., Hartung D., Innes M., Kerem B., Nowaczyk M.J. Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2006;79:965–972. doi: 10.1086/508902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fisher S.E. Tangled webs: Tracing the connections between genes and cognition. Cognition. 2006;101:270–297. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fisher S.E., Scharff C. FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends Genet. 2009;25:166–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fisher S.E., Vargha-Khadem F., Watkins K.E., Monaco A.P., Pembrey M.E. Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nat. Genet. 1998;18:168–170. doi: 10.1038/ng0298-168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fox S.B., Brown P., Han C., Ashe S., Leek R.D., Harris A.L., Banham A.H. Expression of the forkhead transcription factor FOXP1 is associated with estrogen receptor alpha and improved survival in primary human breast carcinomas. Clin. Cancer Res. 2004;10:3521–3527. doi: 10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-03-0461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Friedman J.I., Vrijenhoek T., Markx S., Janssen I.M., van der Vliet W.A., Faas B.H., Knoers N.V., Cahn W., Kahn R.S., Edelmann L. CNTNAP2 gene dosage variation is associated with schizophrenia and epilepsy. Mol. Psychiatry. 2008;13:261–266. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gabel L.A., Gibson C.J., Gruen J.R., LoTurco J.J. Progress towards a cellular neurobiology of reading disability. Neurobiol. Dis. 2010;38:173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Galaburda A.M., Sherman G.F., Rosen G.D., Aboitiz F., Geschwind N. Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Ann. Neurol. 1985;18:222–233. doi: 10.1002/ana.410180210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gauthier J., Joober R., Mottron L., Laurent S., Fuchs M., De Kimpe V., Rouleau G.A. Mutation screening of FOXP2 in individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. A. 2003;118A:172–175. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.10105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Grimbert P., Valanciute A., Audard V., Pawlak A., Le gouvelo S., Lang P., Niaudet P., Bensman A., Guellaën G., Sahali D. Truncation of C-mip (Tc-mip), a new proximal signaling protein, induces c-maf Th2 transcription factor and cytoskeleton reorganization. J. Exp. Med. 2003;198:797–807. doi: 10.1084/jem.20030566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Grimbert P., Valanciute A., Audard V., Lang P., Guellaën G., Sahali D. The Filamin-A is a partner of Tc-mip, a new adapter protein involved in c-maf-dependent Th2 signaling pathway. Mol. Immunol. 2004;40:1257–1261. doi: 10.1016/j.molimm.2003.11.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hannenhalli S., Kaestner K.H. The evolution of Fox genes and their role in development and disease. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2009;10:233–240. doi: 10.1038/nrg2523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Harel S., Greenstein Y., Kramer U., Yifat R., Samuel E., Nevo Y., Leitner Y., Kutai M., Fattal A., Shinnar S. Clinical characteristics of children referred to a child development center for evaluation of speech, language, and communication disorders. Pediatr. Neurol. 1996;15:305–311. doi: 10.1016/s0887-8994(96)00222-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hayiou-Thomas M.E. Genetic and environmental influences on early speech, language and literacy development. J. Commun. Disord. 2008;41:397–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Heng J.I., Chariot A., Nguyen L. Molecular layers underlying cytoskeletal remodelling during cortical development. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Horn D., Kapeller J., Rivera-Brugués N., Moog U., Lorenz-Depiereux B., Eck S., Hempel M., Wagenstaller J., Gawthrope A., Monaco A.P. Identification of FOXP1 deletions in three unrelated patients with mental retardation and significant speech and language deficits. Hum. Mutat. 2010 doi: 10.1002/humu.21362. in press. Published online September 16, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jackman C., Horn N.D., Molleston J.P., Sokol D.K. Gene associated with seizures, autism, and hepatomegaly in an Amish girl. Pediatr. Neurol. 2009;40:310–313. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2008.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jais J.P., Haioun C., Molina T.J., Rickman D.S., de Reynies A., Berger F., Gisselbrecht C., Brière J., Reyes F., Gaulard P., Groupe d'Etude des Lymphomes de l'Adulte The expression of 16 genes related to the cell of origin and immune response predicts survival in elderly patients with diffuse large B-cell lymphoma treated with CHOP and rituximab. Leukemia. 2008;22:1917–1924. doi: 10.1038/leu.2008.188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Jin Y., Birlea S.A., Fain P.R., Mailloux C.M., Riccardi S.L., Gowan K., Holland P.J., Bennett D.C., Wallace M.R., McCormack W.T. Common variants in FOXP1 are associated with generalized vitiligo. Nat. Genet. 2010;42:576–578. doi: 10.1038/ng.602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kamal M., Valanciute A., Dahan K., Ory V., Pawlak A., Lang P., Guellaen G., Sahali D. C-mip interacts physically with RelA and inhibits nuclear factor kappa B activity. Mol. Immunol. 2009;46:991–998. doi: 10.1016/j.molimm.2008.09.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kamal M., Pawlak A., BenMohamed F., Valanciuté A., Dahan K., Candelier M., Lang P., Guellaën G., Sahali D. C-mip interacts with the p85 subunit of PI3 kinase and exerts a dual effect on ERK signaling via the recruitment of Dip1 and DAP kinase. FEBS Lett. 2010;584:500–506. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2009.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kang C., Riazuddin S., Mundorff J., Krasnewich D., Friedman P., Mullikin J.C., Drayna D. Mutations in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway and persistent stuttering. N. Engl. J. Med. 2010;362:677–685. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0902630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Konstantoulas C.J., Parmar M., Li M. FoxP1 promotes midbrain identity in embryonic stem cell-derived dopamine neurons by regulating Pitx3. J. Neurochem. 2010;113:836–847. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06650.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lai C.S., Fisher S.E., Hurst J.A., Levy E.R., Hodgson S., Fox M., Jeremiah S., Povey S., Jamison D.C., Green E.D. The SPCH1 region on human 7q31: Genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2000;67:357–368. doi: 10.1086/303011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lai C.S., Fisher S.E., Hurst J.A., Vargha-Khadem F., Monaco A.P. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature. 2001;413:519–523. doi: 10.1038/35097076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Laroche F., Ramoz N., Leroy S., Fortin C., Rousselot-Paillet B., Philippe A., Colleaux L., Bresson J.L., Mogenet A., Golse B. Polymorphisms of coding trinucleotide repeats of homeogenes in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Psychiatr. Genet. 2008;18:295–301. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283060fa5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Law J., Boyle J., Harris F., Harkness A., Nye C. Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2000;35:165–188. doi: 10.1080/136828200247133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lennon P.A., Cooper M.L., Peiffer D.A., Gunderson K.L., Patel A., Peters S., Cheung S.W., Bacino C.A. Deletion of 7q31.1 supports involvement of FOXP2 in language impairment: Clinical report and review. Am. J. Med. Genet. A. 2007;143:791–798. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lesch K.P., Timmesfeld N., Renner T.J., Halperin R., Röser C., Nguyen T.T., Craig D.W., Romanos J., Heine M., Meyer J. Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: Converging evidence from genome-wide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. J. Neural Transm. 2008;115:1573–1585. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0119-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lewis B.A., Freebairn L.A., Hansen A.J., Miscimarra L., Iyengar S.K., Taylor H.G. Speech and language skills of parents of children with speech sound disorders. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2007;16:108–118. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Li S., Weidenfeld J., Morrisey E.E. Transcriptional and DNA binding activity of the Foxp1/2/4 family is modulated by heterotypic and homotypic protein interactions. Mol. Cell. Biol. 2004;24:809–822. doi: 10.1128/MCB.24.2.809-822.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Li H., Yamagata T., Mori M., Momoi M.Y. Absence of causative mutations and presence of autism-related allele in FOXP2 in Japanese autistic patients. Brain Dev. 2005;27:207–210. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2004.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Li B., Samanta A., Song X., Iacono K.T., Brennan P., Chatila T.A., Roncador G., Banham A.H., Riley J.L., Wang Q. FOXP3 is a homo-oligomer and a component of a supramolecular regulatory complex disabled in the human XLAAD/IPEX autoimmune disease. Int. Immunol. 2007;19:825–835. doi: 10.1093/intimm/dxm043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lu M.M., Li S., Yang H., Morrisey E.E. Foxp4: A novel member of the Foxp subfamily of winged-helix genes co-expressed with Foxp1 and Foxp2 in pulmonary and gut tissues. Gene Expr. Patterns. 2002;2:223–228. doi: 10.1016/s1567-133x(02)00058-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lunetta K.L., Faraone S.V., Biederman J., Laird N.M. Family-based tests of association and linkage that use unaffected sibs, covariates, and interactions. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2000;66:605–614. doi: 10.1086/302782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. MacDermot K.D., Bonora E., Sykes N., Coupe A.M., Lai C.S., Vernes S.C., Vargha-Khadem F., McKenzie F., Smith R.L., Monaco A.P., Fisher S.E. Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2005;76:1074–1080. doi: 10.1086/430841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Manolio T.A., Collins F.S., Cox N.J., Goldstein D.B., Hindorff L.A., Hunter D.J., McCarthy M.I., Ramos E.M., Cardon L.R., Chakravarti A. Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature. 2009;461:747–753. doi: 10.1038/nature08494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Marui T., Koishi S., Funatogawa I., Yamamoto K., Matsumoto H., Hashimoto O., Nanba E., Kato C., Ishijima M., Watanabe K. No association of FOXP2 and PTPRZ1 on 7q31 with autism from the Japanese population. Neurosci. Res. 2005;53:91–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Maunakea A.K., Chepelev I., Zhao K., Bruneau B. Epigenome mapping in normal and disease States. Circ. Res. 2010;107:327–339. doi: 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.110.222463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. McCarthy M.I., Hirschhorn J.N. Genome-wide association studies: Potential next steps on a genetic journey. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2008;17(R2):R156–R165. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Meaburn E., Dale P.S., Craig I.W., Plomin R. Language-impaired children: No sign of the FOXP2 mutation. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1075–1077. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200206120-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mefford H.C., Muhle H., Ostertag P., von Spiczak S., Buysse K., Baker C., Franke A., Malafosse A., Genton P., Thomas P. Genome-wide copy number variation in epilepsy: Novel susceptibility loci in idiopathic generalized and focal epilepsies. PLoS Genet. 2010;6:e1000962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Miscimarra L., Stein C., Millard C., Kluge A., Cartier K., Freebairn L., Hansen A., Shriberg L., Taylor H.G., Lewis B., Iyengar S.K. Further evidence of pleiotropy influencing speech and language: Analysis of the DYX8 region. Hum. Hered. 2007;63:47–58. doi: 10.1159/000098727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Monaco A.P. Multivariate linkage analysis of specific language impairment (SLI) Ann. Hum. Genet. 2007;71:660–673. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.2007.00361.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Nagase T., Kikuno R., Hattori A., Kondo Y., Okumura K., Ohara O. Prediction of the coding sequences of unidentified human genes. XIX. The complete sequences of 100 new cDNA clones from brain which code for large proteins in vitro. DNA Res. 2000;7:347–355. doi: 10.1093/dnares/7.6.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Newbury D.F., Bonora E., Lamb J.A., Fisher S.E., Lai C.S., Baird G., Jannoun L., Slonims V., Stott C.M., Merricks M.J., International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2002;70:1318–1327. doi: 10.1086/339931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Newbury D.F., Winchester L., Addis L., Paracchini S., Buckingham L.L., Clark A., Cohen W., Cowie H., Dworzynski K., Everitt A. CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2009;85:264–272. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. O'Brien E.K., Zhang X., Nishimura C., Tomblin J.B., Murray J.C. Association of specific language impairment (SLI) to the region of 7q31. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2003;72:1536–1543. doi: 10.1086/375403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. O'Dushlaine C., Kenny E., Heron E., Donohoe G., Gill M., Morris D., Corvin A., The International Schizophrenia Consortium Molecular pathways involved in neuronal cell adhesion and membrane scaffolding contribute to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder susceptibility. Mol. Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.7. in press. Published online February 16, 2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Paik K.H., Song S.M., Ki C.S., Yu H.W., Kim J.S., Min K.H., Chang S.H., Yoo E.J., Lee I.J., Kwan E.K. Identification of mutations in the GNPTA (MGC4170) gene coding for GlcNAc-phosphotransferase alpha/beta subunits in Korean patients with mucolipidosis type II or type IIIA. Hum. Mutat. 2005;26:308–314. doi: 10.1002/humu.20205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Palmesino E., Rousso D.L., Kao T.J., Klar A., Laufer E., Uemura O., Okamoto H., Novitch B.G., Kania A. Foxp1 and lhx1 coordinate motor neuron migration with axon trajectory choice by gating Reelin signalling. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Panaitof S.C., Abrahams B.S., Dong H., Geschwind D.H., White S.A. Language-related Cntnap2 gene is differentially expressed in sexually dimorphic song nuclei essential for vocal learning in songbirds. J. Comp. Neurol. 2010;518:1995–2018. doi: 10.1002/cne.22318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Pariani M.J., Spencer A., Graham J.M., Jr., Rimoin D.L. A 785kb deletion of 3p14.1p13, including the FOXP1 gene, associated with speech delay, contractures, hypertonia and blepharophimosis. Eur. J. Med. Genet. 2009;52:123–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmg.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Perl D.P., Olanow C.W. The neuropathology of manganese-induced Parkinsonism. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2007;66:675–682. doi: 10.1097/nen.0b013e31812503cf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Peterson R.L., Pennington B.F., Shriberg L.D., Boada R. What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009;52:1175–1188. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Poliak S., Salomon D., Elhanany H., Sabanay H., Kiernan B., Pevny L., Stewart C.L., Xu X., Chiu S.Y., Shrager P. Juxtaparanodal clustering of Shaker-like K+ channels in myelinated axons depends on Caspr2 and TAG-1. J. Cell Biol. 2003;162:1149–1160. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200305018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Poot M., Beyer V., Schwaab I., Damatova N., Van't Slot R., Prothero J., Holder S.E., Haaf T. Disruption of CNTNAP2 and additional structural genome changes in a boy with speech delay and autism spectrum disorder. Neurogenetics. 2010;11:81–89. doi: 10.1007/s10048-009-0205-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Prasse J.E., Kikano G.E. Stuttering: An overview. Am. Fam. Physician. 2008;77:1271–1276. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Raas-Rothschild A., Cormier-Daire V., Bao M., Genin E., Salomon R., Brewer K., Zeigler M., Mandel H., Toth S., Roe B. Molecular basis of variant pseudo-hurler polydystrophy (mucolipidosis IIIC) J. Clin. Invest. 2000;105:673–681. doi: 10.1172/JCI5826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Raitano N.A., Pennington B.F., Tunick R.A., Boada R., Shriberg L.D. Pre-literacy skills of subgroups of children with speech sound disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2004;45:821–835. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Raza M.H., Riazuddin S., Drayna D. Identification of an autosomal recessive stuttering locus on chromosome 3q13.2-3q13.33. Hum. Genet. 2010;128:461–463. doi: 10.1007/s00439-010-0871-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Riaz N., Steinberg S., Ahmad J., Pluzhnikov A., Riazuddin S., Cox N.J., Drayna D. Genomewide significant linkage to stuttering on chromosome 12. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2005;76:647–651. doi: 10.1086/429226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rice M.L., Smith S.D., Gayán J. Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language Impairment. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1:264–282. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9031-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Rossi E., Verri A.P., Patricelli M.G., Destefani V., Ricca I., Vetro A., Ciccone R., Giorda R., Toniolo D., Maraschio P., Zuffardi O. A 12Mb deletion at 7q33-q35 associated with autism spectrum disorders and primary amenorrhea. Eur. J. Med. Genet. 2008;51:631–638. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmg.2008.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Rousso D.L., Gaber Z.B., Wellik D., Morrisey E.E., Novitch B.G. Coordinated actions of the forkhead protein Foxp1 and Hox proteins in the columnar organization of spinal motor neurons. Neuron. 2008;59:226–240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Scerri T.S., Schulte-Körne G. Genetics of developmental dyslexia. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;19:179–197. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0081-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schroeder D.I., Myers R.M. Multiple transcription start sites for FOXP2 with varying cellular specificities. Gene. 2008;413:42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.gene.2008.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Shriberg L.D., Tomblin J.B., McSweeny J.L. Prevalence of speech delay in 6-year-old children and comorbidity with language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1999;42:1461–1481. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4206.1461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shriberg L.D., Ballard K.J., Tomblin J.B., Duffy J.R., Odell K.H., Williams C.A. Speech, prosody, and voice characteristics of a mother and daughter with a 7;13 translocation affecting FOXP2. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2006;49:500–525. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/038). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Shu W., Yang H., Zhang L., Lu M.M., Morrisey E.E. Characterization of a new subfamily of winged-helix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors. J. Biol. Chem. 2001;276:27488–27497. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M100636200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Shu W., Lu M.M., Zhang Y., Tucker P.W., Zhou D., Morrisey E.E. Foxp2 and Foxp1 cooperatively regulate lung and esophagus development. Development. 2007;134:1991–2000. doi: 10.1242/dev.02846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Shugart Y.Y., Mundorff J., Kilshaw J., Doheny K., Doan B., Wanyee J., Green E.D., Drayna D. Results of a genome-wide linkage scan for stuttering. Am. J. Med. Genet. A. 2004;124A:133–135. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.20347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Sices L., Taylor H.G., Freebairn L., Hansen A., Lewis B. Relationship between speech-sound disorders and early literacy skills in preschool-age children: Impact of comorbid language impairment. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2007;28:438–447. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31811ff8ca. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. SLI Consortium A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2002;70:384–398. doi: 10.1086/338649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. SLI Consortium (SLIC) Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2004;74:1225–1238. doi: 10.1086/421529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Smith S.D., Pennington B.F., Boada R., Shriberg L.D. Linkage of speech sound disorder to reading disability loci. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2005;46:1057–1066. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01534.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Spiteri E., Konopka G., Coppola G., Bomar J., Oldham M., Ou J., Vernes S.C., Fisher S.E., Ren B., Geschwind D.H. Identification of the transcriptional targets of FOXP2, a gene linked to speech and language, in developing human brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;81:1144–1157. doi: 10.1086/522237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Stein C.M., Schick J.H., Gerry Taylor H., Shriberg L.D., Millard C., Kundtz-Kluge A., Russo K., Minich N., Hansen A., Freebairn L.A. Pleiotropic effects of a chromosome 3 locus on speech-sound disorder and reading. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2004;74:283–297. doi: 10.1086/381562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Stein C.M., Millard C., Kluge A., Miscimarra L.E., Cartier K.C., Freebairn L.A., Hansen A.J., Shriberg L.D., Taylor H.G., Lewis B.A., Iyengar S.K. Speech sound disorder influenced by a locus in 15q14 region. Behav. Genet. 2006;36:858–868. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9090-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Strauss K.A., Puffenberger E.G., Huentelman M.J., Gottlieb S., Dobrin S.E., Parod J.M., Stephan D.A., Morton D.H. Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006;354:1370–1377. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Suresh R., Ambrose N., Roe C., Pluzhnikov A., Wittke-Thompson J.K., Ng M.C., Wu X., Cook E.H., Lundstrom C., Garsten M. New complexities in the genetics of stuttering: Significant sex-specific linkage signals. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2006;78:554–563. doi: 10.1086/501370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Tan G.C., Doke T.F., Ashburner J., Wood N.W., Frackowiak R.S. Normal variation in fronto-occipital circuitry and cerebellar structure with an autism-associated polymorphism of CNTNAP2. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1030–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Terracciano A., Sanna S., Uda M., Deiana B., Usala G., Busonero F., Maschio A., Scally M., Patriciu N., Chen W.M. Genome-wide association scan for five major dimensions of personality. Mol. Psychiatry. 2010;15:647–656. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Tomblin J.B., O'Brien M., Shriberg L.D., Williams C., Murray J., Patil S., Bjork J., Anderson S., Ballard K. Language features in a mother and daughter of a chromosome 7;13 translocation involving FOXP2. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009;52:1157–1174. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0162). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Vargha-Khadem F., Watkins K., Alcock K., Fletcher P., Passingham R. Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1995;92:930–933. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.3.930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Verkerk A.J., Mathews C.A., Joosse M., Eussen B.H., Heutink P., Oostra B.A., Tourette Syndrome Association International Consortium for Genetics CNTNAP2 is disrupted in a family with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder. Genomics. 2003;82:1–9. doi: 10.1016/s0888-7543(03)00097-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Vernes S.C., Spiteri E., Nicod J., Groszer M., Taylor J.M., Davies K.E., Geschwind D.H., Fisher S.E. High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2, a gene mutated in speech and language disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;81:1232–1250. doi: 10.1086/522238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Vernes S.C., Newbury D.F., Abrahams B.S., Winchester L., Nicod J., Groszer M., Alarcón M., Oliver P.L., Davies K.E., Geschwind D.H. A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N. Engl. J. Med. 2008;359:2337–2345. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Vernes S.C., MacDermot K.D., Monaco A.P., Fisher S.E. Assessing the impact of FOXP1 mutations on developmental verbal dyspraxia. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2009;17:1354–1358. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2009.43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wadman R., Durkin K., Conti-Ramsden G. Self-esteem, shyness, and sociability in adolescents with specific language impairment (SLI) J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008;51:938–952. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/069). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Wassink T.H., Piven J., Vieland V.J., Pietila J., Goedken R.J., Folstein S.E., Sheffield V.C. Evaluation of FOXP2 as an autism susceptibility gene. Am. J. Med. Genet. 2002;114:566–569. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.10415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Watkins K.E., Dronkers N.F., Vargha-Khadem F. Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: Comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain. 2002;125:452–464. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wittke-Thompson J.K., Ambrose N., Yairi E., Roe C., Cook E.H., Ober C., Cox N.J. Genetic studies of stuttering in a founder population. J. Fluency Disord. 2007;32:33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zeesman S., Nowaczyk M.J., Teshima I., Roberts W., Cardy J.O., Brian J., Senman L., Feuk L., Osborne L.R., Scherer S.W. Speech and language impairment and oromotor dyspraxia due to deletion of 7q31 that involves FOXP2. Am. J. Med. Genet. A. 2006;140:509–514. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Zweier C., de Jong E.K., Zweier M., Orrico A., Ousager L.B., Collins A.L., Bijlsma E.K., Oortveld M.A., Ekici A.B., Reis A. CNTNAP2 and NRXN1 are mutated in autosomal-recessive Pitt-Hopkins-like mental retardation and determine the level of a common synaptic protein in Drosophila. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2009;85:655–666. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES