Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2010 Sep 13;19(11):2758–2764. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-10-0623

False-positive results in the randomised controlled trial of mammographic screening from age 40 (‘Age’ trial)

Louise E Johns 1,*, Sue Moss 1, Howard Cuckle 2, Lynda Bobrow 3, Andrew Evans 4, Elisabeth Kutt 5, Carol Record 6, Barbara Thomas 7, Age Trial Management Group
PMCID: PMC2978129  EMSID: UKMS31861  PMID: 20837718

Abstract

Background

False-positive recall is a recognised disadvantage of mammographic breast screening and the rate of such recalls may be higher in younger women, potentially limiting the value of screening below age 50.

Methods

Attendance and screening outcome data for 53 884 women in the intervention arm of the UK Age trial were analysed to report observed false-positive recall rates during 13 years of trial fieldwork. The Age trial was a randomised controlled trial of the effect of mammographic screening from age 40 on breast cancer mortality, conducted in 23 NHS Screening centres between 1991 and 2004. Women randomised to the intervention arm were offered annual invitation to mammography from age 40/41 to age 48.

Results

Overall, 7893 women (14.6% of women the intervention arm and 18.1% of women attending at least one routine screen) experienced one or more false-positive screen during the trial. The rates of false-positive mammography at first and subsequent routine screens were 4.9% and 3.2%, respectively. The cumulative false-positive rate over seven screens was 20.5%. Eighty-nine percent of women who had a false-positive recall at their previous screen attended their next invitation to routine screening.

Conclusions

Rates of false-positive recall in the Age trial were comparable with the national screening programme, however, the positive predictive value of referral was lower. Experiencing a false-positive screen did not appear to lessen the likelihood of re-attendance in the trial.

Impact

The question of greatly increased false-positive rates in this age group and of their compromising re-attendance is refuted by the findings of this study.

Keywords: breast, screening, false-positive, age, trial

INTRODUCTION

The effectiveness of mammographic screening in reducing mortality from breast cancer in women aged 50-69 is widely accepted (1). However, debate continues regarding the risks and benefits of mammography, especially for women in their forties. Disadvantages of screening, which need to be weighed against any benefit, include false-positive results; the recall of women for further investigations that do not result in a diagnosis of breast cancer. False-positive recall can result in inconvenience, increased anxiety, particularly in the short-term, and will usually lead to further investigations, involving biopsy in some instances. Collectively, these contribute significantly to the overall cost of screening programmes.

False-positive rates of screening mammography tend to fall with age, but reported rates vary widely; 0.9% to 6.5% in published randomised trials, case-control studies and demonstration studies of annual mammography in women aged 40 to 64 (2). European organised breast screening programmes tend to have relatively low false-positive rates, whilst US rates are higher. The UK breast screening programme recorded false-positive rates of 7.9% and 3.2% at first and subsequent routine screens, respectively, between 1999 and 2005 (3). In contrast, the Netherlands breast screening programme, reported false-positive rates of only 2% at both initial and subsequent screens for women aged 50-69 between 1990 and 1995 (4).

Importantly, false-positive results may discourage women from re-attending for screening. This appears to vary considerably between countries. In the US, women were more likely to return for screening after a false-positive result, whereas in Canada they were less likely to re-attend and in Europe no significant impact of false-positive screens on re-attendance was seen (5). Many studies have sought to address the psychological consequences of false-positive mammograms and, while some have suggested lasting effects, most found that anxiety returns to previous levels once the outcome is known (6).

The UK Age trial was the only trial designed specifically to investigate the effect of annual invitation to mammography starting at age 40. It has recently been announced that the UK National Health Service Breast Screening Programme (NHSBSP) will lower the minimum invitation age for its three-yearly screening from 50 to 47 by 2012. Information from the UK Age trial, which invited women up to age 48, has therefore assumed increased relevance for the NHSBSP. In 2006, we reported mortality results from the UK Age trial after 10 years of follow-up (7). The present paper reports information on false-positive recalls at routine mammographic screening among women in the intervention arm of the Age trial during 13 years of fieldwork between 1991 and 2004.

METHODS

The trial

The Age trial was conducted in 23 NHS breast screening units in England, Wales and Scotland. The design of the study has been previously described in detail (8). Briefly, between 1991 and 1997, 160 921 women aged 39-41 years were randomised to an intervention arm or a control arm, in a ratio of 1:2. The intervention arm provided information on 53 884 women for this analysis. Women in the intervention arm were offered annual screening by mammography up to and including the calendar year of their 48th birthday, while those in the control arm received standard medical care. All women in the intervention arm, including previous non-responders, were re-invited annually unless they specifically requested not to be invited again.

The analysis included trial screening occurring between date of entry to the trial and 31st December 2004. All screening in the trial had been completed by this date.

Screening and breast cancer incidence data

Individual-level data on screening invitation, attendance, procedures performed and their outcomes were downloaded from the national breast screening computer system at participating screening units using extraction software written specifically for the trial. In this analysis, ‘ever attenders’ are defined as women who attended at least one routine screen and ‘regular attenders’ are those who attended seven or more routine trial screens. Data on breast cancer diagnoses in the trial population were obtained from cancer registries, pathology laboratories, breast screening units and the Office for National Statistics.

False-positive screens

True positive screening episodes were those where initial mammographic findings led to recall for further procedures and breast cancer (insitu or invasive) was found. False-positive screens were defined as routine trial screens where initial mammographic findings led to recall for additional procedures, but further assessment did not result in a diagnosis of breast cancer at that episode. Recalls on the basis of technically inadequate films were not included as positive mammographic results.

Cumulative false-positive recall rate

The cumulative false-positive recall rate over seven screens was calculated for a sub-group of women who attended at least seven routine screens during the trial (i.e. regular attenders). The number of women in this group who experienced at least one false-positive recall in their first seven screens was expressed as a proportion of the total number of women in the group. False-positive rates in the regular attenders were extrapolated to estimate a cumulative false-positive rate over 10 screens. The additional number of women who would be expected to have a false-positive result was derived for rounds 8 to 10 from the average number of regular attenders who experienced their first false-positive result at screens four to seven.

The cumulative independent risk over seven screens was defined by:

1probability of never having a false-positive recall over seven screens=1[(1p(FPS1))×(1p(FPSS))6]

where p(FPS1) = probability of false-positive recall at the first screen and p(FPSS) = probability of false-positive recall at subsequent screens (two to seven).

Socio-economic status

Socio-economic status of the study population was estimated using the Townsend index of social and material deprivation (9); a summary score calculated using unemployment, car ownership, owner occupation, and overcrowding variables derived from the 1991 census. Pre-calculated Townsend values by enumeration district (10) were mapped to each woman’s postcode of residence at the time they entered the trial. Data from the 1991 census data were most current for the period of trial entry which took place between 1991 and 1996. Townsend quintiles used in analyses were defined according to the distribution of scores for the whole of England and Wales.

Statistical tests

All statistical tests were conducted using STATA (version 10.0, Stata Corporation, TX, USA) statistical software. Differences between proportions were assessed by Chi-squared tests. A P value of 0.05 (2-sided) was considered statistically significant in all analyses.

RESULTS

False-positive rates

Of the 53 884 women randomised to the intervention arm of the trial, 7893 (14.6%) experienced one or more false-positive recall during the course of their routine trial screening and 1114 (2.1%) experienced two or more; equivalent to 18.1% and 2.5% in the ever attenders. Among the regular attenders, 5272 women (22.7%) experienced at least one false-positive recall and 855 (3.7%) had two or more (Table 1).

Table 1.

Number of false-positive screens per woman (screening rounds 1 to 10)

Number of false-positives All intervention arm Ever attenders

(≥1 attendance)
Regular attenders

(≥7 attendances)
Number of women % Number of women % Number of women %
53 884 100.0 43 709 100.0 23 245 100.0
At least 1 7893 14.6 7893 18.1 5272 22.7
0 45 991 85.4 35 816 81.9 17 973 77.3
1 6779 12.6 6779 15.5 4417 19.0
2 963 1.8 963 2.2 723 3.1
3 137 0.3 137 0.3 121 0.5
4 11 <0.1 11 <0.1 8 <0.1
5 3 <0.1 3 <0.1 3 <0.1

Table 2 shows the number of false-positive screens in women as a proportion of screens attended, and the additional procedures performed. There were 2134 (4.9%) false positives at first screens and 7041 (3.2%) at subsequent screens; the overall false-positive rate at routine screens was 3.5%. At first screens, 237 women with false-positive recalls (0.5% of first screens; 11.1% of false-positive first screens) had cytology and 86 women (0.2% of first screens; 4.0% of false-positive first screens) underwent surgical or non-surgical biopsy. These proportions fell at subsequent screens when 346 women (0.2% of subsequent screens; 4.9% of false-positive subsequent screens) had cytology and 214 (0.1% of subsequent screens; 3.0% of false-positive subsequent screens) underwent biopsy.

Table 2.

Cytology and biopsy procedures in women with false-positive results

Routine screen Number of screens False-positive screens* Cytology Biopsy Cytology and biopsy
N % N % N % N %
First 43 709 2134 4.9 237 0.5 86 0.2 32 0.07
Subsequent 216 930 7041 3.2 346 0.2 214 0.1 52 0.02

Total 260 639 9175 3.5 583 0.2 300 0.1 84 0.03
*

Includes 21 screens (14 first, 7 subsequent) where mammography was positive, but the women failed to attend for further procedures at the trial screening unit. None of these 21 women were diagnosed with breast cancer in the following five years.

Cumulative false-positive rates

Among the 23 245 women who were regular attenders 4.7% had a false-positive finding at their first screen and 3.2% at subsequent screens (2.9%-3.3% with no evidence of a decrease with increasing round number). Over their first seven attendances for routine mammographic screening, women in this group had a 20.5% risk of experiencing at least one false-positive recall. Calculated on the basis of independent probability, the cumulative risk of false positive was 21.6% over seven screens.

The average number of regularly attending women experiencing their first false-positive screen at rounds four to seven was 581 per round. Using this estimate as the number expected at rounds 8 to 10 gives a total of 6505 false-positives in the regular attenders; a 28.0% risk of experiencing at least one false-positive recall over ten attendances for routine mammographic screening.

Impact of false-positive results on re-attendance for screening

Table 3 shows the impact of a false-positive recall on subsequent re-attendance. Uptake of the routine invitation following a false-positive screen was 88.8%. Uptake of the routine invitation following a true-negative screen was 88.5% in women who had never had a false-positive result, compared with 91.5% in women who had prior experience of a false-positive (P<0.0001); 88.8% among all women. There was also no difference in re-attendance after false-positive compared with true-negative screens when uptake was calculated as a proportion of the number of women in each group, as opposed to the number invited in each group.

Table 3.

Impact of false-positive result on uptake of next routine invitation

Number invited Uptake

%
Invitation following a false-positive screen 7852 88.8
Invitation following a true-negative screen
   No history of false-positive screen 194 806 88.5
   History of false-positive screen 18 977 91.5
   Total 213 791 88.8

False-positive results and measures of socio-economic status

Table 4 shows the cumulative false-positive status of regular attenders, by deprivation group. There was no significant difference in risk of at least one false-positive screen between the lowest and highest Townsend quintile (risk ratio=0.94; 95% CI: 0.86-1.03; P=0.20).

Table 4.

Cumulative false-positive status and socio-economic status* among regular attenders

False-positive
status
Townsend quintile Total
1

(least deprived)
2 3 4 5

(most deprived)
n/k
N % N % N % N % N % N % N %
Yes
(1 or more)
1595 23.2 1207 23.1 926 21.3 817 22.9 507 21.9 220 23.6 5272 22.7
No 5281 76.8 4010 76.9 3419 78.7 2743 77.1 1806 78.1 714 76.4 17 973 77.3

Total 6876 100.0 5217 100.0 4345 100.0 3560 100.0 2313 100.0 934 100.0 23 245 100.0
*

Townsend deprivation score 1991, based on earliest postcode of residence during the trial

DISCUSSION

A review for the American College of Physicians (ACP) concluded that routine mammographic screening in women aged 40 to 49 years reduced the risk of death from breast cancer, but lead to an increase in additional clinical procedures and anxiety (11). Rates of false-positive mammographic screening vary widely, but tend to be lower in organised breast screening programmes. To our knowledge, the only data on false-positive results for mammographic screening in women under age 50 in Europe are from Navarra, Spain, reporting prevalent screen false-positive rates of 23%, however, initial screening was by single view mammography and recall was frequently limited to the requirement for a second view (12).

The sensitivity and specificity of mammography decrease with increasing breast density. In a study of women aged 40-44 (13), 10% of women with very dense breasts had a false positive recall, compared with 4% of women with fatty breasts, and mammography sensitivity was 62% and 88%, respectively. As women in their forties have denser breasts than women over 50, false-positive results might be expected to be higher in the Age trial than in the UK’s national programme (NHSBSP), which currently invites women aged 50-70. The false-positive rates of 5% and 3% at first and subsequent screens in the trial compare favourably with rates of 8% and 3% reported for equivalent rounds in the NHSBSP (3), however, the difference in breast cancer incidence rates in women of different ages will affect the overall rate of recall for assessment. The positive predictive value (PPV) of recall, which also reflects the specificity of the screening test, may provide a better measure for comparing false-positive results between different age groups. The PPV of recall for assessment in the trial was 2% at first screens (1st invitation) and 3-5% at subsequent screens, increasing with age. The NHSBSP PPV is 8% at first screens (1st invitation) and 16% at subsequent screens (3).

By 2012 the NHSBSP will be inviting women for their prevalent screen from age 47. Only 356 women in the Age trial received their first trial screen at age 47 and these screens will not be typical of prevalent screens in this age group since the women involved will have been previous non-attenders. More informative for the new national policy is to compare the incident round false-positive rate in Age trial women aged 47 or above with the rate of false-positives at NHSBSP incident screens in women aged 50-54 (currently the youngest age group). These women will have had their prevalent round NHSBSP screen before age 52. The incident screen false-positive rate among these women between 1999 and 2005 was 3.56% (data from Department of Health routine statistics provided annually by breast screening units). The Age trial incident round false-positive rate in women aged 47 or above was 3% (in 40 355 screens), suggesting no great increase in false-positive results should be expected from the policy of inviting younger women.

The ACP review concluded that the psychological consequences of false-positive screens were greater among women who underwent biopsy than women with further imaging only (11). Only a small number of studies have reported the rates of extra diagnostic procedures arising from false-positive screens. In a US study, 631 false-positives lead to a 26% increase in outpatient visits, 89% more imaging procedures and 20% more biopsies (14). In the Stockholm trial, in women aged 40-49, 231 false-positives resulted in 648 physician visits, 234 FNACs, 92 extra mammograms and 55 (24%) excision biopsies (15). The UK Million Women Study showed that 15% of false-positives in women aged 50 and above had a negative biopsy (FNA, wide bore needle or surgical biopsy) (16). In our data, it was not possible to distinguish between surgical and non-surgical biopsy, but of the total 9175 false-positive screens, the proportion undergoing additional invasive procedures was relatively low at 6% and 3% for cytology and biopsy, respectively.

Recent literature, both in the media and in peer-reviewed journals, has emphasised the need for more information on the potential harms of breast screening to be made routinely available to women (17, 18). The NHSBSP is currently revising the information provided to women invited for screening, which will include more information on the issues of over-diagnosis, over-treatment and false-positive recalls.

The observed cumulative risk of a regularly attending woman experiencing a false-positive recall over seven routine Age trial screens was 20%, and estimated to be 28% over ten screens. This is higher than the cumulative false-positive rates over 10 screens of 8-21%, predicted on the basis of 3-5 screening rounds in women aged 50 to 69 in Denmark (19), and higher than the 21% risk over 20 years of biennial screening for women aged over 50 in Norway (20). The Age trial rate is also higher than the 10-11% cumulative false-positive rate estimated from NHSBSP cancer detection and cumulative assessment rate data for four screening rounds at five units published by Wallis et al (21). However, the Age trial rate is significantly lower than most reported rates for US women aged under 50 which are of the order of 34% after five screens for women aged 40-49 (22).

There is an ongoing debate about whether the risk of having a false-positive screen depends, at least in part, on previous screening history (19). The independent rate may be an over-estimate if access to previous mammograms aids interpretation and reduces the chance of unnecessary recall. Conversely, the existence of a previous false-positive screen might reflect characteristics that place a woman at higher risk of a further false-positive result, for example having particularly dense breast tissue. In the trial, the observed cumulative risk of one or more false-positive recalls over seven screens was 20%, compared with an independent probability of 22% over the same period. This suggests that in this trial, previous experience of a false-positive result does not make another any more likely.

Evidence of the influence of a false-positive experience on subsequent screening attendance is contradictory; ranging from women being less likely to return for routine screening, being unaffected or more likely to attend future screens (23); (24); (25); (26). A study of women aged 50 and above invited by the UK NHSBSP in East Anglia found that 83% of women with a false-positive result re-attended compared with 96% of women with true-negative results (27). By contrast, a study of women aged 40 and over in Washington state, US, reported a 1.2-fold increase in attendance at the next scheduled screen for women with a false-positive versus women with a true-negative (28). Based on re-invitation, Age trial women who experienced a false-positive result were just as likely to attend their next screen as those who had had a true-negative result. Indeed, uptake of invitation following a true-negative result was higher (91%) among women who had had a false-positive screen in the past than among women who had never experienced a false-positive recall. The study by McCann et al identified that it was only when re-attendance was based on the number of women, rather than the number re-invited, that lower re-attendance following false-positive recall was apparent (27). When this method was applied to the Age trial data, there was no difference in uptake following a false-positive screen compared with overall uptake following a true-negative screen.

In the Age trial, as has been reported for the NHSBSP (29); (30), there was an inverse relationship between screening attendance and socio-economic status (31). Since the number of attendances will influence whether a woman ever receives a false-positive result in her cumulative screening history, the effect of deprivation was examined in this study for the regular attenders only. Among women who attended at least seven routine trial screens, the likelihood of experiencing a false-positive screen was not related to socio-economic status.

Methodological issues and limitations

The cumulative false-positive recall rate over a given number of screens has been calculated using data for women who attended at least seven screens. Women accepting all, or almost all their invitations are a selected group, so cumulative estimates based on their experience might not be valid for all women (19). However, in this trial, we found that the risk at each screen was independent of previous false-positive experience.

No information was collected on the level of increased anxiety caused by false-positive recalls in the Age trial.

The relationship between false-positive recalls and subsequent interval cancer diagnosis is not covered here, but will be addressed by a future paper

The fact that the trial was set completely within the NHSBSP framework should ensure the applicability of the results to the UK programme. The Age trial can therefore provide useful information on rates of false-positive recall to be expected among women at the youngest end of the age-range once the NHSBSP begins inviting women from age 47.

CONCLUSIONS

Results from the Age trial do not suggest that the new national policy of inviting women for breast screening from age 47 by 2012 will result in a large increase in false-positive results. Experiencing a false-positive screen did not appear to prevent women from re-attending, but the negative psychological impact should not be discounted. False-positive recalls are a disadvantage of breast cancer screening and their likelihood and implications should be fully communicated to the target population, so that women can make an informed choice about whether or not to attend screening. Whether screening should be implemented in this age group is a separate issue, but the question of greatly increased false-positive rates in this age group and of their compromising re-attendance is refuted by the findings of this study and should be taken into account when determining screening policy.

Acknowledgements

This work is supported by the Medical Research Council and Cancer Research UK, and has also received funding from the Department of Health and the US National Cancer Research Institute.

The authors would like to acknowledge that the collection of the large volumes of information required for this trial is made possible by the enthusiasm and dedication of screening office managers, clerical, secretarial and computing staff working at participating trial centres* and administrative staff at relevant Primary Care Trusts. Contributions from Derek Coleman and Nicola Kingston for data processing and analysis at the Trial Co-ordinating centre are gratefully acknowledged.

*Age trial Centres: Avon Breast Screening, Aylesbury Breast Screening Service, Breast Test Wales, Cumbria Breast Screening Service, Epping Breast Screening Service, Greater Manchester Breast Screening Service, Hanley Breast Screening, Humberside Breast Screening Service, Newcastle Breast Screening Service, North of London Breast Screening Service, Nottingham Breast Screening, Pennine Breast Screening Programme, Peterborough Breast Imaging, Sheffield Breast Screening, South Derbyshire Breast Screening Service, South East London Breast Screening Service, South East Scotland Breast Screening Programme, Surrey Breast Screening Service, Warrington Breast Screening Service, Welcome Centre Breast Unit, West Sussex Breast Screening Service, Wiltshire Breast Screening Service, Wirral Breast Centre.

The authors are grateful to the Census Dissemination Unit at the University of Manchester for the provision of pre-calculated enumeration-level Townsend scores based on 1991 census area statistics.

The Age trial was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council and Cancer Research UK, and has also received funding from the Department of Health and the US National Cancer Research Institute.

Reference List

  • 1.International Agency for Research on Cancer . Breast Cancer Screening. IARC Press; Lyon: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mushlin AI, Kouides RW, Shapiro DE. Estimating the accuracy of screening mammography: a meta-analysis. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14:143–53. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(97)00019-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bennett RL, Blanks RG, Patnick J, Moss SM. Results from the UK NHS Breast Screening Programme 2000-05. J Med Screen. 2007;14:200–4. doi: 10.1258/096914107782912068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fracheboud J, de Koning HJ, Beemsterboer PM, et al. Nation-wide breast cancer screening in The Netherlands: results of initial and subsequent screening 1990-1995. National Evaluation Team for Breast Cancer Screening. Int J Cancer. 1998;75:694–8. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0215(19980302)75:5<694::aid-ijc6>3.0.co;2-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brewer NT, Salz T, Lillie SE. Systematic review: the long-term effects of false-positive mammograms. Ann Intern Med. 2007;146:502–10. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-146-7-200704030-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Aro AR, Pilvikki AS, van Elderen TM, van der PE, van der Kamp LJ. False-positive findings in mammography screening induces short-term distress - breast cancer-specific concern prevails longer. Eur J Cancer. 2000;36:1089–97. doi: 10.1016/s0959-8049(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Moss SM, Cuckle H, Evans A, Johns L, Waller M, Bobrow L. Effect of mammographic screening from age 40 years on breast cancer mortality at 10 years’ follow-up: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2006;368:2053–60. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69834-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Moss S. A trial to study the effect on breast cancer mortality of annual mammographic screening in women starting at age 40. Trial Steering Group. J Med Screen. 1999;6:144–8. doi: 10.1136/jms.6.3.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Phillimore P, Beattie A, Townsend P. Widening inequality of health in northern England, 1981-91. BMJ. 1994;308:1125–8. doi: 10.1136/bmj.308.6937.1125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Census Dissemination Unit, University of Manchester; http://cdu.mimas.ac.uk/related/deprivation.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Armstrong K, Moye E, Williams S, Berlin JA, Reynolds EE. Screening mammography in women 40 to 49 years of age: a systematic review for the American College of Physicians. Ann Intern Med. 2007;146:516–26. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-146-7-200704030-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ascunce N, del Moral A, Murillo A, et al. Early detection programme for breast cancer in Navarra, Spain. Eur J Cancer Prev. 1994;3(Suppl 1):41–8. doi: 10.1097/00008469-199401001-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carney PA, Miglioretti DL, Yankaskas BC, et al. Individual and combined effects of age, breast density, and hormone replacement therapy use on the accuracy of screening mammography. Ann Intern Med. 2003;138:168–75. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-138-3-200302040-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Elmore JG, Barton MB, Moceri VM, Polk S, Arena PJ, Fletcher SW. Ten-year risk of false positive screening mammograms and clinical breast examinations. N Engl J Med. 1998;338:1089–96. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199804163381601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lidbrink E, Elfving J, Frisell J, Jonsson E. Neglected aspects of false positive findings of mammography in breast cancer screening: analysis of false positive cases from the Stockholm trial. BMJ. 1996;312:273–6. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7026.273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Banks E, Reeves G, Beral V, et al. Impact of use of hormone replacement therapy on false positive recall in the NHS breast screening programme: results from the Million Women Study. BMJ. 2004;328:1291–2. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7451.1291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gotzsche PC, Hartling OJ, Nielsen M, Brodersen J, Jorgensen KJ. Breast screening: the facts--or maybe not. BMJ. 2009;338:b86. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jorgensen KJ, Gotzsche PC. Who evaluates public health programmes? A review of the NHS Breast Screening Programme. J R Soc Med. 2010;103:14–20. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2009.090342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Njor SH, Olsen AH, Schwartz W, Vejborg I, Lynge E. Predicting the risk of a false-positive test for women following a mammography screening programme. J Med Screen. 2007;14:94–7. doi: 10.1258/096914107781261891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hofvind S, Thoresen S, Tretli S. The cumulative risk of a false-positive recall in the Norwegian Breast Cancer Screening Program. Cancer. 2004;101:1501–7. doi: 10.1002/cncr.20528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wallis M, Neilson F, Hogarth H, Whitaker C, Faulkner K. Cumulative attendance, assessment and cancer detection rate over four screening rounds in five English breast-screening programmes: a retrospective study. J Public Health (Oxf) 2007;29:275–80. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdm020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Christiansen CL, Wang F, Barton MB, et al. Predicting the cumulative risk of false-positive mammograms. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2000;92:1657–66. doi: 10.1093/jnci/92.20.1657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Brett J, Austoker J. Women who are recalled for further investigation for breast screening: psychological consequences 3 years after recall and factors affecting re-attendance. J Public Health Med. 2001;23:292–300. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/23.4.292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Currence BV, Pisano ED, Earp JA, et al. Does biopsy, aspiration or six-month follow-up of a false-positive mammogram reduce future screening or have large psychosocial effects? Acad Radiol. 2003;10:1257–66. doi: 10.1016/s1076-6332(03)00382-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lampic C, Thurfjell E, Sjoden PO. The influence of a false-positive mammogram on a woman’s subsequent behaviour for detecting breast cancer. Eur J Cancer. 2003;39:1730–7. doi: 10.1016/s0959-8049(02)00451-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pinckney RG, Geller BM, Burman M, Littenberg B. Effect of false-positive mammograms on return for subsequent screening mammography. Am J Med. 2003;114:120–5. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(02)01438-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.McCann J, Stockton D, Godward S. Impact of false-positive mammography on subsequent screening attendance and risk of cancer. Breast Cancer Res. 2002;4:R11. doi: 10.1186/bcr455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Burman ML, Taplin SH, Herta DF, Elmore JG. Effect of false-positive mammograms on interval breast cancer screening in a health maintenance organization. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131:1–6. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-1-199907060-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Banks E, Beral V, Cameron R, et al. Comparison of various characteristics of women who do and do not attend for breast cancer screening. Breast Cancer Res. 2002;4:R1. doi: 10.1186/bcr418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Maheswaran R, Pearson T, Jordan H, Black D. Socioeconomic deprivation, travel distance, location of service, and uptake of breast cancer screening in North Derbyshire, UK. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:208–12. doi: 10.1136/jech.200X.038398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Johns LE, Moss SM. Randomized controlled trial of mammographic screening from age 40 (‘Age’ trial): patterns of screening attendance. J Med Screen. 2010;17:37–43. doi: 10.1258/jms.2010.009091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES