Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2011 Nov 3;7(11):e1002233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002233

How Landscape Heterogeneity Frames Optimal Diffusivity in Searching Processes

E P Raposo 1,*, F Bartumeus 2, M G E da Luz 3, P J Ribeiro-Neto 1, T A Souza 1,4, G M Viswanathan 5,6
Editor: Mercedes Pascual7
PMCID: PMC3207935  PMID: 22072951

Abstract

Theoretical and empirical investigations of search strategies typically have failed to distinguish the distinct roles played by density versus patchiness of resources. It is well known that motility and diffusivity of organisms often increase in environments with low density of resources, but thus far there has been little progress in understanding the specific role of landscape heterogeneity and disorder on random, non-oriented motility. Here we address the general question of how the landscape heterogeneity affects the efficiency of encounter interactions under global constant density of scarce resources. We unveil the key mechanism coupling the landscape structure with optimal search diffusivity. In particular, our main result leads to an empirically testable prediction: enhanced diffusivity (including superdiffusive searches), with shift in the diffusion exponent, favors the success of target encounters in heterogeneous landscapes.

Author Summary

Understanding how animals search for food is crucial for animal ecology. Although much has been learned about the main aspects of the so-called foraging problem, some important questions still remain unanswered. In this work we address the issue of the relevance of heterogeneity in the resources distribution to efficient animal foraging behavior. Our results unveil the key mechanism coupling landscape heterogeneity dynamics with optimal search diffusivity. Indeed, although the effect of (global) resource density on animal foraging behavior is well documented, much less has been known about how spatiotemporal landscape heterogeneity affects the efficiency of encounter interactions by foraging organisms. In this sense, we propose a new empirically testable theoretical prediction on the dynamics (e.g. diffusion exponent) of foraging organisms in heterogeneous environments. We also show that the conditions in which Lévy strategies are optimal are much broader than previously considered.

Introduction

The random search problem has lately received a great deal of attention [1], [2]. This is partly due to its broad interdisciplinary range of applications, which include, e.g., enhanced diffusion of regulatory proteins while “searching” for specific DNA spots [3], [4] and the finding of binding sites on transmembrane proteins by neurotransmitters in the brain [5]. Recently, this problem has also found interesting connections with human mobility and related topics [6][9].

A classical context in which the random search problem has been applied in the last four decades is animal foraging [1], [2], [10][27], with the searcher (i.e. forager) typically represented by an animal species in quest of target sites (prey, food, other individuals, shelter, etc.) in a search landscape.

Among the most studied random walk models proposed as plausible search strategies, we cite correlated random walks [12], [28], [29], Lévy flights and walks [13][17], [19], [20], [24], [25], [27], [30][39], intermittent walks [40][46], and composite Brownian walks [47], [48]. In particular, Lévy random searchers, with probability distribution of step lengths Inline graphic, for Inline graphic, have successfully explained [34] the emergence of optimal searches in landscapes with randomly and scarcely distributed target sites. On the other hand, when resources are plentiful Lévy strategies are unnecessary [34], and efficient Brownian optimal searches may arise with, e.g., a Poisson-like exponential distribution Inline graphic [24], [25]. Lévy flights and walks have been also shown to be relevant in several other contexts [1], such as in proteins searching for specific DNA sites [49], in which the optimal Lévy mechanism emerges directly from the underlying physics of the problem (polymer scaling theory in three dimensions).

In the regime of low density of resources of the random search problem, two limiting situations have been extensively considered [34]: (i) non-destructive searches, in which the searcher always departs from a position at the vicinity of the last target found with unrestricted revisits; and (ii) destructive searches, in which, once found, the target becomes inaccessible to future visits, so that the starting point of the searcher is, on average, faraway from all targets. In the former case the maximum efficiency is achieved [34] for Inline graphic (a “compromise” superdiffusive solution), whereas in the latter Inline graphic (ballistic motion). It is important to observe, nevertheless, that by varying the searcher's starting point [44], [48] or the degree of target revisitability or temporal regeneration [50], [51], intermediate values of the optimal Lévy exponent arise, Inline graphic.

It is also interesting to comment on the effect of an energy cost function on the efficiency of search strategies. Indeed, as reported in [50], [51], the range of Inline graphic-values associated with search paths in which the net energy gain (the balance between the energy income due to the finding of targets and the energy cost of the search process itself) remains always positive is actually limited. In such a case, low values of Inline graphic giving rise to very large search jumps might not be acceptable, since they imply a high energy cost, with intermediate values of Inline graphic emerging as the best strategy. In addition, we also refer to the study reported in [52] in which exact results for the first passage time and leapover statistics of Lévy flights are presented. In this case, the targets might not be always detected, being thus overshoot by jumps whose length distribution displays infinite variance.

Despite the intense progress in the fields of random searches and animal foraging, a number of relevant issues still remain open. A particularly important one is to understand the coupling mechanism between landscape spatiotemporal dynamics and efficient search motility, when resources are scarce and environmental information is limited. In this sense, the pervasiveness of different animal search strategies is expected to strongly depend on a few but essential features of actual landscapes. For instance, targets distributions in realistic search processes usually present heterogeneous properties through time and space, such as diverse degrees of temporal regeneration and spatial aggregation [26], [53], [54]. Although the effect of (global) resource density on animal foraging behavior is well documented [25], [26], [37], [42], [55], much less is known about how spatiotemporal landscape heterogeneity dynamics affects the target revisitability and/or searcher-to-targets distances, both known to be key properties to optimize perception-limited searches [44], [48], [50], [51]. Thus, a mechanistic understanding of how and which landscape features are related to search efficiency should be a relevant step towards a comprehensive view of animal foraging behavior.

Here we address the question of how the landscape heterogeneity influences the encounter success and search efficiency under conditions of constant (global) density of scarce resources. We develop a random search model in which diverse degrees of inhomogeneities are considered by introducing fluctuations in the starting distances to target sites. We thus ask what happens to the optimal search strategy in an heterogeneous landscape, as the searcher's initial distances to the targets fluctuate along the search. We answer to this query qualitatively for the general case and quantitatively for Lévy random searches in particular, in the constant density regime of scarce resources. In patchy or aggregated landscapes, we find that enhanced diffusivity (including superdiffusive strategies) favors the encounter of targets and the success of foraging. Eventually, for strong enough fluctuations in the starting distances to nearby targets a crossover to ballistic strategies might emerge.

These predictions are empirically testable through feasible experiments which investigate the dynamics (e.g. diffusion exponent) of foraging organisms in specially designed low-density environments of controlled heterogeneity.

Materials and Methods

Distributions of starting positions: General considerations

We consider a random search model in which diverse degrees of landscape heterogeneity are taken into account by introducing fluctuations in the starting distances to target sites in a one-dimensional (1D) search space, with absorbing boundaries separated by the distance Inline graphic. Every time an encounter occurs the search resets and restarts over again. Thus, the overall search trajectory can be viewed as the concatenated sum of partial paths between consecutive encounters. The targets' positions are fixed – targets are in fact the boundaries of the system. Fluctuations in the starting distances to the targets are introduced by sampling the searcher's departing position after each encounter from a probability density function (pdf) Inline graphic of initial positions Inline graphic. Importantly, Inline graphic also implies a distribution of starting (a)symmetry conditions regarding the relative distances between the searcher and the boundary targets.

This approach allows the typification of landscapes that, on average, depress or boost the presence of nearby targets in the search process. Diverse degrees of landscape heterogeneity can thus be achieved through suitable choices of Inline graphic.

For example, a pdf providing a distribution of nearly symmetric conditions can be assigned to a landscape with a high degree of homogeneity in the spatial arrangement of targets. In this sense, the mentioned destructive search represents the fully symmetric limiting situation, with the searcher's starting location always equidistant from all boundary targets. On the other hand, a distribution Inline graphic which generates a set of asymmetric conditions is related to a patchy or aggregated landscape. Indeed, in a patchy landscape it is likely that a search process starts with an asymmetric situation in which the distances to the nearest and farthest targets are very dissimilar. Analogously, the non-destructive search corresponds to the highest asymmetric case, in which at every starting search the distance to the closest (farthest) target is minimum (maximum). Finally, a pdf Inline graphic giving rise to an heterogeneous set of initial conditions (combining symmetric and asymmetric situations) can be associated with heterogeneous landscapes of structure in between the homogeneous and patchy cases.

More specifically, the limiting case corresponding to the mentioned destructive search can be described by the pdf with fully symmetric initial condition,

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e017.jpg (1)

where Inline graphic denotes Dirac Inline graphic-function. This means that every destructive search starts exactly at half distance from the boundary targets. In this context, it is possible to introduce fluctuations in Inline graphic by considering, e.g., a Poisson-like pdf [56] exponentially decaying with the distance to the point at the center of the search space, Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e022.jpg (2)

where Inline graphic, with Inline graphic the “radius of vision” of the searcher (see below), Inline graphic the normalization constant, and Inline graphic due to the symmetry of the search space.

On the other hand, the highest asymmetric non-destructive limiting case is represented by

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e027.jpg (3)

so that every search starts from the point of minimum distance in which the nearest target is undetectable, Inline graphic. Similarly, fluctuations in Inline graphic regarding this case can be introduced by considering a Poisson-like pdf decreasing with respect to the point Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e031.jpg (4)

where Inline graphic, Inline graphic is a normalization constant, and Inline graphic. In Eqs. (2) and (4), the parameter Inline graphic controls the range and magnitude of the fluctuations. Actually, the smaller the value of Inline graphic, the less disperse are the fluctuations around Inline graphic and Inline graphic in Eqs. (2) and (4), respectively.

Random search model in 1D

When looking for boundary target sites in a 1D interval, the searcher's step lengths Inline graphic are taken from a general pdf Inline graphic. At each step the probabilities to move to the right or to the left are equal. We define the “radius of vision” Inline graphic as the distance below which a target becomes detectable by the searcher. Thus, if the targets are located at the boundary positions Inline graphic and Inline graphic, the search keeps on as long as the walker's position lies in the range Inline graphic. Here we are interested in searches in environments scarce in targets, i.e. for Inline graphic. In this case, leaving the present position to look randomly for targets should occur much more frequently than simply detecting a site in the close vicinity, a regime favored when targets are plentiful.

Suppose initially that, as a target is found, the search always restarts from the same position Inline graphic in the interval Inline graphic. As discussed, the highest asymmetric (non-destructive) and fully symmetric (destructive) cases correspond respectively to setting Inline graphic (or Inline graphic, due to symmetry) and Inline graphic. After the encounter of a statistically large number of targets, the efficiency of the search, Inline graphic, is evaluated [34] as the ratio of the number of sites found to the total distance traversed by the searcher. Since this distance is equal to the product of the number of encounters and the average distance traveled between consecutive findings, Inline graphic, then Inline graphic.

Consider now that, instead of always departing from the same location after an encounter, the searcher can restart from any initial position Inline graphic in the range Inline graphic, chosen from a pdf Inline graphic. The fluctuating values of Inline graphic imply a distribution of Inline graphic values. Since searches starting at Inline graphic are statistically indistinguishable from searches starting at Inline graphic (in both cases the closest and farthest targets are at distances Inline graphic and Inline graphic from the starting location), the symmetry of the search space regarding the position Inline graphic implies Inline graphic. The average efficiency thus becomes

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e065.jpg (5)

where Inline graphic due to the above mentioned symmetry.

To study the effect of fluctuations in the starting distances of a searcher, we note that the exact average distance Inline graphic in Eq. (5) can be formally expressed [57], [58] as

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e068.jpg (6)

where the integral operator Inline graphic acts as follows:

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e070.jpg (7)

and Inline graphic and Inline graphic are, respectively, the unity operator and the average length of a single step starting at Inline graphic. Specifically, we can write for a general pdf Inline graphic

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e075.jpg (8)

The second and third integrals above represent steps to the left and to the right which are not truncated by the encounter of a target site at the boundaries; the first and last ones concern steps truncated by the detection of the targets at Inline graphic and Inline graphic, respectively (what actually happens at Inline graphic and Inline graphic, due to the searcher's “radius of vision”).

Despite the formal aspect of Eq. (6), the numerical calculation of Inline graphic with a given Inline graphic can be performed by discretizing [57], [58] the search interval Inline graphic, i.e. Inline graphic, with Inline graphic integer and Inline graphic. In this procedure, integrals are approximated by summations, and so on.

In the next section, we use this model to study the role of landscape heterogeneity on the search efficiency and diffusivity. The presented analysis is qualitative for the general case and quantitative for Lévy random searches.

Results

Efficient search strategies with a general pdf of step lengths

Consider, first, the limiting case with no fluctuation in the starting distances. The underlying mechanisms of efficient searches with asymmetric and symmetric initial conditions are fundamentally distinct. In the fully symmetric (destructive) case Inline graphic the closest sites are located at equal initial distances Inline graphic from the searcher in the low-density regime. Thus, for a general distribution of step lengths Inline graphic characterized by a set of parameters Inline graphic, the one Inline graphic that leads to the largest efficiency Inline graphic must present the fastest possible diffusivity in order to reach these faraway targets. For example, in the case of the single-parameter power-law pdf Inline graphic, Inline graphic is maximized with ballistic strategy [34]: Inline graphic.

In contrast, in the highest asymmetric (non-destructive) situation Inline graphic or Inline graphic the most efficient search must compromise between performing large steps to access the farthest site and sweeping in detail at the vicinity of the closest site. In the parameter space, this solution, related to a set Inline graphic, displays intermediate diffusivity between normal (Brownian) and the fastest possible one, assigned to the set Inline graphic. In the same example, this implies [34] Inline graphic, in contrast with Brownian diffusion resulting from Inline graphic (see Figs. 1 and 2).

Figure 1. Robustness of the ballistic optimal search strategy with respect to fluctuations in the distances to faraway target sites.

Figure 1

In the case of Lévy random searchers, for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, the average search efficiency Inline graphic, Eq. (5), is always highest for Inline graphic (ballistic dynamics), for any value of the parameter Inline graphic of the Poissonian fluctuations around the maximum allowed distance, Inline graphic, Eq. (2). Cases with uniform and without any (Inline graphic-function) fluctuation are also shown. Solid lines are a visual guide.

Figure 2. Shift in the optimal search strategy towards an enhanced superdiffusive dynamical regime, as landscapes with distinct degrees of heterogeneity are considered.

Figure 2

For Lévy random searchers, using Inline graphic and Inline graphic (solid symbols), the average search efficiency Inline graphic, Eq. (5), is maximized for smaller Inline graphic (faster diffusivity) in the case of wider (larger-Inline graphic) Poissonian fluctuations in the distances to nearby target sites, Eq. (4). Cases with uniform and without any (Inline graphic-function) fluctuation are also shown (solid lines are a visual guide). Empty symbols locate the maximum Inline graphic obtained from the condition Inline graphic. For strong enough fluctuations, with Inline graphic, a crossover to ballistic dynamics (Inline graphic) emerges.

When the starting positions are not fixed, heterogeneous landscapes with stronger fluctuations in the distances to nearby targets lead to optimal search strategies with faster dynamics (enhanced diffusivity). The arguments giving rise to this general conclusion are as follows.

On one hand, sampling starting positions around Inline graphic corresponds to introduce fluctuations in the initial distances to the faraway boundary targets in the low-density regime, as discussed. In this case, we expect that starting positions far away from Inline graphic are chosen with smaller probabilities. This implies a decreasing pdf Inline graphic from Inline graphic to Inline graphic, such as found in Eq. (2). Consequently, both Inline graphic and Inline graphic increase monotonically from Inline graphic to Inline graphic (Fig. 3). The most relevant contribution to the product Inline graphic in Eq. (5) thus comes from positions near Inline graphic. No qualitative difference is expected to occur between Inline graphic and Inline graphic, indicating that searches with fully symmetric (fixed) initial condition and those comprising fluctuations in the faraway targets present similar optimal dynamics, related to the set Inline graphic, namely ballistic, if supported by Inline graphic.

Figure 3. Nice adjustment of the average distance .

Figure 3

Inline graphic traversed between consecutive findings by a Lévy random searcher starting at position Inline graphic . Results obtained by numerical discretization of Eq. (6) (solid lines) and multiple regression (symbols), for Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

On the other hand, in the asymmetric case fluctuations in the starting distances to the nearby boundary target can be introduced by a decreasing pdf Inline graphic from Inline graphic to Inline graphic, such as in Eq. (4). Therefore, as Inline graphic increases and Inline graphic diminishes, the initial position associated with the most relevant contribution to Inline graphic in Eq. (5) crosses over to somewhere in between Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Indeed, the slower Inline graphic decays, the larger such position becomes. As a consequence, the asymmetric optimal set Inline graphic in the absence of fluctuations might give away the role of the most efficient search strategy to some other intermediate compromising solution Inline graphic, which is closer to the symmetric set Inline graphic in the parameter space and, therefore, presents enhanced dynamics (e.g., a larger diffusion exponent). Eventually, for some proper choice of Inline graphic encompassing strong fluctuations with large weight near Inline graphic, the justification for such compromising solution might even fade away, so that Inline graphic, with strategies of fastest possible diffusivity becoming optimal. In this uttermost case fluctuations lose their local character, and a crossover from superdiffusive to ballistic search behavior may take place.

We observe that the above rationale should also apply, at least qualitatively, to searches in higher-dimensional spaces. In this situation, as the search path can be approximated by a sequence of nearly rectilinear moves, the general qualitative features of 1D random searches usually hold true in higher dimensions [34], [39]. Nevertheless, the finding of targets in 2D and 3D occurs with considerably lower probability, since the extra spatial directions yield a larger exploration space, resulting in lower encounter rates and search efficiencies. The impact of target spatial fluctuations on high-dimensional search strategies should also reduce [39]. We can thus conclude that, beyond representing the realistic exploration space of some animal species [27], the 1D analysis presented here is also useful in establishing upper limits for the influence of landscape heterogeneities in random searches. Therefore, the understanding of animal foraging behavior in 2D and 3D, as well as other practical realizations of the random search problem, might also benefit from the present results.

We next apply the above arguments, valid for a general pdf Inline graphic, to the particular case of Lévy random searchers.

Lévy searches in heterogeneous landscapes

We now specifically consider a random searcher with step lengths chosen from the pdf

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e153.jpg (9)

and Inline graphic otherwise, with Inline graphic representing a lower cutoff length. We assign a “negative step length” Inline graphic if the searcher moves to the left and take Inline graphic for simplicity. Equation (9) for Inline graphic corresponds to the long-range asymptotical limit of Lévy Inline graphic-stable distributions with index Inline graphic, characterized by the existence of rare, large steps alternating between sequences of many short-length jumps [13], [14], [16]. As its second moment diverges the central limit theorem does not hold, and anomalous (superdiffusive) dynamics governed by the generalized central limit theorem takes place. Indeed, Lévy random walks and flights are related to a Hurst exponent [13], [14] Inline graphic, with ballistic dynamics in the case Inline graphic, whereas diffusive behavior (Inline graphic) emerges for Inline graphic. For Inline graphic pdf (9) is not normalizable and Inline graphic corresponds to the Cauchy distribution.

The search path eventually comprises truncated steps due to the encounter of targets, so that the power-law decay of Eq. (9) cannot extend all the way to infinity, thus implying an effective truncated Lévy distribution [59]. In spite of this, in the regime Inline graphic the search should retain the most relevant properties of a non-truncated Lévy walk to a considerable extent. Indeed, the ratio Inline graphic of the number of truncated steps to the non-truncated ones, essentially equal to the inverse of the average number of steps performed between consecutive targets, is given by Inline graphic and Inline graphic, for Inline graphic, in the highest asymmetric (non-destructive) and fully symmetric (destructive) cases, respectively [34], [57], [58]. Thus, except for Inline graphic ballistic walks, one has that Inline graphic if Inline graphic. Further, the justification for truncated distributions also arises naturally in the context of animal foraging since directional persistence due to scanning is likely to be broken at the finding of targets [19]. Indeed, infinitely long rectilinear paths are not allowed for searching organisms.

By inserting Eq. (9) into Eqs. (6) and (7), we numerically calculate Inline graphic through the discretization of the search space (see previous section). Results are displayed in solid lines in Fig. 3. Notice first the presence of the symmetry Inline graphic discussed above. In the absence of fluctuations in the initial distances, the existence of a maximum efficiency with an intermediate exponent Inline graphic (see Fig. 2) for searches starting at fixed Inline graphic (highest asymmetric condition) can be understood as follows: strategies with Inline graphic might access the farthest target at Inline graphic in a ballistic way after a small number of very large steps, implying a large Inline graphic and low efficiency; in contrast, searches with Inline graphic tend on average to find the closest site at Inline graphic after a great number of small steps, also giving rise to a large Inline graphic; the efficient compromise between these two trends, leading to the lowest Inline graphic and maximum Inline graphic, is therefore represented by a strategy with an intermediate value, Inline graphic.

In the presence of fluctuations in the starting distances, the integral (5) must be evaluated. Although the explicit expression for Inline graphic, Eq. (6), is not known up to the present, a multiple regression can be successfully performed,

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e189.jpg (10)

as indicated by the nice adjustment shown in Fig. 3, obtained with Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Thus, the integral (5) can be done using Eqs. (2), (4) and (10), with results displayed in Figs. 1 and 2 for several values of the parameter Inline graphic.

By considering fluctuations in the starting distances to faraway targets through Eq. (2), we notice in Fig. 1 that the efficiency is qualitatively similar to that of the fully symmetric condition, Eq. (1), in agreement with the general arguments of the previous section. Indeed, in both cases the maximum efficiency is achieved as Inline graphic. For Inline graphic the presence of fluctuations only slightly improve the efficiency. These results indicate that ballistic strategies remain robust to fluctuations in the distribution of faraway targets.

On the other hand, fluctuations in the starting distances to nearby targets, Eq. (4), are shown in Fig. 2 to decrease considerably the search efficiency, in comparison to the highest asymmetric case, Eq. (3). In this regime, since stronger fluctuations increase the weight of starting positions far from the target at Inline graphic, the compromising optimal Lévy strategy displays enhanced superdiffusion, observed in the location of the maximum efficiency in Fig. 2, which shifts from Inline graphic, for the delta pdf and Eq. (4) with small Inline graphic, towards Inline graphic, for larger Inline graphic (slower decaying Inline graphic). Indeed, both the pdf of Eq. (4) with a vanishing Inline graphic and Eq. (3) are very acute at Inline graphic. It is also worth noticing that a lower Inline graphic is related to a larger Hurst exponent [1], [13], [14], and therefore to a larger diffusion exponent, as argued in the previous section.

As even larger values of Inline graphic are considered, fluctuations in the starting distances to the nearby target become non-local, and Eq. (4) approaches the Inline graphic limiting case of the uniform distribution, Inline graphic (see Fig. 2). In this situation, search paths departing from distinct Inline graphic are equally weighted in Eq. (5), so that the dominant contribution to the integral (and to the average efficiency Inline graphic as well) comes from search walks starting at positions near Inline graphic. Since for these walks the most efficient strategy is ballistic, a crossover from superdiffusive to ballistic optimal searches emerges, induced by such strong fluctuations. Consequently, the efficiency curves for very large Inline graphic (Fig. 2) are remarkably similar to that of the fully symmetric case (Fig. 1).

We can quantify this crossover shift in Inline graphic by defining a function Inline graphic that identifies the location in the Inline graphic-axis of the maximum in the efficiency Inline graphic, for each curve in Fig. 2 with fixed Inline graphic. As discussed, eventually a compromising solution with Inline graphic cannot be achieved, and an efficiency function Inline graphic monotonically decreasing with increasing Inline graphic arises for Inline graphic. In this sense, the value Inline graphic for which such crossover occurs marks the onset of a regime dominated by ballistic optimal search strategies.

The value of Inline graphic for each Inline graphic can be determined from the condition Inline graphic, so that, by considering Eqs. (4), (5) and (10),

graphic file with name pcbi.1002233.e224.jpg (11)

with Inline graphic. Solutions are displayed in Fig. 4 and also in Fig. 2 as empty symbols, locating the maximum of each efficiency curve. In addition, the crossover value can be determined through Inline graphic. In the case of pdf (4), we obtain (Fig. 4) Inline graphic for Inline graphic and Inline graphic (regime Inline graphic).

Figure 4. Determination of the optimal search strategy of Lévy random searchers with Poissonian fluctuations in the distances to nearby targets, Eq. (4).

Figure 4

The condition Inline graphic, for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, provides the optimal Lévy exponent, Inline graphic, associated with the strategy of maximum average efficiency. Inset: since strategies with Inline graphic are not allowed (non-normalizable pdf of step lengths), the highest efficiency is always obtained for Inline graphic as fluctuations with Inline graphic are considered, marking the onset of a regime dominated by ballistic optimal search dynamics.

We also note that the scale-dependent interplay between the target density and the range of fluctuations implies a value of Inline graphic which is a function of Inline graphic. For instance, a larger Inline graphic (i.e., a lower target density) leads to a larger Inline graphic and a broader regime in which superdiffusive Lévy searchers are optimal. Nevertheless, the above qualitative picture should still hold as long as low target densities are considered.

Moreover, since ballistic strategies lose efficiency in higher dimensional spaces [44], it might be possible that in 2D and 3D the crossover to ballistic dynamics becomes considerably limited. In spite of this, enhanced superdiffusive searches, with Inline graphic, should still conceivably emerge due to fluctuations in higher-dimensional heterogeneous landscapes.

From these results we conclude that, in the presence of Poissonian-distributed fluctuating starting distances with Inline graphic, Lévy search strategies with faster (enhanced) superdiffusive properties, i.e. Inline graphic, represent optimal compromising solutions. In this sense, as local fluctuations in nearby targets give rise to landscape heterogeneity, Lévy searches with enhanced superdiffusive dynamics actually maximize the search efficiency in aggregate and patchy environments. On the other hand, for strong enough fluctuations with Inline graphic, a crossover to the ballistic strategy emerges in order to access efficiently the faraway region where targets are distributed. These findings are in full agreement with the general considerations discussed in the previous section.

At last, to further test the robustness of these results we have also considered the power-law distribution of starting positions, Inline graphic, with Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic as the normalization constant. Differently from distributions (2) and (4), the long tail in this pdf confers self-affine scale-invariant properties over a long spatial range in the low-density regime, Inline graphic. The evidence of scale-free distributions of targets has been reported in the context of animal foraging, e.g. in [24]. In the present analysis we have essentially verified all the general features previously discussed. In particular, all strategies with Inline graphic are ballistic, with compromising superdiffusive solutions arising for Inline graphic.

Discussion

The effect of limited resources on animal motility is well documented in ecology. Scarcity coming from resource competition is known to induce higher dispersal rates [60], [61] and larger home ranges [62], [63]. Habitat fragmentation also reshapes dispersal kernels, often increasing dispersal distances [64]. In the context of foraging behavior, the role of (global) resource density has been considerably investigated, with strong evidence pointing to shifts from Brownian to superdiffusive search strategies as animals move from high to low productive areas. Examples range from microorganisms [37] to large marine predators [25], [26], [55]. In contrast, much less is known about the influence of heterogeneity in the resource distribution on the foraging success.

Most theoretical efforts relying on core random search theory have by far provided only a limited approach to the issue of optimal searches, since they mostly assume oversimplified landscapes [2], [40]. Nonetheless, a few simulation studies have addressed the effect of environmental heterogeneity, including target motion, on encounter success for different searcher types [19], [24], [39], [65], [66]. These works give support to the hypothesis that search processes are linked to target distributions and dynamics, thus agreeing with our results in that the optimal strategy can actually change, e.g. from superdiffusive to ballistic motion, depending on the landscape heterogeneity. In a more recent example, it was shown [65] that Lévy optimal foragers can be evolutionarily optimal in heterogeneous environments, for suitable details of the simulations and definition of efficiency. Our work advances on this topic by pinpointing a very general mechanism which seems essential to understand previous simulation results [19], [24], [39], [65].

By comprehensively describing the key mechanism coupling landscape dynamics and search diffusivity, we have shown that statistical fluctuations in the set of initial search conditions play a crucial role for determining which strategy is optimal. The presence of such fluctuations sets a clear basis for the non-universality of search patterns, and shows that enhanced diffusivity (including superdiffuse strategies) favors random encounter success in patchy and aggregated landscapes. As a consequence, the foraging conditions in which Lévy strategies appear as optimal are much broader than previously suggested [40], [44][46].

In dynamic and complex landscapes with scarcity of resources neither ballistic nor Lévy strategies should be considered as universal (see, e.g., [45], [46]), since realistic fluctuations in the targets distribution may induce switches between these two regimes. This observation has been confirmed by recent empirical results [25], [27], showing that foragers in the wild do not exhibit movement patterns that can be approximated, at all times, by Lévy, ballistic or exponential models. Nevertheless, the relevant finding is that in the low-density regime superdiffusive Lévy strategies remain as the optimal solution in a broad range of heterogeneous landscape conditions, with the optimal exponent Inline graphic dependent on specific environment properties. Crossovers between superdiffusive and ballistic strategies may also emerge depending on whether strong target spatial fluctuations are local or not, and if they depress or boost the presence of nearby targets. For instance, recent data on a species of jellyfish have reported [27] on Lévy flight foraging strategies with optimal index as low as Inline graphic. Moreover, studies on marine predators have also found [24] small values as Inline graphic. Such rather fast, enhanced superdiffusion (with respect to Inline graphic) suggests the occurrence of foraging activity in a highly dynamic and heterogeneous landscape, as it is clearly the case for marine prey landscapes [25], [26], [67].

In the present work, the question of how the landscape heterogeneity affects the search efficiency in encounter interactions is addressed under conditions of constant global density of scarce resources. In such conditions we predict that efficient strategies with larger diffusion exponents (including superdiffusive ones) should arise, as heterogeneous environments with wider distributions of starting distances between the foraging organism and the nearby targets are considered. Similarly to what occurs in homogeneous landscapes [42], we do not expect density fluctuations in the scarcity regime to modify optimal Lévy solutions per se, but only to the extent that fluctuations in density modify the initial searcher-to-targets distances. In other words, provided that the asymmetry in the searcher-to-targets distances is maintained as density changes, optimal Lévy strategies should result insensitive to target density fluctuations. This means that for a Lévy searcher is less important to have advanced knowledge of the density than of the relative positions of the targets. Clearly, robustness to changes in environmental parameters (i.e. density) should be considered as an advantage in non-informed optimal search solutions [42].

If we acknowledge the presence of selective pressures responsible for the evolution and maintenance of non-oriented motility in organisms [68], our results lead to a neat empirically testable prediction: patchy and heterogeneous landscapes should promote the emergence of enhanced diffusivity and compromising optimal Lévy strategies. Even though the empirical inference of large scale movement patterns from heterogeneity properties of the landscape is a difficult task [26], specifically designed and controlled large scale experiments are feasible in the laboratory [68][71] and even in the field [54].

We hope the present study might shed light on unsettled issues related to the efficiency and associated dynamics of organisms performing random searches. Besides the well documented dependence of search efficiency on resource density [25], [26], [34], [37], [55], our results suggest another relevant aspect of non-universal random search behavior: landscape heterogeneity frames optimal diffusivity.

Acknowledgments

We thank A. S. Ferreira and H. A. Araújo for fruitful discussions.

Footnotes

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

This work was supported by CNPq, CAPES, F. Araucária, FAPEAL, FACEPE (Brazilian agencies). FB acknowledges the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (Grants RyC09-449-06-01, BFU2010-22337). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Viswanathan GM, da Luz MGE, Raposo EP, Stanley HE. The physics of foraging: an introduction to random searches and biological encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.da Luz MGE, Grosberg A, Raposo EP, Viswanathan GM, editors. The randomsearch problem: trends and perspectives. J Phys A 42, no. 43. Bristol: IOPscience; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lomholt MA, van den Broek B, Kalisch S-MJ, Wuite GLJ, Metzler R. Facilitated diffusion with DNA coiling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:8204–8208. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903293106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.van den Broek B, Lomholt MA, Kalisch S-MJ, Metzler R, Wuite GLJ. How DNA coiling enhances target localization by proteins. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:15738–15742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804248105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mandell AJ, Selz KA, Shlesinger MF. Mode matches and their locations in the hydrophobic free energy sequences of peptide ligands and their receptor eigenfunctions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94:13576–13581. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.25.13576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hufnagel L, Brockmann D, Geisel T. Forecast and control of epidemics in a globalized world. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:15124–15129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308344101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brockmann D, Hufnagel L, Geisel T. The scaling laws of human travel. Nature. 2006;439:462–465. doi: 10.1038/nature04292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Shlesinger MF. Random walks: follow the money. Nature Phys. 2006;2:69–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.González MC, Hidalgo CA, Barabási A-L. Understanding individual human mobility patterns. Nature. 2008;453:779–782. doi: 10.1038/nature06958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stephens DW, Krebs JR. Foraging theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Berg HC. Random walks in biology. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Turchin P. Quantitative analysis of movement: measuring and modelling population redistribution in animal and plants. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates Inc; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shlesinger MF, Klafter J. Lévy walks versus Lévy flights. In: Stanley HE, Ostrowsky N, editors. On growth and form. Dordrecht: Nijhoff; 1986. pp. 279–283. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Shlesinger MF, Zaslavsky G, Frisch U, editors. Lévy flights and related topics in physics. Berlin: Springer; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Levandowsky M, Klafter J, White BS. Swimming behavior and chemosensory responses in the protistan microzooplankton as a function of the hydrodynamic regime. Bull Marine Sci. 1988;43:758–763. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Raposo EP, Buldyrev SV, da Luz MGE, Viswanathan GM, Stanley HE. Lévy flights and random searches. J Phys A. 2009;42:434003. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Viswanathan GM, Raposo EP, da Luz MGE. Lévy flights and superdiffusion in random search: the biological encounters context. Phys Life Rev. 2008;5:133–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Petrovskii S, Mashanova A, Jansen VAA. Variation in individual walking behavior creates the impression of a Lévy flight. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:8704–8707. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015208108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bartumeus F, Levin S. Fractal reorientation clocks: linking animal behavior to statistical patterns of search. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:19072–19077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801926105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bartumeus F, Catalan J. Optimal search behavior and classic foraging theory. J Phys A. 2009;42:434002. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stocker R, Seymour JR, Samadani A, Hunt DE, Polz M. Rapid chemotactic response enables marine bacteria to exploit ephemeral microscale nutrient patches. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:4209–4214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709765105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Seymour JR, Marcos, Stocker R. Resource patch formation and exploitation throughout the marine microbial food web. Am Nat. 2008;173:E15–E29. doi: 10.1086/593004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Oshanin G, Vasilyev O, Krapivsky PL, Klafter J. Survival of an evasive prey. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;109:13696–13671. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904354106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sims DW, Southall EJ, Humphries NE, Hays GC, Bradshaw CJA, et al. Scaling laws of marine predator search behaviour. Nature. 2008;451:1098–1102. doi: 10.1038/nature06518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Humphries NE, Queiroz N, Dyer JRM, Pade NG, Musyl MK, et al. Environmental context explains Lévy and Brownian movement patterns of marine predators. Nature. 2010;465:1066–1069. doi: 10.1038/nature09116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sims DW, Witt MJ, Richardson AJ, Southall EJ, Metcalfe JD. Encounter success of freeranging marine predator movements across a dynamics prey landscape. Proc R Soc B. 2006;273:1095–1201. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hays GC, Bastian T, Doyle TK, Fossette S, Gleiss AC, et al. High activity and Lévy searches: jellyfish can search the water column like fish. 2011. E-pub ahead of print. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 28.Zollner PA, Lima SL. Search strategies for landscape-level interpatch movements. Ecology. 1999;80:1019–1030. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bartumeus F, Catalan J, Viswanathan GM, Raposo EP, da Luz MGE. The role of turning angle distributions in animal search strategies. J Theor Bio. 2008;252:43–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Shlesinger MF. Search research. Nature. 2006;443:281–282. doi: 10.1038/443281a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Shlesinger MF. Random searching. J Phys A. 2009;42:434001. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Metzler R, Koren T, van den Broek B, Wuite GJL, Lomholt MA. And did he search for you, and could not find you? J Phys A. 2009;42:434005. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Viswanathan GM, Afanasyev V, Buldyrev SV, Murphy EJ, Prince PA, et al. Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses. Nature. 1996;381:413–415. doi: 10.1038/nature06199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Viswanathan GM, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, da Luz MGE, Raposo EP, et al. Optimizing the success of random searches. Nature. 1999;401:911–914. doi: 10.1038/44831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.da Luz MGE, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Raposo EP, Stanley HE, et al. Improvements in the statistical approach to random Lévy flight searches. Physica A. 2001;295:89–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Edwards AM, Phillips RA, Watkins NW, Freeman MP, Murphy EJ, et al. Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature. 2007;449:1044–1048. doi: 10.1038/nature06199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bartumeus F, Peters F, Pueyo S, Marrasé C, Catalan J. Helical Lévy walks: adjusting searching statistics to resource availability in microzooplankton. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:12771–12775. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2137243100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bartumeus F. Behavioral intermittence, Lévy patterns, and randomness in animal movement. Oikos. 2009;118:488–494. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bartumeus F, Fernandez P, da Luz MGE, Catalan J, Sole RV, et al. Superdiffusion and encounter rates in diluted, low dimensional worlds. Eur Phys J Special Topics. 2008;157:157–166. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bénichou O, Loverdo C, Moreau M, Voituriez R. Intermittent search strategies. Rev Mod Phys. 2011;83:81–129. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.80.031146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bénichou O, Coppey M, Moreau M, Suet P-H, Voituriez R. A stochastic model for intermittent search strategies. J Phys Cond Matter. 2005;17:S4275–S4286. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lomholt MA, Koren T, Metzler R, Klafter J. Lévy strategies in intermitent search processes are advantageous. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:11055–11059. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Reynolds AM. On the intermitent behavior of foraging animals. Europhys Lett. 2006;75:517–520. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Reynolds AM, Bartumeus F. Optimising the success of random destructive searches: Lévy walks can outperform ballistic motions. J Theor Biol. 2009;260:98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.05.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Plank MJ, James A. Optimal foraging: Lévy pattern or process? J R Soc Interface. 2008;5:1077–1086. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2008.0006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.James A, Planck MJ, Brown R. Optimizing the encounter rate in biological interactions: ballistic versus Lévy versus Brownian strategies. Phys Rev E. 2008;78:051128. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.78.051128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Benhamou S. How many animals really do the Lévy walk? Ecology. 2007;88:1962–1969. doi: 10.1890/06-1769.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Reynolds AM. Balancing the competing demands of harvesting and safety from predation: Lévy walk searches outperform composite Brownian walk searches but only when foraging under the risk of predation. Physica A. 2010;389:4740–4746. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lomholt MA, Ambjörnsson T, Metzler R. Optimal target search on a fast-folding polymer chain with volume exchange. Phys Rev Lett. 2005;95:260603. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.95.260603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Raposo EP, Buldyrev SV, da Luz MGE, Santos MC, Stanley HE, et al. Dynamical robustness of Lévy search strategies. Phys Rev Lett. 2003;91:240601. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.91.240601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Santos MC, Raposo EP, Viswanathan GM, da Luz MGE. Optimal random searches of revisitable targets: crossover from superdiffusive to ballistic random walks. Europhys Lett. 2004;67:734–740. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Koren T, Lomholt MA, Chechkin AV, Klafter J, Metzler R. Leapover lengths and first passage time statistics for Lévy flights. Phys Rev Lett. 2007;99:160602. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.99.160602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Johnson CJ, Parker KL, Heard DC. Foraging across a variable landscape: behavioral decisions made by woodland caribou at multiple spatial scales. Oecologia. 2001;127:590–602. doi: 10.1007/s004420000573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McIntyre NE, Wiens JA. Interactions between landscape structure and animal behavior: the roles of heterogeneously distributed resources and food deprivation on movement patterns. Landsc Ecol. 1999;14:437–447. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bartumeus F, Giuggioli L, Louzao M, Bretagnolle V, Oro D, et al. Fishery discards impact on seabird movement patterns at regional scales. Curr Bio. 2010;20:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kardar M. Statistical physics of particles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. 37 [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Kazakov AY, da Luz MGE, Raposo EP, et al. Average time spent by Lévy flights and walks on an interval with absorbing boundaries. Phys Rev E. 2001;64:041108. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.64.041108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Buldyrev SV, Gitterman M, Havlin S, Kazakov AY, da Luz MGE, et al. Properties of Lévy flights on an interval with absorbing boundaries. Physica A. 2001;302:148–161. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.64.041108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Mantegna RN, Stanley HE. Stochastic process with ultraslow convergence to a Gaussian: the truncated Lévy flight. Phys Rev Lett. 1994;73:2946–2949. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.73.2946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Byers JE. Effects of body size and resource availability on dispersal in a native and a nonnative estuarine snail. J Exp Mar Bio Ecol. 2000;248:133–150. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0981(00)00163-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sutherland WJ, Gill JA, Norris K. Density-dependent dispersal in animals: concepts, evidence, mechanisms, and consequences. In: Bullock JM, Kenward RE, Hails RS, editors. Dispersal ecology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing; 2002. pp. 129–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.South A. Extrapolating from individual movement behaviour to population spacing patterns in a ranging mammal. Ecol Model. 1999;117:343–360. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Borger L, Dalziel BD, Fryxell JM. Are there general mechanisms of animal home range behaviour? A review and prospects for future research. Ecol Lett. 2008;11:637–650. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01182.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Van Houtan KS, Pimm SL, Halley JM, Bierregaard RO, Jr, Lovejoy TE. Dispersal of Amazonian birds in continuous and fragmented forest. Ecol Lett. 2007;10:219–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2007.01004.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Preston MD, Pitchford JW, Wood AJ. Evolutionary optimality in stochastic search problems. J R Soc Interface. 2010;7:1301–1310. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2010.0090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Pasternak Z, Bartumeus F, Grasso FW. Lévy-taxis: a novel search strategy for finding odor plumes in turbulent flow-dominated environments. J Phys A. 2009;42:434010. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Makris NC, Ratilal P, Symonds DT, Jagannathan S, Lee S, et al. Fish population and behavior revealed by instantaneous continental shelf-scale imaging. Science. 2006;311:660–663. doi: 10.1126/science.1121756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wei Y, Wang X, Liu J, Nememan I, Singh AH, et al. The population dynamics of bacteria in physically structured habitats and the adaptive virtue of random motility. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:4047–4052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013499108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Giuffre C, Hinow P, Vogel R, Ahmed T, Stocker R, et al. The ciliate Paramecium shows higher motility in non-uniform chemical landscapes. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e15274. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Seymour JR, Simó R, Ahmed T, Stocker R. Chemoattraction to dimethylsulfoniopropionate throughout the marine microbial food web. Science. 2010;329:342–345. doi: 10.1126/science.1188418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Mashanova A, Oliver TH, Jansen VA. Evidence for intermittency and a truncated power law from highly resolved aphid movement data. J R Soc Interface. 2009;7:199–208. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES