Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Intellect Disabil Res. 2011 Sep 15;56(5):441–452. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01484.x

Self-injurious behaviour in intellectual disability syndromes: evidence for aberrant pain signalling as a contributing factor

K A Peebles 1, T J Price 1
PMCID: PMC3272540  NIHMSID: NIHMS333800  PMID: 21917053

Abstract

Background

In most individuals, injury results in activation of peripheral nociceptors (pain-sensing neurons of the peripheral nervous system) and amplification of central nervous system (CNS) pain pathways that serve as a disincentive to continue harmful behaviour; however, this may not be the case in some developmental disorders that cause intellectual disability (ID). Moreover, individuals affected by ID disorders may initiate self-injurious behaviour to address irritating or painful sensations. In normal individuals, a negative feedback loop decreases sensation of pain, which involves descending inhibitory neurons in the CNS that attenuate spinal nociceptive processing. If spinal nociceptive signalling is impaired in these developmental disorders, an exaggerated painful stimulus may be required in order to engage descending anti-nociceptive signals.

Methods

Using electronic databases, we conducted a review of publications regarding the incidence of chronic pain or altered pain sensation in ID patients or corresponding preclinical models.

Results

There is a body of evidence indicating that individuals with fragile X mental retardation and/or Rett syndrome have altered pain sensation. These findings in humans are supported by mechanistic studies using genetically modified mice harbouring mutations consistent with the human disease. Thus, once self-injurious behaviour is initiated, the signal to stop may be missing. Several developmental disorders that cause ID are associated with increased incidence of gastroesophageal reflux disease (GERD), which can cause severe visceral pain. Individuals affected by these disorders who also have GERD may self-injure as a mechanism to engage descending inhibitory circuits to quell visceral pain. In keeping with this hypothesis, pharmacological treatment of GERD has been shown to be effective for reducing self-injurious behaviour in some patients. Hence, multiple lines of evidence suggest aberrant nociceptive processing in developmental disorders that cause ID.

Conclusions

There is evidence that pain pathways and pain amplification mechanisms are altered in several preclinical models of developmental disorders that cause ID. We present hypotheses regarding how impaired pain pathways or chronic pain might contribute to self-injurious behaviour. Studies evaluating the relationship between pain and self-injurious behaviour will provide better understanding of the mechanisms underlying self-injurious behaviour in the ID population and may lead to more effective treatments.

Keywords: central sensitization, diffuse noxious inhibitory control, fragile X, pain, Rett syndrome, self-injury


Despite the prevalence of self-injurious behavior (SIB) in many genetic developmental disorders associated with severe intellectual impairment, very little is known about the neurobiological underpinnings of this comorbidity. SIB occurs in different sectors of the normal population, but its frequency is much higher among individuals with developmental disorders that negatively influence brain function. Several recent advances in preclinical models of such disorders have led to a greater understanding of how mutations in genes that cause these diseases lead to changes in the structure and function of the central nervous system (CNS). At the same time, a greater appreciation of plasticity in the CNS as it pertains to chronic pain conditions has led to the recognition that molecular mechanisms of pain amplification are remarkably similar to those of learning and memory ( Ji et al. 2003). Here we will summarise recent evidence for aberrant pain processing in humans affected by developmental disorders that cause intellectual disability (ID) and in preclinical models of these diseases. We will place these findings into the context of what is currently known about the initiation and persistence of SIB and discuss possible molecular mechanisms and anatomic pathways that may be disrupted in these diseases. We will outline an experimental framework for further work in this area to test the role of defective pain amplification in SIB that can potentially point to new therapeutic avenues. We will also discuss chronic pain as a potential contributing factor in SIB, which is a complex behaviour. Understanding the role of the nociceptive system in SIB may improve therapeutic outcomes.

Amplification is a key feature of pain pathways

Pain is a cortically dependent sensory experience with an affective component arising from activation of peripheral pain-sensing neurons (nociceptors). In a healthy individual, experiencing pain is important for survival. In the acute setting, activation of nociceptors and CNS neurons that receive input from these peripheral neurons elicits a withdrawal reflex from noxious insults to avoid further tissue damage. Nociceptors signal to supraspinal CNS structures involved in fear, emotion, and other components of avoidance in order to reduce the likelihood of subsequent dangerous encounters. Inflammation of a wound sensitises the surrounding area and encourages guarding of the affected tissue to allow healing. This last example involves pain amplification in which pain thresholds are decreased through multiple mechanisms. Pain amplification is part of the healthy self-protective response to noxious insult, but can be dysregulated in multiple pathologies (Woolf 2010).

Ronald Melzack and Patrick Wall were among the first to recognise that transmission of pain information from primary nociceptor to higher CNS structures was not always faithful and that a network of neurons in the spinal dorsal horn was responsible for gating pain information (Melzack & Wall 1965). We now have a much more advanced understanding of the anatomy and physiology underlying transmission of pain signals to the CNS and factors that influence the severity of perceived pain. The primary nociceptor is a single cell with an axon that extends from a target tissue to the dorsal horn of the spinal cord where it forms a synapse with a second-order neuron. The pain signal can then be transmitted directly from the second-order neuron to higher centres or be filtered by additional spinal neurons before projecting to the brainstem and brain (Melzack & Wall 1965). The pain signal is transmitted to multiple CNS structures that exert pronounced control over pain signalling by sending descending projections that inhibit or amplify the excitability of neurons at earlier stages in the pathway (Ossipov et al. 2010). Thus, cellular feedback loops filter pain signalling. Pain amplification can occur at multiple stages in this pathway: nociceptor firing threshold can be decreased, synaptic efficiency increased (Woolf & Salter 2000), or activity of inhibitory or amplifying descending neurons modified (Ossipov et al. 2010). While an exhaustive description of mechanisms responsible for pain amplification is beyond the scope of this review, we will explain two mechanisms that are dysregulated in mouse models of ID: wind-up and long-term potentiation (LTP), which represent two well defined neurophysiological events involved in sensitization of CNS pain pathways – collectively called central sensitisation (Latremoliere & Woolf 2009; Woolf 2010).

Wind-up

When a noxious stimulus, such as tissue damage, is applied, it causes an initial stinging or sharp pain with a short latency (called ‘first pain’) and is followed by a more persistent pain that commonly possesses a burning quality. This so-called ‘second pain’ has a longer latency and is thought to be associated with wind-up of dorsal horn neurons (Price 1972; Price et al. 1977). Wind-up involves a progressive increase in action potential frequency in second-order spinal dorsal horn neurons with repetitive firing of peripheral nociceptors. This takes less than 1 s to begin and can be observed in most dorsal horn neurons that receive a nociceptive input. While the molecular mechanisms of windup are complex, its basic mechanisms involve glutamatergic neurotransmission and postsynaptic glutamate receptors of the N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) type. Existing evidence points to progressive depolarisation through NMDA channels as a primary means through which frequency-dependent amplification of dorsal horn neuron firing is augmented (Dickenson & Sullivan 1987). In addition to increased input–output firing of dorsal horn neurons, wind-up can also lead to after-discharge in these neurons (continued firing despite the absence of continued input). Wind-up is commonly viewed as a primary mechanism for short-term plasticity in the nociceptive system because it takes place over such a short time-course (Herrero et al. 2000).

Long-term potentiation

Unlike wind-up, LTP involves an increase in synaptic efficacy that has a longer latency to onset and can persist for days to weeks and may even be permanent. As such, most work on LTP has focused on LTP as a mechanism of learning and memory. Several lines of evidence suggest that LTP occurs during learning and memory (Whitlock et al. 2006) and inhibition of molecular maintenance mechanisms of LTP reverse established memories (Pastalkova et al. 2006; Shema et al. 2007). Moreover, LTP is impaired in preclinical models of Rett (Moretti et al. 2006) and Fragile X mental retardation (FxS) (Zhao et al. 2005; Wilson & Cox 2007) syndromes. In terms of pain signalling, LTP has recently been recognised as an important synaptic amplifier mechanism in the dorsal horn (Ikeda et al. 2006; Sandkuhler 2007). While LTP can be induced in dorsal horn neurons by artificial high-frequency stimulation of nociceptors, it can also be observed after natural stimulation that mimics persistent inflammation and/or injury to the peripheral nervous system (Ikeda et al. 2006) pointing to the physiological importance of this type of plasticity in chronic pain states. While the ability of LTP to explain the full sequelae of chronic pain symptoms is still controversial (Latremoliere & Woolf 2010; Sandkuhler 2010), it is, nevertheless, a critical amplification mechanism for pain pathways that leads to enhanced pain perception in human subjects (Klein et al. 2004; Lang et al. 2007).

The significance of these mechanisms for organism fitness is reflected in their evolutionary conservation. Several studies have demonstrated that wind-up can be observed across divergent vertebrate species (Herrero et al. 2000), indicating that it is an evolutionarily conserved mechanism for amplifying incoming pain signals. Similarly, an LTP-like mechanism, termed long-term facilitation, can be observed in sea snails and shares molecular mechanisms with mammalian LTP (Martin et al. 1997). While this neurophysiological event has been extensively studied as a primitive learning and memory mechanism, it is evoked by a painful stimulus and its behavioural endpoint is a facilitation of a withdrawal reflex. Hence, pain amplification mechanisms are evolutionarily conserved across the animal kingdom in both their short- and longer-term forms. As pain is ultimately a teaching signal for limiting the extent of injury and promoting protection of an injured site (Woolf 2010), these amplification mechanisms serve an important evolutionary purpose in protecting the individual.

Evidence for altered pain sensation and/or pain amplification in developmental disorders where self-injurious behaviour is observed

While the status of wind-up and LTP has not been examined in ID patients that engage in SIB, we anticipate that sensitisation due to wind-up and/or LTP would strongly discourage SIB. Moreover, it is formally possible that deficits in overall pain perception might allow increased incidence or persistence of SIB in the ID population. Here we will review evidence for changes in global pain responses and/or amplification of pain signalling in two genetic disorders where SIB is commonly observed, Rett syndrome (Sansom et al. 1993; Matson et al. 2008) and FxS (Symons et al. 2003; Hall et al. 2008).

Rett syndrome

Rett syndrome is an X-linked disorder caused by mutation in the methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 (MeCP2) gene (Van den Veyver & Zoghbi 2000). MeCP2 is a DNA binding protein that modifies the transcription of multiple gene targets. One significant gene target for MeCP2 is the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. Loss of MeCP2 results in decreased BDNF gene expression and this is associated with several neurological deficits in the disease. Interestingly, overexpression of the BDNF gene in a mouse model of the disease ameliorates several key deficits, while complete loss of BDNF and MeCP2 leads to enhanced disease progression (Chang et al. 2006). BDNF plays a key role in neuronal development and is an important factor for synaptic plasticity at adult synapses (e.g. LTP). As such, deficits in LTP are found in the mouse model of Rett syndrome (Moretti et al. 2006). Moreover, MeCP2 has been found to play a key role in pain plasticity (Géranton et al. 2007, 2008). In a preclinical model of inflammation, enhanced nociception was associated with increased phosphorylation of MeCP2. This MeCP2 phosphorylation correlated with derepression of several established gene targets suppressed by MeCP2 and these gene targets were further shown to play a causative role in the onset of inflammatory pain in this preclinical model. Although pain amplification mechanisms have not been examined in mice lacking the MeCP2 gene, mice harbouring a transgene that leads to a 50% reduction in MeCP2 expression do show deficits in acute thermal pain thresholds (Samaco et al. 2008). These preclinical investigations led to a close examination of potential decreases in pain sensitivity in humans with Rett syndrome. A large survey of families enrolled in the Australian Rett Syndrome Database found that decreased pain sensitivity, as measured by parental recall, was a common feature of Rett syndrome, suggesting that these preclinical findings may indicate a disruption in pain signalling in the human population (Downs et al. 2010). Further studies to assess pain thresholds will be required in this population. The potential intersection of decreased pain signalling and/or amplification with SIB in this disease has not been examined.

Fragile X mental retardation

Fragile X mental retardation is caused by silencing of the fragile X mental retardation gene (Fmr1). This gene encodes a protein, fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP), which plays a multifunctional role in protein synthesis and neuronal development (Bagni & Greenough 2005). FMRP binds to mRNAs and is involved in repressing their translation while transporting them to distal sites in cells. In neurons, upon intense synaptic stimulation, FMRP is thought to dissociate from its target mRNA leading to a derepression of translation (Bassell & Warren 2008). Synaptic synthesis of new proteins plays a key role in initiation and maintenance of plasticity and all evidence indicates that FMRP plays a crucial role in this process (Bassell & Warren 2008). Two forms of synaptic plasticity are altered in several brain regions in the Fmr1-knockout mouse, a model of FxS: long-term depression is enhanced (Bear et al. 2004) and LTP is absent in some, but not all, brain regions (Li et al. 2002; Larson et al. 2005; Wilson & Cox 2007; Hu et al. 2008). The long-standing existence of this mouse model for FxS (The Dutch-Belgian Fragile X Consortium 1994), coupled with interest in the role of translation regulation in synaptic plasticity (Kelleher et al. 2004) and the high prevalence of FxS (Turner et al. 1996), has led to an extraordinarily in-depth understanding of the role of FMRP in synaptic plasticity.

One of us, TJP, conducted a detailed study of altered nociception in the mouse model of FxS (Price et al. 2007). While these mice had normal acute pain thresholds to mechanical and thermal stimuli, they showed a marked reduction in assays designed to assess amplification in pain pathways. One of the strongest phenotypes of the FMRP-knockout mouse was a lack of wind-up in dorsal horn neurons. As mentioned above, wind-up is a major mechanism of short-term plasticity in pain pathways and its near absence in this mouse model suggests that a severe deficit in spinal pain processing is present in this disease. In fact, not only did we observe a lack of wind-up in these mice, but we also noted wind-down in several preparations, indicating that there may be a loss of synaptic responsiveness with repetitive stimulation of nociceptors in the absence of FMRP. In addition to this deficit in wind-up, longer-term forms of pain plasticity were also lacking in these mice. These included a long delay (3 weeks in Fmr1-knockout mice vs. immediate sensitisation in wild-type animals) in the development of neuropathic pain after peripheral nerve injury and a lack of metabotropic glutamate receptor 1/5 (mGluR1/5)-mediated nociceptive sensitisation. Because FMRP is strongly expressed in neurons of the pain pathway (Price et al. 2006) and translation regulation is known to play a key role in pain amplification processes (Price & Geranton 2009; Melemedjian et al. 2010; Asiedu et al. 2011), these findings are likely directly linked to FMRP-mediated control of nociceptive plasticity and not associated with other behavioural deficits present in this mouse model of the human disorder.

In contrast to Rett syndrome, studies directly assessing pain thresholds in FxS have not been conducted. Based on studies in the preclinical model, we would not anticipate changes in acute pain thresholds in FxS, but rather that pain amplification would be impaired. In keeping with this hypothesis, the fragile X premutation tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS) does provide some interesting potential insight into this hypothesis. FXTAS, unlike the full mutation that leads to FxS (Hagerman & Hagerman 2002), does not repress FMRP expression but rather leads to an increase in FMRP mRNA expression (Hessl et al. 2005). Humans with FXTAS frequently develop peripheral neuropathies that have a high frequency of associated pain (Berry-Kravis et al. 2007; Brega et al. 2009). Moreover, the incidence of the functional pain amplification disorder, fibromyalgia, is significantly increased in patients with FXTAS (Coffey et al. 2008). Taken together, the preclinical and clinical evidence strongly suggests a major role for the Fmr1 gene in pain amplification.

From preclinical evidence to self-injurious behaviour

While the available evidence clearly points to deficits in either acute pain perception or pain amplification in the absence of MeCP2 or FMRP, a link between these proteins, their disorders and SIB has yet to be established. One limitation is that mouse models of Rett syndrome and FxS do not demonstrate SIB or even repetitive grooming behaviours which may be interpreted as SIB in mice (Silverman et al. 2010). On the other hand, we feel that the preclinical evidence detailed above creates a useful framework through which testable hypotheses can be made. It is feasible that decreased pain thresholds and/or a lack of amplification underlie the persistence of SIB. In such a model, initiation of nociceptor discharge by SIB would be expected to lead to both wind-up and LTP in pain pathways based on the intensity and duration of SIB bouts (Newell et al. 2002a). This would lead to an amplification of pain signalling initiating a protective reflex, an evolutionarily conserved process that would be self-limiting. In contrast, in the absence of such amplification mechanisms, as is likely in Rett syndrome and demonstrated in a preclinical model of FxS, these protective reflex mechanisms might fail to engage leading to the persistence of SIB either within bout or as a consistent stereotypy. While this model would have to be tested in human subjects, there are several methods that have been employed in other settings that could be utilised. LTP can be induced in human pain pathways through stimulation of dermatomes with electrical current. This procedure leads to brief pain and a longer lasting primary hyperalgesia and secondary allodynia, on the order of 30 min (Klein et al. 2004, 2008; Lang et al. 2007). This procedure, coupled with the advent of a validated quantitative sensory testing score for non-verbal adults with neurodevelopmental disorders (Symons et al. 2010), could be utilised to test for an absence of perceptual correlates of LTP in Rett syndrome and FxS. Another potential approach could involve functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The LTP protocol mentioned above would be expected to stimulate activity in the so-called pain axis (Apkarian et al. 2005) in control subjects, while activation of these brain centres in Rett syndrome or FxS would be attenuated. Less invasive procedures, such as a brief noxious pinch or wind-up protocols, may be capable of revealing major differences in engagement of brain centres involved in pain processing in these syndromes. Furthermore, the utilisation of these techniques to compare individuals with these or similar disorders that self-injure versus those that do not may reveal additional differences in pain processing within syndrome populations that would be informative in understanding the potential connection between decreased pain amplification mechanisms and SIB. Finally, and most simply, SIB should lead to the presence of both hyperalgesia and secondary allodynia directly following its termination if pain amplification pathways are intact. We would hypothesise that such effects would not be observed in individuals with Rett syndrome, FxS or other similar disorders. This hypothesis is immediately testable in many observational settings through the use of non-verbal pain inventories and quantitative sensory testing (Symons et al. 2010). While we do not deny that these approaches may present major technical (fMRI) and complex ethical hurdles, gaining a further understanding of the role of pain amplification deficits in SIB may lead to new treatment approaches and a better grasp on the neurobiological underpinnings of the problem.

There is ongoing discussion in the ID community regarding reduced pain sensation and ID patients. Several studies have indicated that baseline pain thresholds are normal in ID patients (Hennequin et al. 2000; Defrin et al. 2004). To the best of our knowledge, all studies in the ID population address baseline pain thresholds and not pain amplification. A key aspect of the proposal discussed herein is that pain amplification may be dysregulated in certain ID patients, even if baseline pain thresholds are normal. In addition, very few published reports on pain sensation in ID patients specify the type of ID under investigation. Differences in pain sensation among ID syndromes could explain conflicting interpretations in the literature. For example, it is possible that patients with Rett syndrome have reduced pain sensation as observed in the Australian study (Downs et al. 2010), while those with Down syndrome have normal pain thresholds, as demonstrated by Defrin et al. (2004). It is important to note that the Australian study was a review of parental recall regarding pain thresholds in Rett patients (Downs et al. 2010), while the study by Defrin et al. employed formal sensory threshold testing (Defrin et al. 2004); however, including ID type in future publications assessing pain and SIB would shed light on conflicting data from studies that have pooled all patients with severe ID into a single group. Also, we wish to acknowledge the risk in speculating about capacity to experience pain in this sensitive population that often cannot adequately express their needs or may have cortical deficits that fundamentally change the pain experience. Any clinical study addressing pain amplification should be thoroughly reviewed for ethical integrity and measures should be standardised to avoid a priori bias of investigators. Investigators must take great care in interpreting and reporting their data because many caregivers believe that children with severe ID do not experience pain normally and these beliefs may influence the quality of care provided (Breau et al. 2003b). It is possible that pain signalling is defective in a subset of ID patients and identifying those individuals most likely to have altered pain processing may allow for tailored treatment for SIB.

Self-injury as a coping mechanism for pain in patients with intellectual disability

Several groups have proposed that SIB may serve as a coping mechanism for severe pain because self-injury should activate descending inhibitory mechanisms in the pain pathway (Bosch et al. 1997; Breau et al. 2003a; Symons & Danov 2005). In a cohort of ID children with and without various types of chronic pain and exhibiting varying degrees of SIB, Breau et al. (2003a) found children with chronic pain tended to engage in SIB less frequently, injure a smaller body area and target the site where pain originated, while those patients without identifiable pain who engaged in SIB tended to self-injure more frequently and in a diffuse pattern. These findings raise the possibility that there are qualitatively different forms of SIB initiated and propagated for different reasons (Breau et al. 2003a). Compulsive SIB directed towards painful areas also can occur in children (Symons & Danov 2005) and adults (Mailis 1996) with intact mental status suffering from neuropathic pain and treatment of the underlying pain condition resolves SIB in many cases (Mailis 1996). Thus, the occurrence of SIB as a coping mechanism for chronic pain may not be limited to the ID population. Multiple factors contribute to initiation, nature and perpetuation of SIB, thus coping with pain should be considered as a motive on an individual basis. It may be beneficial to include type of ID syndrome and nature of self-injury (e.g. diffuse vs. targeted and which sites) in future studies to identify factors indicative of SIB as a coping mechanism for pain.

Diffuse noxious inhibitory control

In cases where SIB is associated with chronic pain, self-injury may be working in a similar fashion to the counter-irritation principle as proposed by several groups in the 1980s (Le Bars et al. 1979a; Pertovaara et al. 1982; Willer et al. 1982). Based on the intensity and duration of self-injury exhibited by ID patients (Newell et al. 2002b), this behavior should engage reflexive diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC). DNIC was first described as an experimental paradigm in which a painful conditioning stimulus increases hetero- or homotypic pain thresholds in distal locations in rats (Le Bars et al. 1979a) and humans (Willer et al. 1984) as measured by withdrawal reflexes. In the healthy population, DNIC is activated by noxious stimuli very close to the detection threshold. However, if pain processing is attenuated in ID patients, the stimulus required to engage DNIC may be much higher than what is observed in normal individuals. DNIC is a result of inhibitory signalling derived from the brain and brainstem because spinalised animals [animals with brain/spinal cord connections disrupted (Le Bars et al. 1979b)] and humans with lesions that interrupt communication between higher structures and the spinal cord (Roby-Brami et al. 1987; De Broucker et al. 1990) do not exhibit altered reflex withdrawal thresholds under conditions that stimulate DNIC in intact animals or individuals (Le Bars et al. 1979a). DNIC depends in part upon the endogenous opioid system as a low dose of the µ-opioid receptor antagonist naloxone that does not affect reflexes in the absence of conditioning stimulus (Boureau et al. 1978; Willer et al. 1990) completely blocks DNIC in rats (Le Bars et al. 1981) and humans (Willer et al. 1990). A meta-analysis has indicated that the µ-opioid receptor antagonist naltrexone improves SIB in 50–80% of ID patients (Symons et al. 2004). This study supports the hypothesis that engaging the endogenous opioid system is a motivating factor for some ID patients, consistent with a scenario in which ID patients activate DNIC through SIB to cope with pain.

Gastroesophageal reflux disease

Gastroesophageal reflux disease (GERD) is one of the best studied causes for SIB in ID patients with chronic pain. GERD causes severe visceral pain and occurs with greater frequency among children with different types of severe ID compared to mentally healthy children (Sondheimer & Morris 1979; Cates et al. 1989; Spitz et al. 1993; Motil et al. 1999; Gössler et al. 2007). Multiple studies have linked the presence of GERD with SIB in ID children and in some cases GERD treatment reduced the incidence of SIB (Bosch et al. 1997; Luzzani et al. 2003). Swender et al. were among the first to specifically test the a priori hypothesis that GERD may be causally related to handmouthing in Rett patients. This group also identified a correlation between GERD and SIB in ID children; however, they found that GERD treatment did not alter behaviour and concluded that SIB may take on secondary functions in this population (Swender et al. 2006). Nevertheless, pharmacological intervention to alleviate pain due to GERD may be an important component of treatment of SIB, and GERD status should be assessed in susceptible patients.

Mechanistic work (Mittal et al. 1995; Young et al. 2007) has revealed a promising pharmacological target for GERD treatment. GERD is caused by transient relaxation of the lower oesophageal sphincter (LES) due to poor neural control (Holloway & Dent 1990; Dent 1998). The vagus nerve senses stomach distension after eating, triggering LES relaxation (Mittal et al. 1995). Inhibiting the metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) decreases the sensitivity of vagal mechanoreceptors reducing frequency of LES relaxations (Young et al. 2007). Treatment with the mGluR5 antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine (MPEP) reduces transient LES relaxations by up to 90% in animal models (Frisby et al. 2005; Jensen et al. 2005) and the negative allosteric modulator of mGluR5, ADX10059 has demonstrated efficacy for GERD treatment in phase I (Keywood et al. 2009; Zerbib et al. 2010) and phase II (Zerbib et al. 2011) clinical trials in otherwise healthy individuals. Excessive mGluR5 signalling is implicated in the pathology of FxS and the inhibitors under investigation in clinical studies for FxS have been well tolerated in this population (Berry-Kravis et al. 2009; Jacquemont et al. 2011). These trials were not designed to test the effect of mGluR5 inhibition on GERD-associated visceral pain and SIB. Including these measures as primary endpoints in subsequent trials will provide needed evidence to test the hypothesis that SIB might be managed through better GERD control in the ID population.

Ongoing pain should be considered as a potential driving factor for SIB in ID patients, but further work is required to assess the relationship between self-injury, chronic pain and DNIC. Discovery of mechanisms linking self-injury to pain and DNIC may reveal additional pharmacological targets, similar to the mGluR5 inhibitors mentioned above. It is worth noting that pain treatment would be useful only in the subset of ID patients that initiate self-injury in response to pain, and that each patient should be evaluated individually for the contribution of pain to SIB. Furthermore, if SIB is initiated due to pain but adopts secondary functions, pain management may improve behaviour, but resolution of SIB will likely require a multipronged approach. Improved methodologies for pain assessment in children with severe ID (Symons et al. 2010) can facilitate the study of the relationship between pain and self-injury. Further clinical studies addressing the role of pain in self-injury initiation and persistence should lead to improved strategies for controlling SIB.

Concluding remarks

Pain and pain amplification are evolutionarily conserved phenomena critical for survival but can be dysregulated in some disease states (Woolf 2010). The extent of pain experienced by an individual depends upon spinal gating (Melzack & Wall 1965; Woolf & Salter 2000), which is in turn regulated by higher centres in the brainstem and brain (Ossipov et al. 2010). Preclinical and some clinical studies indicate that pain amplification may be diminished in certain syndromes that cause severe ID such as FxS (Price et al. 2007) and Rett syndrome (Downs et al. 2010). There is conflicting evidence regarding pain thresholds in the ID population with Rett patients appearing to have decreased sensitivity, albeit as assessed by parental recall and requiring further threshold testing (Downs et al. 2010) and Down syndrome patients having normal or possibly enhanced pain thresholds (Defrin et al. 2004). This could reflect differences in pain processing among ID syndromes as individual molecular defects leading to clinical features of each disease may impact the nociceptive system differently. Furthermore, we are unaware of any published clinical studies evaluating aberrant pain amplification in ID patients. Baseline thresholds may remain normal in certain ID patients, but without effective pain amplification, pain due to self-injury may not be sufficient to deter SIB. Studies evaluating pain amplification in ID patients have the potential to reveal the extent to which pain amplification may be dysregulated in ID patients and linking altered pain processing to SIB may reveal new pharmacological targets for this difficult behaviour (e.g. mGluR5 antagonists). Some existing clinical data support the hypothesis that SIB may be a coping mechanism for certain ID patients dealing with chronic pain. In this paradigm, self-injury activates DNIC, a centrally mediated, opioid-dependent dampening of spinal pain processing that decreases the severity of pain sensation. Identifying ID patients suffering from chronic pain and incorporating pain management into their treatment may improve therapeutic outcomes for SIB. Evaluating pain amplification and/or processing as well as chronic pain status in clinical studies of ID patients may provide insight into mechanisms driving SIB in this population.

References

  1. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. European Journal of Pain. 2005;9:463–484. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asiedu MN, Tillu DV, Melemedjian OK, Shy A, Sanoja R, Bodell B, et al. Spinal protein kinase M {zeta} underlies the maintenance mechanism of persistent nociceptive sensitization. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:6646–6653. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6286-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bagni C, Greenough WT. From mRNP trafficking to spine dysmorphogenesis: the roots of fragile X syndrome. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2005;6:376–387. doi: 10.1038/nrn1667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bassell GJ, Warren ST. Fragile X syndrome: loss of local mRNA regulation alters synaptic development and function. Neuron. 2008;60:201–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bear MF, Huber KM, Warren ST. The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27:370–377. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berry-Kravis E, Goetz CG, Leehey MA, Hagerman RJ, Zhang L, Li L, et al. Neuropathic features in fragile X premutation carriers. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 2007;143:19–26. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berry-Kravis E, Hessl D, Coffey S, Hervey C, Schneider A, Yuhas J, et al. A pilot open label, single dose trial of fenobam in adults with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics. 2009;46:266–271. doi: 10.1136/jmg.2008.063701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bosch J, Van Dyke C, Smith SM, Poulton S. Role of medical conditions in the exacerbation of self-injurious behavior: an exploratory study. Mental Retardation. 1997;35:124–130. doi: 10.1352/0047-6765(1997)035<0124:ROMCIT>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boureau F, Willer JC, Dauthier C. Study of naloxone in normal awake man: effects on spinal reflexes. Neuropharmacology. 1978;17:565–568. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(78)90149-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Breau LM, Camfield CS, Symons FJ, Bodfish JW, Mackay A, Finley GA, et al. Relation between pain and self-injurious behavior in nonverbal children with severe cognitive impairments. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2003a;142:498–503. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2003.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Breau LM, MacLaren J, McGrath PJ, Camfield CS, Finley GA. Caregivers’ beliefs regarding pain in children with cognitive impairment: relation between pain sensation and reaction increases with severity of impairment. The Clinical Journal of Pain. 2003b;19:335–344. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200311000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brega AG, Reynolds A, Bennett RE, Leehey MA, Bounds LS, Cogswell JB, et al. Functional status of men with the fragile X premutation, with and without the tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS) International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;24:1101–1109. doi: 10.1002/gps.2231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cates M, Billmire DF, Bull MJ, Grosfeld JL. Gastroesophageal dysfunction in Cornelia de Lange syndrome. Journal of Pediatric Surgery. 1989;24:248–250. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3468(89)80004-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chang Q, Khare G, Dani V, Nelson S, Jaenisch R. The disease progression of Mecp2 mutant mice is affected by the level of BDNF expression. Neuron. 2006;49:341–348. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coffey SM, Cook K, Tartaglia N, Tassone F, Nguyen DV, Pan R, et al. Expanded clinical phenotype of women with the FMR1 premutation. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 2008;146A:1009–1016. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. De Broucker T, Cesaro P, Willer JC, Le Bars D. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in man. Involvement of the spinoreticular tract. Brain. 1990;113(Pt 4):1223–1234. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.4.1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Defrin R, Pick CG, Peretz C, Carmeli E. A quantitative somatosensory testing of pain threshold in individuals with mental retardation. Pain. 2004;108:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dent J. Gastro-oesophageal reflux disease. Digestion. 1998;59:433–445. doi: 10.1159/000007521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dickenson AH, Sullivan AF. Evidence for a role of the NMDA receptor in the frequency dependent potentiation of deep rat dorsal horn nociceptive neurones following C fibre stimulation. Neuropharmacology. 1987;26:1235–1238. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(87)90275-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Downs J, Geranton SM, Bebbington A, Jacoby P, Bahi-Buisson N, Ravine D, et al. Linking MECP2 and pain sensitivity: the example of Rett syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 2010;152A:1197–1205. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.33314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Frisby CL, Mattsson JP, Jensen JM, Lehmann A, Dent J, Blackshaw LA. Inhibition of transient lower esophageal sphincter relaxation and gastroesophageal reflux by metabotropic glutamate receptor ligands. Gastroenterology. 2005;129:995–1004. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2005.06.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Géranton SM, Morenilla-Palao C, Hunt SP. A role for transcriptional repressor methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 and plasticity-related gene serum- and glucocorticoid-inducible kinase 1 in the induction of inflammatory pain states. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:6163–6173. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1306-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Géranton SM, Fratto V, Tochiki KK, Hunt SP. Descending serotonergic controls regulate inflammation-induced mechanical sensitivity and methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 phosphorylation in the rat superficial dorsal horn. Molecular Pain. 2008;4:35. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-4-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gössler A, Schalamon J, Huber-Zeyringer A, Höllwarth ME. Gastroesophageal reflux and behavior in neurologically impaired children. Journal of Pediatric Surgery. 2007;42:1486–1490. doi: 10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2007.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hagerman RJ, Hagerman PJ. The fragile X premutation: into the phenotypic fold. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 2002;12:278–283. doi: 10.1016/s0959-437x(02)00299-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL. Compulsive, self-injurious, and autistic behavior in children and adolescents with fragile x syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation. 2008;113:44–53. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[44:CSAABI]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hennequin M, Morin C, Feine JS. Pain expression and stimulus localisation in individuals with Down’s syndrome. Lancet. 2000;356:1882–1887. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(00)03259-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Herrero JF, Laird JM, Lopez-Garcia JA. Wind-up of spinal cord neurones and pain sensation: much ado about something? Progress in Neurobiology. 2000;61:169–203. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(99)00051-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hessl D, Tassone F, Loesch DZ, Berry-Kravis E, Leehey MA, Gane LW, et al. Abnormal elevation of FMR1 mRNA is associated with psychological symptoms in individuals with the fragile X premutation. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2005;139:115–121. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Holloway RH, Dent J. Pathophysiology of gastroesophageal reflux. Lower esophageal sphincter dysfunction in gastroesophageal reflux disease. Gastroenterology Clinics of North America. 1990;19:517–535. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hu H, Qin Y, Bochorishvili G, Zhu Y, van Aelst L, Zhu JJ. Ras signaling mechanisms underlying impaired GluR1-dependent plasticity associated with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7847–7862. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1496-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ikeda H, Stark J, Fischer H, Wagner M, Drdla R, Jager T, et al. Synaptic amplifier of inflammatory pain in the spinal dorsal horn. Science. 2006;312:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.1127233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jacquemont S, Curie A, des Portes V, Torrioli MG, Berry-Kravis E, Hagerman RJ, et al. Epigenetic modification of the FMR1 gene in fragile X syndrome is associated with differential response to the mGluR5 antagonist AFQ056. Science Translational Medicine. 2011;3:64ra1. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jensen J, Lehmann A, Uvebrant A, Carlsson A, Jerndal G, Nilsson K, et al. Transient lower esophageal sphincter relaxations in dogs are inhibited by a metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 antagonist. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2005;519:154–157. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ji RR, Kohno T, Moore KA, Woolf CJ. Central sensitization and LTP: do pain and memory share similar mechanisms? Trends in Neurosciences. 2003;26:696–705. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kelleher RJ, 3rd, Govindarajan A, Tonegawa S. Translational regulatory mechanisms in persistent forms of synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2004;44:59–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Keywood C, Wakefield M, Tack J. A proof-of-concept study evaluating the effect of ADX10059, a metabotropic glutamate receptor-5 negative allosteric modulator, on acid exposure and symptoms in gastrooesophageal reflux disease. Gut. 2009;58:1192–1199. doi: 10.1136/gut.2008.162040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klein T, Magerl W, Hopf H-C, Sandkühler J, Treede R-D. Perceptual correlates of nociceptive long-term potentiation and long-term depression in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:964–971. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1222-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Klein T, Stahn S, Magerl W, Treede R. The role of heterosynaptic facilitation in long-term potentiation (LTP) of human pain sensation. Pain. 2008;139:507–519. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lang S, Klein T, Magerl W, Treede R-D. Modality-specific sensory changes in humans after the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in cutaneous nociceptive pathways. Pain. 2007;128:254–263. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Larson J, Jessen RE, Kim D, Fine AK, du Hoffmann J. Age-dependent and selective impairment of long-term potentiation in the anterior piriform cortex of mice lacking the fragile X mental retardation protein. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9460–9469. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2638-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Latremoliere A, Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. The Journal of Pain. 2009;10:895–926. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Latremoliere A, Woolf CJ. Synaptic plasticity and central sensitization: author reply. The Journal of Pain. 2010;11:801–803. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC)I. Effects on dorsal horn convergent neurones in the rat. Pain. 1979a;6:283–304. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90049-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC). II. Lack of effect on non-convergent neurones, supraspinal involvement and theoretical implications. Pain. 1979b;6:305–327. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Le Bars D, Chitour D, Kraus E, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Effect of naloxone upon diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) in the rat. Brain Research. 1981;204:387–402. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90597-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Li J, Pelletier MR, Perez Velazquez JL, Carlen PL. Reduced cortical synaptic plasticity and GluR1 expression associated with fragile X mental retardation protein deficiency. Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences. 2002;19:138–151. doi: 10.1006/mcne.2001.1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Luzzani S, Macchini F, Valadè A, Milani D, Selicorni A. Gastroesophageal reflux and Cornelia de Lange syndrome: typical and atypical symptoms. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 2003;119A:283–287. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.20191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mailis A. Compulsive targeted self-injurious behaviour in humans with neuropathic pain: a counterpart of animal autonomy? Four case reports and literature review. Pain. 1996;64:569–578. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00173-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Martin KC, Casadio A, Zhu H, Yaping E, Rose JC, Chen M, et al. Synapse-specific, long-term facilitation of aplysia sensory to motor synapses: a function for local protein synthesis in memory storage. Cell. 1997;91:927–938. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80484-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Matson JL, Dempsey T, Wilkins J. Rett syndrome in adults with severe intellectual disability: exploration of behavioral characteristics. European Psychiatry. 2008;23:460–465. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Melemedjian OK, Asiedu MN, Tillu DV, Peebles KA, Yan J, Ertz N, et al. IL-6- and NGF-induced rapid control of protein synthesis and nociceptive plasticity via convergent signaling to the eIF4F complex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:15113–15123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3947-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science. 1965;150:971–979. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3699.971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mittal RK, Holloway RH, Penagini R, Blackshaw LA, Dent J. Transient lower esophageal sphincter relaxation. Gastroenterology. 1995;109:601–610. doi: 10.1016/0016-5085(95)90351-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Moretti P, Levenson JM, Battaglia F, Atkinson R, Teague R, Antalffy B, et al. Learning and memory and synaptic plasticity are impaired in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:319–327. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2623-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Motil KJ, Schultz RJ, Browning K, Trautwein L, Glaze DG. Oropharyngeal dysfunction and gastroesophageal dysmotility are present in girls and women with Rett syndrome. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 1999;29:31–37. doi: 10.1097/00005176-199907000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Newell KM, Challis JH, Boros RL, Bodfish JW. Further evidence on the dynamics of self-injurious behaviors: impact forces and limb motions. American Journal of Mental Retardation. 2002a;107:60–68. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0060:FEOTDO>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Newell KM, Challis JH, Boros RL, Bodfish JW. Further evidence on the dynamics of self-injurious behaviors: impact forces and limb motions. American Journal of Mental Retardation. 2002b;107:60–68. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0060:FEOTDO>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ossipov MH, Dussor GO, Porreca F. Central modulation of pain. The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2010;120:3779–3787. doi: 10.1172/JCI43766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pastalkova E, Serrano P, Pinkhasova D, Wallace E, Fenton AA, Sacktor TC. Storage of spatial information by the maintenance mechanism of LTP. Science. 2006;313:1141–1144. doi: 10.1126/science.1128657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pertovaara A, Kemppainen P, Johansson G, Karonen SL. Ischemic pain nonsegmentally produces a predominant reduction of pain and thermal sensitivity in man: a selective role for endogenous opioids. Brain Research. 1982;251:83–92. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)91276-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Price DD. Characteristics of second pain and flexion reflexes indicative of prolonged central summation. Experimental Neurology. 1972;37:371–387. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(72)90081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Price DD, Hu JW, Dubner R, Gracely RH. Peripheral suppression of first pain and central summation of second pain evoked by noxious heat pulses. Pain. 1977;3:57–68. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(77)90035-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Price TJ, Geranton SM. Translating nociceptor sensitivity: the role of axonal protein synthesis in nociceptor physiology. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:2253–2263. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06786.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Price TJ, Flores CM, Cervero F, Hargreaves KM. The RNA binding and transport proteins staufen and fragile X mental retardation protein are expressed by rat primary afferent neurons and localize to peripheral and central axons. Neuroscience. 2006;141:2107–2116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.05.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Price TJ, Rashid MH, Millecamps M, Sanoja R, Entrena JM, Cervero F. Decreased nociceptive sensitization in mice lacking the fragile X mental retardation protein: role of mGluR1/5 and mTOR. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:13958–13967. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Roby-Brami A, Bussel B, Willer JC, Le Bars D. An electrophysiological investigation into the pain-relieving effects of heterotopic nociceptive stimuli. Probable involvement of a supraspinal loop. Brain. 1987;110(Pt 6):1497–1508. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.6.1497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Samaco RC, Fryer JD, Ren J, Fyffe S, Chao HT, Sun Y, et al. A partial loss of function allele of methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 predicts a human neurodevelopmental syndrome. Human Molecular Genetics. 2008;17:1718–1727. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sandkuhler J. Understanding LTP in pain pathways. Molecular Pain. 2007;3:9. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-3-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sandkuhler J. Central sensitization versus synaptic long-term potentiation (LTP): a critical comment. The Journal of Pain. 2010;11:798–800. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sansom D, Krishnan VH, Corbett J, Kerr A. Emotional and behavioural aspects of Rett syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1993;35:340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1993.tb11646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shema R, Sacktor TC, Dudai Y. Rapid erasure of long-term memory associations in the cortex by an inhibitor of PKM zeta. Science. 2007;317:951–953. doi: 10.1126/science.1144334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Silverman JL, Tolu SS, Barkan CL, Crawley JN. Repetitive self-grooming behavior in the BTBR mouse model of autism is blocked by the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:976–989. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sondheimer JM, Morris BA. Gastroesophageal reflux among severely retarded children. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1979;94:710–714. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(79)80135-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spitz L, Roth K, Kiely EM, Brereton RJ, Drake DP, Milla PJ. Operation for gastro-oesophageal reflux associated with severe mental retardation. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1993;68:347–351. doi: 10.1136/adc.68.3.347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Swender SL, Matson JL, Mayville SB, Gonzalez ML, Mcdowell D. A functional assessment of handmouthing among persons with severe and profound intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability. 2006;31:95–100. doi: 10.1080/13668250600710880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Symons FJ, Danov SE. A prospective clinical analysis of pain behavior and self-injurious behavior. Pain. 2005;117:473–477. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Symons FJ, Clark RD, Hatton DD, Skinner M, Bailey DB., Jr Self-injurious behavior in young boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 2003;118:115–121. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.10078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Symons FJ, Thompson A, Rodriguez MC. Self-injurious behavior and the efficacy of naltrexone treatment: a quantitative synthesis. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2004;10:193–200. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Symons FJ, Harper V, Shinde SK, Clary J, Bodfish JW. Evaluating a sham-controlled sensory-testing protocol for nonverbal adults with neurodevelopmental disorders: self-injury and gender effects. The Journal of Pain. 2010;11:773–881. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. The Dutch-Belgian Fragile X Consortium. Fmr1 knockout mice: a model to study fragile X mental retardation. The Dutch-Belgian Fragile X Consortium. Cell. 1994;78:23–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Turner G, Webb T, Wake S, Robinson H. Prevalence of fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1996;64:196–197. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960712)64:1<196::AID-AJMG35>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Van den Veyver IB, Zoghbi HY. Methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 mutations in Rett syndrome. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 2000;10:275–279. doi: 10.1016/s0959-437x(00)00083-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313:1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Willer JC, Roby A, Boulu P, Boureau F. Comparative effects of electroacupuncture and transcutaneous nerve stimulation on the human blink reflex. Pain. 1982;14:267–278. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(82)90133-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Willer JC, Roby A, Le Bars D. Psychophysical and electrophysiological approaches to the pain-relieving effects of heterotopic nociceptive stimuli. Brain. 1984;107(Pt 4):1095–1112. doi: 10.1093/brain/107.4.1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Willer JC, Le Bars D, De Broucker T. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in man: involvement of an opioidergic link. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1990;182:347–355. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(90)90293-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wilson BM, Cox CL. Absence of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated plasticity in the neocortex of fragile X mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:2454–2459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610875104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2010;152(3 Suppl):S2–S15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Woolf CJ, Salter MW. Neuronal plasticity: increasing the gain in pain. Science. 2000;288:1765–1769. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Young RL, Page AJ, O’Donnell TA, Cooper NJ, Blackshaw LA. Peripheral versus central modulation of gastric vagal pathways by metabotropic glutamate receptor 5. American Journal of Physiology. Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology. 2007;292:G501–G511. doi: 10.1152/ajpgi.00353.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Zerbib F, Keywood C, Strabach G. Efficacy, tolerability and pharmacokinetics of a modified release formulation of ADX10059, a negative allosteric modulator of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5: an esophageal pH-impedance study in healthy subjects. Neurogastroenterology and Motility. 2010;22:859–865. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2010.01484.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Zerbib F, Bruley des Varannes S, Roman S, Tutuian R, Galmiche JP, Mion F, et al. Randomised clinical trial: effects of monotherapy with ADX10059, a mGluR5 inhibitor, on symptoms and reflux events in patients with gastro-oesophageal reflux disease. Alimentary Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2011;33:911–921. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2036.2011.04596.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zhao MG, Toyoda H, Ko SW, Ding HK, Wu LJ, Zhuo M. Deficits in trace fear memory and long-term potentiation in a mouse model for fragile X syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:7385–7392. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1520-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES