Skip to main content
CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association Journal logoLink to CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association Journal
. 2012 Apr 17;184(7):755–762. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.111619

Influence of relative age on diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children

Richard L Morrow 1,, E Jane Garland 1, James M Wright 1, Malcolm Maclure 1, Suzanne Taylor 1, Colin R Dormuth 1
PMCID: PMC3328520  PMID: 22392937

Abstract

Background:

The annual cut-off date of birth for entry to school in British Columbia, Canada, is Dec. 31. Thus, children born in December are typically the youngest in their grade. We sought to determine the influence of relative age within a grade on the diagnosis and pharmacologic treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children.

Methods:

We conducted a cohort study involving 937 943 children in British Columbia who were 6–12 years of age at any time between Dec. 1, 1997, and Nov. 30, 2008. We calculated the absolute and relative risk of receiving a diagnosis of ADHD and of receiving a prescription for a medication used to treat ADHD (i.e., methylphenidate, dextroamphetamine, mixed amphetamine salts or atomoxetine) for children born in December compared with children born in January.

Results:

Boys who were born in December were 30% more likely (relative risk [RR] 1.30, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.23–1.37) to receive a diagnosis of ADHD than boys born in January. Girls born in December were 70% more likely (RR 1.70, 95% CI 1.53–1.88) to receive a diagnosis of ADHD than girls born in January. Similarly, boys were 41% more likely (RR 1.41, 95% CI 1.33–1.50) and girls 77% more likely (RR 1.77, 95% CI 1.57–2.00) to be given a prescription for a medication to treat ADHD if they were born in December than if they were born in January.

Interpretation:

The results of our analyses show a relative-age effect in the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD in children aged 6–12 years in British Columbia. These findings raise concerns about the potential harms of overdiagnosis and overprescribing. These harms include adverse effects on sleep, appetite and growth, in addition to increased risk of cardiovascular events.


Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most commonly diagnosed neurobehavioural disorder in children and is widely treated with stimulant and nonstimulant medications.1,2 Although the specific causes of the disorder are unknown, both genetic and environmental factors are thought to contribute and may interact.3,4 Several studies have investigated the potential association between season of birth and incidence of ADHD.59 Season of birth has been studied for a number of mental and behavioural disorders, including schizophrenia, for which an association is well-established,10 and autism, for which the evidence of an association is weaker.1113 Among studies hypothesizing a causal relation between season of birth and ADHD, a consistent seasonal pattern has not been found.59

Two recent studies using survey and private health plan data in the United States reported that date of birth was a predictor of diagnosis and treatment of ADHD based on the age at which the patient started school.14,15 Because of the cut-off dates of birth for entry to kindergarten, children within the same grade may be almost one year apart in age; that is, children with birthdates just before the cut-off will be younger and may be less mature than their classmates born at other times of year. These studies found that children who were relatively younger than their peers within the same grade were more likely to receive a diagnosis or a prescription for the pharmacologic treatment of ADHD. This pattern persisted across states with school entry dates at different times of year and depended on the cut-off date rather than the season in which the child was born. This phenomenon has been termed the relative-age effect and has been found to affect outcomes in education and athletics.16,17

There have been few studies of the potential effect of children’s relative age within a grade on the treatment and diagnosis of ADHD, and it has not been analyzed in populations outside of the US. The reported prevalence of ADHD in the US is high relative to international comparisons, and American use of medications to treat ADHD exceeds that of other developed countries, such as Canada.18,19

We were interested in knowing whether the effect of relative age was inherent in the American context (owing to underlying factors such as differences in the health care system, cultural attitudes or marketing by the pharmaceutical industry), or whether it would be present in the different context of Canada. We conducted a cohort study to explore the influence of relative age on the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD in a large sample of children with data from a nearly universal public health plan in British Columbia, Canada. The cut-off date of birth that allows entry into kindergarten or grade 1 in this province is December 31. Consequently, children born in December are typically the youngest in their grade. We hypothesized that the younger children within each grade would be more likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD and to have received prescriptions for the medications used to treat ADHD.

Methods

Data sources

We used linked data from provincial administrative health databases for prescription drugs (PharmaNet), physician services (Medical Services Plan) and hospital admissions (Canadian Institute for Health Information Discharge Abstracts Database). PharmaNet contains records of all medications dispensed at community pharmacies in British Columbia.20 Rates of underreporting and misclassification were expected to be minimal, because the quality of the data is checked when claims are transmitted.20 Similarly, data on physician services and admissions to hospital were expected to be reliable on the basis of studies comparing patient charts with administrative data in Canada.21,22 Our data were representative of most of the province’s population and excluded only about 4% of residents; that is, those who are federally insured and not covered by the province’s health plan, the British Columbia Medical Services Plan (i.e., Aboriginal people, prisoners, military personnel and members of the Royal Canadian Mounted Police).

Study design

The study period was Dec. 1, 1997, to Nov. 30, 2008. The study cohort comprised children between the ages of 6 and 12 years any time during the study period and who had been registered in the provincial health plan for at least one year before their entry into the cohort. Children entered the cohort when the latest of the following events occurred: their sixth birthday, the start of the study period or having been covered by the provincial health plan for one year. Follow-up continued until the earliest of the following events occurred: the day before their thirteenth birthday, the end of the study period or the end of coverage under the provincial health plan.

For our analysis of prescribed medications, we included methylphenidate, dextroamphetamine, mixed amphetamine salts and atomoxetine.

We placed children in categories based on their month of birth, then calculated the proportion of children who received a prescription for a medication used to treat ADHD and the proportion of patients with a diagnosis of ADHD for each month. To estimate the influence of relative age, we used these proportions to calculate the absolute risk difference (RD) and relative risk (RR) of receiving an ADHD diagnosis and of receiving a prescription for a medication to treat ADHD for children who were born in December compared with those born in January. We used a Cochran–Armitage trend test to determine the influence of relative age on diagnosis and prescribing across all months of birth.

In addition to our main analysis, we conducted analyses stratified by year and by age, and a series of sensitivity analyses. In the analysis stratified by year, we analyzed data in one-year categories spanning December to November (e.g., 1997–98 refers to Dec. 1, 1997, to Nov. 30, 1998). In the analysis stratified by age, we used one-year age groups from 6 to 12 years. As sensitivity analyses, we conducted the same risk calculations as in our main analysis, but changed the birthdates used to define our cohorts. We analyzed cohorts of children born Dec. 17–31 in relation to children born Jan. 1–15 (in which a relative-age effect was predicted) and compared the result with control analyses comparing the birthdate cohorts of Dec. 2–16 with Dec. 17–31, and Jan. 1–15 with Jan. 16–30 (in which no effect was expected). In addition, we analyzed the cohort of children born Dec. 28–31 relative to the cohort born Jan. 1–3.

Results

A total of 937 943 children were included in our study. The percentages of children who received a diagnosis of ADHD or a prescription for medication to treat ADHD are summarized by month of birth in Table 1. The numbers of children in the cohorts born in January and December were similar. Children in both groups had a mean age of 7.8 years upon entry to the cohort (data not shown). The proportion of children receiving a diagnosis of ADHD or treatment for ADHD increased with each birth month from January to September, for both boys and girls, then plateaued until December (the percentage of boys receiving a diagnosis decreased slightly in December). These rising trends in diagnosis and treatment by birth month were confirmed with Cochran–Armitage tests (all one-sided p values < 0.001). The variation in ADHD treatment by birth month is presented in Figure 1, which shows an increase in the percentage of children receiving treatment by birth month.

Table 1:

Percentages of boys and girls aged 6–12 years who received a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or treatment for the condition from a cohort of 937 943 children, by birth month

Boys Girls


Month of birth Children, no. Received diagnosis, % Received treatment, % Children, no. Received diagnosis, % Received treatment, %
January 39 136 5.7 4.4 37 448 1.6 1.1

February 36 586 5.9 4.6 34 791 1.8 1.3

March 41 512 6.0 4.7 38 658 1.9 1.4

April 40 605 6.1 5.0 38 529 1.9 1.4

May 42 724 6.5 5.2 40 621 1.9 1.4

June 40 720 6.7 5.3 38 808 2.2 1.7

July 41 829 7.3 5.8 40 149 2.3 1.7

August 40 859 7.3 5.9 38 670 2.4 1.8

September 41 111 7.6 6.2 38 739 2.6 1.9

October 39 773 7.9 6.2 37 967 2.6 1.9

November 37 409 7.8 6.2 35 535 2.6 1.9

December 38 977 7.4 6.2 36 787 2.7 1.9

Overall 481 241 6.9 5.5 456 702 2.2 1.6

Trend test (p value)* −19.7 (< 0.0001) −19.3 (< 0.0001) −16.5 (< 0.0001) −14.1 (< 0.0001)

RD (95% CI)
Dec. v. Jan.
1.71 (1.36 to 2.05) 1.80 (1.48 to 2.11) 1.09 (0.88 to 1.30) 0.84 (0.66 to 1.01)

RR (95% CI)
Dec. v. Jan.
1.30 (1.23 to 1.37) 1.41 (1.33 to 1.50) 1.70 (1.53 to 1.88) 1.77 (1.57 to 2.00)

Note: CI = confidence interval, RD = risk difference, RR = relative risk.

*

Cochran–Armitage trend test with one-sided p value.

Risk of receiving a diagnosis of or treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder for children born in December compared with children born in January.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

Percentage of children aged 6 to 12 years receiving pharmacologic treatment for ADHD, by month of birth. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

The risk of diagnosis and treatment for ADHD among children born in December versus those born in January is reported in terms of absolute RD and RR in Table 1. Boys who were born in December were 30% more likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD than boys born in January, and girls born in December were 70% more likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD than girls born in January. Similarly, boys born in December were 41% more likely, and girls 77% more likely, to have a prescription for a medication to treat ADHD than their peers born in January.

The prevalence of diagnosis and treatment for ADHD increased gradually from 1997–98 to 2007–08 for both boys and girls, peaking in the most recent years (Table 2). The relative risk of diagnosis and treatment for ADHD among children born in December as compared with those born in January remained relatively stable over time, with no statistically significant heterogeneity in risk across yearly strata (Table 2). An elevated risk of diagnosis and treatment for ADHD among children born in December compared with January was present for all ages from 6 to 12 years (Table 3). The RR for girls born in December diminished at the higher end of this age range for both diagnosis and treatment (χ2 tests for heterogeneity of RR p < 0.05 for diagnosis and p < 0.001 for treatment).

Table 2:

Risk of receiving a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or treatment for the condition for children born in December compared with children born in January, by year

Diagnosis Treatment


Year of birth* Children, % Risk, December v. January Children, % Risk, December v. January


RD (95% CI) RR (95% CI) RD (95% CI) RR (95% CI)
Boys

1997–98 2.6 0.62 (0.30 to 0.95) 1.30 (1.13 to 1.48) 3.4 1.04 (0.67 to 1.41) 1.39 (1.23 to 1.56)

1998–99 2.6 0.91 (0.58 to 1.23) 1.46 (1.28 to 1.68) 3.2 0.97 (0.61 to 1.32) 1.40 (1.24 to 1.59)

1999–00 2.6 0.72 (0.40 to 1.04) 1.36 (1.19 to 1.57) 3.1 1.07 (0.72 to 1.42) 1.47 (1.29 to 1.66)

2000–01 2.5 0.45 (0.13 to 0.78) 1.21 (1.06 to 1.39) 3.1 0.88 (0.53 to 1.23) 1.37 (1.21 to 1.55)

2001–02 2.7 0.78 (0.44 to 1.11) 1.36 (1.19 to 1.56) 3.2 1.28 (0.92 to 1.64) 1.56 (1.38 to 1.77)

2002–03 3.0 0.77 (0.42 to 1.13) 1.32 (1.16 to 1.49) 3.3 1.25 (0.87 to 1.62) 1.50 (1.33 to 1.70)

2003–04 3.2 0.75 (0.37 to 1.12) 1.27 (1.13 to 1.43) 3.5 1.02 (0.63 to 1.41) 1.36 (1.21 to 1.53)

2004–05 3.4 1.01 (0.62 to 1.40) 1.36 (1.21 to 1.54) 3.6 1.22 (0.82 to 1.62) 1.42 (1.27 to 1.60)

2005–06 3.5 1.04 (0.64 to 1.44) 1.35 (1.20 to 1.52) 3.6 1.05 (0.64 to 1.45) 1.35 (1.20 to 1.51)

2006–07 3.7 0.94 (0.54 to 1.35) 1.31 (1.17 to 1.47) 3.7 1.01 (0.59 to 1.42) 1.32 (1.17 to 1.47)

2007–08 3.7 0.62 (0.20 to 1.03) 1.19 (1.06 to 1.34) 3.7 1.02 (0.61 to 1.44) 1.33 (1.18 to 1.49)

Girls

1997–98 0.7 0.31 (0.13 to 0.48) 1.58 (1.21 to 2.06) 0.8 0.36 (0.18 to 0.55) 1.65 (1.27 to 2.13)

1998–99 0.7 0.36 (0.19 to 0.53) 1.78 (1.35 to 2.36) 0.8 0.33 (0.16 to 0.50) 1.72 (1.30 to 2.28)

1999–00 0.7 0.29 (0.12 to 0.47) 1.57 (1.19 to 2.06) 0.8 0.38 (0.19 to 0.56) 1.70 (1.31 to 2.21)

2000–01 0.7 0.32 (0.14 to 0.50) 1.61 (1.23 to 2.10) 0.8 0.37 (0.19 to 0.56) 1.69 (1.30 to 2.19)

2001–02 0.8 0.36 (0.18 to 0.55) 1.69 (1.30 to 2.21) 0.8 0.49 (0.30 to 0.68) 1.94 (1.49 to 2.52)

2002–03 0.9 0.35 (0.15 to 0.55) 1.55 (1.21 to 1.99) 0.9 0.50 (0.29 to 0.70) 1.79 (1.41 to 2.29)

2003–04 1.0 0.51 (0.30 to 0.72) 1.80 (1.41 to 2.29) 1.0 0.43 (0.21 to 0.64) 1.57 (1.25 to 1.98)

2004–05 1.1 0.57 (0.35 to 0.78) 1.83 (1.44 to 2.31) 1.1 0.63 (0.41 to 0.85) 1.93 (1.53 to 2.44)

2005–06 1.1 0.48 (0.26 to 0.70) 1.64 (1.30 to 2.06) 1.1 0.61 (0.39 to 0.84) 1.86 (1.48 to 2.36)

2006–07 1.1 0.69 (0.45 to 0.92) 1.97 (1.56 to 2.48) 1.1 0.67 (0.44 to 0.90) 1.96 (1.55 to 2.48)

2007–08 1.2 0.63 (0.38 to 0.87) 1.74 (1.40 to 2.18) 1.1 0.75 (0.51 to 0.98) 2.06 (1.63 to 2.60)

Note: CI = confidence interval, RD = risk difference, RR = relative risk.

*

Data are grouped from December to November of the following year (e.g., 1997–98 refers to Dec. 1, 1997, to Nov. 30, 1998).

Children born in any month of the year who received a diagnosis of or treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

χ2 tests for heterogeneity of relative risk across yearly strata. Among boys, χ2 = 8.3 (p = 0.6) for diagnosis and χ2 = 7.6 (p = 0.7) for treatment; among girls, χ2 = 3.7 (p > 0.9) for diagnosis and χ2 = 5.1 (p = 0.9) for treatment.

Table 3:

Risk of receiving a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or treatment for the condition for children born in December compared with children born in January, by age

Diagnosis Treatment


Age, yr Children, %* Risk, December v. Jananuary Children, (%)* Risk, December v. January


RD (95% CI) RR (95% CI) RD (95% CI) RR (95% CI)
Boys

6 2.3 0.82 (0.56 to 1.08) 1.48 (1.31 to 1.68) 1.6 0.71 (0.48 to 0.93) 1.59 (1.37 to 1.85)

7 3.1 0.96 (0.65 to 1.26) 1.38 (1.25 to 1.53) 2.6 1.02 (0.75 to 1.30) 1.53 (1.36 to 1.72)

8 3.4 1.12 (0.81 to 1.43) 1.42 (1.28 to 1.56) 3.4 1.30 (0.99 to 1.62) 1.50 (1.36 to 1.65)

9 3.6 0.90 (0.58 to 1.22) 1.30 (1.18 to 1.43) 4.0 1.24 (0.91 to 1.57) 1.41 (1.29 to 1.54)

10 3.5 0.60 (0.29 to 0.92) 1.20 (1.09 to 1.31) 4.3 1.24 (0.90 to 1.58) 1.37 (1.26 to 1.49)

11 3.3 0.73 (0.43 to 1.03) 1.27 (1.15 to 1.40) 4.3 1.32 (0.97 to 1.66) 1.38 (1.27 to 1.50)

12 3.0 0.63 (0.35 to 0.92) 1.25 (1.13 to 1.38) 4.2 1.14 (0.81 to 1.48) 1.34 (1.23 to 1.46)

Girls

6 0.7 0.39 (0.24 to 0.54) 1.83 (1.44 to 2.32) 0.4 0.21 (0.10 to 0.33) 1.74 (1.28 to 2.37)

7 0.9 0.41 (0.24 to 0.57) 1.68 (1.36 to 2.08) 0.7 0.45 (0.30 to 0.59) 2.04 (1.60 to 2.60)

8 1.0 0.68 (0.50 to 0.86) 2.11 (1.73 to 2.57) 0.9 0.62 (0.45 to 0.79) 2.19 (1.76 to 2.71)

9 1.1 0.67 (0.49 to 0.86) 1.90 (1.59 to 2.28) 1.2 0.87 (0.68 to 1.06) 2.25 (1.88 to 2.70)

10 1.0 0.43 (0.26 to 0.60) 1.60 (1.32 to 1.93) 1.2 0.74 (0.55 to 0.93) 2.01 (1.68 to 2.41)

11 0.9 0.38 (0.21 to 0.54) 1.54 (1.27 to 1.87) 1.2 0.55 (0.37 to 0.74) 1.69 (1.41 to 2.01)

12 0.8 0.20 (0.04 to 0.36) 1.29 (1.06 to 1.57) 1.1 0.25 (0.08 to 0.43) 1.29 (1.08 to 1.54)

Note: CI = confidence interval, RD = risk difference, RR = relative risk.

*

Children born in any month of the year.

χ2 tests for heterogeneity of relative risk across age strata. Among boys, χ2 = 12.2 (p = 0.06) for diagnosis and χ2 = 7.7 (p = 0.3) for treatment; among girls, χ2 = 15.3 (p = 0.02) for diagnosis and χ2 = 26.5 (p < 0.001) for treatment.

Sensitivity analyses

In our sensitivity analyses, children born during the last two weeks of the year were at higher risk of receiving a diagnosis of ADHD or treatment for the condition than were children born in the first two weeks of the year (analysis 1 in Table 4), in results similar to those of our main analyses. Our control analyses of periods within December and January produced results close to the null (analyses 2 and 3 in Table 4). When we considered cohorts of children born within three days before and after the cut-off for entry to school, the results were similar to those of our main analyses (analysis 4 in Table 4). Risk of diagnosis and treatment among boys was comparable with the results of our analysis by birth month; for girls, the risks were in the expected direction, but our sample was small and the risk of treatment was not statistically significant.

Table 4:

Sensitivity analyses for risk of receiving a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or treatment for the condition for children aged 6–12 years who were born in different periods

Exposed cohort Unexposed cohort RD (95% CI) RR (95% CI)
Analysis 1, date of birth Dec. 17–31 Jan. 1–15
Boys, no. 14 853 15 068
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 1 085 (7.3) 856 (5.7) 1.62 (1.07 to 2.18) 1.29 (1.18 to 1.40)
 Treatment, no. (%) 884 (6.0) 666 (4.4) 1.53 (1.03 to 2.03) 1.35 (1.22 to 1.49)
Girls, no. 13 960 14 529
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 376 (2.7) 231 (1.6) 1.10 (0.77 to 1.44) 1.69 (1.44 to 1.99)
 Treatment, no. (%) 261 (1.9) 150 (1.0) 0.84 (0.56 to 1.12) 1.81 (1.48 to 2.21)
Analysis 2, date of birth Dec. 2–16 Dec. 17–31
Boys, no. 18 957 14 853
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 1 420 (7.5) 1 085 (7.3) 0.19 (−0.38 to 0.75) 1.03 (0.95 to 1.11)
 Treatment, no. (%) 1 190 (6.3) 884 (6.0) 0.33 (−0.19 to 0.84) 1.05 (0.97 to 1.15)
Girls, no. 17 924 13 960
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 469 (2.6) 376 (2.7) −0.08 (−0.43 to 0.28) 0.97 (0.85 to 1.11)
 Treatment, no. (%) 347 (1.9) 261 (1.9) 0.07 (−0.24 to 0.37) 1.04 (0.88 to 1.21)
Analysis 3, date of birth Jan. 1–15 Jan. 16–30
Boys, no. 15 068 19 154
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 856 (5.7) 1 103 (5.8) −0.08 (−0.57 to 0.42) 0.99 (0.90 to 1.08)
 Treatment, no. (%) 666 (4.4) 837 (4.4) 0.05 (−0.39 to 0.49) 1.01 (0.92 to 1.12)
Girls, no. 14 529 18 179
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 231 (1.6) 276 (1.5) 0.07 (−0.20 to 0.34) 1.05 (0.88 to 1.25)
 Treatment, no. (%) 150 (1.0) 194 (1.1) −0.03 (−0.26 to 0.19) 0.97 (0.78 to 1.20)
Analysis 4, date of birth Dec. 29–31 Jan. 1–3
Boys, no. 3 907 3 609
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 272 (6.7) 190 (5.3) 1.70 (0.62 to 2.78) 1.32 (1.10 to 1.58)
 Treatment, no. (%) 252 (6.5) 155 (4.3) 2.16 (1.14 to 3.17) 1.50 (1.24 to 1.82)
Girls, no. 3 710 3 491
 Diagnosis, no. (%) 93 (2.5) 59 (1.7) 0.82 (0.16 to 1.48) 1.48 (1.07 to 2.05)
 Treatment, no. (%) 74 (2.0) 49 (1.4) 0.59 (0.00 to 1.19) 1.42 (0.99 to 2.03)

Note: CI = confidence interval, RD = risk difference, RR = relative risk.

Interpretation

Main findings

Our results show a pattern of treating and diagnosing ADHD among children aged 6–12 years in British Columbia that is consistent with a relative-age effect. Children born during the month preceding the province’s cut-off date for entry to school are typically the youngest and least mature within their grade, and are at a higher risk for treatment and diagnosis of ADHD. Our sensitivity analyses support this interpretation and discourage the alternative interpretation that an underlying seasonality related to the cause of the disorder is producing this effect. A sudden change in risk appears to coincide with the cut-off date for entry to school at the end of the calendar year. Even children born during the three days before the end of the calendar year had a higher risk than children born during the first three days of the following year, despite being born in the same season (i.e., they were all born within a six-day period).

The attenuation in rising risk for the birth months from October to December, seen in Figure 1 and Table 1, may suggest that children predisposed to ADHD have early behaviour problems that cause them to be held back from school for a year, thus allowing them more time to develop sociable behaviour.

Comparison with other studies

The relative-age effect we report, based on population-wide public data in British Columbia, is consistent with analyses of relative age in two studies using survey and private health plan data in the US.14,15

In our study, a sudden change in the percentage of children who received a diagnosis of ADHD or treatment for the condition occurs near Dec. 31, the cut-off date for entry to school in the province of British Columbia. In the American studies, a discontinuity occurred around the dates for entry to school at the different times of year for the different states. In interpreting their findings, the authors of those studies considered that the relative immaturity of younger students within a grade may lead to the inappropriate diagnosis of ADHD.

Although the alternative interpretations that underdiagnosis takes place in older children or that the social pressures on younger children amplify the symptoms of the disorder must also be considered, the evidence of a relative-age effect in our study raises the concern of overdiagnosis and overtreatment of ADHD in younger children within a grade. The potential harms of overdiagnosis and unnecessary pharmacologic treatments are important to consider. Children who are given medications to treat ADHD are exposed to adverse effects on sleep, appetite and growth, in addition to an increased risk of cardiovascular events.23 Inappropriate diagnosis of ADHD in a child born late in the year might lead parents and teachers to treat the child differently or adversely change the child’s self-perceptions. Our analyses add weight to concerns about the medicalization of the normal range of childhood behaviours, particularly for boys.

Our data underscore the dimensional and developmental nature of the symptoms of ADHD24 and the impact of contextual expectations on the likelihood of the diagnosis being made. Age-corrected rating scales and developmentally appropriate evaluation are therefore essential, but such a strategy may not be enough to fully eliminate the relative-age effect. Confounding influences may still exist, such as the expectations of parents and teachers, or the child’s self-perception in the classroom. For example, inadvertent reinforcement may magnify the apparently inattentive, distracting or impulsive behaviours of the youngest children in a class, such as escaping from a difficult academic task (negative reinforcement) or receiving attention from teachers and peers for disruptive behaviour (positive reinforcement). A previous study found that teachers’ perceptions of child behaviour were more strongly related to a child’s age within a grade than were parental perceptions, suggesting that

[T]eachers’ opinions of children are the key mechanisms driving the relationship between school starting age and ADHD diagnoses.14

It is possible that closer consideration of a child’s behaviour in multiple contexts, including those outside of school, may lessen the risk of unnecessary diagnosis when assessing children for ADHD.

Limitations

We aimed to determine the influence of birth month on diagnosing and prescribing medications for ADHD, but our study faced certain limitations. To interpret the increased risk of diagnosis and treatment among younger children within a grade as overdiagnosis or overprescribing implies an assumption as to appropriate levels of diagnosis and treatment, such as that children born in January received appropriate levels of diagnoses and treatment. However, the true incidence of ADHD is unknown. The proportion of children born in January who received a diagnosis of ADHD might underestimate incidence of the disorder if more mature children within each grade are better able to cope with an underlying disorder. However, it is possible that the January proportion overestimates the incidence if a larger issue of overmedicalization of childhood behaviour exists.

Conclusion

Our analyses show a relative-age effect in the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD in children aged 6–12 years in the province of British Columbia. The strength of this effect remained relatively stable for the duration of our 11-year study. Although the prevalence of diagnosis and treatment for ADHD remains considerably higher among boys, it increased over time for children of both sexes in this age range.

Although the influence of relative age on diagnosis and treatment may lessen for older children, we found the effect was present at all ages from 6 to 12 years, for girls and boys alike. The potential harms of overdiagnosis and overprescribing and the lack of an objective test for ADHD strongly suggest caution be taken in assessing children for this disorder and providing treatment. Greater emphasis on a child’s behaviour outside of school may be warranted when assessing children for ADHD to lessen the risk of inappropriate diagnosis. Further research into the determinants of ADHD and approaches to its assessment and treatment should consider a child’s age within a grade.

Footnotes

Competing interests: Richard Morrow, E. Jane Garland, James Wright, Malcolm Maclure and Colin Dormuth received a Catalyst grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research to study postmarket drug safety and effectivness. Malcolm Maclure is employed by the University of British Columbia and the British Columbia Ministry of Health. Suzanne Taylor is employed by the British Columbia Ministry of Health.

This article has been peer reviewed.

Contributors: All of the authors participated in the conception and design of the study, and the analysis and interpretation of the results. Richard Morrow and Colin Dormuth drafted the manuscript, and all of the authors critically revised it and approved the final version submitted for publication.

Funding: This work was funded by a Catalyst Post-Market Drug Safety and Effectiveness grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (grant DSA-103525), and by a five-year renewable grant to the University of British Columbia from the British Columbia Ministry of Health.

References

  • 1.Increasing prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children — United States, 2003 and 2007. MMWR Morb Mortal Wekly Rep 2010;59:1439–43 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Pastor PN, Reuben CA. Diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disability: United States, 2004–2006. Vital Health Stat 10 2008; July:1–14 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Nigg J, Nikolas M, Burt SA. Measured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:863–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thapar A, Langley K, Asherson P, et al. Gene–environment interplay in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the importance of a developmental perspective. Br J Psychiatry 2007;190:1–3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Liederman J, Flannery KA. Fall conception increases the risk of neurodevelopmental disorder in offspring. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1994;16:754–68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mick E, Biederman J, Faraone SV. Is season of birth a risk factor for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35:1470–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Seeger G, Schloss P, Schmidt MH, et al. Gene-environment interaction in hyperkinetic conduct disorder (HD + CD) as indicated by season of birth variations in dopamine receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism. Neurosci Lett 2004;366:282–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Brookes KJ, Neale B, Xu X, et al. Differential dopamine receptor D4 allele association with ADHD dependent of proband season of birth. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008; 147B:94–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Atladóttir HO, Parner ET, Schendel D, et al. Variation in incidence of neurodevelopmental disorders with season of birth. Epidemiology 2007;18:240–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tochigi M, Okazaki Y, Kato N, et al. What causes seasonality of birth in schizophrenia? Neurosci Res 2004;48:1–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hebert KJ, Miller LL, Joinson CJ. Association of autistic spectrum disorder with season of birth and conception in a UK cohort. Autism Res 2010;3:185–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kolevzon A, Weiser M, Gross R, et al. Effects of season of birth on autism spectrum disorders: Fact or fiction? Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1288–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hauschild KM, Mouridsen SE, Nielsen S. Season of birth in Danish children with language disorder born in the 1958–1976 period. Neuropsychobiology 2005;51:93–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Elder TE. The importance of relative standards in ADHD diagnoses: evidence based on exact birth dates. J Health Econ 2010; 29:641–56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Evans WN, Morrill MS, Parente ST. Measuring inappropriate medical diagnosis and treatment in survey data: the case of ADHD among school-age children. J Health Econ 2010;29:657–73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bedard K, Dhuey E. The persistence of early childhood maturity: international evidence of long-run age effects. Q J Econ 2006; 121:1437–72 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Cobley S, Baker J, Wattie N, et al. Annual age-grouping and athlete development: a meta-analytical review of relative age effects in sport. Sports Med 2009;39:235–56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, et al. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:942–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Scheffler RM, Hinshaw SP, Modrek S, et al. The global market for ADHD medications. Health Aff (Millwood) 2007;26:450–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dormuth CR, Glynn RJ, Neumann P, et al. Impact of two sequential drug cost-sharing policies on the use of inhaled medications in older patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease or asthma. Clin Ther 2006;28:964–78, discussion 2–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Humphries KH, Rankin JM, Carere RG, et al. Co-morbidity data in outcomes research: Are clinical data derived from administrative databases a reliable alternative to chart review? J Clin Epidemiol 2000;53:343–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Steele LS, Glazier RH, Lin E, et al. Using administrative data to measure ambulatory mental health service provision in primary care. Med Care 2004;42:960–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gould MS, Walsh BT, Munfakh JL, et al. Sudden death and use of stimulant medications in youths. Am J Psychiatry 2009;166:992–1001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, et al. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:737–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association Journal are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association

RESOURCES