Abstract
The -function and the -function are phenomenological models that are widely used in the context of timing interceptive actions and collision avoidance, respectively. Both models were previously considered to be unrelated to each other: is a decreasing function that provides an estimation of time-to-contact (ttc) in the early phase of an object approach; in contrast, has a maximum before ttc. Furthermore, it is not clear how both functions could be implemented at the neuronal level in a biophysically plausible fashion. Here we propose a new framework – the corrected modified Tau function – capable of predicting both -type (“”) and -type (“”) responses. The outstanding property of our new framework is its resilience to noise. We show that can be derived from a firing rate equation, and, as , serves to describe the response curves of collision sensitive neurons. Furthermore, we show that predicts the psychophysical performance of subjects determining ttc. Our new framework is thus validated successfully against published and novel experimental data. Within the framework, links between -type and -type neurons are established. Therefore, it could possibly serve as a model for explaining the co-occurrence of such neurons in the brain.
Author Summary
In 1957, Sir Fred Hoyle published a science fiction novel in which he described humanity's encounter with an extraterrestrial life form. It came in the shape of a huge black cloud which approached the earth. Hoyle proposed a formula (“”) for computing the remaining time until contact (“ttc”) of the cloud with the earth. Nowadays in real science, serves as a model for ttc -perception for animals and humans, although it is not entirely undisputed. For instance, seems to be incompatible with a collision-sensitive neuron in locusts (the Lobula Giant Movement Detector or LGMD neuron). LGMD neurons are instead better described by the -function, which differs from . Here we propose a generic model (“”) that contains and as special cases. The validity of the model was confirmed with a psychophysical experiment. Also, we fitted many published response curves of LGMD neurons with our new model and with the -function. Both models fit these response curves well, and we thus can conclude that and possibly result from a generic neuronal circuit template such as it is described by .
Introduction
Monocular presentation of a looming object elicits escape or avoidance reactions in many species, including humans [1]–[4]. When a planar object travels perpendicular to a surface toward an observer (i.e. the object approaches the observer on a direct collision course), it projects a symmetrically expanding image on the retina. Notice that in the present paper we only focus on a subset of approaches where the approaching object eventually collides with the observer. We assume that collision happens at time (time to contact, “ttc”). At time before , the image subtends an angle , and its outer contours expand with angular velocity . Both angular variables grow nearly exponentially with decreasing distance between object and eye (assuming a constant velocity ). With knowledge of a predator's or object's typical size [5], it is therefore possible to trigger a behavioral response as soon as or , respectively, crosses a threshold [1], [6], [7].
The visual systems of various species are also known to “compute” functions of and (see e.g. [8] for a recent review). The Tau-function (“”) is defined by . Under the assumption that the object is a rigid sphere that approaches with , has several interesting properties [9], [10]: First, provides a running estimation of ttc during the approach. Second, the ttc estimation is largely independent of physical object size, provided that and are noise-free. Third, decreases approximately linearly with time with a constant slope of , but eventually linearity is compromised, as has a minimum shortly before ttc. It therefore would be comparatively easy to track the remaining time until impact, and to precisely time avoidance reactions, for example as soon as is below a certain threshold value.
These three properties, however, are valid only for “sufficiently small” angular sizes . Any quantitative criterion for “sufficiently small” implicates an error threshold for the deviation of from linearity, that is . For example, according to Text S6 a corresponding threshold for the visual angle can be defined as with some constant . Notice that the -criterion is independent from stimulus parameters such as object diameter or approach velocity.
Because is well suited for the estimation of , it could in principal serve as a universal mechanism for guiding motor actions during object approaches or during self-motion towards static objects. Indeed, several studies related to behavioral responses in this context, thus asserting that many organisms, including humans, rely on for their timing of motor actions (e.g. [10]–[12]). But a critical re-evaluation of the -hypothesis arrived at the conclusion that does not necessarily play a unique role for ttc estimation [13], [14]. For example, humans also rely on the rate of change of relative disparity, particularly in the late phase of an approach, for small object sizes [15]–[18], for low speeds [19], [20], or if knowledge of object size is available [7]. In addition, the task at hand (e.g. catching a ball or eluding a meteorite) seems to dictate the information that will eventually be used for action timing [14], [18], [21]. Further inconsistencies with respect to were reported with psychophysical results, where tended to be underestimated [16]. In addition, ttc -estimation reveals a certain dependence on object size [22], which is also not predicted by at “sufficiently small” angular sizes.
The Tau-function is often studied in the context of ttc -estimation. It appears, however, that in order to describe the responses of collision-sensitive neurons in certain species is inadequate. For example, the Lobula Giant Movement Detector (LGMD) neuron in locusts responds with increasing activity to a stimulation with a symmetrically expanding image, if the expansion pattern is consistent with an approaching object [23], [24]. The response curve of the LGMD neuron gradually increases to a maximum and then abruptly ceases (often to a nonzero baseline response). Because does not have a maximum, a different function has been proposed for modeling LGMD responses: The Eta-function (“”). It is defined as , with a constant [25]. Theoretically, the time when the activity peak occurs depends linearly on the ratio of object half-size to object velocity . The peak will shift closer to for smaller or faster objects, and always occurs at angular size , independent of [26]. The LGMD activity peak could in principle signal a critical angular size for escaping. Indeed, a recent study with freely behaving locust suggests that the time of peak firing rate of the Descending Contralateral Movement Detector (DCMD) predicts that of jump [27] (each LGMD spike triggers a spike in the postsynaptic DCMD as well, because the LGMD is strongly coupled to the DCMD by a combined electrical and chemical synapse [28], [29]).
It has nevertheless been argued that – in some ecologically meaningful situations (small ) – there is no guarantee for the peak to occur before [2], [5]. This statement may be true to the extent that in freely behaving locusts, a reliable escape jump is triggered before collision only in the range of to [30]. For , the jump would occur after projected collision, and this value thus may reflect the typical sizes and speeds of predators.
Apart from the locust, other species have collision-sensitive neurons with -like properties, for instance fruitflies [31] and bullfrogs [32]. In pigeons, the response properties of one of three classes of neurons in the dorsal posterior zone of the nucleus rotundus also seems to be compatible with the -function [1]. (The two remaining classes seem to compute and , respectively). In the goldfish, responses of the M-cell to looming stimuli also appear to follow a version of the -function, in which replaces , such that the new function does only depend on [33].
The Tau-function and the Eta-function are the two prevailing models for studying ttc -perception and (interceptive) action timing on the one hand, and escape behavior and collision avoidance on the other. In other words, we have two different models for two seemingly separated contexts. Each model brings about some hitherto unresolved issues, which are subsequently described.
From a computational point of view, is numerically unstable: In the presence of noise, we have to reckon with the fact that can get very small – or even reach zero – at certain instants during the initial phase of the approach (cf. [17]). As a consequence, fluctuations of with large amplitudes may occur. If, however, noise levels are constant in time, and noise is not multiplicative, the signal to noise ratio continuously improves as is approached. It is furthermore not entirely clear how could be biophysically implemented in a neuron.
As for the -function, the LGMD neuron seems to bypass a direct multiplication or division by computing with subsequent exponentiation of the result [34]. From a mathematical viewpoint, however, taking the logarithm introduces an instability for , although neuronal circuits with divisive inhibition can be adjusted such that no stability problems occur [35]. Moreover, Gabbiani et al. [34] found that a third-order power law fitted the mean instantaneous firing rate of the LGMD better than an exponential or a linear function (see also reference [36]).
Our original motivation was to improve the stability of with a simple modification. This modification led us to the modified Tau function . Similar to the -function, the -function also reveals a maximum before ttc. We were able to fit the response curves of -type neurons with (Text S4). Our -function represents the equilibrium solution of an equation for describing neuronal firing rate. Because of this, is based on a biophysically plausible mechanism.
But comes with a disadvantage: Unlike , it no longer provides a running value of ttc. In order to recover the ttc prediction, we needed to add a correction term to . This so-defined corrected modified Tau function () recovers the ttc prediction of the original -function, but suppresses noise better than . Most importantly, the corrected m-Tau function predicts the results of a psychophysical experiment, requiring subjects to estimate ttc.
Theoretically, we therefore can explain -type and -type responses within the framework, which contains (but also !) as a special case. Until now, and did not have any obvious relationship with each other (although we show in Text S6 how could formally be related to ). The -function could thus serve to explain why -type and -type neurons could be found alongside each other in the pigeon brain [1].
Results
The corrected modified Tau function “” (equation 5) contains the modified Tau function “” (equation 1) as a special case. We nevertheless first introduce the model, as this makes its relation to the original -function much easier understood.
The modified model (“”)
Behavioral and neural responses to optical variables (e.g., , , , ) in the initial part of a trajectory are very noisy signals. Signal fluctuations may occur as a consequence of the discrete structure of the retinal photoreceptor array and its limited spatial resolution. The signal-to-noise ratio continuously improves as ttc is approached (Text S3).
Our first step adds computational stability to the model. Let be a constant (in units of ). The modified Tau model is defined as:
(1) |
Biophysically, can be interpreted as leakage conductance (equation S2 in Text S1). According to equation (1), can formally be expressed in terms of multiplied with a gain control factor , which depends only on angular velocity. Notice, however, that the multiplicative version “” would again compromise stability, because appears as one of the factors in the product. Figure 1 a juxtaposes and the factors and , respectively.
Let the initial distance between the eye and a circular object (diameter ) be denoted by . Then, choosing will create a maximum of at time (i.e., a maximum before ):
(2) |
(the previous equation is derived in the Methods Section). The time when assumes its maximum can thus be controlled by specifying , where bigger values will place the maximum closer to . The maximum depends as follows on approach velocity and object diameter, respectively.
Assume fixed values for and . Then, will have an activity maximum at (default case). Now increase approach velocity and initial distance, such that remains constant. As a consequence, the peak will shift closer to with respect to the default case (triangle symbols in Figure 1a ; further figures in Text S2). This is the velocity effect.
Now increase the object diameter. The maximum of will then occur earlier compared to the default case (circle symbols in Figure 1). This is the size effect.
Assuming that the peak signals an imminent collision, this shifting behavior is consistent with larger objects being perceived to have an earlier ttc than smaller ones [22]. Note that the original -function (i.e. and noise-free angular variables) does not show a strong dependence on object size where holds (but see Text S6).
The -function is the prevailing model for describing responses from collision sensitive neurons to object approaches with constant velocity. Its characteristic feature is its maximum. Because also has a maximum, we fit previously published neuronal response curves with the -function and (Text S4). Figure 2 summarizes these fits by comparing the response maxima of the experimental curves (“”) with the maxima predicted by the best fits achieved with the two functions (“”). Predictions of are slightly better with -fits, both in terms of mean and median of absolute differences (). With respect to goodness of fit measures (root-mean-square-errors, , F-statistics), both functions perform again on par with each other. Therefore, both and the -function describe neuronal responses of object approaches with constant velocity.
The experimental maxima at time depend linearly on [26]. The -function predicts this linear relationship (equation S5 in Text S2), where slope is identified by , and intercept by a temporal delay of corresponding line fits (Figure 3a ).
The maximum of the -function , however, depends in a nonlinear way on (equation 2 & equation S6 in Text S2; illustration: Figure 4). (Nonlinearity means that the slope depends on , and linearity means that it does not). Linearity is approached with increasing values of , eventually reaching a slope of one for (equation S9 in Text S2). This is nevertheless inconsistent with experimental evidence, as the experimental values for are underestimated (typically ).
We thus explored a different possibility: Can the nonlinear function be hidden by noise? Figure 3b suggests that it nearly can, as seen when fitting a line to a version of with additive Gaussian noise. Noise levels were set as reported in [26]. This hide-and-seek works quite well, and the fitting statistics (, KS-test on residuals, F-statistics) are consistent with linearity in many random trials (detailed analysis: Text S2).
Figure 4 suggests a correlation between intercept and slope of line fits for different values of . We thus fit lines to the noisified version of for various values of . As before, noise levels were set as reported, and we again identified intercept and slope of the line fits to with and , respectively. The result of this procedure is shown in Figure 5, and agrees well with Figure 4 in [26]. Thus, consistently predicts a good correlation between intercepts and slopes both in the presence and in the absence of noise.
The corrected modified model (“”)
Maximum detection of in the initial phase of an object approach (i.e., for small values of ) is problematic, due to the signal's poor signal-to-noise ratio and the rather “shallow” curvature around the maximum. The situation gets progressively better if we place the maximum closer to , that is for bigger values of : The signal-to-noise ratio is better, and curvature is higher. With , however, we fell short of explaining the results of our psychophysical experiment (which is below described further). This led us to modify as follows.
Observe that for all , and thus
(3) |
is a positive correction factor to , such that . As with , the correction factor per se is again susceptible to fluctuations in the angular variable , and we would have gained no improvement by simply adding it to .
Now, the crucial idea is to render insensitive to such fluctuations. This is achieved with a first order low-pass filter (a short introduction is given in Text S8). Low-pass filtering of and transforms into a slowly varying signal, which is eventually added to :
(4) |
and are low-pass filtered visual angle and angular velocity, respectively, and is the system's integration time constant. In order to avoid initial filter transients, the filter variables were initialized with and , respectively. The are filter memory coefficients with for . No filtering would take place for (no memory), and the filters would never change their initial state for (infinite memory).
The corrected, modified model (“corrected m-Tau”) is then defined as:
(5) |
where is a small constant, such that possible division-by-zero errors are avoided in the simulation. Nevertheless,in the presence of noise, division-by-zero errors do not typically represent a problem during an approach with , because if the following two conditions hold: (i) appropriate initialization of , and (ii) “sufficiently strong” lowpass filtering. The offset is included for the sake of completeness. It was only considered for simulating our psychophysical experiment (described below), where it turned out to be negligibly small. In general, therefore, it is safe to assume .
Similar to , the new corrected m-Tau-model also computes an estimation of ttc for “sufficiently small” angular sizes. But the principal advantage of over is that it is less sensitive to noise. The noise suppression of the corrected m-Tau-model is constrained by the noise suppression performance of two “limit functions”, which are approached dependent on the values of , , and (Figure 6). For the derivation of these limit functions, assume (to simplify matters) that in equation (5) with (and ). Then, as we will show subsequently, the constraining functions are the ordinary function for , on the one hand (equation 6), and for a version of with lowpass-filtered angular variables, on the other (equation 8). Thus, , where , provided that we exclude the case , , which would imply that is unbounded.
Case I:
For very small (more precisely ), the first term of the equation (5) is approximately
(6) |
which is just the ordinary function. For the second term , which implies that it can be neglected because its denominator is approximately equal to . Furthermore, during an object approach with constant velocity, angular size and angular velocity are increasing, and , as is monotonically decreasing (except at times very close to ttc, see Text S6). The last arguments hold also for and , respectively, which are the lowpass-filtered optical variables, where . We eventually arrive at the approximation
(7) |
Summarizing the above, if , then the noise suppression performance of the corrected m-Tau -model is comparable with that of ordinary (Figure 6a ).
Case II:
For (more precisely ), the situation is just the opposite of Case I. The first term of equation (5) can be neglected, because . Given that in the denominator of the term, we obtain
(8) |
This is the ordinary -function but with lowpass filtered optical variables (“”, Figure 6b ).
Predicting psychophysical performance
Details on our psychophysical experiment are spelled out in the Methods Section. In a nutshell, subjects viewed approaching balls on a monitor. The balls had two different sizes (big & small, corresponding to object diameters & , respectively), and disappeared after (presentation time) until . A beep sounded always at the same time, , in order to indicate a reference time to the subjects. Approaches with different values of were presented, where could occur before or after . Subjects were asked to judge whether they were hit by the ball before or after . Responses were pooled, and the “proportion of later responses” for each presentation time (corresponding to “ball hit me after ”) was computed as a function of ttc. Figure 7a shows the corresponding data points for , along with the best matching Gaussian cumulative density function (“GCDF”-fit) for each object diameter. The GCDF-fits represent an estimate of the underlying psychometric curves or psychometric functions, respectively. Figure 7b suggests that subjects did not respond to the average of the stimulus set, because the mean of the distribution (point of subjective simultaneity) shifted with presentation time. In addition, the variance of the distribution decreased with increasing presentation time. The small object diameter is furthermore associated with a higher variance than the big one.
The full set of data points is shown in Figure 8, where each figure panel corresponds to a different presentation time (small object size: circles; big: triangles). The curves shown in Figure 8 do not represent GCDF-fits (as in Figure 7a ), but rather display simulation results from the -model. For short presentation times, subjects show near-random performance across ttc (Figure 8a, b ), thereby revealing a bias towards later responses (i.e. “ball hit me after ”). The GCDF-fits reveal a higher bias for the small object diameter (Figure 7b ). The corresponding psychometric functions (not shown) and -predictions for the shortest presentation time (; Figure 8a ) are thus rather flat and noisy. This bias is progressively reduced with increasing , indicating improving performance: For , the point of subjective simultaneity approaches for both object diameters, and psychometric functions get closer to a step-wise increase at (Figure 7a ).
We already mentioned that we simulated the psychometric functions with the corrected m-Tau -model (equation 5), at which we added noise to angular size and angular velocity (equation 9). By assuming a constant approach velocity, one could compute an estimation of ttc with equation (12). Note that this estimation should be constant throughout the approach in a noise-free situation and for “sufficiently small” angular sizes. As a consequence of having noise, however, the ttc estimation fluctuates. We therefore computed an average estimation with equation (14), by taking the mean value across a time interval (the interval contained the last estimates). The average ttc estimation was evaluated at presentation time , and compared with the reference . With a total number of such trials, we then counted occurrences where the average estimate occurred after . The simulated proportion of later responses is then obtained by dividing by (equation 13).
In order to find the appropriate -parameters for predicting psychophysical performance, the error between -predictions and psychophysical data points was minimized. We refer to this procedure as optimization. Optimization was carried out separately for object diameters big and small. The first step of the optimization procedure consisted in parsing the parameter space, and recording the error associated with each set of -parameters. The error was determined with two measures (“score measures”): The root mean square error (), and an outlier-insensitive robust error (). In the second step, the parameter sets were sorted in ascending order with respect to their associated score measure. Sorting took place separately for and , leading to corresponding tables where the best set of parameters was assigned rank one (1st table row), the second best rank two (2nd table row), and so on (Tables S1 & S2 in Text S5).
A third table of -parameters was then computed which was optimal for both object diameters simultaneously (combined; Table S3 in Text S5). This could be done in a straightforward way, simply by averaging the score measures of big and small of corresponding parameter sets, and subsequently sorting the averaged errors (more details on finding parameters are given in Text S5).
For the computation of and , all psychophysical data points that represent the proportion of later responses entered equivalently, in the sense that no weighting coefficients were used to bias the optimization process toward longer presentation times (as GCDF-fits at longer presentation times have a smaller variance, see Figure 7b ). Notice that parameter optimization for the combined diameter naturally implicates a trade off – the errors with respect to big and small will be bigger compared to individual parameter optimization.
Figure 8 shows that the corrected m-Tau -model adjusts fairly well to the psychophysical data of both object diameters. Nevertheless, the -predictions for are somewhat worse with the combined parameter optimization (Figure 8e ) when compared to a separate optimization for big and small (corresponding figures in Text S7). The most likely explanation for this discrepancy (individual versus combined parametrizations) is that each object size is associated with a different noise level (noise levels are represented by the -parameters with ; see equation 9). We investigated this hypothesis by comparing the corresponding values of for big and small, as a function of their rank. Figure 9 shows that the for small are consistently higher than for big. Therefore, the corrected m-Tau -model generally supports the notion that smaller object diameters imply higher noise levels in angular size and angular velocity, respectively.
We also studied two models with less degrees of freedom than corrected m-Tau : The first was , and the second was with for (). The best (i.e. smallest) score measures achieved with these reduced models were consistently higher than the best values achieved by the corrected m-Tau -model (Text S5), and their best-ranked parameter sets resulted in psychometric curve predictions that were also inferior by visual inspection (not shown).
Discussion
With the corrected m-Tau -model equation (5), we proposed a general framework that comprises the -function and several properties of the -function. By means of adjusting only a single parameter (), the corrected m-Tau -model can approximate and , respectively. Moreover, the -approximation is less sensitive to noise than the original -function, and accounts well for the performance of the psychophysical experiment that we carried out.
In the experiment, subjects had to decide whether a (displayed) ball reached them before or after a reference signal at time . However, balls were only presented until , and disappeared afterwards. In other words, subjects had to estimate ( could occur before or after ). With respect to our experiment, the corrected m-Tau -model suggests the following conclusions:
Subjects relied on a -based mechanism for judging ttc (). We use the term “-based” as a synonym for any timing-based mechanism. The full corrected m-Tau -model better predicted our psychophysical results than any of the two alternative models that we considered ( and ).
The decision about whether perceived occurs before or after is based on information at (or immediately around) , as the only information used was from until for predicting ttc (see equation 14).
Subjects' performance improves with increasing , indicating that the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) that is associated with the computation of (perceived) improves during an object approach. Such an improvement can be brought about by two mechanisms. First, the noise level is signal-independent and thus stays the same during an object approach. As angular size and angular velocity increase monotonically with time, the SNR would improve accordingly. Second, noise may increase with the signal [37], but is concurrently suppressed by low-pass filtering. Low-pass filtering may be adaptive, such that it adjusts to signal variability in each moment. We are not aware of any such signal-dependent noise suppression, and we therefore deem the first mechanism to be the more likely. Accordingly, we propose that approaching objects with smaller size lead to decreased SNRs in the signals that represent and , respectively.
The perception of ttc in humans reveals a certain dependence on object size [22]. Thus, one might argue that -based mechanisms are not an adequate model for ttc perception, because they are largely object-size-independent in the early phase of an object approach when is still “sufficiently small”. However, this argument ignores noise. As long as the noise-induced fluctuations in and do not cancel (“correlated noise”), the SNR of will depend on object size (Figure 6). Therefore, any decision based on computing with a -based mechanisms will be limited by the SNR at time (ttc can be computed by adding to , because in the early phase of an approach decreases linearly with time for , see equation 12). The SNR improves with increasing object size and with decreasing (initial) distance between object and observer. Thus, bigger objects will imply better accuracy in estimating . Similarly, smaller distances will imply better estimation accuracy. Both effects are observed in our psychophysical experiment, where a better “estimation accuracy” translates into psychometric curves that adjust better to a step-wise increase from zero to one at (because of ; Figure 7a ). Without noise, however, -based mechanisms cannot predict such dependence on object size for small angular sizes.
The modified -model (“”) constitutes a special case of . It is obtained from equation (5) for ( by default). Its distinguishing feature is a maximum before , which can be shifted via (equation 2). The -maximum decreases as it is positioned closer to , because this implies bigger values of . The time of the -maximum depends furthermore on size and velocity (Figure 1). The curve shape of is reminiscent of the -function, since both functions have a maximum. We thus decided to fit previously published response curves from collision sensitive neurons to both functions, and observed that both functions fit the neural curves well in terms of goodness-of-fit criteria (Text S4). We must not forget, however, two important differences between and .
First, since reveals a minimum shortly before (Text S6) and derives from , the -response is more precisely biphasic. The biphasic structure gets pronounced in some of the curve fits, especially when is close to (see corresponding figures in Text S4). Then, the amplitude of the -maximum is small, and consequently the fitting algorithm has to scale it to the maximum of the neuronal recording data. In this way, the minimum is also scaled.
Second, depends in a nonlinear way on the size-to-velocity ratio (see Figure 4 for an illustration). This is contradictory to several studies that found a linear dependence. A linear dependence is also predicted by the -function (equation S5 in Text S2). The contradiction can be alleviated by adding noise to relative time of the -maximum (; equation S10 in Text S2), with noise amplitudes as reported in [26]. As a consequence of noise, the nonlinear relationship can be literally hidden (Figure 3), such that statistical tests would affirm an underlying linear process (Text S2). Masking by noise is more effective for bigger values of , because the noise level is proportional to .
The -function in its original form cannot explain the neuronal response curves for an approach with (“linear approach”) [25]: Rather than predicting a decreasing response with time, the -function would linearly increase. In contrast, the -function makes correct predictions. Correct predictions with can nevertheless be made by including an additional inhibitory process in the firing rate equation of (equation S3 in Text S1, where a full proof of concept is described). Important, this extension of (i) is based on a power function with an exponent between and , but not on an exponential function as with , and in this regard it may hence be considered as being biophysically more plausible than (see also reference [36]); (ii) does not interfere with the “normal” behavior (i.e. normal object approaches are not affected); and (iii) tolerates high noise levels (i.e., the mechanism is robust).
What about alternative models which also have a response peak? In Text S6 we studied two such functions, namely “inverse ” (), and angular acceleration (). Both of them reveal a linear dependence of on (equations S24 & S26, respectively, in Text S6). The maximum of always precedes that of . However, does not make correct predictions for the “linear approach”, as we would obtain ab initio for (although a dynamical version may predict the decreasing LGMD-activity on the basis of temporal filtering effects).
In contrast, would make consistent predictions in that case. Without further modifications, though, neither nor seems to be adequate for fitting the response curves of collision sensitive neurons, because there is no free model parameter to shift their respective maximum. Although the occurrence of their maxima could principally be controlled by a global shift of the time scale , the corresponding values (obtained by fitting the neuronal response curves) would overestimate experimental values (Text S6). Similarly, when “fitting” the -function to and the so obtained values of would underestimate experimental values: The -maximum would coincide with the maximum of for , and with the maximum of for .
In conclusion, is no replacement for the -function, at least for describing neuronal responses of collision sensitive neurons in insects. However, in the nucleus rotundus of pigeons three classes of neurons were reported [1], [38]. They conform to -like, -like, and -like responses. The fact that is just a special case of could possibly explain why neurons with -like and -like properties can be found in a single brain. Within the -framework, the function corresponds to , and is obtained for choosing . Thus, the adjustment of only a single weight () is necessary to go from one function to the other. The corrected m-Tau -framework could thus offer a parsimonious yet full-fledged explanation of the implementation of -like and -like neurons at the circuit level.
Methods
Psychophysical experiment
Subjects
Four subjects that were members of the Basic Psychology Department of the University of Barcelona participated in the experiment. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive with respect to the aims of the experiment. Two of the subjects were well-trained psychophysical subjects in similar tasks. None of the subjects was stereo blind (StereoFly test, Stereo Optical Co.). They all signed an informed consent. The psychophysical experiment was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty for Psychology of the University of Barcelona, in agreement with the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki in 1954.
Stimuli and apparatus
Stimuli were displayed on a Phillips 22 inch monitor (Brilliance 202P4) at a refresh rate of 118 Hz and a screen resolution of pixels. A 3Dlabs VP870 video card controlled the stereo shutter spectacles (CristalEyes). Simulated targets were uniform disks that moved on a collision trajectory along a line that passed the midpoint between the subjects' eyes. The screen was at one meter distance from subjects' eyes.
Seven time-to-contact values (experimentally fixed values ) were combined with two different object sizes (diameter and ), and five presentation times (), totaling different combinations. In order to ensure that the subjects used the judged time to contact rather than some other correlated measure, we varied the initial simulated starting distances (from to ), and set velocities to .
Procedure
Each simulated object appeared at its initial distance on the monitor. After one second, the object started approaching the observer at the designated constant velocity , and was visible until (presentation time). The reference time was indicated to subjects with an acoustic signal (beep) [16]. The reference time remained unchanged throughout the experiment. Subjects were instructed to press one of two buttons to indicate whether they thought being hit by the object before or after . In each session, the complete set of stimuli was shown to subjects in random order (five repetitions times the combinations). Each subject took part in five sessions. Feedback on incorrect responses was provided after each trial.
Simulation of our psychophysical experiment
We simulated our psychophysical experiment with the corrected m-Tau -model (equation 3), where we plugged in noisified versions of the optical variables (i.e. ),
(9) |
with noise probabilities (), and with the dot denoting the time derivative. The are random variables, which at each instant return a value that is drawn from a centered normal distribution. In the last equations, we used the explicit expression for angular size,
(10) |
and angular velocity (rate of expansion)
(11) |
with and . The values of and are the psychophysical stimulus parameters. Simulations were carried out with a temporal resolution of .
The corrected m-Tau -model is constrained by two limit functions: Ordinary on the one hand (equation 6), and on the other (equation 8). Both limit functions decrease approximately as (illustration: Figure 6). Thus, a ttc estimation at time can be computed as
(12) |
(Nomenclature: is the model prediction for ttc at time , and is the experimentally set parameter). In the psychophysical study, subjects were asked to estimate whether they were hit by the approaching object before or after . We accordingly define their proportion of later responses as the number of trials (where subjects responded with being struck after ) divided by the total number of trials :
(13) |
is represented by circle and triangle symbols in Figure 7 and 8. The corresponding predictions from the model are denoted by . Specifically, with and , and analogous for . Computation of is required for , which we did with equation (12) as per
(14) |
Notice that, due to noise (equation 9), will be subjected to random jitter with each trial . Therefore, in order to obtain a more robust estimate of ttc , we do not use only : The integral in the last equation computes – in the discrete case – the mean value across the last time steps until (typically , what amounts to a time interval for averaging of , cf. first figure in Text S7). In order to illustrate the noise level at each , we also computed the standard deviation of the last values of . The shaded areas in the figures which visualize & correspond to . Predictions of the corrected m-Tau -model are shown as curves in Figure 8, as well as in Text S7.
Parameters of the corrected m-Tau model
The corrected m-Tau -model has eight free parameters: , , , , , , , . The parameter space was parsed with constant step widths. For each set of parameter values , -predictions for the proportion-of-later-response curves were computed according to the procedure described in the previous section. The corresponding “goodness of prediction” (or “prediction performance”) was evaluated with the root mean square error (rmse, ), and the outlier insensitive, robust error (robe, ), see equation S18 in Text S5. The “goodness of prediction” measures are referred to as score-measures (rmse-scores & robe-scores, respectively). Parameter values were sorted according to their scores. In this way we ended up with several score tables, which list the best set of parameters, according to object size: Table S1 in Text S5 for small object diameter (), Table S2 in Text S5 for big object diameter (), & Table S3 in Text S5 for combined object diameter. The scores for the combined size were computed by averaging the scores of big & small for corresponding parameter values, and then sorting the averaged scores in ascending order. More details on parameter finding and analysis are given in Text S5.
Derivation of Equation 2
Consider a rigid sphere (object radius or half-size ) that approaches an observer on a direct collision course. If the approach proceeds at a constant velocity , the object-observer distance at time is . Thus, the initial distance is .
Now, consider the gain control factor from equation (1)
(15) |
where we plug in the explicit expression for angular velocity equation (11) and obtain
(16) |
Especially in the initial phase of the approach, when visual angle and angular velocity are sufficiently small, decreases approximately linearly with time (cf. Text S6),
(17) |
Because of , the m-Tau function becomes approximately
(18) |
A maximum of the m-Tau function implies that its first time derivative is zero. We define . The first time derivative of the (approximate) m-Tau function
(19) |
disappears if , or
(20) |
The last equation is the distance (positive sign) where the approximated m-Tau function attains its maximum during an object approach. Thus, the time when the -maximum occurs is
(21) |
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors like to thank Eric Johnson for his help in proofreading our manuscript.
Funding Statement
MSK acknowledges support from a Ramon & Cajal grant from the Ministry of Science and Innovation of the Spanish government, and from the national grant DPI2010-21513. JLM acknowledges support by grant PSI2010-15867 from the Ministry of Science and Innovation of the Spanish government and an ICREA Academia Distinguished Professorship award. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Sun H, Frost B (1998) Computation of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons. Nat Neurosci 1: 296–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Rind F, Simmons P (1999) Seeing what is coming: building collision-sensitive neurones. Trends Neurosci 22: 215–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Yonas A, Bechthold A, Frankel D, Gordon F, McRoberts G, et al. (1977) Development of sensitivity to information for impending collision. Percept Psychophys 21: 97–104. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Schiff W, Caviness J, Gibson J (1962) Persistent fear responses in rhesus monkeys to optical stimulus of looming. Science 136: 982–983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Rind F, Santer D (2004) Collision avoidance and a looming sensitive neuron: size matters but biggest is not necessarily best. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 271: S27–S29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Robertson R, Johnson A (1993) Collision avoidance of ying locusts: steering torques and behaviour. J Exp Biol 183: 35–60. [Google Scholar]
- 7. López-Moliner J, Field D, Wann J (2007) Interceptive timing: Prior knowledge matters. J Vis 7: 11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Fotowat H, Gabbiani F (2011) Collision detection as a model for sensory-motor integration. Annu Rev Neurosci 34: 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hoyle F (1957) The Black Cloud. London: Pinguin Books. 26–29 pp.
- 10. Lee D (1976) A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5: 437–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lee D, Reddish P (1981) Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics. Nature 293: 293–294. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Regan D, Hamstra S (1993) Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion. Vision Res 33: 447–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Wann J (1979) Anticipating arrival: is the tau-margin a specious theory? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22: 1031–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Tresilian J (1999) Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? Trends Cogn Sci 3: 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Regan D, Beverley K (1979) Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: changing disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage. Vision Res 19: 1331–1342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Gray R, Regan D (1998) Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information. Vision Res 38: 499–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Rushton S, Wann J (1999) Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing ball catch. Nat Neurosci 2: 186–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Regan D, Gray R (2000) Visually guided collision avoidance and collision achievement. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 99–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Regan D, Beverley K (1973) Some dynamic features of depth perception. Vision Res 13: 2369–2379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. McKee S, Watamaniuk S, Harris J, Smallman H, Taylor D (1997) Is stereopsis e_ective in breaking camouage for moving targets? Vision Res 37: 2047–2055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Tresilian J (1994) Approximation of information sources and perceptual variables in interceptive timing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20: 154–173. [Google Scholar]
- 22. DeLucia P, Warren R (1994) Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20: 783–798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Judge S, Rind F (1997) The locust DCMD, a movement-detecting neurone tightly tuned to collision trajectories. J Exp Biol 200: 2209–2216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Rind F, Simmons P (1997) Signaling of object approach by the DCMD neuron of the locust. J Neurophysiol 77: 1029–1033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Hatsopoulos N, Gabbiani F, Laurent G (1995) Elementary computation of object approach by a wide-field visual neuron. Science 270: 1000–1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Gabbiani F, Krapp H, Laurent G (1999) Computation of object approach by a wide-field, motion sensitive neuron. J Neurosci 19: 1122–1141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Fotowat H, Harrison R, Gabbiani F (2011) Multiplexing of motor information in the discharge of a collision detecting neuron during escape behaviors. Neuron 69: 147–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. O'Shea M, Rowell C (1975) A spike-transmitting electrical synapse between visual interneurons in the locust movement detector system. J Comp Physiol A 97: 143–158. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Rind F (1984) A chemical synapse between two motion detecting neurones in the locust brain. J Exp Biol 110: 143–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Fotowat H, Gabbiani F (2007) Relationship between the phases of sensory and motor activity during a looming-evoked multistage escape behavior. J Neurosci 27: 10047–10059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Fotowat H, Fayyazuddin A, Bellen H, Gabbiani F (2009) A novel neuronal pathway for visually guided escape in drosophila melanogaster. J Neurophysiol 102: 875–885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Nakagawa H, Hongjian K (2010) Collision-sensitive neurons in the optic tectum of the bullfrog, rana catesbeiana. J Neurophysiol 104: 2487–2499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Preuss T, Osei-Bonsu P, Weiss S, Wang C, Faber D (2006) Neural representation of object approach in a decision-making motor circuit. J Neurosci 26: 3454–3464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Gabbiani F, Krapp H, Koch C, Laurent G (2002) Multiplicative computation in a visual neuron sensitive to looming. Nature 420: 320–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Grossberg S, Repin D (2003) A neural model of how the brain represents and compares multi-digit numbers: spatial and categorical processes. Neural Netw 16: 1107–1140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Keil M (2011) Emergence of multiplication in a biophysical model of a wide-field visual neuron for computing object approaches: Dynamics, peaks, & fits. Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS) foundation. Available: http://books.nips.cc. http://arxiv.org/abs/1110.0433.
- 37. Harris C, Wolpert D (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394: 780–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Frost B, Sun H (2003) The biological bases of time-to-collision computation. In: Hecht H, JPSavelsbergh G, editors, Time-to-Contact (Advances in Psychology Series). Amsterdam - North Holland: Elsevier. pp. 13–38.
- 39. Gabbiani F, Mo C, Laurent G (2001) Invariance of angular threshold computation in a wide-field looming-sensitive neuron. J Neurosci 21: 314–329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.