Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2012 Aug 27;3:295. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00295

Resting State Networks and Consciousness

Alterations of Multiple Resting State Network Connectivity in Physiological, Pharmacological, and Pathological Consciousness States

Lizette Heine 1, Andrea Soddu 1, Francisco Gómez 1, Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse 1, Luaba Tshibanda 2, Marie Thonnard 1, Vanessa Charland-Verville 1, Murielle Kirsch 3, Steven Laureys 1, Athena Demertzi 1,*
PMCID: PMC3427917  PMID: 22969735

Abstract

In order to better understand the functional contribution of resting state activity to conscious cognition, we aimed to review increases and decreases in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) functional connectivity under physiological (sleep), pharmacological (anesthesia), and pathological altered states of consciousness, such as brain death, coma, vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, and minimally conscious state. The reviewed resting state networks were the DMN, left and right executive control, salience, sensorimotor, auditory, and visual networks. We highlight some methodological issues concerning resting state analyses in severely injured brains mainly in terms of hypothesis-driven seed-based correlation analysis and data-driven independent components analysis approaches. Finally, we attempt to contextualize our discussion within theoretical frameworks of conscious processes. We think that this “lesion” approach allows us to better determine the necessary conditions under which normal conscious cognition takes place. At the clinical level, we acknowledge the technical merits of the resting state paradigm. Indeed, fast and easy acquisitions are preferable to activation paradigms in clinical populations. Finally, we emphasize the need to validate the diagnostic and prognostic value of fMRI resting state measurements in non-communicating brain damaged patients.

Keywords: default mode network, resting state networks, consciousness, sleep, anesthesia, coma, hypnosis

Introduction

In the past decades, neuroimaging research has been focusing on studying brain function in “resting” conditions, when subjects receive no external stimulation. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) resting state connectivity studies stress that the brain at rest is characterized by coherent fluctuations in the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal. These BOLD fluctuations can be detected in the low frequency range (<0.1 Hz; Cordes et al., 2001), they are distinct from respiratory and cardiovascular signal contribution (De Luca et al., 2006) and organize the brain in large-scale cerebral networks (Damoiseaux et al., 2006). The most widely studied resting state network (RSN) is the default mode network (DMN), encompassing precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), mesiofrontal/anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and temporoparietal junction areas (Figure 1). This network of areas was initially identified in positron emission tomography (PET) studies as regions less active when performance on cognitive tasks was compared to resting control condition, such as eye fixation or eyes closed (Shulman et al., 1997; Mazoyer et al., 2001). Later, the DMN was also identified in fMRI and in terms of cognitive function, its activity has been linked to self-related and internal processes, such as stimulus-independent thoughts (McKiernan et al., 2006), mind-wandering (Mason et al., 2007), social cognition (Schilbach et al., 2008), introspection (Goldberg et al., 2006), monitoring of the “mental self” (Lou et al., 2004), and integration of cognitive processes (Greicius et al., 2003). Interestingly, areas of the DMN can be assigned to specific cognitive functions, for example the PCC seems to be important in autobiographical memory while the frontal areas may be important for self-reference (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2011). In that respect, resting state acquisitions can, at least to a certain degree, be informative of cognitive function. Importantly for clinical studies, the resting state paradigm is particularly appealing because it does not require sophisticated experimental setup to administer external stimuli and surpasses the need for patients’ contribution (e.g., language comprehension and/or production or motor responses; Soddu et al., 2011). Hence, resting state protocols are a suitable means to study clinical populations, in which communication cannot be established at the bedside, such as patients with disorders of consciousness [e.g., coma, “vegetative state (VS)”/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS), minimally conscious state (MCS)]. It has been suggested that resting state analyses can be used in a clinical setting to identify group differences, to obtain patient-specific diagnostic and prognostic information, to perform longitudinal studies and monitor treatment effects, to cluster heterogeneous diseases such as schizophrenia or even to guide treatments, such as surgical interventions (Fox and Greicius, 2010).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Multiple cerebral networks can be identified with fMRI in healthy controls (n = 10) during normal wakeful resting state using independent component analysis. These networks reflect “higher-order” cognitive (i.e., default mode, left and right executive control, salience networks), and “lower-order” sensorimotor, and sensory (auditory, visual) function. For illustrative purposes, group-level spatial maps (z values) are rendered on a structural T1 magnetic resonance template and x, y, and z values indicate the Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates of the represented sections.

With an aim to better determine the functional role of resting state activity in healthy conditions and to further comprehend its contribution to clinical states, the present review will adopt a “lesion” approach. Indeed, patients’ neurological data can give us information about the functional role of the resting state activity to consciousness. We will review changes in functional connectivity in the DMN under physiological (sleep, hypnosis), pharmacological (sedation, anesthesia), and pathological (coma-related states) alteration of consciousness. The functional contribution of the anticorrelated activity between DMN and the “extrinsic” system to (un)conscious states will also be discussed. We will further focus on functional connectivity changes in multiple RSNs, such as the bilateral executive control, salience, sensorimotor, auditory, and visual networks (Beckmann et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; De Luca et al., 2006; Fox and Raichle, 2007; Smith et al., 2009). With regards to resting state assessments of severely brain-injured patients, we will highlight some methodological issues mainly in terms of hypothesis-driven seed-based correlation analysis and data-driven independent components analysis. Finally, we will attempt to contextualize our discussion within theoretical frameworks around conscious processes.

(Un)Conscious States and Resting State Default Mode Network Activity

To date, there is no universal definition for consciousness covering all its essential characteristics (Zeman, 2001). Here, we define consciousness in an operational way based on clinical practice, stressing that consciousness can be reduced to two components, arousal and awareness (Posner et al., 2007). Arousal refers to the level of alertness and it is clinically evidenced by eyes opening. Awareness refers to the content of consciousness and it is clinically evidenced by command following or by observing non-reflex motor behavior, such as eye tracking and localized responses to pain (Posner et al., 2007). Sleep is the best example to describe the relationship between these two components: the drowsier we become as we move toward deep sleep, the less aware we get of our surroundings and ourselves (a notorious exception is the oneiric activity during rapid eye movement sleep during which we remain behaviorally unconscious; Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002). Based on this definition, subjects in pathological and pharmacological coma (i.e., anesthesia) are not conscious because they are not awake (American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Intraoperative Awareness, 2006). Similarly, under sedation (a drug-dose dependent impairment of consciousness) and hypnotic state (a suggestion-dependent alteration of conscious experience; The Executive Committee of the American Psychological Association – Division of Psychological Hypnosis, 1994) subjects report an altered state of awareness as they move toward lower wakefulness levels. A unique dissociation between arousal and awareness is observed in patients in a VS (also called UWS; Laureys et al., 2010) who recover wakefulness but their motor responses are merely reflexive and, hence, not indicative of conscious awareness (Laureys et al., 2005). Patients in VS/UWS should not be mistaken with patients in a MCS. Patients in MCS, although unable to functionally communicate with their environment, do show fluctuating remnants of willful behavior (Giacino et al., 2002). Based on the level of their purposeful behavioral repertoire, MCS patients were recently subcategorized as MCS+ (i.e., showing command following,) and MCS− (i.e., showing visual pursuit, localization of noxious stimulation, or non-contingent behaviors, such as appropriate smiling or crying to emotional stimuli; Bruno et al., 2011). This kind of clinical distinction highlights the importance of motor output to the evaluation of consciousness. Patients with a locked-in syndrome (LIS), however, have no means of producing speech, limb, or facial movements but still are awake and conscious (Posner et al., 2007). Evidently, by solely measuring motor responses, these patients can be mistaken for unconscious (Laureys et al., 2005). Similarly, consciousness in patients with aphasia can be underestimated if the clinician does not account for such deficit. As a consequence, valid motor- and language-independent assessment of residual brain function in non-communicating patients is of both clinical and ethical importance.

The resting state paradigm surpasses the requirement for motor output or language comprehension. To date, neuroimaging protocols investigating connectivity of the DMN during resting state are not conclusive as to its exact functional role. Nevertheless, resting state fMRI studies suggest that activity of this network is generally reduced as a function of the level of consciousness (Table 1). For example, it has been shown that with the advancement of sleep, connectivity between the frontal and posterior parts of the DMN decreases yet persists (Horovitz et al., 2009). Decreases in functional connectivity were also observed in PCC of the DMN under pharmacological unconsciousness with propofol (Boveroux et al., 2010; Schrouff et al., 2011) and sevoflurane (Martuzzi et al., 2010). Importantly for clinical populations, connectivity in the PCC was shown to be indistinguishable between controls and LIS patients, relatively preserved in MCS, significantly reduced in VS/UWS patients (Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2010) and could not be identified in brain death (i.e., irreversible coma with absent brainstem reflexes; Boly et al., 2009). Similarly during a passive auditory task, DMN deactivations, which are thought to interrupt ongoing introspective processes, showed a reduction in MCS whereas VS/UWS patients did not show such task-induced deactivations (Crone et al., 2011). These studies suggest that DMN functional connectivity correlates, at least partially, with the level of ongoing conscious cognition. This is in agreement with functional connectivity studies on intermediate states of awareness. For example, in hypnotic state there is only relative (Demertzi et al., 2011b) or no connectivity decreases in the DMN (McGeown et al., 2009). Similarly, during moderate sedation, little (Greicius et al., 2008) or no changes (Stamatakis et al., 2010) in DMN connectivity have been observed. During light sleep there is no change (Horovitz et al., 2008; Larson-Prior et al., 2009). Nevertheless, in deep sleep brain activity shows increased modularity, which hinders the brain to integrate information and therefore might account for decreased consciousness during dreamless sleep (Boly et al., 2012).

Table 1.

FMRI studies showing alterations in resting state functional connectivity of multiple networks in physiological (sleep, hypnosis), pharmacological (sedation), and pathological states of unconsciousness.

N Functional connectivity change Method Study
DMN Light sleep 14 Connectivity persists Seed-based Horovitz et al. (2008)
10 Connectivity persists Seed-based Larson-Prior et al. (2009)
Slow wave sleep 14 ↑: PCC correlation with IPC. Correlation within nodes persistent Seed-based Horovitz et al. (2009)
↓: Correlation PCC with MPFC became absent
25 ↓: PCC, PHG, MPFC ICA Sämann et al. (2011)
Light sedation 16 ↑: PCC and areas outside of the DMN Seed-based Stamatakis et al. (2010)
12 ↓: General deceased connectivity, focal decreases PCC ICA Greicius et al. (2008)
Anesthesia 20 ↓: PCC/precuneus, MPFC, superior frontal sulci, parahippocampal gyrus, and bilateral TPJ Seed-based and ICA Boveroux et al. (2010)
14 ↑: PCC and STG Seed-based Martuzzi et al. (2010)
↓: PCC and adjacent areas
18 ↓: Reduction connectivity within the DMN and between the DMN and other networks ICA Schrouff et al. (2011)
Hypnosis 18 ↓ right middle and superior frontal gyrus Seed-based McGeown et al. (2009)
12 ↑: Middle frontal and bilateral angular gyri ICA Demertzi et al. (2011b)
↓: PCC and bilateral parahippocampal areas
Comatose states 2 ↓: Connectivity is absent in brain dead, decreased PCC, and thalamus connectivity ICA Boly et al. (2009)
Preserved cortico-cortical connectivity
11 ↓: Connections between PCC and MPFC Seed-based and ICA Soddu et al. (2012)
Locked-in patients showed near to normal connectivity
14 ↓: All areas showed less connectivity in disorders of consciousness, decrease of connectivity was negatively correlated with consciousness. PCC most significant decrease ICA Vanhaudenhuyse et al. (2010)
13 Presence of DMN has prognostic value ICA Norton et al. (2012)
Executive control network Light sleep 10 No difference Seed-based Larson-Prior et al. (2009)
Slow wave sleep 25 Correlations within the network persist but decrease ICA Sämann et al. (2011)
Light sedation 20 ↓: Right: middle frontal and posterior parietal cortices. Seed-based and ICA Boveroux et al. (2010)
Left: residual in middle frontal, PCC, and temporo-occipital cortices
Salience Slow wave sleep 14 ↑: Connectivity between insula and left ACC Seed-based Martuzzi et al. (2010)
↓: Decrease between connectivity in the insula and supplementary motor cortex and left middle frontal gyrus
Hypnosis 8 ↑: Increases in mid-insula, primary sensory, and orbitofrontal cortex Seed-based Derbyshire et al. (2004)
Sensorymotor network Light sleep 10 No difference Seed-based Larson-Prior et al. (2009)
Slow wave sleep 14 ↑: Connectivity within the network Seed-based Martuzzi et al. (2010)
Light sedation 12 ↑: Within-network increases ICA Greicius et al. (2008)
Auditory Slow wave sleep 14 No difference Seed-based Martuzzi et al. (2010)
Light sedation 20 No difference Seed-based and ICA Boveroux et al. (2010)
Visual Light sleep 10 No difference Seed-based Larson-Prior et al. (2009)
Light sedation 14 ↑: Primary visual area with the cuneus and lingual gyrus Seed-based Martuzzi et al. (2010)
Anesthesia 20 No difference Seed-based and ICA Boveroux et al. (2010)

Upper arrow denotes increases in functional connectivity; lower arrow denotes decreases in functional connectivity. (DMN, default mode network; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; IPC, inferior parietal cortex; MPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; TPJ, temporoparietal junction; STG, superior temporal gyrus, ICA, independent component analysis).

Taken together, changes in the DMN functional connectivity in altered consciousness states could suggest modified self-related conscious mentation. Indeed, it has been suggested that in normal waking conditions, resting state activity in the posterior cingulate and frontal areas accounts for self-referential thoughts (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2011). Therefore, it could be inferred that decreased connectivity in the DMN reflects restricted abilities for self-referential processing, like in patients with disorders of consciousness. One should keep in mind, though, that our limited understanding of the dynamic neural complexity underlying consciousness and its resistance to quantification in the absence of communication make it difficult to establish strong claims about self-consciousness in non-communicating subjects.

DMN functional anticorrelations

Since the early studies of resting state, it was suggested that the brain’s baseline activity can be organized in two brain networks showing anticorrelated activity to each other: an “intrinsic” and an “extrinsic” network (Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005; Golland et al., 2007; Tian et al., 2007). The “intrinsic” network coincides with the DMN and is involved in the same cognitive processes as the DMN. The “extrinsic” system encompasses lateral frontoparietal areas resembling the brain activations during goal-directed behavior and it has been linked to cognitive processes of external sensory input, such as somatosensory (e.g., Boly et al., 2007), visual (e.g., Dehaene and Changeux, 2005), and auditory (e.g., Brunetti et al., 2008). Previous studies showed that these two systems are of a competing character in the sense that they can disturb or even interrupt each other (e.g., Tian et al., 2007). Such anticorrelated pattern is also illustrated in activation studies on motor performance (Fox et al., 2007), perceptual discrimination (Sapir et al., 2005), attentional lapses (Weissman et al., 2006), and somatosensory perception of stimuli close to somatosensory threshold (Boly et al., 2007). We recently determined the cognitive-behavioral counterpart of such “resting state” activity and showed that activity in the DMN corresponded to behavioral reports of “internal” awareness (i.e., self-related thoughts). Conversely, subjective ratings for “external” awareness (i.e., perception of the environment through the senses) correlated with the activity of an “extrinsic” system (encompassing lateral frontoparietal cortices; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2011). These findings depict that the anticorrelated pattern between DMN and the extrinsic system is of functional significance to conscious cognition. With an aim to further characterize the role of these two systems to subjective awareness, we sought to modulate their relationship by means of hypnosis. We found that, as compared to a control condition of autobiographical mental imagery, there was a hypnosis-related reduction in connectivity in the “extrinsic” system, reflecting a decreased sensory or perceptual awareness. Interestingly, this modulated activity was paralleled to subjective reports of increased sense of dissociation from the environment and reduced intensity of “external thoughts” (Demertzi et al., 2011b).

Taken together these data indicate that DMN and anticorrelated extrinsic system activity underlies (at least partially) conscious ongoing mentation. It should be mentioned that fMRI anticorrelations were previously subject to debate in the literature. It has been argued, for instance, that fMRI functional anticorrelations are nothing more than noise in the signal due to regression of the brain’s global activity during data preprocessing (Anderson et al., 2010). Other data, however, suggest that the anticorrelations persist both with and without global signal regression, suggesting some underlying biological origins for this anticorrelated pattern (Fox et al., 2009; Chai et al., 2012). We would agree with the latter evidence which is supported by studies in unconscious conditions, such as anesthesia (Boveroux et al., 2010; Figure 2), sleep (Sämann et al., 2011), and in unresponsive patients (Boly et al., 2009) where these anticorrelations generally reduce or even disappear, accounting for their functional contribution to conscious cognition.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Spontaneous fMRI BOLD activity in the default mode network (in blue; considered to reflect self-related mentation) anticorrelates with the activity of a lateral frontoparietal system (in red; considered to mediate conscious perception of the external world). Here, this anticorrelated activity is shown for normal wakefulness, hypnotic state, and during deep anesthesia. Of note is the absence of the activity in the “extrinsic” frontoparietal system in the two conditions of altered sense of awareness (hypnosis, anesthesia) which is considered as suggestive of a diminished “external” awareness (i.e., the perception of the environment through the senses). Statistical maps are thresholded at a false discovery error rate p < 0.05 and rendered on a structural T1 magnetic resonance image of a healthy subject (x and z values indicate Talairach coordinates of the represented sections).

Beyond the DMN: Resting State Activity in Multiple Cerebral Networks

Importantly, the different functions of a brain region cannot be understood in isolation, meaning in terms of functional segregation, but only in conjunction with regions it interacts with, that is in terms of functional integration (Seghier et al., 2010). Therefore, we further focus our review on other “large-scale higher-order” (bilateral executive control and salience networks) and sensorimotor-sensory (auditory, visual) RSNs, which can be consistently identified in healthy conditions (Figure 2).

The executive control network during normal wakefulness encompasses bilateral middle, inferior and superior frontal cortices, bilateral inferior parietal lobes, ACC/supplementary motor area (SMA), and bilateral insular cortices (Figure 1). Resting state independent components analysis identified this network in a lateralized manner. The left executive control network is thought to be more involved in cognitive and “language” paradigms while the right executive control network relates to perceptual, somesthetic, and nociception processing (Smith et al., 2009; Laird et al., 2011). Activity in both these two networks is reduced during deep sleep (Sämann et al., 2011) and anesthesia (Boveroux et al., 2010) whereas light sleep does not seem to mediate functional connectivity in these networks (Larson-Prior et al., 2009; Table 1). Taken together, these results highlight the involvement of the executive control networks in the perception of the external world, in line with previous suggestion that activity of these areas is a necessary condition for conscious (i.e., reportable) visual perception (Dehaene et al., 2003).

The salience network encompasses fronto-insular and ACCs (Figure 1) with connections to subcortical and limbic structures. In normal conditions, this network is implicated in the orientation toward salient emotional stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007), conflict monitoring, information integration, and response selection (Cole and Schneider, 2007; Roberts and Hall, 2008). It has been proposed that the salience network enables the switch between internal attention (the default mode) and task-related states (Menon and Uddin, 2010). The salience network has also been linked to pain-related processes both during acute stimulus-induced pain (Tracey and Mantyh, 2007), during resting state while anticipating pain (Ploner et al., 2010; Wiech et al., 2010), and after hypnotic suggestions for creating pain experiences in the absence of a noxious stimulus (Derbyshire et al., 2004). Under light sevoflurane sedation, increased connectivity between the ACC and the insula was observed, although connectivity between the insula and the secondary somatosensory cortex was reduced (Martuzzi et al., 2010). Analysis of the salience network in comatose states could be beneficial for the study of pain and possible suffering in these patients in the absence of external stimulation. Indeed, such stimulations are not always feasible due to sophisticated setups or due to patients’ clinical picture. Hence, salience network resting state analysis could shed light on the cerebral substrate that could account for patients’ orientation to salient stimuli, including painful ones.

The sensorimotor network resembles the activations seen in motor tasks (Biswal et al., 1995). In normal wakefulness it encompasses the SMA/midcingulate cortex, bilateral primary motor cortex, and bilateral middle frontal gyri (Biswal et al., 1995; Greicius et al., 2008; Figure 1). During light sedation the sensorimotor network shows increases in functional connectivity (Greicius et al., 2008; Martuzzi et al., 2010). To date, the above networks have not been further investigated under other unconscious states.

The auditory network, important in audition, such as tone/pitch discrimination, music, and speech (Laird et al., 2011) in normal wakefulness, encompasses primary and secondary auditory cortices, including Heschl’s gyrus, bilateral superior temporal gyri, and posterior insular cortex (Figure 1). During normal wakefulness, resting state independent component analysis (ICA) also identifies the visual network in three independent components (Figure 1). One network, the lateral visual network includes the middle temporal visual association area at the temporo-occipital junction and is most important in complex (emotional) stimuli (Laird et al., 2011). The other networks include medial and occipital visual networks, important in simple visual (e.g., a flickering checkerboard), and higher-order visual stimuli (e.g., orthography), respectively (Beckmann et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Allen et al., 2011; Laird et al., 2011). No difference in connectivity was identified between both these primary auditory and visual sensory networks and light sleep (Larson-Prior et al., 2009), or between awake and sedation (Boveroux et al., 2010; Martuzzi et al., 2010). One study showed increased temporal synchrony in auditory and visual areas in light midazolam sedation (Kiviniemi et al., 2005). The visual cortex has been shown to possess higher amplitude of BOLD fluctuations when asleep (Fukunaga et al., 2006). This indicates that resting state activity continues in these areas during sleep, and thus transcends consciousness. Finally, reliably indicated as possessing functional connectivity is the cerebellum. This network is associated with action and somesthesis (Laird et al., 2011), but not yet thoroughly studied in altered states of consciousness.

Analyzing Resting State Data from Pathological Brains: Methodological Issues

The clinical neuro-investigation of severely brain-injured patients with the resting state paradigm is technically easier compared to activation (Schiff et al., 2005) or “active” mental imagery protocols (e.g., Monti et al., 2010). This is because patients do not have to perform any task, and such data can have faster translation into clinical practice (Soddu et al., 2011). Depending on the adopted methodology, several issues need to be taken into account when analyzing resting state acquisitions from clinical populations. To date, two main approaches are employed; hypothesis-driven seed-voxel correlation analysis and data-driven ICA (see Table 1 for the adopted approach by each reviewed study). Each method has its own advantages, yet their methodological difficulties, especially in non-collaborative patients, which merit to be acknowledged.

Hypothesis-driven method: Seed-based correlation analysis

The seed-voxel approach uses extracted BOLD time course from a region of interest and determines the temporal correlation between this signal (the seed) and the time course from all other brain voxels (Fox et al., 2005). This creates a whole-brain voxel-wise functional connectivity map of covariance with the seed region. It is the most straightforward method to analyze functional connectivity of a particular brain region. The method gives direct answers to specific hypotheses about functional connectivity of that region. It is attractive and elegant for many researchers as the data can be interpreted relatively easily when a well-defined seed area is used. When applying this approach to the study of resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness, several controversial issues arise. A first general issue concerns regressing out the global activity from the BOLD signal, which might induce spurious anticorrelations (Fox et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2009). However, in the case of brain death (a condition where the brain totally lacks neuronal activity and arterial blood flow), this type of regression is an important step to obtain the obvious zero connectivity in this condition (Boly et al., 2009). Alternatively, a non-zero BOLD signal measured in brain death can be taken to be artifactual, contaminated by head motion or heart beating (Soddu et al., 2011). Next, patients with severe brain injuries may suffer from structural deformations resulting from traumatic brain injury and focal hemorrhages. Additionally, patients with severe chronic brain injuries usually develop atrophy and secondary hydrocephalus (i.e., ex vacuo dilation of the ventricles). This implies that even if a statistical structural normalization procedure has been performed, the selection of a proper seed region can become difficult and will require visual inspection by an expert eye. This issue adds to the already intrinsic challenges of an priori selection of the seed region which, in principle, can lead to as many possible overlapping networks as the number of possible seeds (Cole et al., 2010). Using seed-based analysis, other noisy confounds might be influencing the data (e.g., head motion, vascular activity, scanner artifacts). To reduce such noise, the BOLD signal can be preprocessed by regressing out head motion curves as well as ventricular and white matter signal, and each of their first-order derivative terms (Fox et al., 2005). Finally, as for all group-level analyses, one has to take into account the between-subject variability, such as cortical folding or functional localization between individuals or groups (Cole et al., 2010) which can be extremely challenging in severely deformed brains.

Data-driven method: Independent component analysis

Data-driven methods are used to analyze whole-brain connectivity patterns without the need of a priori seed regions. ICA is the most widely used methodology with high level of consistency in results within subjects (van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010). ICA divides an entire dataset into different maximally statistical independent components and thus is able to isolate cortical connectivity maps from non-neural signals (Beckmann et al., 2005). Spontaneous activity is therefore automatically separated from noise, such as head motion or physiological confounds (e.g., cardiac pulsation, respiratory, and slow changes in the depth and rate of breathing; Beckmann and Smith, 2004). This method has the advantage that it can evaluate and compare the coherence of activity in multiple distributed voxels (Cole et al., 2010). The advantage is that it divides different RSNs into different components. However, ICA does not provide any classification or ordering of the independent components. It is therefore perceived as more difficult to understand due to the complex representation of the data. The most straightforward method for labeling the components is by visual inspection, but this lacks reproducibility and could be hard to perform in cases with a large component dimensionality. Alternatively, an automatic selection is preferable but the way to choose the right independent component remains a delicate issue. By merely performing a spatial similarity test with a predefined template has been shown not to be successful for choosing the right component (Soddu et al., 2012). Some automatic approaches for component selection have been proposed, based on template matching using the “goodness of fit” as an outcome index. However, these methods have to be interpreted with care especially in cases of deformed brains as in patients with a traumatic brain injury or comatose state. It was recently proposed that when selecting independent components in patients populations, spatial, temporal, and a “compromise” between spatial and temporal properties of the network of interest need to be met (Soddu et al., 2012). For example, a component can be erroneously selected as the RSN of interest if the selection is based on the spatial pattern ignoring the properties in the time domain (Figure 3, bottom right panel). Additionally, the determination of the proper dimensionality (i.e., the “right” number of estimated components) remains unclear. Extracting many components can result in the spatial segregation of the network of interest into multiple sub-networks (Smith et al., 2009). It was shown, for example, that the use of 75 components can reduce the DMN into four components and the sensorimotor network in six (Allen et al., 2011). When applying ICA in pathological brains it is probably more useful not to select a large quantity of components, because high component dimensionality can further reduce the chances of identifying a network due to decrease in spatial pattern and spectral properties (Tohka et al., 2008).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The challenge of selecting the “right” independent component as the resting state network of interest in pathological conditions. The figure illustrates the spatial pattern (brain maps, z values 0.8–10) and spatial-temporal properties (fingerprints: a representation of the component in a multidimensional space of parameters; De Martino et al., 2007) of the default mode network in healthy consciousness states (healthy subject, patient with locked-in syndrome; upper row) and in two patients with vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (VS/UWS; lower row). For the healthy control, the locked-in syndrome and the VS/UWS patient in the lower left corner, the default mode network shows the characteristic properties in both the spatial and the temporal domain (i.e., the fingerprints pick in the 0.02–0.05 Hz frequency band labeled with the number 9) even if for the VS/UWS patient the spatial pattern is only partially preserved. Of note is that the second VS/UWS patient exhibits the spatial pattern of the default mode network but importantly the time course of this component is characterized by high frequency fluctuations, in the 0.1–0.25 Hz frequency band and high spatial entropy (labeled, respectively, with the number 11 and 4 in the fingerprint). Therefore, such activity cannot be considered of neuronal origin. As a consequence, if the component selection was merely based on a spatial similarity test (e.g., with a predefined template), then this component could be erroneously selected and further statistically analyzed. A “compromise” in the selection of the appropriate network of interest in the space and time domain is needed to will eventually exclude non-neuronal contributions [Fingerprint labels: (1) degree of clustering, (2) skewness, (3) kurtosis, (4) spatial entropy, (5) autocorrelation, (6) temporal entropy, power: (7) 0–0.008 Hz, (8) 0.008–0.02 Hz, (9) 0.02–0.05 Hz, (10) 0.05–0.1 Hz, (11) 0.1–0.25 Hz].

Other techniques to analyze resting state data exist such as methods that focus on the (fractional) amplitude of low frequency fluctuations [(f)ALFF; Zang et al., 2007; Zuo et al., 2010), or on the small world characteristics using correlations and graph analysis (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Zalesky et al., 2012].

Conclusions and Perspectives

The default mode network is the most widely studied network in the resting state literature and has been linked to self-related processes. To date, fMRI resting state studies show that DMN connectivity is reduced under altered states of consciousness, such as sleep, sedation/anesthesia, hypnotic state, and clinical states of disorders of consciousness (VS/UWS, MCS, coma, and brain death). Such connectivity alterations can be discussed in two non-mutually exclusive ways. On one hand, one can refer to these reductions in resting state connectivity during altered conscious states as reflecting reduced capacities for (conscious) cognitive processing (e.g., Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2010). On the other hand, we can equally talk about persistent (albeit reduced) functional connectivity pattern in unconscious states, which transcends the level of consciousness, and which is considered as a physiologic baseline (e.g., Raichle et al., 2001). In any case, it seems that the purposes and questions of each study will eventually determine how such alterations can be further discussed and interpreted. Both the scientific and clinical implications for cognition seem to be the essence of resting state connectivity measurements.

At the scientific level, resting state analyses shed light on the necessary conditions needed for conscious awareness to take place. In other words, in the absence of external stimulation, resting state functional connectivity paradigms could quantify the minimal prerequisites under which cognitive processes can become “conscious.” This could mean that in the presence of an adequate neural substrate (i.e., the RSN), one could infer preserved capacities for conscious cognition. Of course the absence of functional connectivity cannot be taken as a proof for incapacity for conscious awareness. Indeed, it has been suggested that functional connections are best recruited after external stimulation (Honey et al., 2009). In any case, the sufficiency of the RSNs integrity to consciousness remains to be further determined with studies measuring effective connectivity (Churchland, 2007).

In summary, we here reviewed studies in resting state fMRI connectivity of “higher-order” associative cerebral networks (default mode, right and left executive control, and salience) and “lower-level” sensory (auditory and visual) and sensorimotor networks under various altered states of consciousness. As previously proposed, in order for humans to be conscious of something, incoming information (via sensory networks) needs to be made globally available to multiple brain systems via long-range neurons associative networks (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011). Here, the reviewed studies suggest that resting state connectivity is preserved but altered in most RSNs under physiological and pharmacological states, impeding information integration. It should be noted here that it was not among our aims to exhaustively review all spectrum of altered states of consciousness. Much research has been conducted in states of altered sense of awareness, such as in neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g., dementias and schizophrenia; for a review see Buckner et al., 2008), meditation (Brewer et al., 2011; Josipovic et al., 2012), and drug-related states such as alcohol (Esposito et al., 2010), amphetamine (Roberts and Garavan, 2010), or psychedelic drugs (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012) which in general show changes in the connection between the posterior cingulate and frontal areas. Resting state investigations have also been attempted using other modalities, such as electroencephalography (e.g., Lehembre et al., 2012). Rather, we here focused on RSNs obtained using fMRI. We reviewed changes in functional connectivity as a function of various states of wakefulness. This aim lies within our ultimate clinical goal to better document, manage and predict residual brain functioning of patients with disorders of consciousness. As these patients are incapable of functional communication with their environment, they might be wrongly diagnosed as unconscious when locked-in (Laureys et al., 2005) or when suffering from aphasia (Majerus et al., 2009). The ethical implications of erroneous diagnostics are apparent, especially when pain (Demertzi et al., 2009, 2012) and end-of-life issues (Demertzi et al., 2011a) are discussed.

At the clinical level, the study of resting state activity in pathological states of consciousness can become demanding due to both clinical and methodological issues. For example, patients who show increased prescan motion activity will need to be anesthetized to reduce the noise during data acquisition. Apart from the clinical issue of applying anesthetics to these vulnerable patients, the effect of anesthesia will need to be accounted for in the acquired data. This is added to the methodological challenge of the spatial normalization of severely deformed brains (Shen et al., 2007). Additionally, identified resting state connectivity patterns need to be interpreted according to the studied population. In brain death, for instance, it was shown that resting state fMRI activity is absent in line with the clinical neurological criteria for the diagnosis of death (Boly et al., 2009). Therefore, in cases where resting state activity, in the DMN for example, is identified, such findings can be pertained to motion and other artifacts, not indicative of neuronal activity (Soddu et al., 2011). The characterization of the fMRI functional connectivity of other RSNs in comatose states remains to be further elucidated. It can be expected, though, that in such severely constrained situations, like in disorders of consciousness, the functional integrity of most RSNs is considerably restricted accounting for patients’ limited capacities for conscious cognition.

Despite intrinsic limitations, resting state data are technically easier to obtain in patients’ population, as compared to auditory (Schiff et al., 2005) or visual (Monti et al., 2012) activation protocols or “active” mental imagery protocols (Monti et al., 2010; Bardin et al., 2011). The challenge now is twofold: first, to unravel the relationship (i.e., correlations, anticorrelations) between and among the RSNs under various conscious conditions. The second challenge is to move from static functional connectivity measurements to the assessment of the temporal dynamics of such associations, meaning looking at changes in functional connectivity across time. Such imperatives are justified when considering the nature of intrinsic brain activity, which is ongoing and which characterizes most areas of the brain, beyond the DMN (Raichle and Snyder, 2007).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS), the European Commission, the James McDonnell Foundation, the Mind Science Foundation, the French Speaking Community Concerted Research Action (ARC-06/11-340), the Public Utility Foundation “Université Européenne du Travail,” “Fondazione Europea di Ricerca Biomedica,” and the University Hospital of Liège.

References

  1. Allen E. A., Erhardt E. B., Damaraju E., Gruner W., Segall J. M., Silva R. F., Havlicek M., Rachakonda S., Fries J., Kalyanam R., Michael A. M., Caprihan A., Turner J. A., Eichele T., Adelsheim S., Bryan A. D., Bustillo J., Clark V. P., Feldstein Ewing S. W., Filbey F., Ford C. C., Hutchison K., Jung R. E., Kiehl K. A., Kodituwakku P., Komesu Y. M., Mayer A. R., Pearlson G. D., Phillips J. P., Sadek J. R., Stevens M., Teuscher U., Thoma R. J., Calhoun V. D. (2011). A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:2. 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Intraoperative Awareness (2006). Practice advisory for intraoperative awareness and brain function monitoring a report by the American society of anesthesiologists task force on intraoperative awareness. Anesthesiology 104, 847–864 10.1097/00000542-200604000-00031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson J. S., Druzgal T. J., Lopez-Larson M., Jeong E. K., Desai K., Yurgelun-Todd D. (2010). Network anticorrelations, global regression, and phase-shifted soft tissue correction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 919–934 10.1002/hbm.21079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bardin J. C., Fins J. J., Katz D. I., Hersh J., Heier L. A., Tabelow K., Dyke J. P., Ballon D. J., Schiff N. D., Voss H. U. (2011). Dissociations between behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging-based evaluations of cognitive function after brain injury. Brain 134, 769–782 10.1093/brain/awr005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beckmann C. F., Deluca M., Devlin J. T., Smith S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1001–1013 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beckmann C. F., Smith S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beckmann C. F., Smith S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biswal B., Yetkin F. Z., Haughton V. M., Hyde J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541 10.1002/mrm.1910340409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boly M., Balteau E., Schnakers C., Degueldre C., Moonen G., Luxen A., Phillips C., Peigneux P., Maquet P., Laureys S. (2007). Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 12187–12192 10.1073/pnas.0611404104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boly M., Perlbarg V., Marrelec G., Schabus M., Laureys S., Doyon J., Pelegrini-Issac M., Maquet P., Benali H. (2012). Hierarchical clustering of brain activity during human nonrapid eye movement sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 5856–5861 10.1073/pnas.1111133109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Boly M., Tshibanda L., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Noirhomme Q., Schnakers C., Ledoux D., Boveroux P., Garweg C., Lambermont B., Phillips C., Luxen A., Moonen G., Bassetti C., Maquet P., Laureys S. (2009). Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2393–2400 10.1002/hbm.20672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Boveroux P., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Bruno M. A., Noirhomme Q., Lauwick S., Luxen A., Degueldre C., Plenevaux A., Schnakers C., Phillips C., Brichant J. F., Bonhomme V., Maquet P., Greicius M. D., Laureys S., Boly M. (2010). Breakdown of within- and between-network resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Anesthesiology 113, 1038–1053 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181f697f5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brewer J. A., Worhunsky P. D., Gray J. R., Tang Y. Y., Weber J., Kober H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20254–20259 10.1073/pnas.1112029108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brunetti M., Della Penna S., Ferretti A., Del Gratta C., Cianflone F., Belardinelli P., Caulo M., Pizzella V., Olivetti Belardinelli M., Romani G. L. (2008). A frontoparietal network for spatial attention reorienting in the auditory domain: a human fMRI/MEG study of functional and temporal dynamics. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1139–1147 10.1093/cercor/bhm145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bruno M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Thibaut A., Moonen G., Laureys S. (2011). From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. J. Neurol. 258, 1373–1384 10.1007/s00415-011-6114-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Buckner R. L., Andrews-Hanna J. R., Schacter D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bullmore E., Sporns O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198 10.1038/nrn2618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carhart-Harris R. L., Erritzoe D., Williams T., Stone J. M., Reed L. J., Colasanti A., Tyacke R. J., Leech R., Malizia A. L., Murphy K., Hobden P., Evans J., Feilding A., Wise R. G., Nutt D. J. (2012). Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 2138–2143 10.1073/pnas.1119598109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chai X. J., Castanon A. N., Ongur D., Whitfield-Gabrieli S. (2012). Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 59, 1420–1428 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Churchland P. S. (2007). The necessary-and-sufficient boondoggle. Am. J. Bioeth. 7, 54–55; discussion W51–W54. 10.1080/15265160701462509 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cole D. M., Smith S. M., Beckmann C. F. (2010). Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:8. 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cole M. W., Schneider W. (2007). The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage 37, 343–360 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cordes D., Haughton V. M., Arfanakis K., Carew J. D., Turski P. A., Moritz C. H., Quigley M. A., Meyerand M. E. (2001). Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in “resting-state” data. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 22, 1326–1333 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Crone J. S., Ladurner G., Höller Y., Golaszewski S., Trinka E., Kronbichler M. (2011). Deactivation of the default mode network as a marker of impaired consciousness: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 6, e26373. 10.1371/journal.pone.0026373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Damoiseaux J. S., Rombouts S. A., Barkhof F., Scheltens P., Stam C. J., Smith S. M., Beckmann C. F. (2006). Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 13848–13853 10.1073/pnas.0601417103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. De Luca M., Beckmann C. F., De Stefano N., Matthews P. M., Smith S. M. (2006). fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29, 1359–1367 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. De Martino F., Gentile F., Esposito F., Balsi M., Di Salle F., Goebel R., Formisano E. (2007). Classification of fMRI independent components using IC-fingerprints and support vector machine classifiers. Neuroimage 34, 177–194 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dehaene S., Changeux J. P. (2005). Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: a neuronal model for inattentional blindness. PLoS Biol. 3, e141. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dehaene S., Changeux J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dehaene S., Sergent C., Changeux J. P. (2003). A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 8520–8525 10.1073/pnas.1332574100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Demertzi A., Ledoux D., Bruno M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Gosseries O., Soddu A., Schnakers C., Moonen G., Laureys S. (2011a). Attitudes towards end-of-life issues in disorders of consciousness: a European survey. J. Neurol. 258, 1058–1065 10.1007/s00415-010-5882-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Demertzi A., Soddu A., Faymonville M. E., Bahri M. A., Gosseries O., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Phillips C., Maquet P., Noirhomme Q., Luxen A., Laureys S. (2011b). Hypnotic modulation of resting state fMRI default mode and extrinsic network connectivity. Prog. Brain Res. 193, 309–322 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00020-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Demertzi A., Racine E., Bruno M. A., Ledoux D., Gosseries O., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Thonnard M., Soddu A., Moonen G., Laureys S. (2012). Pain perception in disorders of consciousness: neuroscience, clinical care, and ethics in dialogue. Neuroethics 1–14 10.1007/s12152-011-9149-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Demertzi A., Schnakers C., Ledoux D., Chatelle C., Bruno M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Boly M., Moonen G., Laureys S. (2009). Different beliefs about pain perception in the vegetative and minimally conscious states: a European survey of medical and paramedical professionals. Prog. Brain Res. 177, 329–338 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17722-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Derbyshire S. W., Whalley M. G., Stenger V. A., Oakley D. A. (2004). Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage 23, 392–401 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Esposito F., Pignataro G., Di Renzo G., Spinali A., Paccone A., Tedeschi G., Annunziato L. (2010). Alcohol increases spontaneous BOLD signal fluctuations in the visual network. Neuroimage 53, 534–543 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fox M. D., Greicius M. (2010). Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:19. 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Fox M. D., Raichle M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711 10.1038/nrn2201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fox M. D., Snyder A. Z., Vincent J. L., Corbetta M., Van Essen D. C., Raichle M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 9673. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Fox M. D., Snyder A. Z., Vincent J. L., Raichle M. E. (2007). Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial variability in human behavior. Neuron 56, 171–184 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fox M. D., Zhang D., Snyder A. Z., Raichle M. E. (2009). The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 3270–3283 10.1152/jn.90777.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Fransson P. (2005). Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 15–29 10.1002/hbm.20113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fukunaga M., Horovitz S. G., van Gelderen P., de Zwart J. A., Jansma J. M., Ikonomidou V. N., Chu R., Deckers R. H., Leopold D. A., Duyn J. H. (2006). Large-amplitude, spatially correlated fluctuations in BOLD fMRI signals during extended rest and early sleep stages. Magn. Reson. Imaging 24, 979–992 10.1016/j.mri.2006.04.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Giacino J. T., Ashwal S., Childs N., Cranford R., Jennett B., Katz D. I., Kelly J. P., Rosenberg J. H., Whyte J., Zafonte R. D., Zasler N. D. (2002). The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 58, 349–353 10.1212/WNL.58.3.349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Goldberg I. I., Harel M., Malach R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron 50, 329–339 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Golland Y., Bentin S., Gelbard H., Benjamini Y., Heller R., Nir Y., Hasson U., Malach R. (2007). Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 17, 766–777 10.1093/cercor/bhk030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Greicius M. D., Kiviniemi V., Tervonen O., Vainionpää V., Alahuhta S., Reiss A. L., Menon V. (2008). Persistent default-mode network connectivity during light sedation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 839–847 10.1002/hbm.20537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Greicius M. D., Krasnow B., Reiss A. L., Menon V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 253. 10.1073/pnas.0135058100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hobson J. A., Pace-Schott E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness, and learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 679–693 10.1038/nrn915 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Honey C. J., Sporns O., Cammoun L., Gigandet X., Thiran J. P., Meuli R., Hagmann P. (2009). Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 2035–2040 10.1073/pnas.0811168106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Horovitz S. G., Braun A. R., Carr W. S., Picchioni D., Balkin T. J., Fukunaga M., Duyn J. H. (2009). Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 11376–11381 10.1073/pnas.0901435106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Horovitz S. G., Fukunaga M., De Zwart J. A., Van Gelderen P., Fulton S. C., Balkin T. J., Duyn J. H. (2008). Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: a simultaneous EEG-fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 671–682 10.1002/hbm.20428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Josipovic Z., Dinstein I., Weber J., Heeger D. J. (2012). Influence of meditation on anti-correlated networks in the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:183. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00183 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kiviniemi V. J., Haanpää H., Kantola J.-H., Jauhiainen J., Vainionpää V., Alahuhta S., Tervonen O. (2005). Midazolam sedation increases fluctuation and synchrony of the resting brain BOLD signal. Magn. Reson. Imaging 23, 531–537 10.1016/j.mri.2004.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Laird A. R., Fox P. M., Eickhoff S. B., Turner J. A., Ray K. L., Mckay D. R., Glahn D. C., Beckmann C. F., Smith S. M., Fox P. T. (2011). Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 4022–4037 10.1162/jocn_a_00077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Larson-Prior L. J., Zempel J. M., Nolan T. S., Prior F. W., Snyder A. Z., Raichle M. E. (2009). Cortical network functional connectivity in the descent to sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 4489–4494 10.1073/pnas.0900924106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Laureys S., Celesia G. G., Cohadon F., Lavrijsen J., Leon-Carrrion J., Sannita W. G., Sazbon L., Schmutzhard E., Von Wild K. R., Zeman A., Dolce G., Disorders of Consciousness T. E. (2010). Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 8, 68. 10.1186/1741-7015-8-68 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Laureys S., Pellas F., Van Eeckhout P., Ghorbel S., Schnakers C., Perrin F., Berre J., Faymonville M.-E., Pantke K.-H., Damas F., Lamy M., Moonen G., Goldman S. (2005). The locked-in syndrome: what is it like to be conscious but paralyzed and voiceless? Prog. Brain Res. 150, 495–511 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50034-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lehembre R., Bruno M., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Chatelle C., Cologan V., Leclercq Y., Soddu A., Macq B., Laureys S., Noirhomme Q. (2012). Resting state EEG study of comatose patients: a connectivity and frequency analysis to find differences between vegetative and minimally conscious states. Funct. Neurol. 27, 41–47 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lou H. C., Luber B., Crupain M., Keenan J. P., Nowak M., Kjaer T. W., Sackeim H. A., Lisanby S. H. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 6827–6832 10.1073/pnas.0400049101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Majerus S., Bruno M. A., Schnakers C., Giacino J. T., Laureys S. (2009). The problem of aphasia in the assessment of consciousness in brain-damaged patients. Prog. Brain Res. 177, 49–61 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17705-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Martuzzi R., Ramani R., Qiu M., Rajeevan N., Constable R. T. (2010). Functional connectivity and alterations in baseline brain state in humans. Neuroimage 49, 823–834 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mason M. F., Norton M. I., Van Horn J. D., Wegner D., Grafton S. T., Macrae C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393–395 10.1126/science.1131295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mazoyer B., Zago L., Mellet E., Bricogne S., Etard O., Houde O., Crivello F., Joliot M., Petit L., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. (2001). Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 287–298 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00437-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. McGeown W. J., Mazzoni G., Venneri A., Kirsch I. (2009). Hypnotic induction decreases anterior default mode activity. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 848–855 10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. McKiernan K. A., D’angelo B. R., Kaufman J. N., Binder J. R. (2006). Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 29, 1185–1191 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Menon V., Uddin L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Monti M. M., Pickard J. D., Owen A. M. (2012). Visual cognition in disorders of consciousness: from V1 to top-down attention. Hum. Brain Mapp. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/hbm.21507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Monti M. M., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Coleman M. R., Boly M., Pickard J. D., Tshibanda L., Owen A. M., Laureys S. (2010). Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N. Engl. J. Med. 362, 579–589 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Murphy K., Birn R. M., Handwerker D. A., Jones T. B., Bandettini P. A. (2009). The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage 44, 893–905 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Norton L., Hutchison R. M., Young G. B., Lee D. H., Sharpe M. D., Mirsattari S. M. (2012). Disruptions of functional connectivity in the default mode network of comatose patients. Neurology 78, 175–181 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823fcd61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ploner M., Lee M. C., Wiech K., Bingel U., Tracey I. (2010). Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 355–360 10.1073/pnas.0906186106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Posner J., Saper C., Schiff N. D., Plum F. (eds). (2007). Plum and Posner’s Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. New York: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  74. Raichle M. E., Macleod A. M., Snyder A. Z., Powers W. J., Gusnard D. A., Shulman G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Raichle M. E., Snyder A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37, 1083–1090; discussion 1097–1089. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Roberts G. M., Garavan H. (2010). Evidence of increased activation underlying cognitive control in ecstasy and cannabis users. Neuroimage 52, 429–435 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Roberts K. L., Hall D. A. (2008). Examining a supramodal network for conflict processing: a systematic review and novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for related visual and auditory stroop tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1063–1078 10.1162/jocn.2008.20074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sämann P. G., Wehrle R., Hoehn D., Spoormaker V. I., Peters H., Tully C., Holsboer F., Czisch M. (2011). Development of the brain’s default mode network from wakefulness to slow wave sleep. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2082–2093 10.1093/cercor/bhq295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sapir A., D’Avossa G., Mcavoy M., Shulman G. L., Corbetta M. (2005). Brain signals for spatial attention predict performance in a motion discrimination task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 17810–17815 10.1073/pnas.0504678102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Schiff N. D., Rodriguez-Moreno D., Kamal A., Kim K. H., Giacino J. T., Plum F., Hirsch J. (2005). fMRI reveals large-scale network activation in minimally conscious patients. Neurology 64, 514–523 10.1212/01.WNL.0000150883.10285.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schilbach L., Eickhoff S. B., Rotarska-Jagiela A., Fink G. R., Vogeley K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 457–467 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Schrouff J., Perlbarg V., Boly M., Marrelec G., Boveroux P., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Bruno M.-A., Laureys S., Phillips C., Pélégrini-Issac M., Maquet P., Benali H. (2011). Brain functional integration decreases during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Neuroimage 57, 198–205 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., Reiss A. L., Greicius M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Seghier M. L., Zeidman P., Neufeld N. H., Leff A. P., Price C. J. (2010). Identifying abnormal connectivity in patients using dynamic causal modeling of FMRI responses. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:142. 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Shen S., Szameitat A. J., Sterr A. (2007). VBM lesion detection depends on the normalization template: a study using simulated atrophy. Magn. Reson. Imaging 25, 1385–1396 10.1016/j.mri.2006.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Shulman G. L., Fiez J. A., Corbetta M., Buckner R. L., Miezin F. M., Raichle M. E., Petersen S. E. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 648–663 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Smith S. M., Fox P. T., Miller K. L., Glahn D. C., Fox M. P., Mackay C. E., Filippini N., Watkins K. E., Toro R., Laird A. R., Beckmann C. F. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 13040–13045 10.1073/pnas.0908706106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Soddu A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Bahri M. A., Bruno M. A., Boly M., Demertzi A., Tshibanda J. F., Phillips C., Stanziano M., Ovadia-Caro S., Nir Y., Maquet P., Papa M., Malach R., Laureys S., Noirhomme Q. (2012). Identifying the default-mode component in spatial IC analyses of patients with disorders of consciousness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 778–796 10.1002/hbm.21249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Soddu A., Vanhaudenhuyse A., Demertzi A., Marie-Aurelie B., Tshibanda L., Di H., Melanie B., Papa M., Laureys S., Noirhomme Q. (2011). Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness. Funct. Neurol. 26, 37–43 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Stamatakis E. A., Adapa R. M., Absalom A. R., Menon D. K. (2010). Changes in resting neural connectivity during propofol sedation. PLoS ONE 5, e14224. 10.1371/journal.pone.0014224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. The Executive Committee of the American Psychological Association – Division of Psychological Hypnosis (1994). Definition and description of hypnosis. Contemp. Hypn. 11, 142–162 [Google Scholar]
  92. Tian L., Jiang T., Liu Y., Yu C., Wang K., Zhou Y., Song M., Li K. (2007). The relationship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems indicated by resting state correlational patterns of sensory cortices. Neuroimage 36, 684–690 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Tohka J., Foerde K., Aron A., Tom S., Toga A. (2008). Automatic independent component labeling for artifact removal in fMRI. Neuroimage 39, 1227–1245 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tracey I., Mantyh P. W. (2007). The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron 55, 377–391 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. van den Heuvel M. P., Hulshoff Pol H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 519–534 10.1016/S0924-977X(10)70095-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vanhaudenhuyse A., Demertzi A., Schabus M., Noirhomme Q., Bredart S., Boly M., Phillips C., Soddu A., Luxen A., Moonen G., Laureys S. (2011). Two distinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 570–578 10.1162/jocn.2010.21488 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Vanhaudenhuyse A., Noirhomme Q., Tshibanda L. J., Bruno M. A., Boveroux P., Schnakers C., Soddu A., Perlbarg V., Ledoux D., Brichant J. F., Moonen G., Maquet P., Greicius M. D., Laureys S., Boly M. (2010). Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain 133, 161–171 10.1093/brain/awq228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Weissman D. H., Roberts K., Visscher K., Woldorff M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978 10.1038/nn1727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Moran J. M., Nieto-Castanon A., Triantafyllou C., Saxe R., Gabrieli J. D. (2011). Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage 55, 225–232 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wiech K., Lin C. S., Brodersen K. H., Bingel U., Ploner M., Tracey I. (2010). Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. J. Neurosci. 30, 16324–16331 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zalesky A., Fornito A., Bullmore E. (2012). On the use of correlation as a measure of network connectivity. Neuroimage 60, 2096–2106 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zang Y. F., He Y., Zhu C. Z., Cao Q. J., Sui M. Q., Liang M., Tian L. X., Jiang T. Z., Wang Y. F. (2007). Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev. 29, 83–91 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zeman A. (2001). Consciousness. Brain 124, 1263–1289 10.1093/brain/124.1.232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zuo X. N., Di Martino A., Kelly C., Shehzad Z. E., Gee D. G., Klein D. F., Castellanos F. X., Biswal B. B., Milham M. P. (2010). The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 49, 1432–1445 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES