Abstract
Mutations in either of two tumor suppressor genes, TSC1 or TSC2, cause tuberous sclerosis complex (TSC), a syndrome resulting in benign hamartomatous tumors and neurological disorders. Cellular growth defects and neuronal disorganization associated with TSC are believed to be due to upregulated TOR signaling. We overexpressed Rheb, an upstream regulator of TOR, in two different subsets of D. melanogaster central brain neurons in order to upregulate the Tsc-Rheb-TOR pathway. Overexpression of Rheb in either the mushroom bodies or the insulin producing cells resulted in enlarged axon projections and cell bodies, which continued to increase in size with prolonged Rheb expression as the animals aged. Additionally, Rheb overexpression in the mushroom bodies resulted in deficiencies in 3 hr but not immediate appetitive memory. Thus, Rheb overexpression in the central brain neurons of flies causes not only morphological phenotypes, but behavioral and aging phenotypes that may mirror symptoms of TSC.
Introduction
Tuberous sclerosis complex (TSC) is a multisystem autosomal-dominant syndrome caused by mutations inactivating one of two tumor suppressor genes, TSC1 or TSC2. It is typified by formation of hamartomas, or benign tumors, in multiple organ systems such as the heart, lungs, kidneys, brain and skin [1] and is also commonly connected with a wide array of potentially devastating neurological phenotypes, including intellectual, behavioral and psychiatric disorders. Some of the symptoms associated with TSC are directly attributed to the formation of brain hamartomas, also known as cortical tubers [2]. However, many of TSC's neurological manifestations, such as autism and cognitive impairment, are developmental in origin and may reflect a disorganization of portions of the central nervous system rather than disruption via cortical tubers [2], [3]. The developmental mechanisms disrupted in TSC are still being elucidated.
TSC1 and TSC2 form an obligate heterodimeric protein complex that functions downstream of PI3K/Akt signaling, and is a key regulator of the serine-threonine kinase TOR (Target of Rapamycin) [4], [5]. TSC2 contains a GAP (GTPase activating protein) domain in its c-terminal region, necessary for its function in inhibiting the activity of the small GTPase, Rheb [6]–[8]. Rheb, a homolog of Ras, activates TOR by binding to its kinase domain [9]. Through phosphorylation of ribosomal S6 kinase (S6K), the translational repressor 4E-BP1, and other factors, TOR activation upregulates translation and promotes cell growth [2], [4]. TOR has also been shown to be a potent suppressor of autophagy [10]–[12]. Loss of TSC1 or TSC2 function or experimental overexpression of Rheb results in upregulated TOR activity, as seen both by increased phosphorylated S6K and enhanced cellular growth that can be repressed through administration of rapamycin [6], [13]–[15].
Misregulation of the TOR pathway has been shown to have multiple effects on the neurons and glia of the central nervous system. In addition to disruption of proper brain function through formation of cortical tubers, symptoms of TSC have also been associated with other physiological and developmental malfunctions [3]. Loss of Tsc1 in the pyramidal neurons of mice causes an increase in the size of somata and dendritic spines [13], while mice lacking Tsc1 in all neurons have enlarged cortical and hippocampal neurons akin to tubers, as well as abnormal brain architecture, delays in myelination and seizures [16]. Additionally, astrocyte-specific Tsc1 knockout mice have an increase in astrocyte numbers and abnormal organization of hippocampal neurons [17]. Mice heterozygous for loss-of-function of either Tsc1 or Tsc2 in neurons lack apparent neural morphological defects, but do display cognitive and social defects [18]–[20]. Taken together, the murine data alone shows a surprising phenotypic variability when TSC is modeled within the central nervous system.
The Tsc-Rheb-TOR pathway is highly conserved, and as such can be studied effectively in invertebrates such as the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. Rheb was originally discovered in a Drosophila screen for novel regulators of cell growth [7], [8]; indeed, all of the major genetic players that comprise the human Tsc-Rheb-TOR pathway are present in flies. Overexpression of Rheb in Drosophila photoreceptor cells has been shown to produce axon guidance defects and induce cell death by downregulating autophagy [10], [21], while Rheb overexpression in the external sensory organ produces a cell fate switch from neuron to bristle/socket cells [22]. To investigate the effects of Rheb overexpression specifically within the D. melanogaster central brain, we used the Gal4-UAS system [23] to target two neuronal subsets, the mushroom bodies and insulin producing cells (IPCs). Rheb overexpression within marked central neurons (mushroom bodies) of a living animal allowed us to test behavioral output and investigate overall morphology, and compare our results from a Drosophila model to mammalian models of TSC.
Methods
Drosophila strains
To generate flies overexpressing both Rheb and GFP in the mushroom bodies, we crossed y w mcd8GFP; +, and y w mcd8GFP; UAS-Rheb (generated from Bloomington Stock Center stocks) to w*; OK107-Gal4, respectively. To generate flies overexpressing both Rheb and GFP in the insulin producing cells, we crossed w*; UAS-Rheb to w*; dilp2-Gal4.A UASmcd8-GFP/CyO. w1118, was crossed to w*; OK107-Gal4 and used for behavioral test controls. All stocks are available from Bloomington Stock Center, Indiana University.
Females were collected 1 day post-eclosion for immunohistochemistry. Males were collected 1 day post-eclosion for behavior tests. For aging experiments, 1 day old female adults were collected, then housed in food vials at 25°C for 21 days post-eclosion (PE); vials were changed every 3 to 4 days.
Immunohistochemistry and Imaging
Drosophila brains were dissected in a phosphate-buffered saline solution, fixed for 30 minutes in 4% buffered paraformaldehyde, rinsed several times in phosphate-buffered saline with 0.3% Triton-X (PBS-Tx) and blocked with 5% normal goat serum for 15 minutes. After blocking, they were incubated overnight at 4°C in one or more of the following antibodies: mouse anti-fibrillarin (1∶500) [24], rabbit anti-GFP (1∶10,000; Invitrogen), and mouse anti-FasII 1D4 (1∶100; Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank). They were then rinsed again in PBS-Tx, and incubated overnight at 4°C in secondary antibody (1∶1000; AlexaFluor 488 goat anti-rabbit IgG and AlexaFluor 568 goat anti-mouse IgG; Invitrogen) and DAPI (0.1 µg/mL; Invitrogen). Following secondary incubation, tissues were washed in PBS, attached to polylysine-coated coverslips, dehydrated through an ethanol series, cleared in xylene and mounted in DPX (Fluka BioChemica). All tissues were imaged on a Zeiss LSM 510 META confocal microscope.
EdU Labeling
Staged embryos were collected on grape agar plates and either transferred to a food vial containing 0.02 mM Click-it™ EdU or allowed to hatch and pupate, at which time the newly eclosed adults were transferred to a food vial containing EdU. Larval animals were kept on EdU food until pupation, and were dissected at 1 day PE. Adults that were kept on EdU-containing food for 10 days were dissected on day 10 PE. Immunohistochemistry and imaging was performed as above, using the Click-it™ EdU Alexa Fluor-555 imaging kit from Invitrogen.
Behavioral Tests
Odor sensitivity
Groups of 30 4–5 day old males food deprived for 16–20 hrs on agar vials prior to testing were tested. Odors used were 1∶36 ethyl acetate in mineral oil and 1∶36 iso-amyl alcohol in mineral oil. Choice tests were performed in a 1 cm diameter Y-maze with 13 cm arms, where flies chose between each single odor and air streamed through mineral oil. Preference index was calculated by (# flies in odor vial - # flies in air vial)/total # flies.
Sucrose sensitivity
Groups of 30 4–5 day old males food deprived for 16–20 hours on agar vials prior to testing were tested. Sensitivity chambers consisted of 6 cm Petri dishes with 2.1 cm diameter 3 MM filter paper folded along seam of dish on opposing sides. Filter paper was soaked in water or 2 M sucrose and dried 24 hours prior to experiment. Flies were gently tapped into the dish, and the number of flies on each filter paper was counted 30 seconds after entry. Preference index was calculated by (# flies on sucrose - # flies on water)/total # flies.
Sucrose Conditioning
Sucrose conditioning was performed similarly to Kaun et al [25]. Groups of 30 4–5 day old males food deprived for 16–20 hours on agar vials prior to testing were used. Flies were trained in 14 mL culture vials with mesh lids in 30×15×15 cm training boxes and tested in a 1 cm diameter Y-maze with 13 cm arms. Training consisted of a 5 minute habituation to the training chamber with air (flow rate 130), a 5 minute presentation of odor 1 with plain filter paper pre-soaked in water and dried, then 5 minutes of odor 2 with filter paper pre-soaked in 2 M sucrose and dried. Reciprocal training was performed to ensure that inherent preference for either odor did not affect conditioning scored: a separate group of flies was simultaneously trained using odor 1 as the sucrose-paired odor. Vials of flies from Group 1 and Group 2 were paired according to placement in the training chamber and tested simultaneously 2 minutes or 3 hours following training. Preference index was calculated by (# flies in Odor+ vial - # flies in Odor− vial)/total # flies. Learning index was the average between preference indexes in reciprocal trials. Statistics for behavior experiments was performed using JMP 9.0.2. Statistical significance was determined using one-way ANOVA with two-way Student's-t post-hoc test (p<0.05).
Image Analysis
Confocal image stacks were compiled and analyzed using ImageJ (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/). Cell slice area and cluster volume for the Kenyon cells of the mushroom bodies and cell number for the IPCs were scored blind. For cell slice area, five cells per animal were measured. Volume analysis for both the cell cluster and neuropil volumes was performed using the ImageJ “Measure Stack” volume measurement plugin. IPC number was calculated manually by marking cell nuclei on each slice of an image stack, taking care to count each cell only once. Statistical significance was determined using one-way ANOVA with Tukey post-hoc test (p<0.05; SPSS 17.0, www.spss.com).
Results
To determine if heightened expression of Rheb would affect development of the D. melanogaster nervous system and/or cause behavioral shifts, we overexpressed Rheb in a subset of neurons within the brain. We chose to use Rheb overexpression in the fly nervous system since it has previously been shown to upregulate TOR activity and gives similar phenotypes to Tsc1 null clones [6], [20]–[22]; additionally, use of RNAi constructs seen to reduce Tsc1 or Tsc2 in other tissues were unsuccessful in these two neuronal subsets, presumably due to the resistance of the Drosophila nervous system to RNAi [26].
The mushroom bodies are an associative area within the insect brain and have been implicated in many complex behaviors, most notably in olfactory learning and memory [27]–[29]. In addition, the mushroom bodies are well-organized and characterized structures within the central brain, with a distinct developmental pattern. They are composed of neurons with bilaterally symmetrical axonal and dendritic projections. Within each Drosophila brain hemisphere, the mushroom body cell cluster is composed of approximately 2500 Kenyon cells. Each Kenyon cell extends dendritic projections to form the calyces, while the axonal projections continue their extensions to form two distinct branches, each subdivided into α, α′, β, β′, and γ lobes (Fig. 1A) [29]–[31]. We chose the mushroom bodies to test neuronal response to Rheb overexpression because of their distinct axonal structures and capacity for functional assessment by means of well-defined behavioral assays. Using the Gal4/UAS system [23], we targeted Rheb overexpression along with a membrane-bound GFP construct (CD8::GFP) to the mushroom body neurons using the OK107-Gal4 driver [32]. CD8::GFP was highly expressed in both the cell bodies and neuropil of the mushroom bodies when driven by OK107-Gal4. In addition, OK107-Gal4 also drives UAS transgene expression in the insulin producing cells (IPCs), a set of neurosecretory cells located along the midline of the brain (Fig. 1A, arrowhead), as well as at lower levels in a few other neurons within the brain [32]–[34]. However, the distinct structure of the mushroom bodies combined with OK107-driven expression in relatively few neurons outside of the mushroom bodies facilitated our analysis of mushroom body structures.
Rheb overexpression in the Drosophila mushroom bodies
We evaluated the response to Rheb overexpression within the mushroom bodies by qualitative assessment of axonal lobe shape and size, as well as quantitative measurements of cell body size and cell cluster size. When examined at 1 day post-eclosion (PE), mushroom body axonal lobes overexpressing Rheb (OK107>GFP, Rheb) were greatly expanded as compared to the mushroom bodies of control animals (OK107>GFP; Fig. 1B). All portions of the lobe appeared enlarged. Neuropil volume was significantly larger for OK107>GFP, Rheb animals (173517.6 µm3±20829.3) than for OK107>GFP animals (64514.8 µm3±5186.3) at 1 day PE (Fig. 1E). However, the overall structure and proportionality of the lobes appeared intact. No obvious large-scale misrouting or deletions were observed, and upon close examination of image Z-stacks, the expanded lobes did not invade the opposite brain hemisphere. In addition to enlarged neural projections, the cell bodies of the Kenyon cells were also increased in size (Fig. 2B). Cell size was quantified by measuring the area of an optical slice through the center of the cell body using ImageJ software. Five cells each from >18 samples per genotype were measured, and then all cell measurements were averaged for each genotype. At 1 day PE, the average individual Kenyon cell area for OK107>GFP, Rheb animals (21.69 µm2±5.95) was nearly twice that of control OK107>GFP animals (11.15 µm2±3.12; Fig. 2E). However, when we compared DAPI staining within the Kenyon cells of OK107>GFP, Rheb animals, we did not observe an increase in size or intensity of staining as compared to control animals, indicating a lack of DNA endoreplication (Fig. 2B). We also measured the total volume of the Kenyon cell body cluster within a brain hemisphere by outlining the area of each slice containing cell bodies, then using ImageJ to estimate the entire volume of the cell body cluster across all the image stacks. Cell body cluster volume was significantly larger for OK107>GFP, Rheb animals (96165.67 µm3±38068.65) than for OK107>GFP animals (31952.04 µm3±17296.1) at 1 day PE (Fig. 2F).
Continuous Rheb overexpression in the mushroom bodies of aged animals
We also sought to examine the effects of prolonged Rheb overexpression in the neurons of aged animals. We examined the mushroom bodies of OK107>GFP animals and OK107>GFP, Rheb animals that were collected 1 day PE and housed for 3 weeks before dissection. As seen in Figure 1, the mushroom body axonal lobes of OK107>GFP, Rheb animals at 21 days PE appeared grossly enlarged compared to control animals of the same age (Fig. 1C, D). The average neuropil volume of OK107>GFP, Rheb animals (220499.3 µm3±22516.4) was significantly larger than that of OK107>GFP animals (89337.2 µm3±12487.1) of the same age (Fig. 1E). Additionally, GFP staining of OK107>GFP, Rheb animals at 21 days PE appeared punctuate in some places and diffuse in others. Both the axonal lobes and the cell body clusters were less compact and defined, and also had more irregular and less defined GFP staining than in control animals of the same age (Fig. 2D). The average Kenyon cell body area for 21 days PE OK107>GFP, Rheb animals (31.72 µm2±11.8) was significantly larger than in control animals at either 1 or 21 days PE (11.15 µm2±3.22; 9.05 µm2±3.12; Fig. 2E). Additionally, the average Kenyon cell body area in aged animals overexpressing Rheb was significantly larger than in young animals overexpressing Rheb (21.69 µm2±5.95), indicating that Kenyon cell size continued to increase with continuous Rheb overexpression in aging animals (Fig. 2E). The average Kenyon cell cluster volume for cells overexpressing Rheb at 21 days PE (108332.6 µm3±69221.92) was significantly larger than in control animals at 21 days PE (23748.28 µm3±14688.06); however, the cluster volume did not significantly increase for OK107>GFP, Rheb animals between 1 and 21 days PE (Fig. 2F).
Rheb overexpression in the mushroom bodies causes a memory deficiency
The dramatic increase in mushroom body lobe size caused by overexpression of Rheb prompted us to investigate whether a behavioral phenotype was also present. The mushroom body plays an important role in integration of information and decision-making in the fly [29], [35], [36]. It has been implicated in memory for shock, sucrose, ethanol, and courtship [25], [27], [37]. The relatively stable nature of appetitive sucrose memory is an enticing model to study memory in the mushroom body due to the simplicity of the assay and the stability of the memory after a single training session [38]–[40]. Thus, we chose to test immediate memory (2 min after training) and 3 hr memory (3 hrs after training) using a sucrose reward paired with one of two odors. Neither of the control strains nor the flies overexpressing Rheb showed a preference for either of the two odors used (iso-amyl alcohol or ethyl acetate) when not paired with sucrose (Fig. 3A, B). Overexpression of Rheb did result in an increased sensitivity for sucrose after starvation as compared to controls potentially due to expression in the IPCs (Fig. 3C). Immediately after conditioning, control flies and OK107>GFP, Rheb flies had a similar learning index (Fig. 3D, E). However, 3 hours after conditioning, controls had a significantly higher learning index than OK107>GFP, Rheb flies (Fig. 3F), indicating that flies overexpressing Rheb in the mushroom bodies had normal immediate memory formation, but were deficient in retaining those memories.
Rheb overexpression in the IPCs
As noted previously, the OK107 driver also expresses in the insulin-producing cells (IPCs) of the Drosophila brain (Fig. 1A). The IPCs are a set of neurosecretory cells that lie along the median of the pars intercerebralis and secrete insulin-like peptides that control growth and metabolism [41]–[44]. When comparing the mushroom bodies of OK107>GFP, Rheb animals to control (OK107>GFP) animals, we noticed that the IPCs were also expanded in size. To examine their altered morphology in more detail without overlap of the mushroom bodies, we used the dilp2-Gal4 driver to target Rheb overexpression specifically to the IPCs. As seen in Figure 4, overexpression of Rheb visibly increased cell body size in the IPCs of 1 day PE animals. Nucleolus size (as seen by fibrillarin staining) was also increased, indicating upregulation of ribosome biosynthesis (Fig. 4B′, arrowhead). Additionally, the diameter of the individual neurites projecting from the cell bodies appeared larger, as did the entire axon bundle (Fig. 4B, arrowhead). dilp2-Gal4>Rheb animals did not appear to have enlarged mushroom bodies compared to controls, indicating that the size increases seen previously with the OK107 driver were cell autonomous (not shown). Similarly to the Kenyon cells of the mushroom bodies, IPCs overexpressing Rheb continued to increase in size with continued Rheb overexpression over time. The IPC bodies of 21 days PE dilp2>GFP, Rheb animals appeared dramatically enlarged with huge nucleoli (Fig. 4D′, arrowhead). Again, as in the Kenyon cells, DAPI staining of DNA was not visibly increased (Fig. 4D′, arrow), signifying the UAS-Rheb transgene does not cause DNA endoreplication.
Because of the IPCs' irregular shape, we were unable to quantify cell size between genotypes or ages. However, because of the relatively low number of IPCs, we were able to evaluate cell number to determine if extra cell divisions had occurred. Quantification of the average IPC number per animal resulted in no significant difference between dilp2>GFP, Rheb animals (26.82±4.51) and control dilp2>GFP animals (23±3.38) at 1 day PE (Fig. 4E). Cell number was also not significantly different between dilp2>GFP, Rheb animals (20.1±4.43) and dilp2>GFP animals at 21 days PE (18.5±3.78; Fig. 4E). Animals were also fed 5-ethynyl-2′-deoxyuridine (Click-iT™ EdU) to test for EdU incorporation during S-phase. Larvae were fed EdU during their entire larval life and examined at 1 day PE; adults were also fed EdU during the first 10 days PE and examined on day 10 PE. No EdU incorporation was seen in the IPCs of either case (data not shown), further substantiating the lack of extra cell divisions and endoreplication in the IPCs of dilp2>GFP, Rheb animals.
Discussion
To explore how Rheb overexpression alters neuron growth and morphology, and ultimately how the behavior of the whole organism is affected, we overexpressed Rheb in two specific neuronal subsets of the central brain of D. melanogaster. Selective overexpression of Rheb in two different subsets of central brain neurons induced both enlarged cell bodies and projections, while continuous Rheb overexpression over time enhanced this phenotype. Rheb overexpression in the mushroom bodies also resulted in a decrease in 3 hr but not immediate odor-sucrose memory. These experiments highlight the importance of tight regulation of the upstream components of the TOR pathway for proper neural growth and function throughout development and adulthood.
Tsc-Rheb-TOR signaling is known to have a function in controlling cell body size in the nervous system. Loss of Tsc1 or Tsc2 in mouse pyramidal neurons results in enlarged somata, and activation of the PI3K-Akt pathway also results in larger cultured hippocampal cell bodies; this increase in size is mediated through the Tsc-Rheb-TOR pathway, as seen by upregulation of downstream effectors such as phosphorylated S6K [13], [16], [45], [46]. In flies, both Rheb overexpression and Tsc1 null clones induce a similar enlarged cell phenotype [6], [7], [10], [21]. Additionally, neuronal cell body size increases due to Rheb overexpression have also been seen in another invertebrate, C. elegans [47]. The increase that we observe in Kenyon cell body size with Rheb overexpression is in line with the growth phenotypes seen in these prior studies. Although they could not be quantified due to irregular cell borders, the IPC bodies were also visibly bigger with large nucleoli. The increased size of the nucleoli indicates a rise in rRNA production and ribosome biogenesis, consistent with an increase in protein production during cell growth.
Post-mitotic growth is often due to endocycling, in which the cell undergoes growth and DNA synthesis without mitotic divisions. Cell growth of D. melanogaster salivary gland cells is linked to TOR pathway activation-driven endocycling [48]. However, not all cell growth is linked to DNA content; cells within an organism that have the same genome copy number show great variations in size [49]. The apparent lack of increased DNA content upon examination of DAPI staining in the Kenyon cells and IPCs indicates that the enlargement of cell size is not due to endocycling in these neurons. Additionally, we did not observe any incorporation of EdU in IPCs in animals treated as either larva or adults, signifying that S-phase did not take place in these cells post-embryonically. Therefore, cell growth in these two neuronal subsets likely takes place via TOR activation of non-endocycling coupled growth pathways that promote anabolic processes nutrient uptake, and suppress autophagy.
In addition to increases in cell body size, neurons in which the TOR pathway has been upregulated also show an increase in the size of their projections. Eliminating Tsc1 or Tsc2 activity in the mammalian brain leads to increased neurite outgrowth, formation of ectopic axons, increased size of dendritic spines, and increased dendritic branching and complexity [13], [16], [45], [50], [51]. We also observed an increase in the overall size of the axon bundles of both the mushroom body neurons and the IPCs overexpressing Rheb. Because of the simple projection pattern, low number of axons and high GFP expression in the membrane of the IPCs, we were able to observe an increase in axon diameter in the IPCs overexpressing Rheb. However we did not observe grossly misrouted axons. This is consistent with results from other studies in D. melanogaster; overexpression of Rheb in the motoneuron of the neuromuscular junction results in increased synapse size but no misrouting [21] and PI3K overexpression in cholinergic central neurons causes an increase in axon diameter [52]. Conversely, misrouting has been observed in photoreceptor cells lacking Tsc1 [21]. Additionally, although both the mushroom body neurons and the IPCs showed dramatic size increases due to Rheb overexpression, overexpression of Rheb or deletion of Tsc1 in other classes of neurons, such as the adult-specific neurons of the thoracic ganglia, did not (unpublished data). These data indicate that the effects of Rheb overexpression or Tsc1 null clones in various neuron types may be context-dependent.
Previous studies have shown that activation of the TOR pathway can alter timing of cell divisions. Fingar et al showed that the S6K1 and eIF4E pathways downstream of mTOR promote cell cycle progression [53], while activation of PI3K-TOR in Xenopus embryos results in abnormally rapid cell cycles after the midblastula transition [54]. In D. melanogaster, it has been reported that overexpression of Rheb can lead to cell cycle activation in photoreceptor cells and S2 cells but not wing cells, indicating that effects on cell cycle of the TSC-Rheb-TOR pathway are cell-type dependent. Since the dilp2-Gal4 driver is activated post-mitotically in the IPCs [44], [55], any additional IPCs would have to come from cell cycle re-entry. We did not find any evidence of cell cycle re-entry by the IPCs, either by cell number comparison or EdU staining in dilp2>GFP, Rheb flies. This could be due to the lapse of DNA repair in terminally differentiated neurons that can lead to cell death when mitosis is triggered [56]. This lack of cell divisions is also seen when an input upstream of the TOR pathway is activated; expression of PI3K in a group of cholinergic central neurons causes an increase in cell body size and number of synapses, but not an increase in cell number [52].
A previous study showed that flies with continuous ubiquitous overexpression of Rheb for 30 days post-eclosion show a decrease in geotaxis escape behavior as compared to either control animals of the same age or younger flies (5 days PE) also overexpressing Rheb in all tissues [57]. Additionally, prolonged expression of Rheb in fly photoreceptors results in degeneration of rhabdomeres due to the loss of autophagy [10]. Based on these data, we decided to monitor neuron morphology after 21 days of continuous Rheb expression in adult flies. The significant size increase in the Kenyon cell bodies relative to controls or to 1 day PE Kenyon cells overexpressing Rheb indicates that cell growth, and therefore nutrient uptake, continues. Although cell size could not be measured in the IPCs, we were able to observe an obvious increase in cell body and nucleolus size, indicating that macromolecule synthesis and ribosome biogenesis continue to increase as Rheb overexpression continues. The appearance of the cell bodies and neurite projections (bulging membranes and punctate, patchy GFP expression) in these animals at 21 days PE points to declining neuron health and is consistent with reports of Rheb-triggered neurodegeneration via inhibition of autophagy [10], [11]. Interestingly, increased phosphorylation of the human TSC2 gene which inhibits its function in repressing TOR activity, has been found in the frontal cortex of Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease patients [58]. These studies, in conjunction with our data, indicate that damage to neurons may accumulate with continuous TOR pathway upregulation in TSC patients.
Given the dramatic cell enlargement seen with Rheb overexpression in the mushroom bodies, we sought to examine whether this alteration would affect learning and memory. Because of the dramatic phenotype seen with Rheb overexpression, we expected a correspondingly dramatic behavioral phenotype. As expected, the capacity to form lasting memories of an odor-evoked stimulus was depressed; however, memory tested immediately after training was normal. This suggests that increased mushroom body size and/or altered cell metabolism is more detrimental to longer-lasting memories than immediate memories.
Olfactory information in Drosophila enters the brain through olfactory receptor neurons that connect to projection neurons (PNs) within the antennal lobe. The PNs then innervate the Kenyon cells of the mushroom body (reviewed in [29]). Memory traces have been found to occur in the PNs, the dorsal paired medial neurons (DPMs) that innervate the mushroom body lobes, and the mushroom bodies themselves [59], [60]. Information about appetitive stimuli travels through octopaminergic neurons [61] and dopaminergic PAM neurons [62], while the Neuropeptide F neurons [63] and neurons of the protocerebral posterior lateral 1 dopaminergic cluster [64] are also involved in reward memory formation. Although the circuitry involved in appetitive memory is not well understood, at least two memory traces for odor-sugar association may exist: one in the first-order interneurons (PNs) and another in the second-order interneurons (Kenyon cells) of the olfactory system in Drosophila [59]. The normal 2 min sucrose memory in flies with Rheb overexpression in the mushroom body suggests a mushroom-body independent memory trace, which we hypothesize may be localized to the projection neurons. Intriguingly, the projection neurons have been reported to support a 3 hr memory trace in flies [59]. Our data suggest that the memory trace in the projection neurons may be dependent on a normally functioning mushroom body. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that other neuronal structures can compensate for the defects seen in mushroom body morphology, potentially rewiring some or all of the memory circuit.
The rapid decay of reward memory seen with Rheb overexpression suggests that altered mushroom body morphology and/or Rheb signaling in the mushroom body affects consolidation or retention of memory. This is reminiscent of the phenotype observed in mutants of the Drosophila homolog of pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating peptide (PACAP) called amnesiac (amn) [65]. amn is required in the dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons for consolidation of 3 hr sucrose reward memory [65]. Furthermore, DPM neuron output to the mushroom body α′β′ lobe neurons is required for memory consolidation suggesting that activity in α′β′ neurons establishes a recurrent α′β′ neuron-DPM neuron loop that is necessary for consolidation of memory [66]. Recent data suggests that the DPM neurons form a time-extended and broad olfactory cellular memory trace after appetitive conditioning [60]. Since the OK107 driver is not expressed in the DPM neurons [33], [34], we hypothesize that the grossly enlarged morphology of the mushroom body axonal lobes is either preventing formation of a normal memory trace in the mushroom body, or preventing proper synaptic connectivity with the DPM neurons, resulting in lack of consolidation.
Intriguingly, appetitive memory decay also occurs more quickly when a non-nutritive sugar is used, and this decay correlates with the duration and breadth of the DPM neuron memory trace [60], [67], [68]. Thus, another possibility is that Rheb overexpression in the mushroom body and/or IPCs results in inability to process sugar. We believe however, that the normal body development and size (data not shown), along with the increased sucrose sensitivity in OK107>Rheb flies, makes this possibility less likely.
Disruption of neuronal morphology and function has also been seen in vertebrate models. Deletion of Pten, an upstream negative regulator of TOR, results in mice with ectopic neural processes and increased dendritic spine density. These mice also show decreased learning ability [69]. In addition to growth and misrouting phenotypes, suppression of TSC1 or TSC2 function has been shown to affect neuronal polarity, neurotransmitter receptor expression, neuron hyperexcitability and reduced synaptic plasticity [70]–[73]. Indeed, Eker rats heterozygous for a Tsc2 mutation show reduced long-term potentiation and long-term depression in hippocampal cells, indicating that the synapses may have a reduced ability for activity-dependent synaptic modification, a necessary part of memory formation [73].
The role of the Tsc-Rheb-TOR signaling pathway in determining neuron size, growth, projection pattern and function is complex, and depends on cell type, species, age of the organism, and environmental conditions. However, studies in both vertebrates and invertebrates are yielding clues as to the specific effects tuberous sclerosis has on the various cell types of the nervous system, both in organization and in functional capacity. Our results add to this growing body of work, establishing another model for exploration of the effects of upstream regulators of TOR on neuron morphology and function.
Acknowledgments
Helpful comments on the manuscript were provided by anonymous reviewers.
Funding Statement
Research was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) R01 grant NS058230 to BAE and by the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada Fellowship (to KRK) and NIH-R01 to Ulrike Heberlein (Dept. Anatomy, University of California San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, 94158, USA). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Leung AK, Robson WL (2007) Tuberous sclerosis complex: a review. J Pediatr Health Care 21: 108–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Ehninger D, de Vries PJ, Silva AJ (2009) From mTOR to cognition: molecular and cellular mechanisms of cognitive impairments in tuberous sclerosis. J Intellect Disabil Res 53: 838–851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Swiech L, Perycz M, Malik A, Jaworski J (2008) Role of mTOR in physiology and pathology of the nervous system. Biochim Biophys Acta 1784: 116–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Van Slegtenhorst M, Nellist M, Nagelkerken B, Cheadle J, Snell R, et al. (1998) Interaction between hamartin and tuberin, the TSC1 and TSC2 gene products. Hum Mol Genet 7: 1053–1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Inoki K, Li Y, Zhu T, Wu J, Guan KL (2002) TSC2 is phosphorylated and inhibited by Akt and suppresses mTOR signaling. Nat Cell Biol 4: 648–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Patel PH, Thapar N, Guo L, Martinez M, Maris J, et al. (2003) Drosophila Rheb GTPase is required for cell cycle progression and cell growth. J Cell Sci 116: 3601–3610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Saucedo LJ, Gao X, Chiarelli DA, Li L, Pan D, et al. (2003) Rheb promotes cell growth as a component of the insulin/TOR signaling network. Nat Cell Bio 5: 566–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Zhang Y, Gao X, Saucedo LJ, Ru B, Edgar BA, et al. (2003) Rheb is a direct target of the tuberous sclerosis tumor suppressor proteins. Nat Cell Biol 5: 578–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Long X, Lin Y, Ortiz-Vega S, Yonezawa K, Avruch J (2005) Rheb binds and regulates the mTOR kinase. Curr Biol 15: 702–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Wang T, Lao U, Edgar BA (2009) TOR-mediated autophagy regulates cell death in Drosophila neurodegenerative disease. J Cell Biol 186: 703–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Roscic A, Baldo B, Crochemore C, Marcellin D, Paganetti P (2011) Induction of autophagy with catalytic mTOR inhibitors reduces huntingtin aggregates in a neuronal cell model. J Neurochem 119: 398–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Zhou X, Ikenoue T, Chen X, Li L, Inoki K, et al. (2009) Rheb controls misfolded protein metabolism by inhibiting aggresome formation and autophagy. PNAS 22: 8923–8928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Tavazoie SF, Alvarez VA, Ridenour DA, Kwiatkowski DJ, Sabatini BL (2005) Regulation of neuronal morphology and function by the tumor suppressors Tsc1 and Tsc2. Nat Neurosci 8: 1727–1734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Meikle L, Pollizzi K, Egnor A, Kramvis I, Lane H, et al. (2008) Response of a Neuronal Model of Tuberous Sclerosis to Mammalian Target of Rapamycin (mTOR) Inhibitors: Effects on MTORC1 and Akt Signaling Lead to Improved Survival and Function. J Neurosci 28: 5422–5432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Nobukini T, Thomas G (2004) The mTOR/S6K signaling pathway: the role of the TSC1/2 tumour suppressor complex and the proto-oncogene Rheb. Novartis Found Symp 262: 148–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Meikle L, Talos DM, Onda H, Pollizzi K, Rotenberg A, et al. (2007) A mouse model of Tuberous Sclerosis: Neuronal loss of Tsc1 causes dysplastic and ectopic neurons, reduced myelination, seizure activity and limited survival. J Neurosci 27: 5546–5558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Uhlmann EJ, Wong M, Baldwin RL, Bajenaru ML, Onda H, et al. (2002) Astrocyte-specific TSC1 conditional knockout mice exhibit abnormal neuronal organization and seizures. Ann Neurol 52: 285–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Ehninger D, Han S, Shilyansky C, Zhou Y, Li W, et al. (2008) Reversal of learning deficits in a Tsc2+/− mouse model of tuberous sclerosis. Nat Med 14: 843–848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Goorden SM, van Woerden GM, van der Weerd L, Cheadle JP, Elgersma Y (2007) Cognitive deficits in Tsc1+/− mice in the absence of cerebral lesions and seizures. Ann Neurol 62: 648–655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Wang Y, Greenwood JS, Calcagnotto ME, Kirsch HE, Barbaro NM, et al. (2007) Neocortical hyperexcitability in a human case of Tuberous Sclerosis Complex and mice lacking neuronal expression of TSC1 . Ann Neurol 61: 139–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Knox S, Ge H, Dimitroff BD, Ren Y, Howe KA, et al. (2007) Mechanisms of TSC-mediated control of synapse assembly and axon guidance. PLoS One 2: e375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Karbowniczek M, Zitserman D, Khabibullin D, Hartman T, Yu J, et al. (2010) The evolutionarily conserved TSC/Rheb pathway activates Notch in tuberous sclerosis complex and Drosophila external sensory organ development. J Clin Invest 120: 93–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Brand AH, Perrimon N (1993) Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development 118: 401–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Aris JP, Blobel G (1988) Identification and characterization of a yeast nucleolar protein that is similar to a rat liver nucleolar protein. JCB 107: 17–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kaun KR, Azanchi R, Maung Z, Hirsh J, Heberlein U (2011) A Drosophila model for alcohol reward. Nat Neurosci 14: 612–619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Buckingham SD, Esmaeili B, Wood M, Sattelle D (2004) RNA interference: from model organisms towards therapy for neural and neuromuscular disorders. Hum Mol Gen 13: R275–R288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Heisenberg M, Borst A, Wagner S, Byers D (1985) Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. J Neurogenet 2: 1–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Margulies C, Tully T, Dubnau J (2005) Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol 15: R700–713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Davis RL (2011) Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron 70: 8–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Ito K, Awano W, Suzuki K, Hiromi Y, Yamamoto D (1997) The Drosophila mushroom body is a quadruple structure of clonal units each of which contains a virtually identical set of neurons and glial cells. Development 124: 761–771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Lee T, Lee A, Luo L (1999) Development of the Drosophila mushroom bodies: sequential generation of three distinct types of neurons from a neuroblast. Development 126: 4065–4076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Connolly JB, Roberts IJH, Armstrong JD, Kaiser K, Forte M, et al. (1996) Associative learning disrupted by impaired Gs signaling in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Science 274: 2104–2107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Chen C-C, Wu J-K, Lin H-W, Pai T-P, Fu T-F, et al. (2012) Visualizing Long-Term Memory Formation in Two Neurons of the Drosophila Brain. Science 335: 678–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Aso Y, Grubel K, Busch S, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, et al. (2009) The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J Neurogenet 23: 156–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Zhang K, Guo JZ, Peng Y, Xi W, Guo A (2007) Dopamine-mushroom body circuit regulates saliency-based decision-making in Drosophila. Science 316: 1901–1904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Joseph RM, Devineni AV, King IF, Heberlein U (2009) Oviposition preference for and positional avoidance of acetic acid provide a model for competing behavioral drives in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 11352–11357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. McBride SM, Giuliani G, Choi C, Krause P, Corrleale D, et al. (1999) Mushroom body ablation impairs short-term memory and long-term memory of courtship conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron 24: 967–977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Tempel BL, Bonini N, Dawson DR, Quinn WG (1983) Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci 80: 1482–1486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Krashes MJ, Waddell S (2008) Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. J Neurosci 28: 3103–3113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Coloumb J, Kaiser L, Chabaud MA, Preat Y (2009) Parametric and genetic analysis of Drosophila appetitive long-term memory and sugar motivation. Genes Brain Behav 8: 407–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Cao C, Brown MR (2001) Localization of an insulin-like peptide in brains of two flies. Cell Tissue Res 304: 317–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Brogiolo W, Stocker H, Ikeya T, Rintelen F, Fernandez R, et al. (2001) An evolutionary conserved function of the Drosophila insulin receptor and insulin-like peptides in growth control. Curr Biol 11: 213–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Ikeya T, Galic M, Belawat P, Nairz K, Hafen E (2002) Nutrient-dependent expression of insulin-like peptides from neuroendocrine cells in the CNS contributes to growth regulation in Drosophila . Curr Biol 12: 1293–1300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Rulifson EJ, Kim SK, Nusse R (2002) Ablation of insulin-producing neurons in flies: growth and diabetic phenotypes. Science 296: 1118–1120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Jaworski J, Spangler S, Seeburg DP, Hoogenraad CC, Sheng M (2005) Control of dendritic arborization by the phosphoinoditide-3′-kinase-Akt-mammalian target of rapamycin pathway. J Neurosci 25: 11300–11312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Way SW, McKenna J 3rd, Mietzsch U, Reith RM, Wu HC, et al. (2009) Loss of Tsc2 in radial glia models the brain pathology of tuberous sclerosis complex in the mouse. Human Mol Genet 18: 1252–1265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Goldsmith AD, Sarin S, Lockery S, Hobert O (2010) Developmental control of lateralized neuron size in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Neural Dev 5: 33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Zielke N, Kim KJ, Tran V, Shibutani ST, Bravo MJ, et al. (2011) Control of Drosophila endocycles by E2F and CRL4(CDT2). Nature 480: 123–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Saucedo LJ, Edgar BA (2002) Why size matters: altering cell size. Curr Opin Genet Dev 12: 565–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Floricel f, Higaki K, Maki H, Nanba E, Ninomiya H, et al. (2007) Antisense suppression of TSC1 gene product, hamartin, enhances neurite outgrowth in NGF-treated PC12h cells. Brain Dev 29: 502–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Choi Y-J, Di Nardo A, Kramvis I, Meikle L, Kwiatkowski DJ, et al. (2008) Tuberous sclerosis complex proteins control axon formation. Genes Dev 22: 2485–2495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Martin-Pena A, Acebes A, Rodrigues JR, Sorribes A, dePolavieja GG, et al. (2006) Age-independent synaptogenesis by phosphoinositide 3 kinase. J Neurosci 26: 10199–101208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Fingar DC, Richardson CJ, Tee AR, Cheatham L, Tsou C, et al. (2004) mTOR controls cell cycle progression through its cell growth effectors S6K1 and 4E-BP1/eukaryotic translation initiation factor 4E. Mol Cell Bio 24: 200–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Ueno S, Ueno T, Iwao Y (2011) Role of the PI3K-TOR-S6K pathway in the onset of cell cycle elongation during Xenopus early embryogenesis. Dev Growth Differ 53: 924–933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Wang S, Tulina N, Carlin D, Rulifson E (2007) The origin of islet-like cells in Drosophila identifies parallels to the vertebrate endocrine axis. PNAS 104: 19873–19878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Nouspikel T, Hanawalt PC (2003) When parsimony backfires: neglecting DNA repair may doom neurons in Alzheimer's disease. Bioessays 25: 168–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Patel PH, Tamanoi F (2006) Increased Rheb-TOR signaling enhances sensitivity of the whole organism to oxidative stress. J Cell Sci 119: 4285–4292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Habib SL, Michel D, Masliah E, Thomas B, Ko HS, et al. (2008) Role of tuberin in neuronal degeneration. Neurochem Res 33: 1113–1116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Thum AS, Jenett A, Ito K, Heisenberg M, Tanimoto H (2007) Multiple memory traces for olfactory reward learning in Drosophila. J Neurosci 27: 11132–11138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Cervantes-Sandoval I, Davis RL (2012) Distinct traces for appetitive versus aversive olfactory memories in DPM neurons of Drosophila. Curr Biol 22: 1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Schwaertzel M, Monastirioti M, Scholz H, Friggi-Grelin F, Birman S, et al. (2003) Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci 23: 10495–10502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Liu C, Placais PY, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, et al. (2012) A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature doi: 10.1038/nature11304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Shohat-Ophir G, Kaun KR, Azanchi R, Heberlein U (2012) Sexual deprivation increases ethanol intake in Drosophila. Science 336: 541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, et al. (2009) A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell 139: 416–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Keene AC, Krashes MJ, Leung B, Bernard JA, Waddell S (2006) Drosophila dorsal paried medial neurons provide a general mechanism for memory consolidation. Curr Biol 16: 1524–1530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Krashes MJ, Keene AC, Leung B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S (2007) Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron 53: 103–115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Burke CJ, Waddell S (2011) Remembering nutrient quality of sugar in Drosophila. Curr Biol 21: 746–750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Fujita M, Tanimura T (2011) Drosophila evaluates and learns the nutritive value of sugars. Curr Biol 21: 751–755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Kwon CH, Luikart BW, Powell CM, Zhou J, Matheny SA, et al. (2006) Pten regulates neuronal arborization and social interaction in mice. Neuron 50: 377–388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Talos DM, Kwiatkowski DJ, Cordero K, Black PM, Jensen FE (2008) Cell-specific alterations of glutamate receptor expression in tuberous sclerosis complex cortical tubers. Ann Neurol 63: 454–465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Li Y-H, Werner H, Püschel AW (2008) Rheb and mTOR regulate neuronal polarity through Rap1B. J Biol Chem 283: 33784–33792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Wildonger J, Jan LY, Jan YN (2008) The Tsc1-Tsc2 complex influences neuronal polarity by modulating TORC1 activity and SAD levels. Genes Dev 22: 2447–2453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. von der Brelie C, Waltereit R, Zhang L, Beck H Kirschestein T (2006) Impaired synaptic plasticity in a rat model of tuberous sclerosis. Eur J Neurosci 23: 686–692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]