Skip to main content
British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology logoLink to British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology
. 2013 Feb 5;75(3):738–755. doi: 10.1111/bcp.12058

Nutrition and neurodegeneration: epidemiological evidence and challenges for future research

Sophie Gillette‐Guyonnet 1,2,3, Marion Secher 1, Bruno Vellas 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC3575940  PMID: 23384081

Abstract

The prevention of dementias, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD), is a growing public health concern, due to a lack of effective curative treatment options and a rising global prevalence. Various potential risk or preventive factors have been suggested by epidemiological research, including modifiable lifestyle factors such as diet. Current epidemiological data are in favour of a protective role of certain micronutrients (B vitamins related to homocysteine metabolism, the anti‐oxidant vitamins C and E, flavonoids, polyunsatured omega‐3 fatty acids, vitamin D) and macronutrients (fish) in the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia/AD. Some factors have been targeted by interventions tested in randomized controlled trials (RCTs), but many of the results are conflicting with observational evidence. Epidemiological analysis of the relations between nutrient consumption and cognitive decline is complex and it is highly unlikely that a single component plays a major role. In addition, since multiple factors across the life course influence brain function in late life, multidomain interventions might be more promising in the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia/AD. Designing such trials remains very challenging for researchers. The main objective of this paper is to review the epidemiologic data linking potential protective factors to cognitive decline or dementia/AD, focusing particularly on the roles of adiposity, caloric restriction, micro (group B vitamins related to homocysteine metabolism, the anti‐oxidant vitamins C and E, flavonoids, polyunsatured omega‐3 fatty acids, vitamin D) and macronutrients (fish). Limitations of the current data, divergence with results of interventional prevention studies and challenges for future research are discussed.

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, dietary patterns, multidomain intervention, nutrients, prevention


The ageing of the population which affects all developed countries leads to an increase in age‐related diseases, headed by the dementias and by Alzheimer's disease (AD) in particular. At the present time, AD accounts for 70% of prevalent dementias. Its incidence is increasing markedly and according to current predictions the numbers of persons affected will double every 20 years [1]. In their recently published study, Brookmeyer et al. [1] estimated at 26.6 million the number of Alzheimer's patients worldwide in 2006 (ranging between 11.4 to 59.4 million according to the geographical area considered). Their forecast for the future indicates that this number could be multiplied by four by 2050 and reach 106.8 million (variation of 47.2 to 221.2 million), affecting one in 85 persons. The onset of AD is generally insidious. It appears increasingly evident that the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms are active long before the appearance of the clinical symptoms of the disease. In the absence of curative treatments, prevention appears to open up interesting perspectives. Projections of epidemiological data show us that while the prevalence of AD is 5% after 65 years and 25% after 85 years, delaying the onset of the clinical phase of the disease by just 1 year reduces its prevalence by 25%, and a 5 year delay in onset would decrease the prevalence in the population by 50% after 5 years of application of preventive measures [2]. Cognitive impairment can be influenced by a number of factors and the potential effect of nutrition has become a topic of increasing scientific and public interest. Current epidemiological data are in favour of a protective role of certain micronutrients (group B vitamins related to homocysteine metabolism, the anti‐oxidant vitamins C and E, flavonoids, polyunsatured omega‐3 fatty acids, vitamin D) and macronutrients (fish) in the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia/AD [3]. There is also increasing evidence for the role of total adiposity, usually measured clinically by body mass index (BMI), and central adiposity in AD [4]. In addition, it is now well established that caloric restriction could be used to promote successful brain ageing [5]. Increasing attention is being paid, not to the characteristics of the subjects in the years preceding diagnosis, but in a more global manner to the subject's entire life and in particular to the mid‐life period towards the age of 50 years [6,7]. Clarification of the relation between nutrition and cognition through epidemiological studies is important because consistent evidence of a prospective association would more strongly support the need for intervention trials testing the effectiveness of nutrient therapy in preventing cognitive decline and dementia. The aim of this paper is to review the epidemiological data linking potential nutritional factors to risk of cognitive decline and dementia/AD, focusing particularly on the roles of adiposity, caloric restriction, micro (group B vitamins related to homocysteine metabolism, the anti‐oxidant vitamins C and E, flavonoids, polyunsatured omega‐3 fatty acids, vitamin D) and macronutrients (fish). Results from recent meta‐analyses, reviews, longitudinal studies and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) assessing the prevention of both cognitive decline and dementia/AD are included. Limitation of the current data, divergence with results of interventional prevention studies and challenges for future research are discussed.

Summary of findings from prospective studies and RCTs

BMI, cognitive decline and risk of dementia/AD

There is increasing evidence for the role of total adiposity and central adiposity in AD [818]. Data remain however conflicting. A recent review [4] including analyses of salient studies published in 2007 and 2008 concluded that (i) central adiposity in middle age predicts dementia in old age, (ii) the relation between high adiposity and dementia is attenuated with older age, (iii) waist circumference in old age (a measure of central adiposity) may be a better predictor of dementia than BMI and (iv) lower BMI predicts dementia in elderly people and weight loss may precede dementia diagnosis by decades, which may explain seemingly paradoxical findings [19,20,21,22,23,24]. In a recent paper studying the potential relationship between healthy lifestyle in mid‐life and risk of dementia in 3468 Japanese‐American men followed in the Honolulu Asia Aging study within 25 years, authors found after examining each factor individually that BMI in mid‐life was most strongly associated with greater risk of overall dementia in later‐life (OR = 1.87, 95% CI = 1.26, 2.77; BMI >25.0 vs. <22.6 kg m−2) [b25].

In addition, post‐mortem examinations in 298 individuals, who were followed in the Honolulu‐Asia Aging study, found that low BMI was associated with a higher degree of AD pathology [b26]. Moreover, obesity in individuals with high levels of adiposity in mid‐life is associated with neuronal and/or myelin abnormalities, primarily in the frontal lobe and so might have an effect on accelerated ageing [b18].

In total, prevention and manipulation of adiposity in mid‐life may provide a means to prevent cognitive decline and dementia/AD.

Caloric restriction, cognitive decline and risk of dementia/AD

Accumulation of experimental evidence suggests that caloric restriction may offer protection against age‐related neuronal loss and neurodegenerative disorders. Hypotheses linking caloric restriction to cognitive ability include increase in insulin sensitivity, anti‐inflammatory mechanisms (les 2), reduction of neural oxidative stress, promotion of synaptic plasticity, induction of various stress and neurotrophic/neuroprotective factors [b5,b27]. Data from RCTs in adults are however limited. To our knowledge, there are only two RCTs in humans which examined the effect of caloric restriction alone or in combination with physical exercise on cognitive function. In the GALERIE study (Comprehensive Assessment of long term Effects of reducing Intake of Energy) [b28], 48 participants were randomized to one of four groups: control (weight maintenance) (mean age 37 ± 7 years), caloric restriction (25% restriction) (mean age 39 ± 5 years), caloric restriction plus structured exercise (12.5% restriction plus 12.5% increased energy expenditure via exercise) (mean age 36 ± 6 years) or low calorie diet (890 kcal day−1 diet until 15% weight loss, followed by weight maintenance) (mean age 38 ± 8 years). Cognitive tests (verbal memory, visual memory, attention/concentration) were conducted at baseline and months 3 and 6. No consistent pattern of verbal memory, visual retention/memory, or attention/concentration deficits emerged during the trial. Daily energy deficit was not significantly associated with change in cognitive test performance. These conclusions must be interpreted in the context of study limitations (small sample size and limited statistical power). More recently, in the ENCORE study (Exercise and Nutrition Interventions for Cardiovascular Health) [b29], 124 participants, mean age of 52 ± 9 years, with elevated blood pressure (BP) (systolic BP 130 to 159 mm Hg or diastolic BP 85 to 99 mm Hg) who were sedentary and overweight or obese (BMI 25 to 40 kg m−2) were randomized to the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet alone, DASH combined with a behavioural weight management programme including exercise and caloric restriction or a usual diet control group. Participants completed a battery of neurocognitive tests of executive function‐memory learning and psychomotor speed at baseline and 4 months after the intervention. Participants on the DASH diet combined with a behavioural weight management programme exhibited greater improvements in executive function‐memory learning (P = 0.008) and psychomotor speed (P = 0.023) compared with the usual diet control. Neurocognitive improvements appeared to be mediated by increased aerobic fitness and weight loss.

In total, because the long term effects (over 1 year) of caloric restriction in older people are not yet known, precautions should be taken before engaging in such a caloric restriction regimen. In order to design new RCTs in the future and propose specific recommendations, additional studies are needed to determine if the effects of caloric restriction are long lasting or only active during the restriction period, as well as the length the caloric restriction must be continued to see benefits in adults. Moreover, it seems that ‘multidomain’ interventions will be proposed in the future combining caloric restriction with other lifestyle components, such as physical exercise. Finally, the clarification of the mechanisms through which caloric restriction may beneficially influence AD neuropathology will help in the development of lifestyle therapeutic strategies in AD and possibly other neurodegenerative disorders.

Nutrients, cognitive decline and risk of dementia/AD

Anti‐oxidants

Experimental, clinical, neuropathological and epidemiological investigations have implicated oxidative stress as a possible factor in the pathogenesis of cognitive decline and dementia. Multiple lines of evidence indicate that oxidative stress not only strongly participates in an early stage of AD prior to cytopathology, but plays an important role in inducing and activating multiple cell signalling pathways that contribute to the lesion formations of toxic substances and then promote the development of AD.

Exogenous anti‐oxidants (such as vitamin E and C, carotenoids, flavonoids) decrease free radical mediated damage caused by toxic chain reactions in neuronal cells and reduce the toxicity of beta‐amyloid in in vitro studies of brains of patients with AD [b30]. The use of anti‐oxidants for prevention of dementia has been evaluated in several observational studies with varying results [3137]. Two large prospective studies reported that higher dietary intake of anti‐oxidants was associated with a lower risk of AD [b31,b32]. Nevertheless, these studies had significant limitations such as the use of a food frequency questionnaire to estimate nutrient intake in patients with potential cognitive impairment. Then, neither study found an association between anti‐oxidant supplements and AD risk but only with anti‐oxidant intake from food.

Several RCTs with vitamin E and cognitive decline have been published in older adults with normal cognition, but none has found an association between supplementation and decreased risk of cognitive decline over follow‐up times [b38b40]. A long term supplementation in β‐carotene (50 mg every other day for 18 years) showed a beneficial effect compared with the placebo group [b41].

Some studies examined the relationship between combinations of anti‐oxidants on cognitive performance. A recent RCT on an anti‐oxidants complex supplementation (including 34 elements) has shown an increased cognitive performance during the 4 months of follow‐up compared with the placebo group [b42]. In the SU.VI.MAX study (Supplementation in Vitamins and Mineral Anti‐oxidants) [b43], 4447 French participants aged 45–60 years were randomized to receive daily vitamin C (120 mg), β‐carotene (6 mg), vitamin E (30 mg), selenium (100 μg) and zinc (20 mg) in combination or as a placebo. Six years after the end of the trial, subjects receiving active anti‐oxidant supplementation had better episodic memory scores (mean difference 0.61, 95% CI 0.02, 1.20).

In addition, the neuroprotective effects of natural anti‐oxidants, such as Ginkgo biloba, have been suggested. Results from the Ginkgo Evaluation of Memory (GEM) randomized controlled trial [b44] provided, however, evidence that treatment with Ginkgo biloba does not slow the rate of cognitive decline in older adults. Through examination of the effect modification by baseline cognitive status (mild cognitive impairment vs. normal cognition), the authors concluded that Ginkgo biloba affected neither cognitive changes associated with dementia prodrome nor cognitive changes associated with normal ageing [b44,b45]. An earlier article from the GEM study [b46] reported that Ginkgo biloba was not effective in reducing the overall incidence rate of dementia or AD. A lower risk of progression of cognitive decline was found in a small RCT involving 118 older adults with no cognitive impairment (84 years and over) [b47]. However, treatment was also associated with an increased incidence of strokes in the treatment group [b47]. Results of the French GuidAge trial conducted in 2854 older adults (70 years and over) reporting memory complaints and treated during 5 years, showed that long term use of standardized Ginkgo biloba extract (5 years) did not reduce the risk of progression to AD compared with placebo [b48]. Finally, in a Cochrane review including 45 RCTs (36 small RCTs of duration less than 3 months plus nine RCTs of 6 months duration), authors concluded that Ginkgo biloba appears to be safe in use with no excess side effects compared with placebo but they did not find evidence that Ginkgo biloba has predictable and clinically significant benefit for people with cognitive impairment or dementia [b49]. The authors underlined the unsatisfactory methods used in these trials. A major limitation of this review could be the combined evaluation of cognitive decline and dementia [b50]. A statistically significant advantage of Ginkgo biloba compared with placebo in improving cognition for demented patients was recently described in the meta‐analysis of Weinmann et al. [b50] (nine trials, 12 to 52 weeks duration, 2372 patients).

In total, the results on anti‐oxidant nutrients and cognitive decline or dementia may suggest the importance of having a balanced combination of several anti‐oxidant nutrients in order to exert a significant preventive effect on cognitive decline and dementia. We must, however, use these data cautiously for future recommendations. There is a growing concern regarding the risk of mortality associated with anti‐oxidant supplementation. A recent meta‐analysis [b51], studying the effect of anti‐oxidant supplements on mortality in randomized primary and secondary prevention trials, showed that treatment with β‐carotene, vitamin A, or vitamin E might increase mortality. According to the authors, the potential impact of vitamin C and selenium on mortality needs further study. There is also presently no clear evidence that Ginkgo biloba promotes successful cognitive ageing.

Vitamin B

It has now been proved that vitamin B6, vitamin B12 and folate deficiency, as co‐factors in the methylation of homocysteine are associated with increased homocysteine concentrations. Supraphysiological concentrations of homocysteine or deficits in folate and vitamin B12 should promote amyloid and tau protein accumulation and neuronal death, and also have a direct effect on cognitive decline. The potential mechanisms whereby homocysteine might mediate cognitive decline and dementia include: neurotoxicity induced by activation of N‐methl‐D‐aspartate receptors, promotion of apoptosis, vascular injury from promotion of atherogenesis and proliferation of smooth muscle cells, platelet activation and increased burden of ischaemic strokes and white matter lesions [b52]. While there is some evidence that elevated serum homocysteine and/or low serum concentrations of folate, vitamin B6 and vitamin B12 may be associated with impaired cognition and risk of dementia, there are no convincing data that vitamin supplementation prevents dementia [b53b56].

A possible benefit from folate therapy was suggested in a longitudinal (non‐randomized) study of 965 older individuals, in whom a lower incidence of AD was noted among those in the highest quartile of total folate intake after adjustments [b57]. Neither vitamin B6 nor B12 intake was associated with the risk of AD.

Several RCTs tested the effects of supplementation with one or more supplementations with folate, vitamin B12 or vitamin B6 in older healthy adults [58,59,60,61,62]. Only two trials have shown a positive effect on cognition. In the FACIT trial, 818 participants with elevated serum homocysteine aged 50 to 70 years were randomized to receive daily folate supplementation (800 mg day−1) or placebo. The results showed that folate supplementation for 3 years was associated with improved performance on memory, sensorimotor speed and information processing speed compared with placebo [b59]. More recently, a RCT [b62] tested the effect of daily oral supplementation of folate (400 μg day−1) and vitamin B12 (100 μg day−1) in 900 adults aged 60–74 years. Results showed that supplementation improved the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status‐Modified (TICS‐M) total (P = 0.032, effect size d = 0.17), the TICS‐M immediate (P = 0.046, d = 0.15) and TICS‐M delayed recall (P = 0.013, effect size d = 0.18) scores at 24 months in comparison with placebo. No significant changes were evident in orientation, attention, semantic memory, processing speed or informant reports. We are currently waiting the results of the OPEN study [b63] conducted in 200 older people with vitamin B12 deficiency, aged 75 years or over, who were randomly allocated to receive either a daily oral tablet containing vitamin B12 or a matching placebo tablet. Primary outcomes assessed at 12 months were electrophysiological indices of peripheral and central neurosensory responses required for mobility and sensory function. Cognitive performance (including tests of memory, executive function and psychomotor speed) are part of secondary outcomes.

Supplementation with B vitamins was tested in patients with mild to moderate AD in two RCTs [b64,b65]. Only one has reported a positive impact of supplementation (folate 5 mg day−1 and vitamin B12 1 mg day−1 for 24 months) on cognitive decline among patients with elevated plasma homocysteine (>13 μmol l−1) compared with placebo [b65]. Moreover, the cognitive decline and the accelerated rate of brain atrophy could be slowed by treatment with B vitamins (folate 0.8 mg day−1, vitamin B12 0.5 mg day−1 and vitamin B6 20 mg day−1 for 24 months) in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in particular in those with elevated homocysteine [b66,b67] (the VITACOG study).

The existing epidemiological evidence for protective associations of the B vitamins is a first step but it is still limited. A major limitation of many of the prospective studies of B vitamins that could account for the inconsistent findings is the lack of statistical control for dietary confounders. Confounding bias is particularly likely for folate intake as it is associated with many dietary (e.g. anti‐oxidant nutrients, other B vitamins, dietary fats) and other healthy lifestyle variables that have been implicated as protective factors for AD and cognitive decline. More prospective studies are consequently needed that adequately control for dietary confounders including carotenoids, especially lutein, niacin, dietary fats and indicators of vitamin B12 deficiency such as methylmalonic acid.

Vitamin D

Epidemiologic investigations have revealed a beneficial role of vitamin D in muscle function, cardiovascular health, diabetes and cancer prevention. Low levels of serum 25‐hydroxyvitamin D are also associated with increased odds of prevalent cognitive dysfunction, AD and all cause dementia in a number of studies, raising the possibility that vitamin D plays a role in the aetiology of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Vitamin D contributes to neuroprotection by modulating the production of nerve growth, neurotrophin, glial cell derived neurotrophic factor, nitric oxide synthase and choline acetyl transferase [b68], and neuroprotective mechanisms including vasoprotection and amyloid phagocytosis and clearance [b69,b70]. However, the majority of human studies reporting associations between vitamin D and cognition or dementia have been cross‐sectional or case‐control designs. Two recent large prospective studies established a temporal relationship with cognitive decline. First, Slinin et al. [b71] found little evidence of independent associations between lower 25‐hydroxyvitamin D concentration and baseline global and executive cognitive function or incident cognitive decline in a cohort of 1604 men enrolled in the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study and followed them for an average of 4.6 years. Second, low concentrations of vitamin D were associated with cognitive decline over a 6 year observation period in a population of 858 adults enrolled in the InCHIANTI study [b72]. Moreover, it was recently demonstrated that baseline serum vitamin D deficiency predicted the onset of non‐Alzheimer dementias within 7 years among older women from the EPIDOS cohort [b73]. To our knowledge, no large RCT specifically designed to explore the impact of vitamin D supplementation in the prevention of cognitive decline has been published.

In patients with AD, the AD‐IDEA trial [b74] is currently in recruitment. This RCT tested the hypothesis that the combination of memantine and vitamin D may provide enhanced protection against several degenerative processes linked to AD. All patients received memantine 20 mg daily and one of the two treatment options, vitamin D or placebo. The primary outcome was the change in Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale‐cognitive subscale (ADAS‐cog).

In total, prospective studies and RCTs are needed to confirm that vitamin D deficiency is causally related to cognitive decline and to open up important new possibilities for treatment and prevention.

Fatty acids

Fatty acids have been suggested to play a role in modulating the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia based on observational studies. Polyunsaturated fats (PUFA) comprise two major classes, the n‐6 class (e.g. linoleic acid and arachidonic acid) and the n‐3 class [e.g. α‐linolenic acid, eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA)]. Several mechanisms have been postulated for the possible protective role of n‐3 PUFA in dementia. DHA is a key component of membrane phospholipids in the brain and adequate n‐3 PUFA status may help maintain integrity and neuronal function. The oxidative products of PUFA act as key cellular mediators of inflammation, allergy and immunity, oxidative stress, bronchial constriction, vascular response and thrombosis and may thereby influence risk especially for vascular dementia. DHA may be directly involved in enhancing neuronal health in the ageing brain through a range of potential mechanisms. DHA may modify the expression of genes that regulate a variety of biological functions potentially important for cognitive health, including neurogenesis and neuronal function [b75]. Fatty fish is the primary dietary source of longer chain n‐3 fatty acids, EPA and DHA. In observational studies, high dietary intake of saturated and trans‐unsaturated (hydrogenated) fats may increase the risk of cognitive decline and AD [b76,b77], but evidence is conflicting [b78]. The potential effects of dietary intake of fish and omega‐3 fatty acids on the risk of dementia or cognitive decline have been studied as well. The findings have been mixed, but most studies have shown a benefit for higher fish consumption [b79b82] or on the accumulation of white matter abnormalities measured on brain MRI [b83]. Some studies have investigated the effects of specific omega‐3 fatty acids, especially DHA and found dietary DHA in the highest tertile measured at baseline was associated with lower rates of incident dementia over follow‐up [b84,b85].

Four RCTs have been published on the effects of specific omega‐3 fatty acids in normal healthy adults on cognitive performance [b86b89]. Only one of them [b87] has found positive results by investigating the effect of a daily oral DHA supplementation (900 mg day−1) for 24 weeks in 485 adults aged 55 years and older on cognitive functions. Intention‐to‐treat analysis showed significantly fewer Paired Associate Learning (PAL) six pattern errors with DHA vs. placebo at 24 weeks (difference score, −1.63 ± 0.76, 95% CI −3.1, −0.14, P = 0.03). DHA supplementation was also associated with improved immediate and delayed verbal recognition memory (VRM) scores (P < 0.02), but not working memory or executive function tests.

In patients with AD or MCI, none RCT have supported a therapeutic role for omega‐3 fatty acid supplementation [b90,b91]. Freund‐Levi et al [b91] have reported a positive effect of n‐3 fatty acid supplementation on cognitive functions but in a small group of patients with very mild AD (MMSE >27 points).

High intakes of saturated and trans‐unsaturated (hydrogenated) fats were positively associated with increased risk of AD, whereas high intakes of polyunsaturated and monounsaturated fats were protective against cognitive decline in the elderly in prospective studies. Fish consumption (n‐3 PUFA) has been associated with lower risk of AD in longitudinal cohort studies. n‐3 PUFAs could have a preventive effect against dementia through their anti‐thrombotic and anti‐inflammatory properties in addition to their specific effect on neural functions.

Combinations of nutrients

The hypothesis that combinations of certain nutrients could provide clinically relevant benefits to patients with AD formed the basis of the development of the medical food ‘Souvenaid’, which is a multinutrient drink designed to improve synapse formation [b92]. The efficacy, tolerability and safety of ‘Souvenaid’ was tested in a proof of concept clinical trial conducted in 225 drug‐naïve AD patients (mean MMSE score of 23.9, mean age of 73.7 years). Patients were randomized to active product ‘Souvenaid’ or a control drink, taken once daily for 12 weeks. The active product ‘Souvenaid’ contains a specific formulation of nutrients registered as Fortasyn Connect (EPA 300 mg/daily dose, DHA 1200 mg/daily dose, Vitamin E 40 mg/daily dose, Vitamin C 80 mg/daily dose, vitamin B12 3 μg/daily dose, vitamin B6 1 mg/daily dose and Folate 400 μg/daily dose) plus other vitamins, minerals, trace elements and macronutrients. Primary outcome measures were the delayed verbal recall task of the Wechsler Memory Scale‐revised (WMS‐r) and the 13‐item modified ADAS‐cog at week 12. Results showed a significant improvement in the WMS‐r delayed verbal recall in the active group compared with control (P = 0.026) but not in modified ADAS‐cog. In the subgroup of patients with very mild AD (120 patients with baseline MMSE between 24 and 26), results showed a greater improvement in the WMS‐r delayed verbal recall in the active group compared with control (P = 0.011) and in the WMS‐r immediate verbal recall (P = 0.157) [b92]. Secondary analysis was made in this cohort [b93] where the study population was divided into two subgroups: patients with ‘low’ baseline ADAS‐cog scores (<25.0) and patients with ‘high’ baseline ADAS‐cog scores (≥25.0). Repeated measures models for each patient were used to determine the relationship between ADAS‐cog score and intervention up to 24 weeks. A significant treatment effect (P = 0.046) was shown in patients with ‘high’ baseline ADAS‐cog, but not in patients with ‘low’ baseline ADAS‐cog. Overall, intake adherence was significantly correlated with ADAS‐cog improvement in the active product group (correlation coefficient = −0.260; P = 0.019), but not in the control group.

In addition, the effects of B vitamins and omega‐3 fatty acid supplementation on cognition in a high risk population were tested in an ancillary study of the SU.FOL.OM3 (Supplementation with FOLate, vitamins B6 and B12 and/or omega‐3 fatty acids) secondary prevention trial conducted in France between 2003 and 2009 [b94]. The present sample included 1748 men and women aged 45–80 years with a history of myocardial infarction, unstable angina or ischaemic stroke. Participants were assigned in a 2 × 2 factorial design to one of the following four groups: folate (0, 56 mg), vitamin B6 (3 mg) and vitamin B12 (0, 2 mg); EPA and DHA (600 mg) in a 2:1 ratio; vitamins B and omega‐3 fatty acids; or placebo. No significant main effects on cognitive function (evaluated by the French version of the modified Telephone interview for Cognitive Status) after 4 years of supplementation were found. However, authors found some evidence of disease history‐ and age‐specific effects. In the subgroup with prior stroke, for example, participants assigned to receive B vitamins plus omega‐3 fatty acids were significantly less likely to have a decreased score on the temporal orientation task than those assigned to receive placebo.

Methodological statements

In his paper, Daffner [b95] discussed advantages and limitations of epidemiologic/cohort studies, animal/basic sciences studies, human proof of concept studies, and human intervention studies, the four major lines of evidence available to examine whether a proposed factor my have an impact on the promotion of successful cognitive ageing. According to this author, the advantages of epidemiological/cohort studies include their large number of subjects and their acquisition of information about many factors. Particularly valuable to the study of successful cognitive ageing is the ability of epidemiological studies to follow subjects over years to decades. A major limitation to this line of investigation is that findings can only establish the presence of an association between a designated factor and a clinical outcome, which is not proof of causality. Uncertainty remains about the potential influence of unidentified factors and whether an intervention directed at a particular factor would necessarily change the outcome in clinically significant ways. That can be explain why results found in prospective cohort studies have not been confirmed by RCTs.

Many methodological explanations for divergent observational and experimental results for dementia prevention were also identified in the review proposed by Coley et al. [b96]. The most common methodological limits concern precision to measure some exposures (self or proxy report rather than objective measures leading to misclassification), measurement of exposure variations over time (and not only at one time point), timing of intervention (some interventions may have differential effects according to the time of exposure), confounding factors taken into account (many studies did not consider, for example, the importance of adjusting for the presence of ApoE4). The action of n‐3 PUFA on the ageing brain might therefore differ according to ApoE polymorphism and several epidemiological studies suggest a protective effect of long‐chain n‐3 PUFA on cognitive decline only in those who do not carry the allele 4 but with inconsistent results [b97]), mode of collection of nutritional data used, forms and doses of the vitamins used in randomized controlled studies (Table 1), duration of follow‐up, duration of intervention, insufficient statistical power, variation of outcome definition, and consideration of attrition rate.

Table 1.

Nutrition and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia/Alzheimer's disease: data from randomized controlled trials conducted in cognitively normal elderly subjects

Caloric restriction
 Martin et al. 2007 [28] Outcomes: Cognitive tests (verbal memory, visual memory, attention/concentration) Intervention: Caloric restriction alone or with physical exercise
Follow‐up: 6 months
Participants: n = 48; four groups: control (weight maintenance) (mean age 37 ± 7 years), caloric restriction (25% restriction) (mean age 39 ± 5 years), caloric restriction plus structured exercise (12.5% restriction plus 12.5% increased energy expenditure via exercise) (mean age 36 ± 6 years), or low‐calorie diet (890 kcal day−1 diet until 15% weight loss, followed by weight maintenance) (mean age 38 ± 8 years). Daily energy deficit was not significantly associated with change in cognitive test performance.
 Smith et al. 2010 [29]* Outcomes: Neurocognitive tests of executive function‐memory‐learning and psychomotor speed Intervention: Caloric restriction with physical exercise
Follow‐up: 4 months
Participants: n = 124, mean age of 52 ± 9 years, with elevated blood pressure, sedentary and overweight or obese (BMI 25 to 40 kg m−1); three groups: Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH), diet alone, DASH combined with a behavioural weight management programme including exercise and caloric restriction, or usual diet control group Participants on the DASH diet combined with a behavioural weight management programme exhibited greater improvements in executive function‐memory‐learning (P = 0.008) and psychomotor speed (P = 0.023) compared with the usual diet control.
Anti‐oxidants
 Yaffe et al. 2004 [38] Outcomes: AREDS (Age‐Related Eye Disease Study) cognitive battery included six validated cognitive tests with eight components: The Modified MMSE, Animal Category, Letter Fluency, Logical Memory part I and II, Wechsler Memory Scale Revised, Immediate Recall, Word List Mean; Buschke Selective Reminding Test and Digits Backwards. Intervention: Anti‐oxidants or zinc and copper or both
Follow‐up: Median of 6.9 years
Participants: n = 2 166, mean age of 75 ± 5 years, four groups: daily oral tablets containing anti‐oxidants (vitamin C, 500 mg; vitamin E, 400 IU; beta carotene, 15 mg), zinc and copper (zinc, 80 mg; cupric oxide, 2 mg), anti‐oxidants plus zinc and copper or placebo Compared with placebo, anti‐oxidants with or without zinc and copper did not have a significant effect on cognitive performance
 Kang et al. 2006 [39] Outcomes: Three cognitive assessments of general cognition, verbal memory and category fluency administered by telephone at 2 year intervals. The primary outcome was a global composite score averaging performance on all tests Intervention: Vitamin E
Follow‐up: 2 years
Participants: n = 39 876 women, 65 years or older, two groups: 600 IU of vitamin E on alternate days or placebo No differences in global score between the vitamin E and placebo groups at the first assessment (5.6 years after randomization) or at the last assessment (9.6 years of treatment)
 Grodstein et al. 2007 [41]* Outcomes: General cognition, verbal memory and category fluency. The primary end point was a global score averaging all tests Intervention: Beta carotene
Follow‐up: 18 years
Participants: n = 5 956 men, older than 65 years, two groups: beta carotene (50 mg, alternate days) or placebo No impact of short term (1 year) β‐carotene supplementation on cognitive performance
The mean global score was significantly higher in the beta carotene group than in the placebo group in long term supplementation (18 years) (mean difference in z scores, 0.047 standard units; P = 0.03)
On verbal memory, men receiving long term beta carotene supplementation also performed significantly better than the placebo group (mean difference in z scores, 0.063; P = 0.007)
 Kang et al. 2009 [40] Outcomes: Five cognitive tests assessed by telephone. The primary outcome was a global composite score averaging all scores Intervention: Vitamin E, vitamin C, beta carotene
Follow‐up: 5.4 years
Participants: n = 2 824 women, 65 years or older, with CVD or ≥three coronary risk factors, three groups: vitamin E (402 mg every other day), beta carotene (50 mg every other day), and vitamin C (500 mg daily) Vitamin E supplementation and beta carotene supplementation were not associated with slower rates of cognitive change
Although vitamin C supplementation was associated with better performance at the last assessment (mean difference, 0.13; 95% confidence interval, 0.06 to 0.20; P = 0.0005), it was not associated with cognitive change over time
 Summers et al. 2010 [42]* Outcomes: Memory testing with a 50 part paired association test and a 20‐word immediate recall test Intervention: Complex anti‐oxidant blend (including 34 elements)
Follow‐up: 4 months
Participants: n = 86, aged of 50 to 75 years, two groups: active treatment or placebo Memory testing with a 50 part paired association test and a 20 word immediate recall test were significantly improved, P = 0.015 and P = 0.005 respectively
 Kesse‐Guyot et al. 2011 [43]* Outcomes: four neuropsychological tests (six tasks) Intervention: Complex anti‐oxidant blend
Follow‐up: 6 years
Participants: n = 4 447, aged 45–60 years, two groups: daily vitamin C (120 mg), β‐carotene (6 mg), vitamin E (30 mg), selenium (100 μg), and zinc (20 mg) in combination or placebo Subjects receiving active anti‐oxidant supplementation had better episodic memory scores (mean difference: 0.61; 95% CI 0.02, 1.20)
Gingko biloba
 DeKosky et al. 2008
 Snitz et al. 2009 [44,46]
Outcomes: Rates of change over time in the Modified MMSE, in the ADAS‐Cog, and in neuropsychological domains of memory, attention, visual‐spatial construction, language, and executive functions Intervention: Ginkgo biloba
Follow‐up: Median follow‐up of 6.1 year
Participants: n = 3 069 older adults aged 75 years or older (approximately 15% with MCI at baseline), two groups: Ginkgo biloba extract 120 mg twice daily or placebo After a median of 6 years of follow‐up, treatment did not reduce the risk of all‐causes of dementia in the cohort (HR = 1.12, 95% CI 0.94, 1.33) or in the subset of patients with MCI at baseline (HR = 1.13, CI 0.85, 1.50)
 Dodge et al. 2008 [47]* Outcomes: Risk of progression from CDR = 0 to CDR = 0.5 and decline in episodic memory Intervention: Ginkgo biloba
Follow‐up: 42 months
Participants: n = 118 older adults aged of 84 years or over Treatment group had a lower risk of progression of cognitive decline (HR = 0.33, P = 0.02) but also an increased incidence of strokes (P = 0.01)
 Vellas et al. 2012 [48] Outcomes: conversion to probable AD Intervention: Ginkgo biloba
Follow‐up: 5 years
Participants: n = 2 854 adults aged 70 years or older who spontaneously reported memory complaints, two groups: twice per day dose of 120 mg standardised Ginkgo biloba extract (EGb761) or matched placebo. By 5 years, 61 participants in the Ginkgo group had been diagnosed with probable Alzheimer's disease (1.2 cases per 100 person‐years) compared with 73 participants in the placebo group [1.4 cases per 100 person‐years; hazard ratio (HR) 0.84, 95% CI 0.60, 1.18; P = 0.306], but the risk was not proportional
Vitamins B
 McMahon et al. 2006 [58] Outcomes: plasma homocysteine concentration and tests of cognition Intervention: Folate and vitamin B6 and B12
Follow‐up: 2 years
Participants: n = 276, 65 years or older, with plasma homocysteine concentrations of at least 13 μmol l−1, two groups: daily supplement containing folate (1 000 μg) and vitamins B12 (500 μg) and B6 (10 mg) or placebo On average, the plasma homocysteine concentration was 4.36 μmol l−1 lower in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (P < 0.001)
There were no significant differences between the vitamin and placebo groups in the scores on tests of cognition
 Durga et al. 2007 [59] Outcomes: Change in performance for memory, sensorimotor speed, complex speed, information processing speed and word fluency Intervention: Folate
Follow‐up: 3 years
Participants: n = 818, with elevated serum homocysteine, aged 50 to 70 years, two groups: daily folate supplementation (800 mg day−1) or placebo Folate supplementation for 3 years was associated with improved performance on memory, sensorimotor speed and information processing speed compared with placebo
 Kang et al. 2008 [61] Outcomes: Telephone cognitive function testing was administered up to four times over 5.4 years with five tests of general cognition, verbal memory, and category fluency. Repeated‐measures analyses were conducted, and the primary outcome was a global composite score averaging all test results Intervention: Folate and vitamin B6 and B12
Follow‐up: 5.4 years
Participants: n = 2 009 women, aged 65 years or older, with CVD or ≥three coronary risk factors, two groups: combination of B vitamins (2.5 mg folate day−1, 50 mg vitamin B6 day−1, and 1 mg vitamin B12 day−1) or placebo Mean cognitive change from baseline did not differ between the B vitamin and placebo groups (difference in change in global score: 0.03; 95% CI −0.03, 0.08; P = 0.30)
 Ford et al. 2010 [60] Outcomes: The primary outcome of interest was the change in the ADAS‐cog. A secondary aim of the study was to determine if supplementation with vitamins decreased the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia Intervention: Folate and vitamin B6 and B12
Follow‐up: 8 years
Participants: n = 299 hypertensive men, 75 years and older, two groups: folate (2 mg), vitamin B6 (25 mg), and B12 (500 μg) daily supplementation or placebo over 2 years There was no difference in the ADAS‐cog change from baseline to 24 months between the placebo and vitamins group
There was a non‐significant decrease in the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia over 8 years of follow‐up
 Walker et al. 2012 [62]* Outcomes: Main outcome measures examined change in cognitive functioning by using the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status‐Modified (TICS‐M), the Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (processing speed) and the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly Intervention: Folate and vitamin B12
Follow‐up: 2 years
Participants: n = 900, aged 60–74 years, two groups: daily oral supplementation of folate (400 μg day−1) and vitamin B12 (100 μg day−1) or placebo Supplementation improved the TICS‐M total (P = 0.032; effect size d = 0.17), the TICS‐M immediate (P = 0.046; d = 0.15), and TICS‐M delayed recall (P = 0.013; effect size d = 0.18) scores at 24 months in comparison with placebo
No significant changes were evident in orientation, attention, semantic memory, processing speed, or informant reports
Omega‐3 and fish
 Van de Rest et al. 2008 [86] Outcomes: Extensive neuropsychological test battery that included the cognitive domains of attention, sensorimotor speed, memory, and executive function Intervention: EPA + DHA
Follow‐up: 26 weeks
Participants: n = 302, aged 65 years or older, three groups: 1 800 mg day−1 EPA‐DHA, 400 mg day−1 EPA‐DHA, or placebo At 26 weeks, there were no significant differential changes in any of the cognitive domains for either low dose or high dose fish oil supplementation compared with placebo
 Yurko‐Mauro et al. 2010 [87]* Outcomes: The primary outcome was the CANTAB Paired Associate Learning (PAL) which is a visuospatial learning and episodic memory test Intervention: DHA
Follow‐up: 24 weeks
Participants: n = 485, aged ≥55 years, two groups: 900 mg day−1 of DHA or placebo Intention‐to‐treat analysis demonstrated significantly fewer PAL six pattern errors with DHA vs. placebo at 24 weeks (difference score, −1.63 ± 0.76, 95% CI −3.1, 0.14, P = 0.03)
DHA supplementation was associated with improved immediate and delayed Verbal Recognition Memory scores (P < 0.02), but not working memory or executive function tests (secondary outcomes)
 Dangour et al. 2010 [88] Outcomes: battery of cognitive tests, including the primary outcome, the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Intervention: EPA + DHA
Follow‐up: 24 months
Participants: n = 867, aged 70–79 years, 2 groups: daily 200 mg EPA plus 500 mg DHA or placebo No change in cognitive function scores over 24 months
Intention‐to‐treat analysis showed no significant differences between trial arms at 24 months in the CVLT or any secondary cognitive outcome
 Geleijnse et al. 2011 [89] Outcomes: MMSE Intervention: EPA + DHA or ALA or EPA + DHA + ALA
Follow‐up: 40 months
Participants: n = 2 911 coronary patients aged 60 to 80 years, four groups: margarines that provided 400 mg day−1 of EPA‐DHA, 2 g day−1 of ALA, both EPA‐DHA and ALA, or placebo Changes in MMSE score did not differ significantly between EPA‐DHA and placebo (−0.65 vs. −0.69 points, P = 0.44) or between ALA and placebo (−0.60 vs. −0.74 points, P = 0.12)
The risk of cognitive decline was 1.03 (95% CI 0.84, 1.26, P = 0.80) for EPA‐DHA (vs. placebo) and 0.90 (0.74, 1.10, P = 0.31) for ALA (vs. placebo)
Combination of nutrients
 Andreeva et al. 2011 [94] Outcomes: French version of the modified Telephone interview for Cognitive Status Intervention: Vitamins B9, B6, B12 and omega‐3
Follow‐up: 4 years
Participants: n = 1 748, aged 45–80 years, with a history of myocardial infarction, unstable angina or ischemic stroke, four groups: folate (0, 56 mg), vitamin B6 (3 mg) and vitamin B12 (0, 02 mg); EPA and DHA (600 mg) in a 2 : 1 ratio; vitamins B and omega‐3 fatty acids; placebo No significant main effects on cognitive function after 4 years of supplementation
Physical activity
 Lautenschlager et al. 2008 [122]* Outcomes: ADAS‐cog score Intervention: Physical activity
Follow‐up: 18 months
Participants: n = 138 with reported memory problems but no criteria for dementia, two groups: 24 week home‐based programme of physical activity or education and usual care In an intent‐to‐treat analysis, participants in the intervention group improved 0.26 points (95% CI −0.89, 0.54) and those in the usual care group deteriorated 1.04 points (95% CI 0.32, 1.82) on the ADAS‐Cog at the end of the intervention
The absolute difference of the outcome measure between the intervention and control groups was −1.3 points (95% CI −2.38, −0.22) at the end of the intervention
At 18 months, participants in the intervention group improved 0.73 points (95% CI −1.27, 0.03) on the ADAS‐Cog, and those in the usual care group improved 0.04 points (95% CI −0.46, 0.88). Word list delayed recall and CDR sum of boxes improved modestly as well, whereas word list total immediate recall, digit symbol coding, verbal fluency, Beck depression score, and Medical Outcomes 36‐Item Short‐Form physical and mental component summaries did not change significantly
*

Results are positive only for these studies. AD, Alzheimer's disease; ADAS‐cog, Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale‐Cognitive subscale; ALA, alpha‐linolenic acid; BMI, body mass index; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating; CVD, cardiovascular disease; DHA, docosahexaenoic acid; EPA, eicosapentaenoic acid; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging.

Finally, risk factors can change over time, with different life periods having their own sets of exposures to different risk factors. The influence of different aspects of diet is likely to vary over the course of life. It is important to identify the critical periods during which an individual is a greatest risk of damage if exposed to a specific nutrient deficiency.

Perspective for the future: using optimal interventions (e.g. multidomain approach)

At the present time, it is still difficult to propose specific recommendations for the prevention of AD. Epidemiological analysis of the relations between nutrient consumption and cognitive decline is complex and it is highly unlikely that a single component plays a major role. We need to pursue studies which will improve our knowledge of the biochemical mechanisms underlying the pathophysiological processes and identify potential therapeutic agents, and which in a public health perspective will examine food groups and dietary patterns. There is converging evidence that composite dietary patterns, such as the Mediterranean diet, are related to lower risk for cardiovascular disease, several forms of cancer and overall mortality. Recent papers showed that higher adherence to a diet approaching the Mediterranean diet is associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline and dementia/AD [98,99,100,101,102,103]. Several mechanisms have been proposed underlying the suggested protective role of Mediterranean diet against age‐related changes in cognitive function, predementia syndromes and dementia: vascular variables and non‐vascular biological mechanisms such as metabolic, oxidative and inflammatory processes [b104]. This underlines the need to consider interactions between micro‐ and macronutrients for future research. Detailed study of social, economic and cultural transformations of the end of the life cycle, from cessation of professional activity until the end of life, and the consequences of these transformations on food habits, from obtaining supply to actual food intake, is an area of research which could broaden the classic question of social determinants of cognitive decline by repositioning it within the dynamic material and social process of ageing [3].

Accumulating evidence, mainly from longitudinal observational studies, suggests that higher levels of physical activity may help maintain cognitive function during ageing. A systematic review published in 2004 analyzed the results of 15 longitudinal observational studies that addressed the impact of lifestyle with cognition or dementia [6]. Most studies examining the relationship between physical activities including physical leisure activities and both dementia and AD were positive. Several studies continue to support these findings [103,105115]. One study found that in 69 older adults, higher levels of physical exercise over the previous decade were associated with reduced beta‐amyloid uptake on positron emission tomography [b116].

Several RCTs on the association between physical activity and cognitive decline in patients with dementia or MCI were published recently [117,118,119,120,121]. In one trial, 31 patients with dementia (mean age 81.8 ± 5.3 years) were randomized in two groups: the intervention group received a 5 week physical activity programme involving three 1 h sessions per week and the control group had no physical activity. After 15 weeks, subjects from the intervention group improved their Rapid Evaluation of Cognitive Functions test overall score (P < 0.01), while those in the control group decreased it [b118]. In another RCT in 33 patients with amnestic MCI aged 55 to 85 years, the intervention group received a programme of aerobic physical activity (treadmill, stationary bike or elliptical, 45–60 min day−1, four times a week1 for 6 months). The results showed an improvement of executive functions at 6 months among women in the intervention group [b119]. A more recent trial examined the effects of mind body physical exercise (Tai Chi) on cognitive function in 389 patients with a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) of 0.5 or amnestic‐MCI. Patients were randomized into two groups: the intervention group received Tai Chi sessions or stretching and toning exercise at least three times a week and 30 min per day. At 5 months, both groups showed an improvement in global cognitive function, delayed recall and subjective cognitive complaints (P < 0.05), improvements in visual spans and CDR sum of boxes scores were observed in the intervention group (P < 0.001). Logistic regression analysis controlled for baseline group differences in education and cognitive function suggested that the intervention group was associated with stable CDR (OR = 0.14, 95%CI 0.03, 0.71, P = 0.02) [b121].

To our knowledge, there is only one trial on the association between physical activity and risk of dementia which included subjects with cognitive complaint but without criteria of dementia. In this study, 170 adults aged 49 years or over were randomized to receive a programme of physical activity at home (at least 150 min of moderate activity a week) or to an education/usual care. After 24 weeks, patients in the intervention group improved their cognitive performances compared with patients in the placebo group. After 18 months, results were similar [b122].

It is important to know whether physical activity and diet confer independent associations because individuals who exercise often belong to a higher educational‐socioeconomic strata, are more health conscious, and in general tend to follow healthier eating habits. The magnitude of such potential associations with AD in individuals engaging in such activities is also of great interest from a public health point of view. In their study, Scarmeas et al. [b103] investigated the combined association of diet and physical activity with AD risk. Analyses were based on data collected in two cohorts comprising 1880 community‐dwelling elderly without dementia living in New York. A total of 282 incident AD cases occurred during a mean of 5.4 (3.3) years of follow‐up. When considered simultaneously, both Mediterranean‐type diet adherence and physical activity were associated with lower AD risk. Compared with individuals neither adhering to the diet nor participating in physical activity, those both adhering to the diet and participating in physical activity had a lower risk of AD. The investigation of which particular dimensions of lifestyle are associated with disease risk is important. This work supports the idea that intervention focused only on a single factor in late life is not likely to be effective. Since multiple factors across the life course influence brain function in late life, multidomain interventions might be more promising in the prevention of cognitive decline. Some specific challenges need to be underlined in designing trials involving multidomain interventions, firstly, surrounding the specific selection of subjects [b95]. We can imagine that subjects who accept to modify multiple lifestyle domains are likely to have a higher level of education and better overall health, meaning that it may be difficult to demonstrate an effect of the intervention. Observance in multidomain trials is difficult to assess if the intervention combines on different lifestyle factors. If the intervention is based around lifestyle recommendations, it will be difficult to evaluate precisely actual behavioural modifications. In these interventions, it is impossible to maintain double‐blind conditions, and difficult to define an adequate control group, especially for physical exercise interventions. It is also difficult to identify the independent effects of each factor, because they may act through common mechanisms (e.g. via cardiovascular mechanisms), and there may be between‐group contamination.

Multi‐intervention preventive vascular trials have already been conducted. For example, in The Nurses' Health Study [b123], a multi‐intervention, heart disease trial, participants were asked to adopt many little lifestyle changes rather than make a single dramatic one. They were asked to keep their BMI below 27 kg m−2, restrict alcohol to one half serving per day, eat more than the median for grains, fruit and vegetables, and to exercise moderately. All those who complied had a relative risk for heart disease risk of 0.2. Another good example of a multi‐intervention trial is the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) trial [b124], which combined medication with dietary regulation, including reduced sodium, emphasis on fruit and vegetables, and potassium intake. This trial concluded that these interventions can control blood pressure quite well.

We need now to develop similar trials for cognitive decline. The frail elderly persons, who seem to be more likely to develop cognitive decline, appears to be a first target. In addition, we need to use biomarkers in this new generation of preventive trials to improve diagnostic accuracy, especially early in the course of AD and to help us to stratify AD cases, as well as identifying and monitoring the biochemical effects of the interventions. A recent proposal to include pathologically linked biomarkers of AD in the clinical diagnostic criteria has the potential to reduce the frequency of a false positive diagnosis and increase the validity of a clinical diagnosis of AD, especially at its earliest symptomatic stage [b125]. Potential biomarkers include atrophy on MRI, changes in brain function as assessed by fMRI, EEG or FDG PET which measures brain metabolism, amyloid PET (directly assess brain amyloid) as well as blood and CSF markers of tau and Aβ amyloid and other proteins and metabolites (indirectly estimate levels of brain amyloid plaques). Biomarkers measuring brain Aβ amyloid deposits will be used to identify subjects with a greater risk for cognitive decline and conversion to dementia, and to facilitate clinical design by targeting the ‘at risk’ population, and decreasing sample size and study duration.

To conclude, we report two recent RCTs designed with the aim of testing the effectiveness of mutli domain intervention on the cognitive and functional decline (the MAPT and DO‐HEALTH trials).

The MAPT trial is a multicentre study to assess benefits of multidomain intervention and omega‐3 on prevention of cognitive decline in frail elderly people followed during 3 years. It is a randomized, placebo controlled study, using a four group design including three treatment groups (omega‐3 alone, multidomain intervention alone, omega‐3 plus multi‐domain intervention) and a placebo group (Principal Investigator: Professor Vellas) [b126]. The inclusion period began in May 2008 and was completed in February 2011. Participants were enrolled from various sources, including advertisement in the local media, conference, general practitioners and memory clinics in 13 French cities. A total of 1680 fulfilled the eligibility criteria and were entered into the study. The study is coordinated from the hospital reference centre in Toulouse. The MAPT study treatment period will end in February 2014. The protocol is registered on a public‐access clinical trial database (http://www.clinicaltrials.gov).

The recruitment goal for the MAPT trial is to enrol a sample of frail elderly people, aged 70 years and over, living independently with good functional and cognitive status. Definition of frailty is to date not consensual but we practically used three clinical components to identify frail persons based on epidemiological evidence: spontaneous memory complaint expressed to the general practitioner, limitation in one instrumental activity of daily living (ability to use the telephone, shop, prepare meals, do housekeeping, do one's laundry, use transportation, follow a medication schedule and manage money) (IADL) and slow walking speed.

The multidomain intervention consists of collective training sessions and a yearly personalized preventive consultation which aims to detect dementia risk factors (sensory disturbances, difficulty in walking, nutritional disturbances, vascular risk factors) and to set up management in collaboration with the general practitioner. Training sessions are conducted in small groups (6–8 participants) settings in twelve 120 min sessions over the first 2 months (two sessions a week for the first month and one session a week the second month). Each training session during these ‘intensive period’ includes 60 min for the cognitive training, 45 min for the physical training and 15 min for nutritional advice. After the second month, sessions are planned monthly throughout the 3 year intervention period to reinforce the key messages of the programme and increase compliance. Participants are asked to use a diary to record their cognitive and physical activities each month. Booster training will be delivered in each multidomain group 1 and 2 years after their initial training sessions.

We performed neuroimaging examinations (PET‐scan and MRI) in sub‐groups of participants: (i) to explore the effects of interventions on cerebral atrophy (total brain volume and hippocampal volume) (MRI‐MAPT ancillary study): MRI is performed within the 2 months following the inclusion visit (or the 6 month follow‐up visit) and at 36 months. Currently, 504 subjects have undergone baseline MRI, (ii) to explore the effects of multidomain intervention on cerebral metabolism (FDG‐PET ancillary study): FDG‐PET scans are performed at baseline, 6 months and 1 year in a sub‐group of 68 participants followed in Toulouse centre and (iii) to evaluate brain amyloid deposits (Florbetapir‐PET ancillary study). Florbetapir‐PET imaging is proposed within the 2 years of follow‐up. Currently, 187 participants have been enrolled.

The DO‐HEALTH trial is a multinational study to assess benefits of vitamin D3, omega‐3 and home exercise on prevention of chronic diseases in the elderly. The DO‐HEALTH study (VitaminD3‐Omega3‐Home Exercise‐Healthy Ageing and Longevity Trial) will be Europe's largest healthy ageing study (Principal investigator: Professor Bischoff‐Ferrari). It expects to provide solid evidence for the efficacy and safety of three simple preventive interventions: vitamin D, omega‐3 fatty acids and a simple home exercise programme. DO‐HEALTH hopes to provide definitive evidence that the three interventions, alone or combined, are able to reduce the number of fractures, the functional and cognitive decline, the risk of hypertension and the risk of infections in the elderly population.

Conclusions

Nearly all of the data linking nutrition to cognitive decline or dementia/AD comes from observational studies and results are sometimes conflicting because of methodologic issues. It seems necessary to develop further prospective studies of adequate duration, including subjects whose diet is monitored at a sufficiently early stage or at least before the onset of disease or cognitive decline. RCTs of dietary interventions have yielded mixed findings and need to be conducted that they focus on specific groups of subjects (middle‐aged and elderly populations; subjects with vitamin deficiencies or normal and high levels). Such research identifying the role of specifics nutrients, foods or dietary behaviors, is an indispensable step before we can propose specific recommendations in the future.

In the absence of curative treatment, even if it is difficult to change lifestyle habits, lifestyle factors (diet, social engagement, cognitive stimulation, physical exercise) seem the most reasonable candidates for prevention trials at the current time, in particular due to their safety. Due to the difficulties in conducting a multidomain intervention, RCTs may not represent the gold standard in this field, and large public health interventions at the population level may be required. However, such interventions would have to be feasible, cost‐effective and easily transferable in order to have a real public health impact.

Competing Interests

All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available on request from the corresponding author) and declare: no support from any organisation for the submitted work; no financial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

References

  • 1.Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler‐Graham K, Arrighi HM. Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3:186–191. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Brookmeyer R, Gray S. Methods for projecting the incidence and prevalence of chronic diseases in aging populations: application to Alzheimer's disease. Stat Med. 2000;19:1481–1493. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0258(20000615/30)19:11/12<1481::aid-sim440>3.0.co;2-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gillette Guyonnet S, Abellan Van Kan G, Andrieu S, Barberger Gateau P, Berr C, Bonnefoy M, de Dartigues JF, Groot L, Ferry M, Galan P, Hercberg S, Jeandel C, Morris MC, Nourhashemi F, Payette H, Poulain JP, Portet F, Roussel AM, Ritz P, Rolland Y, Vellas B. IANA task force on nutrition and cognitive decline with aging. J Nutr Health Aging. 2007;11:132–152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Luchsinger JA, Gustafson DR. Adiposity and Alzheimer's disease. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2009;12:15–21. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e32831c8c71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gillette‐Guyonnet S, Vellas B. Caloric restriction and brain function. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2008;11:686–692. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e328313968f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fratiglioni L, Paillard‐Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:343–353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fratiglioni L, von Winblad B, Strauss E. Prevention of Alzheimer's disease and dementia. Major findings from the Kungsholmen Project. Physiol Behav. 2007;92:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Luchsinger JA, Patel B, Tang M‐X, Schupf N, Mayeux R. Measures of adiposity and dementia risk in elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:392–398. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.3.392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sturman MT, de Leon CFM, Bienias JL, Morris MC, Wilson RS, Evans DA. Body mass index and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Neurology. 2008;70:360–367. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000285081.04409.bb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Lopez OL, Diehr P, O'Meara ES, Longstreth WT, Jr, Luchsinger JA. Mid‐life and late‐life obesity and the risk of dementia: cardiovascular health study. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:336–342. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2008.582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Whitmer RA, Gunderson EP, Barrett‐Connor E, Quesenberry CP, Jr, Yaffe K. Obesity in middle age and future risk of dementia: a 27 years longitudinal population based study. BMJ. 2005;330:1360. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38446.466238.E0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, Kåreholt I, Winblad B, Helkala EL, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A. Obesity and vascular risk factors at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:1556–1560. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.10.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Xu WL, Atti AR, Gatz M, Pedersen NL, Johansson B, Fratiglioni L. Midlife overweight and obesity increase late‐life dementia risk: a population‐based twin study. Neurology. 2011;76:1568–1574. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182190d09. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cournot M, Marquié JC, Ansiau D, Martinaud C, Fonds H, Ferrières J, Ruidavets JB. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle‐aged men and women. Neurology. 2006;67:1208–1214. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Whitmer RA, Gustafson DR, Barrett‐Connor E, Haan MN, Gunderson EP, Yaffe K. Central obesity and increased risk of dementia more than three decades later. Neurology. 2008;71:1057–1064. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000306313.89165.ef. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gustafson DR, Bäckman K, Waern M, Ostling S, Guo X, Zandi P, Mielke MM, Bengtsson C, Skoog I. Adiposity indicators and dementia over 32 years in Sweden. Neurology. 2009;73:1559–1566. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c0d4b6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kerwin DR, Gaussoin SA, Chlebowski RT, Kuller LH, Vitolins M, Coker LH, Kotchen JM, Nicklas BJ, Wassertheil‐Smoller S, Hoffmann RG, Espeland MA. Interaction between body mass index and central adiposity and risk of incident cognitive impairment and dementia: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:107–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gazdzinski S, Kornak J, Weiner MW, Meyerhoff DJ. Body mass index and magnetic resonance markers of brain integrity in adults. Ann Neurol. 2008;63:652–657. doi: 10.1002/ana.21377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stewart R, Masaki K, Xue Q‐L, Peila R, Petrovitch H, White LR, Launer LJ. A 32‐year prospective study of change in body weight and incident dementia: the Honolulu‐Asia Aging Study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:55–60. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Shah RC, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Change in body mass index and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2005;65:892–897. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000176061.33817.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Nourhashémi F, Deschamps V, Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Dartigues J‐F, Barberger‐Gateau P. Body mass index and incidence of dementia: the PAQUID study. Neurology. 2003;60:117–119. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038910.46217.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Johnson DK, Wilkins CH, Morris JC. Accelerated weight loss may precede diagnosis in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1312–1317. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.9.1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Knopman DS, Edland SD, Cha RH, Petersen RC, Rocca WA. Incident dementia in women is preceded by weight loss by at least a decade. Neurology. 2007;69:739–746. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000267661.65586.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hughes TF, Borenstein AR, Schofield E, Wu Y, Larson EB. Association between late‐life body mass index and dementia: the Kame Project. Neurology. 2009;72:1741–1746. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a60a58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gelber RP, Petrovitch H, Masaki KH, Abbott RD, Ross GW, Launer LJ, White LR. Lifestyle and the risk of dementia in Japanese‐American men. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:118–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03768.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Buchman AS, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Body mass index in older persons is associated with Alzheimer disease pathology. Neurology. 2006;67:1949–1954. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000247046.90574.0f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Witte AV, Fobker M, Gellner R, Knecht S, Flöel A. Caloric restriction improves memory in elderly humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:1255–1260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808587106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Martin CK, Anton SD, Han H, York‐Crowe E, Redman LM, Ravussin E, Williamson DA. Examination of cognitive function during six months of calorie restriction: results of a randomized controlled trial. Rejuvenation Res. 2007;10:179–190. doi: 10.1089/rej.2006.0502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Craighead L, Welsh‐Bohmer KA, Browndyke JN, Strauman TA, Sherwood A. Effects of the dietary approaches to stop hypertension diet, exercise, and caloric restriction on neurocognition in overweight adults with high blood pressure. Hypertension. 2010;55:1331–1338. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.146795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Feng Y, Wang X. Antioxidant therapies for Alzheimer's disease. Oxid Med Cell Longev. 2012;2012:472932. doi: 10.1155/2012/472932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Engelhart MJ, Geerlings MI, van Ruitenberg A, Swieten JC, Hofman A, Witteman JCM, Breteler MM. Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002;287:3223–3229. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.24.3223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Tangney CC, Bennett DA, Aggarwal N, Wilson RS, Scherr PA. Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a biracial community study. JAMA. 2002;287:3230–3237. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.24.3230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Foley DJ, White LR. Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer disease: food for thought. JAMA. 2002;287:3261–3263. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.24.3261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Luchsinger JA, Tang M‐X, Shea S, Mayeux R. Antioxidant vitamin intake and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:203–208. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zandi PP, Anthony JC, Khachaturian AS, Stone SV, Gustafson D, Tschanz JT, Norton MC, Welsh‐Bohmer KA, Breitner JC. Reduced risk of Alzheimer disease in users of antioxidant vitamin supplements: the Cache County Study. Arch Neurol. 2004;61:82–88. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Dunn JE, Weintraub S, Stoddard AM, Banks S. Serum alpha‐tocopherol, concurrent and past vitamin E intake, and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:670–676. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000255940.13116.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Devore EE, van Grodstein F, Rooij FJA, Hofman A, Stampfer MJ, Witteman JCM, Breteler MM. Dietary antioxidants and long‐term risk of dementia. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:819–825. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Yaffe K, Clemons TE, McBee WL, Lindblad AS. Impact of antioxidants, zinc, and copper on cognition in the elderly: a randomized, controlled trial. Neurology. 2004;63:1705–1707. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000142969.19465.8f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kang JH, Cook N, Manson J, Buring JE, Grodstein F. A randomized trial of vitamin E supplementation and cognitive function in women. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:2462–2468. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.22.2462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kang JH, Cook NR, Manson JE, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. Vitamin E, vitamin C, beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women's Antioxidant and Cardiovascular Study. Circulation. 2009;119:2772–2780. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.816900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Grodstein F, Kang JH, Glynn RJ, Cook NR, Gaziano JM. A randomized trial of beta carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: the Physicians' Health Study II. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167:2184–2190. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.20.2184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Summers WK, Martin RL, Cunningham M, DeBoynton VL, Marsh GM. Complex antioxidant blend improves memory in community‐dwelling seniors. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19:429–439. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kesse‐Guyot E, Fezeu L, Jeandel C, Ferry M, Andreeva V, Amieva H, Hercberg S, Galan P. French adults' cognitive performance after daily supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals at nutritional doses: a post hoc analysis of the Supplementation in Vitamins and Mineral Antioxidants (SU.VI.MAX) trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:892–899. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.007815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Snitz BE, O'Meara ES, Carlson MC, Arnold AM, Ives DG, Rapp SR, Saxton J, Lopez OL, Dunn LO, Sink KM, DeKosky ST. Ginkgo biloba for preventing cognitive decline in older adults: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2009;302:2663–2670. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Feng L. Ginkgo biloba and cognitive decline. JAMA. 2010;303:1477. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.DeKosky ST, Williamson JD, Fitzpatrick AL, Kronmal RA, Ives DG, Saxton JA, Lopez OL, Burke G, Carlson MC, Fried LP, Kuller LH, Robbins JA, Tracy RP, Woolard NF, Dunn L, Snitz BE, Nahin RL, Furberg CD. Ginkgo biloba for prevention of dementia: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2008;300:2253–2262. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dodge HH, Zitzelberger T, Oken BS, Howieson D, Kaye J. A randomized placebo‐controlled trial of Ginkgo biloba for the prevention of cognitive decline. Neurology. 2008;70(19 Pt 2):1809–1817. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000303814.13509.db. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vellas B, Coley N, Ousset P‐J, Berrut G, Dartigues J‐F, Dubois B, Grandjean H, Pasquier F, Piette F, Robert P, Touchon J, Garnier P, Mathiex‐Fortunet H, Andrieu S. Long‐term use of standardised Ginkgo biloba extract for the prevention of Alzheimer's disease (GuidAge): a randomised placebo‐controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11:851–859. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70206-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Birks J, Grimley Evans J. Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009;(1) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003120.pub3. CD003120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Weinmann S, Roll S, Schwarzbach C, Vauth C, Willich SN. Effects of Ginkgo biloba in dementia: systematic review and meta‐analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bjelakovic G, Nikolova D, Gluud LL, Simonetti RG, Gluud C. Mortality in randomized trials of antioxidant supplements for primary and secondary prevention: systematic review and meta‐analysis. JAMA. 2007;297:842–857. doi: 10.1001/jama.297.8.842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Garcia A, Zanibbi K. Homocysteine and cognitive function in elderly people. CMAJ. 2004;171:897–904. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.1031586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kado DM, Karlamangla AS, Huang M‐H, Troen A, Rowe JW, Selhub J, Seeman TE. Homocysteine versus the vitamins folate, B6, and B12 as predictors of cognitive function and decline in older high‐functioning adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Am J Med. 2005;118:161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2004.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hooshmand B, Solomon A, Kåreholt I, Leiviskä J, Rusanen M, Ahtiluoto S, Winblad B, Laatikainen T, Soininen H, Kivipelto M. Homocysteine and holotranscobalamin and the risk of Alzheimer disease: a longitudinal study. Neurology. 2010;75:1408–1414. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f88162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Clarke R, Birks J, Nexo E, Ueland PM, Schneede J, Scott J, Molloy A, Evans JG. Low vitamin B‐12 status and risk of cognitive decline in older adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:1384–1391. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.5.1384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Haan MN, Miller JW, Aiello AE, Whitmer RA, Jagust WJ, Mungas DM, Allen LH, Green R. Homocysteine, B vitamins, and the incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment: results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:511–517. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Luchsinger JA, Tang M‐X, Miller J, Green R, Mayeux R. Relation of higher folate intake to lower risk of Alzheimer disease in the elderly. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:86–92. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.McMahon JA, Green TJ, Skeaff CM, Knight RG, Mann JI, Williams SM. A controlled trial of homocysteine lowering and cognitive performance. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:2764–2772. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa054025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Durga J, van Boxtel MPJ, Schouten EG, Kok FJ, Jolles J, Katan MB, Verhoef P. Effect of 3‐year folic acid supplementation on cognitive function in older adults in the FACIT trial: a randomised, double blind, controlled trial. Lancet. 2007;369:208–216. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60109-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ford AH, Flicker L, Alfonso H, Thomas J, Clarnette R, Martins R, Almeida OP. Vitamins B(12), B(6), and folic acid for cognition in older men. Neurology. 2010;75:1540–1547. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f962c4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kang JH, Cook N, Manson J, Buring JE, Albert CM, Grodstein F. A trial of B vitamins and cognitive function among women at high risk of cardiovascular disease. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88:1602–1610. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Walker JG, Batterham PJ, Mackinnon AJ, Jorm AF, Hickie I, Fenech M, Kljakovic M, Crisp D, Christensen H. Oral folic acid and vitamin B‐12 supplementation to prevent cognitive decline in community‐dwelling older adults with depressive symptoms – the Beyond Ageing Project: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:194–203. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.007799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dangour AD, Allen E, Clarke R, Elbourne D, Fasey N, Fletcher AE, Letley L, Richards M, Whyte K, Mills K, Uauy R. A randomised controlled trial investigating the effect of vitamin B12 supplementation on neurological function in healthy older people: the Older People and Enhanced Neurological function (OPEN) study protocol [ISRCTN54195799] Nutr J. 2011;10:22. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-10-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Aisen PS, Schneider LS, Sano M, van Diaz‐Arrastia R, Dyck CH, Weiner MF, Bottiglieri T, Jin S, Stokes KT, Thomas RG, Thal LJ. High‐dose B vitamin supplementation and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2008;300:1774–1783. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.15.1774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kwok T, Lee J, Law CB, Pan PC, Yung CY, Choi KC, Lam LC. A randomized placebo controlled trial of homocysteine lowering to reduce cognitive decline in older demented people. Clin Nutr. 2011;30:297–302. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2010.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Smith AD, de Smith SM, Jager CA, Whitbread P, Johnston C, Agacinski G, Oulhaj A, Bradley KM, Jacoby R, Refsum H. Homocysteine‐lowering by B vitamins slows the rate of accelerated brain atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e12244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.de Jager CA, Oulhaj A, Jacoby R, Refsum H, Smith AD. Cognitive and clinical outcomes of homocysteine‐lowering B‐vitamin treatment in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27:592–600. doi: 10.1002/gps.2758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Balion C, Griffith LE, Strifler L, Henderson M, Patterson C, Heckman G, Llewellyn DJ, Raina P. Vitamin D, cognition, and dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Neurology. 2012;79:1397–1405. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826c197f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dickens AP, Lang IA, Langa KM, Kos K, Llewellyn DJ. Vitamin D, cognitive dysfunction and dementia in older adults. CNS Drugs. 2011;25:629–639. doi: 10.2165/11593080-000000000-00000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lu'o'ng KVQ, Nguyên LTH. The beneficial role of vitamin D in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2011;26:511–520. doi: 10.1177/1533317511429321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Slinin Y, Paudel ML, Taylor BC, Fink HA, Ishani A, Canales MT, Yaffe K, Barrett‐Connor E, Orwoll ES, Shikany JM, Leblanc ES, Cauley JA, Ensrud KE. 25‐Hydroxyvitamin D levels and cognitive performance and decline in elderly men. Neurology. 2010;74:33–41. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c7197b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Llewellyn DJ, Lang IA, Langa KM, Muniz‐Terrera G, Phillips CL, Cherubini A, Ferrucci L, Melzer D. Vitamin D and risk of cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170:1135–1141. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2010.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Annweiler C, Rolland Y, Schott AM, Blain H, Vellas B, Beauchet O. Serum vitamin D deficiency as a predictor of incident non‐Alzheimer dementias: a 7‐year longitudinal study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;32:273–278. doi: 10.1159/000334944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Annweiler C, Fantino B, Parot‐Schinkel E, Thiery S, Gautier J, Beauchet O. Alzheimer's disease – input of vitamin D with mEmantine assay (AD‐IDEA trial): study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2011;12:230. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-12-230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Sydenham E, Dangour AD, Lim W‐S. Omega 3 fatty acid for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;(6) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005379.pub3. CD005379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Tangney CC, Wilson RS. Dietary fat intake and 6‐year cognitive change in an older biracial community population. Neurology. 2004;62:1573–1579. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000123250.82849.b6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Kalmijn S, van Boxtel MPJ, Ocké M, Verschuren WMM, Kromhout D, Launer LJ. Dietary intake of fatty acids and fish in relation to cognitive performance at middle age. Neurology. 2004;62:275–280. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000103860.75218.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Engelhart MJ, Geerlings MI, Ruitenberg A, Van Swieten JC, Hofman A, Witteman JCM, Breteler MM. Diet and risk of dementia: does fat matter?: the Rotterdam Study. Neurology. 2002;59:1915–1921. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038345.77753.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Huang TL, Zandi PP, Tucker KL, Fitzpatrick AL, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Burke GL, Carlson MC. Benefits of fatty fish on dementia risk are stronger for those without APOE epsilon4. Neurology. 2005;65:1409–1414. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000183148.34197.2e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Morris MC, Evans DA, Tangney CC, Bienias JL, Wilson RS. Fish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community study. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:1849–1853. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.12.noc50161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Barberger‐Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, Alpérovitch A. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three‐City cohort study. Neurology. 2007;69:1921–1930. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000278116.37320.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Beydoun MA, Kaufman JS, Satia JA, Rosamond W, Folsom AR. Plasma n‐3 fatty acids and the risk of cognitive decline in older adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:1103–1111. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.4.1103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Virtanen JK, Siscovick DS, Longstreth WT, Jr, Kuller LH, Mozaffarian D. Fish consumption and risk of subclinical brain abnormalities on MRI in older adults. Neurology. 2008;71:439–446. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000324414.12665.b0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Schaefer EJ, Bongard V, Beiser AS, Lamon‐Fava S, Robins SJ, Au R, Tucker KL, Kyle DJ, Wilson PW, Wolf PA. Plasma phosphatidylcholine docosahexaenoic acid content and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: the Framingham Heart Study. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1545–1550. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.11.1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Lopez LB, Kritz‐Silverstein D, Barrett Connor E. High dietary and plasma levels of the omega‐3 fatty acid docosahexaenoic acid are associated with decreased dementia risk: the Rancho Bernardo study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15:25–31. doi: 10.1007/s12603-011-0009-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.van de Rest O, Geleijnse JM, van Kok FJ, Staveren WA, Dullemeijer C, Olderikkert MGM, de Beekman AT, Groot CP. Effect of fish oil on cognitive performance in older subjects: a randomized, controlled trial. Neurology. 2008;71:430–438. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000324268.45138.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Yurko‐Mauro K, McCarthy D, Rom D, Nelson EB, Ryan AS, Blackwell A, Salem N, Jr, Stedman M. Beneficial effects of docosahexaenoic acid on cognition in age‐related cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Dangour AD, Allen E, Elbourne D, Fasey N, Fletcher AE, Hardy P, Holder GE, Knight R, Letley L, Richards M, Uauy R. Effect of 2‐y n‐3 long‐chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on cognitive function in older people: a randomized, double‐blind, controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91:1725–1732. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.29121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Geleijnse JM, Giltay EJ, Kromhout D. Effects of n‐3 fatty acids on cognitive decline: a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled trial in stable myocardial infarction patients. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:278–287. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Quinn JF, Raman R, Thomas RG, Yurko‐Mauro K, Nelson EB, Van Dyck C, Galvin JE, Emond J, Jack CR, Jr, Weiner M, Shinto L, Aisen PS. Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2010;304:1903–1911. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Freund‐Levi Y, Eriksdotter‐Jönhagen M, Cederholm T, Basun H, Faxén‐Irving G, Garlind A, Vedin I, Vessby B, Wahlund LO, Palmblad J. Omega‐3 fatty acid treatment in 174 patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: omegAD study: a randomized double‐blind trial. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1402–1408. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.10.1402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Scheltens P, Kamphuis PJGH, Verhey FRJ, Olde Rikkert MGM, Wurtman RJ, Wilkinson D, Twisk JW, Kurz A. Efficacy of a medical food in mild Alzheimer's disease: a randomized, controlled trial. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Kamphuis PJGH, Verhey FRJ, Olde Rikkert MGM, Twisk JWR, Swinkels SHN, Scheltens P. Efficacy of a medical food on cognition in Alzheimer's disease: results from secondary analyses of a randomized, controlled trial. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15:720–724. doi: 10.1007/s12603-011-0105-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Andreeva VA, Kesse‐Guyot E, Barberger‐Gateau P, Fezeu L, Hercberg S, Galan P. Cognitive function after supplementation with B vitamins and long‐chain omega‐3 fatty acids: ancillary findings from the SU.FOL.OM3 randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:278–286. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.006320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Daffner KR. Promoting successful cognitive aging: a comprehensive review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19:1101–1122. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Coley N, Andrieu S, Gardette V, Gillette‐Guyonnet S, Sanz C, Vellas B, Grand A. Dementia prevention: methodological explanations for inconsistent results. Epidemiol Rev. 2008;30:35–66. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Barberger‐Gateau P, Samieri C, Féart C, Plourde M. Dietary omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and Alzheimer's disease: interaction with apolipoprotein E genotype. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2011;8:479–491. doi: 10.2174/156720511796391926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang M‐X, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:912–921. doi: 10.1002/ana.20854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Manly JJ, Schupf N, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:216–225. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2008.536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Féart C, Samieri C, Rondeau V, Amieva H, Portet F, Dartigues J‐F, Scarmeas N, Barberger‐Gateau P. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA. 2009;302:638–648. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Gu Y, Nieves JW, Stern Y, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N. Food combination and Alzheimer disease risk: a protective diet. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:699–706. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Mayeux R, Stern Y. Mediterranean diet and Alzheimer disease mortality. Neurology. 2007;69:1084–1093. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000277320.50685.7c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, Brickman AM, Cosentino S, Tang MX, Stern Y. Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2009;302:627–637. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Frisardi V, Panza F, Seripa D, Imbimbo BP, Vendemiale G, Pilotto A, Solfrizzi V. Nutraceutical properties of Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline: possible underlying mechanisms. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22:715–740. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Abbott RD, White LR, Ross GW, Masaki KH, Curb JD, Petrovitch H. Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men. JAMA. 2004;292:1447–1453. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MMB, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292:1454–1461. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Rovio S, Kåreholt I, Helkala E‐L, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M. Leisure‐time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:705–711. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70198-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, Kukull W. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann Intern Med. 2006;144:73–81. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Verghese J, LeValley A, Derby C, Kuslansky G, Katz M, Hall C, Buschke H, Lipton RB. Leisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. Neurology. 2006;66:821–827. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000202520.68987.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Deary IJ, Whalley LJ, Batty GD, Starr JM. Physical fitness and lifetime cognitive change. Neurology. 2006;67:1195–1200. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000238520.06958.6a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Ravaglia G, Forti P, Lucicesare A, Pisacane N, Rietti E, Bianchin M, Dalmonte E. Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology. 2008;70(19 Pt 2):1786–1794. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000296276.50595.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Burns JM, Cronk BB, Anderson HS, Donnelly JE, Thomas GP, Harsha A, Brooks WM, Swerdlow RH. Cardiorespiratory fitness and brain atrophy in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2008;71:210–216. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000317094.86209.cb. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S, Dartigues J‐F, Artero S, Rouaud O, Touchon J, Ritchie K, Berr C. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: results from the Three‐City Study. Neurology. 2009;73:854–861. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b7849b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Vercambre M‐N, Grodstein F, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Kang JH. Physical activity and cognition in women with vascular conditions. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171:1244–1250. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Middleton LE, Manini TM, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Barnes DE, Tylavsky F, Brach JS, Everhart JE, Yaffe K. Activity energy expenditure and incident cognitive impairment in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171:1251–1257. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Liang KY, Mintun MA, Fagan AM, Goate AM, Bugg JM, Holtzman DM, Morris JC, Head D. Exercise and Alzheimer's disease biomarkers in cognitively normal older adults. Ann Neurol. 2010;68:311–318. doi: 10.1002/ana.22096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Eggermont LHP, Swaab DF, Hol EM, Scherder EJA. Walking the line: a randomised trial on the effects of a short term walking programme on cognition in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:802–804. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.158444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Kemoun G, Thibaud M, Roumagne N, Carette P, Albinet C, Toussaint L, Paccalin M, Dugué B. Effects of a physical training programme on cognitive function and walking efficiency in elderly persons with dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;29:109–114. doi: 10.1159/000272435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Baker LD, Frank LL, Foster‐Schubert K, Green PS, Wilkinson CW, McTiernan A, Plymate SR, Fishel MA, Watson GS, Cholerton BA, Duncan GE, Mehta PD, Craft S. Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive impairment: a controlled trial. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:71–79. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Varela S, Ayán C, Cancela JM, Martín V. Effects of two different intensities of aerobic exercise on elderly people with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2012;26:442–450. doi: 10.1177/0269215511425835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Lam LCW, Chau RCM, Wong BML, Fung AWT, Lui VWC, Tam CCW, Leung GT, Kwok TC, Chiu HF, Ng S, Chan WM. Interim follow‐up of a randomized controlled trial comparing Chinese style mind body (Tai Chi) and stretching exercises on cognitive function in subjects at risk of progressive cognitive decline. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:733–740. doi: 10.1002/gps.2602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, van Foster JK, Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, Greenop KR, Almeida OP. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300:1027–1037. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Hu FB, Stampfer MJ, Manson JE, Grodstein F, Colditz GA, Speizer FE, Willett WC. Trends in the incidence of coronary heart disease and changes in diet and lifestyle in women. N Engl J Med. 2000;343:530–537. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200008243430802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Lin P‐H, Appel LJ, Funk K, Craddick S, Chen C, Elmer P, McBurnie MA, Champagne C. The PREMIER intervention helps participants follow the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension dietary pattern and the current Dietary Reference Intakes recommendations. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007;107:1541–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger‐Gateau P, Cummings J, Delacourte A, Galasko D, Gauthier S, Jicha G, Meguro K, O'brien J, Pasquier F, Robert P, Rossor M, Salloway S, Stern Y, Visser PJ, Scheltens P. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: revising the NINCDS‐ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:734–746. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Gillette‐Guyonnet S, Andrieu S, Dantoine T, Dartigues J‐F, Touchon J, Vellas B. Commentary on « A roadmap for the prevention of dementia II. Leon Thal Symposium 2008. » The Multidomain Alzheimer Preventive Trial (MAPT): a new approach to the prevention of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2009;5:114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology are provided here courtesy of British Pharmacological Society

RESOURCES