Abstract
A large number of cellular level abnormalities have been identified in the hippocampus of schizophrenic subjects. Nonetheless, it remains uncertain how these pathologies interact at a system level to create clinical symptoms, and this has hindered the development of more effective antipsychotic medications. Using a 72-processor supercomputer, we created a tissue level hippocampal simulation, featuring multicompartmental neuron models with multiple ion channel subtypes and synaptic channels with realistic temporal dynamics. As an index of the schizophrenic phenotype, we used the specific inability of the model to attune to 40 Hz (gamma band) stimulation, a well-characterized abnormality in schizophrenia. We examined several possible combinations of putatively schizophrenogenic cellular lesions by systematically varying model parameters representing NMDA channel function, dendritic spine density, and GABA system integrity, conducting 910 trials in total. Two discrete “clusters” of neuropathological changes were identified. The most robust was characterized by co-occurring modest reductions in NMDA system function (-30%) and dendritic spine density (-30%). Another set of lesions had greater NMDA hypofunction along with low level GABA system dysregulation. To the schizophrenic model, we applied the effects of 1,500 virtual medications, which were implemented by varying five model parameters, independently, in a graded manner; the effects of known drugs were also applied. The simulation accurately distinguished agents that are known to lack clinical efficacy, and identified novel mechanisms (e.g., decrease in AMPA conductance decay time constant, increase in projection strength of calretinin-positive interneurons) and combinations of mechanisms that could re-equilibrate model behavior. These findings shed light on the mechanistic links between schizophrenic neuropathology and the gamma band oscillatory abnormalities observed in the illness. As such, they generate specific falsifiable hypotheses, which can guide postmortem and other laboratory research. Significantly, this work also suggests specific non-obvious targets for potential pharmacologic agents.
Introduction
Schizophrenia is a debilitating, lifelong illness affecting approximately 1% of the population worldwide [1]. Beginning with Thorazine (chlorpromazine) in the 1950s, antipsychotic medications have been used to treat the condition. However, despite several years of research, and the introduction of a number of new agents, all currently used antipsychotics are far from ideal. They are capable of ameliorating some symptoms in many, though not all, schizophrenic patients, and none represents a cure to the disease. Moreover, these medications carry significant side effect burdens [2].
The absence of development of antipsychotic medications with fundamentally new mechanisms of action [3] stands in stark contrast to the vast amounts that have been learned over the past 20 years on the cellular level abnormalities associated with the disease. The hippocampal neurobiology of the illness has been the subject of a number of recent comprehensive and detailed reviews [4]. Broadly, studies on this and other brain areas have revealed: (1) Dysfunction in the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) system. Deficiencies in GABAergic innervation have been seen, as a result of decreased number of particular subtypes of GABA neurons or GABAergic tone, and a (presumably compensatory) increase in postsynaptic GABA receptor expression [5]; (2) Glutamatergic system deficiency. This is manifested, for example, as decreased expression of N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) receptors, and/or hypofunction of NMDA synaptic activity [6], [7]; and (3) Decreases in brain connectivity. Diminished dendritic spine density has been seen, for example, in postmortem and animal models of the illness [8], [9].
One reason this large and growing body of neurobiological knowledge has not translated into more effective treatments is that we do not have convincing causative links between cellular level abnormalities and particular symptoms or sets of symptoms. This is a problem that is characteristic of psychiatric illnesses in general, and stands in contrast to many other medical illnesses, in which, for example, the underlying genetic abnormality, the dysfunctional protein expressed, the function of this protein, and the manner in which this causes illness pathology are well understood. This is made particularly difficult because function in a given region, such as hippocampus, is likely an emergent phenomenon; it is extremely difficult to intuit the behavior of the overall system by looking at one, or even a few of its constituent cellular level behaviors or interactions in isolation [10]. It is difficult to imagine designing an effective intervention without taking this into account.
Oscillatory brain activity is an emergent, system level behavior that stands at an intermediate level of complexity between the cellular and the clinical. A large amount of recent research has indicated that schizophrenic patients show synchronization deficiencies in neural processing [11], particularly in the gamma frequency band [12]–[17]. Importantly, there is also evidence that gamma activity subserves particular cognitive functions, such as perceptual binding within a particular sensory modality, or integration of information from different sensory modalities [18], to form a coherent percept. Thus, disturbed function may be etiologically related to some of the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, such as hallucinations or compromised reality testing.
Given the complexity of schizophrenia, it is unsurprising that computational modeling has been applied in an attempt to better understand this illness and its possible etiology. While there are exceptions [19], [20], many have been abstract ANN (artificial neural network) style models, and they have tended to examine a single hypothetical pathology, such as connectivity disturbance [21], hyperdopaminergia [22], or deficient perforant path input into hippocampal formation [23]. One reason for this is that creating networks of biophysically detailed cellular models, and running large numbers of parameter assumptions (corresponding to different combinations of neural lesions or medication effects) are very computationally demanding undertakings. However, the development of computers with processing capacity several orders of magnitude greater than those of a generation ago now place us in a position such that we can begin to address these questions via “tissue level” computational work, and this is the approach we have taken here. Using a 72-processor supercomputer, we have created a biophysically detailed computational simulation of hippocampus, and use specific quantitative inability of the model to attune to 40 Hz stimulatory drive as a marker of the schizophrenic phenotype. We then introduce multiple putative schizophrenogenic cellular level abnormalities, as outlined above, into the model in a graded and combinatorial way, and find that two distinct “clusters” of pathologies can create the schizophrenic phenotype. Then, a large number of virtual medication effects are applied to the schizophrenic model, and combinations that return the model to its baseline (non-diseased) state are identified. The potentially ameliorative mechanisms identified are non-obvious, and do not represent simple reversals of the causative lesions.
Methods
Computational Model
The hippocampal model consists of 160 pyramidal cells and interneurons of three subtypes—30 basket cells, 30 chandelier cells, and 20 calretinin-positive (CR+), or interneuron projecting, cells. For pyramidal cells, we used the 64 compartment model described by Traub et al [24]. Interneuron models were based on the 46 compartment model of Traub and Miles [25]. Both include realistic dendritic arbors and incorporate , , , ,andchannels with Hodgkin-Huxley dynamics distributed along the somato-dendritic axis. Interneurons of different subclasses were defined by their axonal projections patterns, based the hippocampal model described by the author [26]. Full details of individual neuron models and their connectivity, a description of the manner in which a simulated EEG was calculated, and details of the model’s hardware implementation can be found in Text S1. Model parameters are given in online Tables S1, S2, S3, and S4.
Implementation of Putative Schizophrenogenic Cellular Level Abnormalities
Glutamatergic system dysfunction:
A number of recent research studies have found decreased density of NMDA synapses in schizophrenic hippocampus and/or hypofunction of these synapses, and have quantified this effect. Tsai et al [27], in postmortem work, found a decrease of 37% in glutamate levels in hippocampus of schizophrenics. This group also found a 55% increase in N-acetylaspartylglutamic acid (NAAG) in this area; together with work by Bergeron et al [28] that demonstrated an inverse relationship between NAAG level and NMDA current, this suggests that schizophrenic patients may experience NMDA hypofunction via decreases in the conductance of the NMDA channel. Law and Deakin [29] found a decrease in the obligatory NMDAR1 subunit of the NMDA receptor of 40%. Similarly, Harrison et al [30], in postmortem work looking at markers of glutamate receptors in schizophrenic hippocampus, found a decrease of 26% in mRNA coding for NR1, a subunit of the NMDA receptor.
Thus, the research suggests a spectrum of possible NMDA deficits. To capture the full range of possible values, we implemented NMDA effects by decreasing maximum conductance (gmax) of the model NMDA receptors by 0 to 45%, in increments of 5%. We also performed trials in which the number of NMDA receptors was decreased through this range, and found quantitatively similar effects (data not shown).
Connectivity disturbances:
A “pruning hypothesis” of schizophrenia has long been suggested [9], [31]. Much of the substantiation for this, however, came from indirect measurements (e.g., decreased neuropil volume). Studies have examined this quantitatively by looking at density of spines on neuronal dendrites. Law et al [32] found a decrease in levels of mRNA for spinophilin (a marker for dendritic spines) of 44.5%, on average. Garey et al [33], in a postmortem study Golgi staining, saw a decreased spine density in temporal lobe of patients of 59.4%. DeVito et al [34], using a genetic knockout model of NMDA receptor hypofunction (a serine racemase knockout mouse), found a decreased dendritic spine density of 40.5%. To capture the full range of possible values, we decreased pyramidal cell spine density from 0 to 60%, in increments of 5%.
GABA system dysregulation:
Heckers et al [35] found decreased expression of mRNA for two isoforms of the GABA-synthesizing enzyme glutamic acid decarboxylase GAD65 and GAD67 decreased by 14% and 4%, respectively, in schizophrenic hippocampi. Bird et al [36], in a postmortem study of brains of psychotic patients found GAD to be decreased by 48.2%. Fatemi et al [37] and Torrey et al [38] found decreases in reelin in schizophrenic hippocampus of 29% and 46%, respectively. However, other studies have found increases in GABA receptor binding. For example, Benes et al [39] showed increases of 45% to 82% depending on subfield of hippocampus. It is felt that this may represent a compensatory upregulation of postsynaptic GABA receptors, in response to decreased GABAergic activity [40].
To apply these changes, we simulated the decrease in GABAergic tone by decreasing the number of GABAergic projections (that is, projections from model interneurons) from 0 to 45% in increments of 7.5%; we simulated increased weight of postsynaptic GABA receptors by increasing the weight factor at these synapses from 0 to 60%. We made these changes in tandem, testing 7 “ordered pairs” of parameter values ([0, 0] to [-45%, +60%,]), where each element is [change in GABAergic tone, GABA postsynaptic weight change]. This was done because when searching large parameter spaces, adding an additional dimension increases the number of trials multiplicatively, and searching four dimensions for the current problem would have been prohibitively time consuming.
Calculation of Illness Metric
We created a metric to quantify the “schizophrenic-ness” of a given model run, based on its quantitative similarity to the experimental findings of Teale et al [16]. This study was used because it employs a steady state evoked potential (SSEP) task, and detailed source localization carried out in the study revealed that the source of the oscillatory activity recorded was temporal lobes, and thus may be hippocampal in origin. Based on their data (their Figure 6, p. 1486), which shows 40 Hz oscillatory activity as a function of time when patients are receiving the stimulus, at maximum patients showed a decrease of approximately 26% at this frequency, compared with controls (this represents an average over left and right hemispheres). The many SSEP experiments that have been carried out on schizophrenic patients indicate that when exposed to 20 or 30 Hz stimulation, patients did not show a response significantly different from controls[41].
Therefore, for a model to said to be schizophrenic: (1) 20 and 30 Hz activity were required to be within a given tolerance of the baseline case. We used +/- 7.5% for this value, based on the standard deviation of our 20 simulated control patients (Figure 1); models that failed for either frequency were given a score of 0. (2) 40 Hz activity was required to be significantly decreased from the baseline condition. To quantify this, percentage decrease of schizophrenic condition vs. control condition was calculated (that is, [power of 40 Hz response, baseline condition] – [power of 40 Hz response, schizophrenic condition] / [power of 40 Hz response, baseline condition]). If this equaled 26%, the model received a score of 1; to the extent that this differed from 26%, in absolute value, the score was decreased. Calculation of the illness metric is presented formally in Text S1 and accompanying Table S5.
To ensure that the most highly schizophrenic case identified represented a robust model behavior, we created 20 simulated schizophrenic subjects and 20 control subjects. In order to create a simulated subject, we re-seeded the random number generator to generate a new model, and had it perform the experimental task. Thus, each simulated subject had different specific cell-to-cell pattern of connectivity, but the projection probabilities between cell types (as defined in Table S4) were identical. Using these data, we ran a mixed model ANOVA, entering Group (control, schizophrenic) as a between subjects factor and Frequency (20 Hz, 30 Hz, 40 Hz) as a repeated measures factor. To test the specificity of putative ANOVA findings, hierarchical regressions were run.
Results
The first section below illustrates the network model’s ability to attune to 20, 30, and 40 Hz stimulation in the baseline condition. Subsequent sections show the results of implementation of schizophrenogenic cellular lesions, and the results of trials that incorporate the effect of both known medications and virtual antipsychotic drugs.
Reproduction of Baseline Oscillatory Activity
After tuning, we drove the hippocampal model at 20, 30, and 40 Hz; a simulated EEG was generated for each and was analyzed via fast Fourier transform (FFT) to determine which frequencies were present. The model reproduced, in a quantitatively similar way, frequency behaviors shown in control subjects (Fig 1A [left panels] and 1B [left panels] experimental; Fig 1C [left panels] model output).
To confirm these model results, we created 20 simulated control subjects, as described in the Methods section. The results of these runs are shown in Figure 1D (blue points). It is clear that the behavior of our simulated index control subject is representative of the group of simulated controls, and that this group is similar to that of the control subjects of experimental studies.
Effects of Putative Schizophrenogenic Cellular Level Abnormalities on Model Behavior
The manner in which the cellular level pathology that has been observed in schizophrenic hippocampus was instantiated as parameter changes in the model is detailed in Methods. Briefly, decreased NMDA activity was operationalized by decreasing maximum conductance (gmax) of the model NMDA receptors (in 10 increments); connectivity deficits were operationalized by decreasing pyramidal cell dendritic spine density (13 increments); and GABA system dysregulation was implemented by a joint decrease in GABA tone and increase in postsynaptic weight (7 increments). Iterations representing all possible levels of the aforementioned cellular level lesions were run: that is, we exhaustively searched the parameter space, running 10×13×7 = 910 iterations in total. Each iteration consisted of three trials; in each, the network was driven at a given frequency (20, 30, or 40 Hz), and a simulated EEG was written to file and was analyzed via fast Fourier transform (FFT) to determine which frequencies were present, and their relative power. The degree to which this matched the pattern seen in the clinical studies (i.e., the degree to which there was a specific deficit in 40 Hz response) was quantified using the illness metric, which ranged from 1 (most schizophrenic) to 0, as described in Methods. Figure 2 graphically depicts the results of these trials.
Clearly, a number of points produce schizophrenia-like results. There is a prominent cluster centered at a point characterized by an NMDA decrease of 30%, a spine density decrease of 30%, and a GABA deficit of 0 (which we will call the “primary point”). There is another point characterized by an NMDA decrease of 45%, a spine density decrease of 30%, and a GABA system defect of (–37.5, +30%), as defined in Methods (which we will call the “secondary point”). For the primary point, power spectra of oscillatory activity in response to 20, 30, and 40 Hz drive is shown in Fig 1C, in comparison with control behavior (Figs 1A, B). 40 Hz response is decreased to about 24% below the control case, calculated as an average of 20 simulated control patients; 20 and 30 Hz responses are roughly the same as those of controls. This again was confirmed by re-running the model with 20 simulated schizophrenic patients.
To more formally test these effects, we ran a Group (control, schizophrenic) x Frequency (20 Hz, 30 Hz, 40 Hz) ANOVA. Both the main effects of Frequency (F [2, 80] = 4812.6, p < 0.001, Greenhouse-Geisser correction: ε = 0.87) and Group (F [2, 80] = 289.05, p < 0.001, ε = 0.87) were significant. Critically, these effects were qualified by a significant Group by Frequency interaction, driven by greatest group differences at 40 Hz (see Fig 1D). Because groups differed in all three frequencies, a set of hierarchical regression analyses was run to test the specificity of the findings. Specifically, in the first regression, we entered power at 20 and 30 Hz in the first step, and Group (dummy-coded) in the second step, in order to predict power at 40 Hz. The model was significant, indicating that Group predicted 40 Hz activity when controlling for power at 20 and 30 Hz (ΔR2 = 0.101, ΔF [1], [38] = 77.64, p < 0.001). Critically, when entering 40 in the first step, Group predicted neither 30 Hz power (ΔR2 = 0.003, ΔF [1], [38] = 0.81, p = 0.375) nor 20 Hz power (ΔR2 = 0.025, ΔF [1], [38] = 0.81, p = 0.193). Thus, group differences were specific to 40 Hz.
In an attempt to understand the relative contributions of each of these neural level abnormalities individually to the functioning of the system, we performed a “partial derivative” analysis for each. That is, we examined the overall behavior of the system in response to one lesion at a time, holding the others constant. The results are shown in Figures 3 and 4. Significantly, no single abnormality alone accounts for the findings.
Analysis of Oscillatory Dynamics
What neural interactions caused the primary point, with a specific deficit in response to 40 Hz drive, to arise, and how did this differ from the secondary point? To answer this, we examined simulated EEG traces and histograms of spiking activity from both cases. Of note, for 40 Hz drive, the EEG traces of the primary point shows a depression of every other peak, effectively creating a mix of 20 and 40 Hz activity, and a decrease in the 40 Hz response (Fig 5). The spiking probability histograms for the primary and secondary points show averages over two cycles at a time, in an attempt to reveal differential contributions from inhibitory interneurons in alternating cycles. Notably, while both points produce a schizophrenic pattern of oscillatory activity when analyzed at the power spectrum level, there are clear differences in underlying neurophysiologic dynamics, as shown in Figure 5C and D. Panel C clearly shows alternating pyramidal cell activity across cycles; it also reveals a somewhat less marked alternation of PV+ cell activity, as well as modest cycle-to-cycle CR+ activity imbalance. Panel D (secondary point) shows a general damping down of pyramidal cell activity that is roughly constant across cycles, and little cycle-to-cycle variation in PV+ or CR+ activity.
Simulation of Medication Effects
Negative controls:
An important goal of this work is to develop a model that can identify novel pharmacologic agents that can potentially treat the symptoms of schizophrenia. Such a model should also be capable of rejecting current medications known to have no known antipsychotic efficacy. Therefore, when applied to the schizophrenic model they should not produce normalization of oscillatory powers. These then serve as “negative controls”. We chose the test agents described below based on the following considerations: (a) Their neurophysiologic effects are well-characterized, and they can therefore be included in the model in a rigorous manner. (b) There is a published literature documenting their non-effectiveness in the illness. (c) There is a history of clinical use, and their effects on (control) subject EEG activity are known.
For these trials, the primary point schizophrenic model, as defined above, is used as our test system. In separate trials, we apply the effects of phenytoin, an antiepileptic drug that has a specific effect at the Na+ channel (Fig 6); nifedipine, an antihypertensive that acts by blocking calcium channels (Fig 7); and ampakines, medications that allosterically bind to AMPA receptors and increase their activity [42]–[44], both by increasing maximum conductance and by increasing the decay time constant (Fig 8). In no case does the agent correct the 40 Hz deficit. Moreover, when applied to our unaffected model, they produce EEG changes comparable to those seen in the clinical literature. This serves as additional confirmation of the validity of the computational model.
Virtual medication trials:
Many experimental medications for schizophrenia act through one particular mechanism of action. However, it is possible that adjustment of a number of cellular level “levers” would be necessary to return the system to a healthy equilibrium state. We examined five such effects, applying each to the model individually, and in combinations with others. Broadly, these mechanisms fall into two categories: those that can be effected with currently known medications (discussed under AMPA gmax, alpha2, and NMDA sections below); and those that, to the knowledge of these authors, cannot be implemented with any currently known agent (discussed under AMPA τ2 and CR+ projection below)—if effective, these would then represent potential targets for drug development efforts. The manner in which these were modeled is briefly described below, and are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Parameter ranges used for simulated medications.
Parameter | Description | Units | Range of Values | Incr |
AMPA gmax | conductance of AMPA channel | % increase | 0, 20, 40, 60, 80 | 5 |
alpha2 | conductance of GABA channel, alpha2 subtype | % increase | 0, 15, 30, 45, 60 | 5 |
NMDA | conductance of NMDA channel1 | % increase | 0, 20, 40, 60, 80 | 5 |
AMPA τ2 | decay time constant of AMPA channel | msec | 1, 3, 5 | 3 |
CR proj | weight of projection of CR cells on postsynaptic targets | % increase | 0, 20, 40, 60 | 4 |
total number of simulated trials: 1,500 |
Resultant increase in intracellular Ca++ also induces LTP; the quantitative manner in which this is implemented is described in the text, and is in addition to the effect shown here.
Using the schizophrenic model, simulated medication trials were run, systematically varying model parameters through the ranges shown. A total of 3×5× 5×5×4 = 1,500 simulated trials were conducted. Incr = number of increments.
AMPA gmax. The effect of drugs that boost AMPA current were modeled by increasing the maximum conductance (gmax) of the AMPA synaptic current. We did this in increments of 20%, increasing gmax from 0% to 80%.
Alpha2. The experimental drug MK-0777 (also known as TPA-023) has partial agonist activity at GABAA receptors, specifically acting at the α2 and α3 subtypes [45], and has shown partial effectiveness in treating some of the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia [46]. These receptor subtypes are located on the initial segment of pyramidal cells, and are thought to be associated with the inhibitory projections of chandelier cells. While dissociation constants have been quantified [45], to our knowledge, MK-0777’s quantitative effect on GABA channel conductance has not been. Electrophysiological studies with mutant mice (knock-in mice selectively expressing GABAA α2 , α3 , etc subtypes), has indicated that benzodiazepines can increase α2 and α3 conductance by as much as 50% [47]. Thus, to capture a plausible range of drug-induced conductance changes, we selectively increased the gmax of the GABA channels that synapse on the initial segment of pyramidal cells in increments of 15%, increasing gmax from 0 to 60%, in five gradations.
MK-0777 is one of the few cases in which a drug was tested in an experimental paradigm that involved schizophrenic patients and measurement of gamma band oscillations [46]. In this work, schizophrenic patients taking this drug showed a trend toward greater gamma band activity, which did not reach statistical significance at the p = 0.05 level (their Figure 1, p. 1589-90). To ensure that our model system behaved similarly, we implemented an MK-0777 effect alone, and observed a very modest increase in 40 Hz resonance within certain dose ranges (data not shown), consistent with the experimental findings.
NMDA. NMDA boosting drugs, such as D-serine, have potential benefit both because they increase NMDA current, and because resulting intracellular calcium increases enhance long term potentiation (LTP). To model the former, we increased gmax of the NMDA conductance in increments of 20%, to a maximum 80% increase, in five gradations. Single cell modeling that we carried out suggested that the ratio of percentage NMDA conductance increase: overall intracellular calcium concentration increase was approximately 2:1. While it is known that increases in intracellular Ca++ concentration trigger LTP, their precise quantitative relationship remains uncertain [48]–[50]. Detailed modeling work by Shouval et al [51] suggests a ratio of approximately 62%: 29% (increase in calcium: degree of synaptic strengthening) (their Figure 1, p. 10832). Based on this, for every NMDA channel, for each 20% increase in NMDA channel conductance, we also increased the synaptic weight factor by 4.7%.
AMPA τ2. Modeling work described above suggests that significantly increasing the AMPA conductance decay time (τ2), in the manner of certain ampakines, does not improve model performance. However, in exploratory modeling work, we found that decreasing this parameter seemed to have positive effects (data not shown). Therefore, we used τ2 values of 1, 3 (control), and 5 msec.
CR+ projections. Exploratory runs of the schizophrenic model indicated that the calretinin cell projections (which impinge only on other interneurons) have a general quantitative modulatory role: increasing the weight of these projections tended to produce greater network activity overall (including 40 Hz oscillatory behavior) and decreasing them lead to generalized decreases in activity. Based on this, we adjusted upward the synaptic weight factor of the CR+ cells onto their postsynaptic targets, increasing it from 0 to 60%, in four gradations.
We ran each of the above effects alone, and in combination with all other effects, for a total of 5×5×5×3×4 = 1,500 trials (Table 1). For each trial, the model was driven at 20, 30, and 40 Hz, as described in our previous trials investigating schizophrenic pathology. To the extent that a simulated medication specifically increased 40 Hz power response to 40 Hz drive, it was considered effective. That is, if a treated schizophrenic model exactly replicated control model behavior, it would receive a score of 1.0; the score was decreased to the extent that it departed from this. Trials that did not produce 20 Hz and 30 Hz power within 10% of control were marked as failed trials, and received a score of 0. Thus, a simulated drug that boosted all frequencies indiscriminately would not be considered effective.
In total, 97 virtual medications, or 6.5% of the 1,500 tried, produced non-zero scores. 24 received scores 0.90 or higher. The characteristics of these drugs are shown via histograms in Figure 9.
It can be seen that of these top performing medications, many decreased AMPA τ2 and modestly increased NMDA activity. Clearly, a number of these effects may have interacted to produce desirable outcomes. To understand this at a fine grained level, we ran a three-way analysis of variance on the 97 scored virtual medications; results are shown in Table 2, with effects showing significance at a level of p < 0.001 indicated. Of note, (decreasing) AMPA τ2 emerged as a significant effect, alone and in combination. A very significant interaction between AMPA τ2 and CR+ projection strength also emerged.
Table 2. Analysis of variance of model response to drug effects.
Simulated drug effect | F value | p value | |
alpha2 | 0.01 | 0.916 | |
AMPA gmax | 1.76 | 0.185 | |
AMPA τ2 | 25.14 | 6.00E-07 | * |
NMDA | 21.17 | 4.55E-06 | * |
CR proj | 0.35 | 0.554 | |
alpha2:AMPA gmax | 0.92 | 0.338 | |
alpha2:AMPA τ2 | 1.23 | 0.268 | |
AMPA gmax:AMPA τ2 | 13.43 | 2.56E-04 | * |
alpha2:NMDA | 6.52 | 0.011 | |
AMPA gmax:NMDA | 5.24 | 0.022 | |
AMPA τ2:NMDA | 0.01 | 0.914 | |
alpha2:CR proj | 1.78 | 0.182 | |
AMPA gmax:CR proj | 10.44 | 0.001 | |
AMPA τ2:CR proj | 68.18 | 3.29E-16 | * |
NMDA:CR proj | 1.91 | 0.167 | |
alpha2:AMPA gmax:AMPA τ2 | 0.84 | 0.358 | |
alpha2:AMPA gmax:NMDA | 0.48 | 0.490 | |
alpha2:AMPA τ2:NMDA | 1.53 | 0.217 | |
AMPA gmax:AMPA τ2:NMDA | 1.57 | 0.211 | |
alpha2:AMPA gmax:CR proj | 0.37 | 0.545 | |
alpha2:AMPA τ2:CR proj | 4.19 | 0.041 | |
AMPA gmax:AMPA τ2:CR proj | 1.11 | 0.292 | |
alpha2:NMDA:CR proj | 2.69 | 0.101 | |
AMPA gmax:NMDA:CR proj | 4.21 | 0.040 | |
AMPA τ2:NMDA:CR proj | 13.34 | 2.70E-04 | * |
p < 0.001
Using the wellness metric (see text), as the outcome variable, 97 of 1,500 simulated medications produced non-zero values. ANOVA of this output, using parameters for drug effects as factors, is shown. Highly significant effects (p < 0.001) are indicated. CR proj = CR+ projection.
Discussion
It is clear that antipsychotics with fundamentally new mechanisms of action are needed. A primary factor hindering their development has been a lack of well specified, detailed models indicating how the myriad cellular level abnormalities that have been identified experimentally interact to create symptoms. Our work represents an initial attempt to address these questions. Models like ours may allow a process of drug development in which the “target” is not necessarily a single neural entity that is abnormal, but rather a system level behavior that has become dysfunctional. It may be possible to treat the illness by acting on biological entities that are not directly causative, but that can serve to re-equilibrate the system.
Mechanistic Implications
While we acknowledge that 40 Hz oscillatory deficit is not a “classical” symptom of schizophrenia, we felt it was a highly appropriate outcome measure for this computational study for two reasons. First, given the likely importance of gamma band activity in subserving perceptual binding within and across sensory modalities and in cognition generally [18], and the core schizophrenic symptoms of compromised reality testing and hallucinations, a strong argument can be made that the gamma band biomarker is tapping into an important characteristic of the illness. Second, a growing body of clinical work suggests that it may represent an important endophenotypic marker of the disease. In a recent review of this literature [52], it was shown that across all frequencies (theta [4–7 Hz], alpha [8–12 Hz], beta [12–30 Hz], and gamma) and testing paradigms (steady state evoked potentials, induced responses, evoked responses, and resting state measurement), studies that showed the most robust and consistent evidence for a schizophrenic patient-specific phenomenon were those looking at steady state evoked responses in the gamma band. Because endophenotypes—as opposed to complex clinical phenotypes—may be more closely related to the genetic underpinnings of the disease, a focus on these markers may be extremely valuable in elucidating etiology and informing treatments [53].
Our modeling suggests that in the hippocampal etiology of schizophrenia, neural level abnormalities are perhaps not simply additive—that is, that more pathology, regardless of type, necessarily creates more illness. Rather, it appears that there may be one or more discrete combinations of abnormalities that give rise to the decreased gamma band activity that is associated with the illness. Of note, both sets of pathology we identified were characterized by co-occurring modest reduction in spine density, as well as reductions in NMDA functionality. Neither of these lesions alone, even occurring at extreme levels, was seen to be associated with schizophrenia-like model behavior. It appeared that particular combinations of GABA system lesions could lead to a specific, and modest, lessening of 40 Hz response; but no combinations of GABA lesions alone resulted in a pattern that was quantitatively similar to the schizophrenic dysfunction seen in the literature.
Significantly, the two clusters we identified were associated with dissimilar underlying neural dynamics. The most pronounced one (the primary point) showed a highly regular “beat skipping” quality, which created increased 20 Hz resonance in addition to decreased 40 Hz activity. A similar mechanism was seen in a previous modeling study [54]. This is particularly significant because that study implemented a very different mechanism—a lengthening of the decay time constant of the projections of PV+ interneurons—to generate similar behavior. This raises the possibility that this behavior may be an important mechanistic trait associated with the illness, at least in particular brain areas, and that different sets of cellular level abnormalities can give rise to it.
The other schizophrenic combination (the secondary point) we identified did not show this behavior, but appeared to arise from a dampening of 40 Hz behavior generally. This is consistent with the apparent inconsistencies in clinical research, in which some studies show a higher 20 Hz response [55], and many studies did not [17], [41] among schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia’s extreme heterogeneity has always been puzzling. This modeling work raises the possibility that different subtypes are associated with particular sets of neural abnormalities.
Post-mortem and other wet lab research methodologies tend to be very labor intensive, and investigating all combinations of possible neural abnormalities in large numbers of samples is not practical. The type of modeling study described here could be used as a guide, or hypothesis-generating tool, for laboratory research. Moreover, if clinical information is known about the tissue source (which is usually the case), the hypothesis that particular clusters of neural abnormalities correspond to particular subtypes can be tested.
Treatment Implications
Many traditional drug discovery efforts have involved identifying a cellular level abnormality associated with an illness, and creating an agent to counteract that particular deficiency. These efforts have not been entirely successful in the case of schizophrenia, and the modeling work here presents an alternative approach. As a test system, we used the primary point schizophrenia model, as described above. First, medications with no know antipsychotic efficacy were introduced to the schizophrenic model, to ensure that the model identified them as such. This is admittedly a “low bar”, but any test system to identify potentially effective agents should, at a minimum, be able to reject those that are clinically known to be inactive. Then, we carried out a series of 1,500 virtual medication trials, using five different potential drug mechanisms.
Perhaps the most surprising outcome of these simulated drug trials was the model’s prediction that medications that decrease the decay time constant of the AMPA channel would be potentially effective agents. This was apparent in looking at “wellness metric” data descriptively; it also emerged on two and three way analyses of variance. Of the five virtual drugs with highly significant p values, four involved an AMPA τ2 effect. This and other computational studies [54] have suggested that lingering or “blurring” of inhibitory processes may prevent the system from attuning to the relatively fast 40 Hz input drive. To the extent that a reduction of AMPA τ2 causes a sharpening of signaling, this could be beneficial.
Also, there were marked interactions between effects. Notably, the ANOVA showed a very weak single factor CR+ projection strength effect, but an extremely strong interactive effect with decreases in AMPA τ2 (the most robust interaction of all combinations tested). There was a very strong interaction between AMPA τ2 decrease and AMPA gmax as well. Increasing AMPA gmax and CR+ projection are similar in that alone, they would have the effect of increasing excitatory activity generally. It is therefore not surprising that ANOVA revealed these particular interactions—these combinations may result in an increased magnitude of a more “precise” signal.
The implications of these findings are threefold: First, it is possible that a given unsuccessfully tested mechanism (e.g. increasing AMPA conductance via ampakines) is not incorrect, strictly speaking, but rather incomplete—that is, in combination with other cellular level effects, amelioration of symptoms could be achieved. However, because of system complexity, it is difficult to determine a priori, based on deductive reasoning alone, which particular combination of “levers” could lead to a re-equilibration. Modeling can help identify the particular combination of mechanisms that will constitute effective medications.
Second, based on the existing literature, CR+ interneurons have not been implicated as a cause of schizophrenia [56]-[58], nor were they altered in our model to render it schizophrenic. The same is true of the decay time constant of the AMPA conductance. Nonetheless, altering these features helped to re-equilibrate the system, and return it to its control state. This implies that the search for effective antipsychotics need not be limited to neural elements that have been demonstrated in postmortem or other wet lab work to be abnormal in the illness.
Finally, this model makes specific, testable hypotheses. To our knowledge, there is no existing drug that specifically increases the weight of CR+ post-synaptic projections or increases the conductance at these synapses, or that specifically decreases the APMA decay time constant. This work suggests that medications that incorporate these effects may be efficacious for schizophrenia.
Supporting Information
Funding Statement
This research was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health grants K08 MH072771 and P50 MH060450. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Stilo SA, Murray RM (2010) The epidemiology of schizophrenia: replacing dogma with knowlege. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12: 305–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Henderson DC, Cagliero E, Copeland PM, Borba CP, Evins E, et al. (2005) Glucose metabolism in patients with schizophrenia treated with atypical antipsychotic agents: a frequently sampled intravenous glucose tolerance test and minimal model analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry 62: 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Coyle JT, Balu D, Benneyworth M, Basu A, Roseman A (2010) Beyond the dopamine receptor: novel therapeutic targets for treating schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12: 359382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Heckers S, Konradi C (2010) Hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences 4: 529–553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Benes F, Berretta S (2001) GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 25: 1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Coyle J (2006) Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 4–6: 365–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Greene R (2001) Circuit analysis of NMDAR hypofunction in the hippocampus, in vitro, and psychosis of schizophrenia. Hippocampus 11: 569–577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hoffman RE, McGlashan TH (2003) Schizophrenia: From psychodynamic to neurodynamic theories. In: Sadock JS, Sadock VA, editors. Kaplan and Sadock's Synopsis of Psychiatry. 9th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Feinberg I (1982) Schizophrenia: caused by a fault in programmed synaptic elimination during adolescence? Journal of Psychiatric Research 17: 319–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Beggs J (2008) The criticality hypothesis: how local cortical networks might optimize information processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Physical, Mathematical and Engineering Sciences 366: 329–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lisman J (2012) Excitation, inhibition, local oscillations, or large-scale loops: what causes the symptoms of schizophrenia? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 22: 537–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Cho RY, Konecky RO, Carter CS (2006) Impairments in frontal cortical gamma synchrony and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 103: 19878–19883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Ferrarelli F, Massimini M, Peterson MJ, Riedner BA, Lazar M, et al. (2008) Reduced evoked gamma oscillations in the frontal cortex in schizophrenia patients: a TMS/EEG study. American Journal of Psychiatry 165: 996–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hirano S, Hirano Y, Maekawa T, Obayashi C, Oribe N, et al. (2008) Abnormal neural oscillatory activity to speech sounds in schizophrenia: a magnetoencephalography study. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 4897–4903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Spencer KM, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW (2008) Gamma-band auditory steady-state responses are imparied in first episode psychosis. Biological Psychiatry 64: 369–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Teale P, Collins D, Maharajh K, Rojas DC, Kronberg E, et al. (2008) Cortical source estimates of gamma band amplitude and phase are different in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 42: 1481–1489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Light GA, Hsu JL, Hsieh MH, Meyer-Gomes K, Sprock J, et al. (2006) Gamma band oscillations reveal neural network cortical coherence dysfunction in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry 60: 1231–1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kopell N, Ermentrout G, Whittington M, Traub R (2000) Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 97: 1867–1872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Wang X (2006) Toward a prefrontal microcircuit model for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry 39 Suppl 1S80–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Durstewitz D, Seamans J (2008) The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function wiht relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 64: 739–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hoffman RE (1987) Computer simulations of neural information processing and the schizophrenia-mania dichotomy. Archives of General Psychiatry 44: 178–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Rolls ET, Loh M, Deco G, Winterer G (2008) Computational models of schizophrenia and dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 696–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Siekmeier PJ, Hasselmo ME, Howard MW, Coyle J (2007) Modeling of context-dependent retrieval in hippocampal region CA1: Implications for cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 89: 177–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Traub RD, Jefferys JG, Miles R, Whittington MA, Toth K (1994) A branching dendritic model of a rodent CA3 pyramidal neurone. Journal of Physiology (London) 481: 79–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Traub RD, Miles R (1995) Pyramidal cell-to-inhibitory cell spike transduction explicable by active dendritic conductances in inhibitory cell. Journal of Computational Neuroscience 2: 291–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Siekmeier PJ (2009) Evidence of multistability in a realistic computer simulation of hippocampus subfield CA1. Behavioural Brain Research 200: 220–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tsai G, Passani L, Slusher B, Carter R, Baer L, et al. (1995) Abnormal excitatory neurotransmitter metabolism in schizophrenic brains. Archives of General Psychiatry 52: 829–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Bergeron T, Coyle J, Tsai G, Greene R (2005) NAAG reduces NMDA receptor current in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons of acute slices and dissociated neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 7–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Law A, Deakin J (2001) Asymmetrical reductions of hippocampal NMDAR1 glutamate receptor mRNA in the psychoses. Neurochemistry 12: 2971–2974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Harrison P, Law A, Eastwood S (2003) Glutamate receptors and transporters in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1003: 94–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Hoffman RE, Dobscha SK (1989) Cortical pruning and the development of schizophrenia: a computer model. Schizophrenia Bulletin 15: 477–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Law A, Weickert C, Hyde T, Kleinman J, Harrison P (2004) Reduced spinophilin but not microtubule-associated protein 2 express in the hippocampal formation in schizohprenia and mood disorders: Molecular evidence for a pathology of dendritic spines. American Journal of Psychiatry 161: 1848–1855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Garey LJ, Ong WY, Patel TS, Kanani M, Davis A, et al. (1998) Reduced dendritic spine density on cerebral cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 65: 446–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Devito L, Balu D, Kanter B, Lykken C, Basu A, et al. (2011) Serine racemase deletion disrupts memory for order and alters cortical dendritic morphology. Genes Brain Behav 10: 210–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Heckers S, Stone D, Walsh J, Shick J, Koul P, et al. (2002) Differential Hippocampal Expression of Glutamic Acid Decarboxylase 65 and 67 Messenger RNA in Bipolar Disorder and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 59: 521–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Bird E, Barnes J, LL I, Spokes E, Mackay A, et al. (1977) Increased brain dopamine and reduced glutamic acid decarboxylase and choline acetyl transferase activity in schizophrenia and related psychoses. The Lancet 2: 1157–1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Fatemi S, Earle J, McMenomy T (2000) Reduction in reelin immunoreactivity in hippocampus of subjects with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depression. Molecular Psychiatry 5: 654–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Torrey E, Barci B, Webster M, Bartko J, Meador-Woodruff J, et al. (2005) Neurochemical markers for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression in postmortem brains. Biological Psychitry 57: 252–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Benes FM, Khan Y, Vincent SL, Wickramasinghe R (1996) Differences in the subregional and cellular distribution of GABAA receptor binding in the hippocampal formation of schizophrenic brain. Synapse 22: 338–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Lisman J, Coyle J, Green R, Javitt D, Benes F, et al. (2008) Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends in Neuroscience 31: 234–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Kwon JS, O'Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, et al. (1999) Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 56: 1001–1005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Johansen TH, Chaudhary A, Verdoorn TA (1995) Interactions among GYKI-52466, cyclothiazide, and aniracetam at recombinant AMPA and kainate receptors. Mol Pharmacol 48: 946–955. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Arai AC, Kessler M, Rogers G, Lynch G (2000) Effects of the potent ampakine CX614 on hippocampal and recombinant AMPA receptors: interactions with cyclothiazide and GYKI 52466. Mol Pharmacol 58: 802–813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Arai AC, Xia YF, Rogers G, Lynch G, Kessler M (2002) Benzamide-type AMPA receptor modulators form two subfamilies with distinct modes of action. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 303: 1075–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Atack JR, Wong DF, Fryer TD, Ryan C, Sanabria S, et al. (2010) Benzodiazepine binding site occupancy by the novel GABAA receptor subtype-selective drug 7-(1,1-Dimethylethyl)-6-(2-ethyl-2H-1,2,4-triazol-3-ylmethoxy)-3-(2-fluorophenyl)-1,2,4-triazolo[4,4-b]pyridazine (TPA023) in rats, primates, and humans. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 332: 17–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Lewis DA, Cho RY, Carter CS, Eklund K, Forster S, et al. (2008) Subunit-selective modulation of GABA type A receptor neurotransmission and cognition in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 165: 1585–1593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Prenosil GA, Gasser EM, Rudolph U, Keist R, Fritschy J-M, et al. (2006) Specific subtypes of GABAA receptors mediate phasic and tonic forms of inhibition in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 96: 846–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Kotaleski JH, Blackwell KT (2010) Modeling the molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity using systems biology approaches. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 239–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Lynch MA (2004) Long-term potentiation and memory. Physiology Reviews 84: 87–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Cavazzini M, Bliss T, Emptage N (2005) Ca2+ and synaptic plasticity. Cell Calcium 38: 355–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Shouval HZ, Bear MF, Cooper LN (2002) A unified model of NMDA receptor-dependent bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99: 10831–10836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W (2010) Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 100–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger D (2006) Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 818–827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Vierling-Claassen D, Siekmeier P, Stufflebeam S, Kopell N (2008) Modeling GABA alterations in schizophrenia: a link between impaired inhibition and altered gamma and beta range auditory entrainment. Journal of Neurophysiology 99: 2656–2671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Kissler J, Muller MM, Fehr T, Rockstroh B, Elbert T (2000) MEG gamma band activity in schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects in a mental arithmetic task and at rest. Clinical Neurophysiology 111: 2079–2087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Beasley CL, Zhang ZJ, Patten I, Reynolds GP (2002) Selective deficits in prefrontal cortical GABAergic neurons in schizophrenia defined by the presence of calcium-binding proteins. Biological Psychiatry 52: 708–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Hashimoto T, Bazmi HH, Mirnics K, Wu Q, Sampson AR, et al. (2008) Conserved regional patterns of GABA-related transcript expression in the neocortex of subjects with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 165: 497–489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Knable MB, Barci BM, Webster MJ, Meador-Woodruff J, Torrey EF (2004) Molecular abnormalities of the hippocampus in severe psychiatric illness: postmortem findings from the Stanley Neuropathology Consortium. Molecular Psychiatry 9: 609–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Berk R, Bickel P, Campbell K, Fovell R, Keller-McNulty S, et al. (2007) Workshop on statistical approaches for the evaluation of complex computer models. Statistical Science 17: 173–192. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Bayarri M, Berger J, Cafeo G, Garcia-Donato F, Liu F, et al. (2007) Computer model validation with functional output. The Annals of Statistics 35: 1874–1906. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Easterling R, Berger J. Statistical foundations for the validation of computer models. In: Pace D, Stevenson S, editors; 2002. Society for Modeling and Simulation International.
- 62. Kilpatrick CJ, Wanwimolruk S, Wing LM (1984) Plasma concentrations of unbound phenytoin in the management of epilepsy. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 17: 539–546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Lampl I, Schwindt P, Crill W (1998) Reduction of Cortical Pyramidal Neuron Excitability by the Action of Phenytoin on Persistent Na+ Current. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 284: 228–237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Fink M, Irwin P, Sannita W, Papakostas Y, Green MA (1979) Phenytoin: EEG effects and plasma levels in volunteers. Therapeutic Drug Monitoring 1: 93–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Chao TI, Alzheimer C (1995) Effects of phenytoin on the persistent Na+ curent of mammalian CNS neurons. NeuroReport 6: 1778–1780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Herkes GK, Lagerlund TD, Sharbrough FW, Eadie MJ (1993) Effects of antiepileptic drug treatment on the background frequency of EEGs in epileptic patients. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 10: 210–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Van Wieringen A, Binnie CD, Meijer JWA, Peck AW, de Vries J (1989) Comparison of the effects of lamotragine and phenytoin on the EEG power spectrum and cortical brainstem-evoked responses of normal human volunteers. Neuropsychobiology 21: 157–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Cunningham MO, Whittington MA, Bibbig A, ZRoopun A, LeBean REN, et al. (2004) A role for fast rhythmic bursting neurons in cortical gamma oscillations in vitro. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 7152–7157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Deutz NEP, Chamuleau RA, Bovee WM, VanderWerf AJ (1986) Effects of nimodipine on EEG and 31P-NMR spectra during and after incomplete forebrian ischemia in the rat. European Journal of Pharmacology 125: 429–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Denolle T, Sassano P, Allain H, Bentue-Ferrer D, Breton S, et al. (2002) Effects of nicardipine and clonidine on cognitive functions and electroencephalography in hypertensive patients. Fundamental and Clinical Pharmacology 16: 527–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Sannita WG, Garbarino S, Gesino D, Massimilla S, Ogliastro C (1999) Plasma concentrations and CNS effects of Ca antagonists darodpine and nimodipine after single-dose oral administration to healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiolgy 40: 158–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Johansen TH, Chaudhary A, Verdoorn TA (1995) Interactions among GYKI-52488, cyclothiazide, and anifacetam at recombinant AMPA and kainate receptors. Molecular Pharmacology 48: 946–955. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Arai AC, Kessler M, Rogers G, Lynch G (2000) Effects of the potent amapkine CX614 on hippocampal and recombinant AMPA receptors: interactions with cyclothiazide and GYKI 52466. Molecular Pharmacology 58: 802–813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Arai AC, Xia Y-F, Gogers G, Lynch G, Kessler M (2002) Benzamide-type AMPA receptor modulators form two subfamilies with distinct modes of action. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 303: 1075–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Grove SJA, Jamieson C, Maclean JKF, Morrow JA, Rankovic Z (2010) Positive allosteric modulators of the alpha-amino-3-hyroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor. Journal of Medicinal Chemistry 53: 7271–7279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Goff DC, Lamberti JS, Leaon AC, Green MF, Miller AL, et al. (2008) A placebo-controlled add-on trial of amapkine, CX516, for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 465–472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.