Skip to main content
BMC Neurology logoLink to BMC Neurology
. 2013 Mar 18;13:27. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-27

Assessment of localisation to auditory stimulation in post-comatose states: use the patient’s own name

Lijuan Cheng 1,#, Olivia Gosseries 2,#, Limei Ying 1, Xiaohua Hu 3, Dan Yu 3, Hongxing Gao 3, Minhui He 1, Caroline Schnakers 2, Steven Laureys 2, Haibo Di 1,2,
PMCID: PMC3606124  PMID: 23506054

Abstract

Background

At present, there is no consensus on how to clinically assess localisation to sound in patients recovering from coma. We here studied auditory localisation using the patient’s own name as compared to a meaningless sound (i.e., ringing bell).

Methods

Eighty-six post-comatose patients diagnosed with a vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome or a minimally conscious state were prospectively included. Localisation of auditory stimulation (i.e., head or eyes orientation toward the sound) was assessed using the patient’s own name as compared to a ringing bell. Statistical analyses used binomial testing with bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Results

37 (43%) out of the 86 studied patients showed localisation to auditory stimulation. More patients (n=34, 40%) oriented the head or eyes to their own name as compared to sound (n=20, 23%; p<0.001).

Conclusions

When assessing auditory function in disorders of consciousness, using the patient’s own name is here shown to be more suitable to elicit a response as compared to neutral sound.

Keywords: Localisation to sound, Auditory localisation, Vegetative state, Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, Minimally conscious state, Own name, Disorders of consciousness

Background

At present, there is no consensus on what auditory stimulus should be employed for the assessment of localisation to sound in disorders of consciousness such as the “vegetative state” (now also coined unresponsive wakefulness syndrome; VS/UWS [1]) and the minimally conscious state (MCS) [2]. Indeed, several behavioural “coma scales” use different stimuli to evaluate auditory localisation. For instance, the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R), the Sensory Modality Assessment Rehabilitation Technique and the Western Neuro-Sensory Stimulation Profile leave the choice open between several auditory stimuli (e.g., noise, voice). The Coma/Near Coma Scale requests to use “5s of bell ringing”, and the Wessex Head Injury Matrix uses a noise (bell, whistle or buzzer) and “a person talking” (for a review, see [3]).

We here propose to use the patient’s own name (as compared to a meaningless noise) in the assessment of localisation to sound. The own name is intrinsically meaningful for each of us because of its personal significance, emotional content and repetition along life. Beyond our day-to-day experience, the extreme salience of being presented one's own name was highlighted in various experimental and clinical studies. Some of these suggest that self-referential stimuli are so potent that they can "capture attention and subsequently bring the stimulus into awareness" [4]. In everyday social interactions, auto-referential stimuli give rise to a sense of self-awareness, as reflected in the cocktail party phenomenon when hearing our own name [5]. This particularly easy detection in usual laboratory experiments with healthy participants is consistent with research that showed powerful detection of the own name in situations of reduced consciousness [6,7]. The aim of the present study is to determine whether the assessment of localisation to sound in patients recovering from coma is influenced by the choice of the auditory stimulus.

Methods

Eighty-six patients recovering from coma were prospectively assessed free of sedative drugs. Each patient was studied in a sitting position and a standardized arousal facilitation protocol (i.e., deep pressure stimulations from the facial musculature to the toes) was employed if needed in order to prolong the length of time the patient maintained arousal [8]. Localisation to sound was evaluated using a standardized methodology as described in the CRS-R [8]. In brief, an auditory stimulus (bell and patient’s own name) was presented from the right and from the left side while the examiner stood next to the patient but out of view. Stimuli were matched for intensity and duration of presentation, and were presented twice for each side. The order of presentation was randomized using “random number” procedure in Excel. Localisation to auditory stimulation was defined as head or eyes orientation toward the location of the stimulus on both trials for at least one side. Special care was made not to present stimuli when spontaneous eye or head movements were occurring. Clinical diagnosis was made according to the Aspen workgroup criteria for disorders of consciousness [2] and based on the CRS-R assessments [8] performed by two trained and experienced neuropsychologists. Note that according to these guidelines auditory localisation is compatible with the diagnosis of VS/UWS. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hangzhou Normal University and Wujing Hospital which complies with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Informed consents were obtained by the patient’s legal surrogates.

Differences between localisation as assessed by bell or patient's own name were measured using binomial testing (Stata Statistical Software; Release 11.2. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP 2009). Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparisons. Results were considered significant at p<0.01.

Results

Out of 86 patients (67 men; mean age 46 (SD 17) years), 47 (55%) were diagnosed in VS/UWS [1] and 37 (45%) were in MCS. Median time between injury and assessment was 5 months (IQR: 3 – 13 months). Aetiology was traumatic in 53 (61%) and non-traumatic in 33 (39%) patients. 37 (43%) out of the 86 studied patients showed localisation to auditory stimulation. Overall, more patients (n=34, 40%) oriented the head or eyes to the own name as compared to sound (n=20, 23%; p<0.001) (Table 1). MCS patients localized more often to their own name as compared to sound (p<0.001). This effect was not significant in the VS/UWS group (p>0.05) (Figure 1).

Table 1.

Auditory localisation according to diagnosis and aetiology

Diagnosis
Localisation response
Aetiology
Total
  Own name Bell Both None Traumatic Non traumatic  
VS/UWS
4 (5%)
1 (1%)
4 (5%)
38 (44%)
26 (30%)
21 (25%)
47
MCS
13 (15%)
2 (2%)
13 (15%)
11 (13%)
27 (31%)
12 (14%)
39
Total 17 (20%) 3 (3%) 17 (20%) 49 (57%) 53 (61%) 33 (39%) 86

Number of patients showing localisation to the own name, ringing bell or both as a function of diagnosis [vegetative state (VS/UWS) versus minimally conscious state (MCS)] and aetiology.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Auditory localisation. Number of patients in vegetative/unresponsive state (VS/UWS) and minimally conscious state (MCS) showing localisation to sound (n=37) as a function of the employed stimulus (own name in black and ringing bell in white).

Tables 2 and 3 show the clinical data for each patient (MCS and VS/UWS patient groups respectively). Localisation preference was not different depending on aetiology or time since insult (p>0.05). The overall behavioural responsiveness assessed by the CRS-R total score tended to be higher when patients localized both stimuli than when they did not show any localisation (Tables 2 and 3). For instance, MCS patients showing both responses to their own name and to the bell had a CRS-R total score between 9 and 18 whereas MCS patients showing no localisation had a score between 6 and 10. Patients localizing to their own name only (or bell only) showed intermediate CRS-R total scores. In the 37 patients showing localisation, 9 patients were considered as being in VS/UWS according to the CRS-R criteria (i.e., they showed no response to command, no orientation to pain and no visual tracking) - 4 of these patients showed orientation to the own name but not to sound.

Table 2.

Clinical data of the MCS patients

Patient Gender Aetiology Time since injury* CRS-R score** Auditory localisation
MCS1
male
trauma
7
6
none
MCS2
male
trauma
73
7
none
MCS3
male
trauma
21
7
none
MCS4
female
trauma
20
7
none
MCS5
male
trauma
155
8
none
MCS6
female
trauma
160
8
none
MCS7
female
trauma
21
8
none
MCS8
male
trauma
205
9
none
MCS9
male
trauma
20
10
none
MCS10
male
stroke
51
10
none
MCS11
male
trauma
45
10
none
MCS12
female
trauma
9
8
bell
MCS13
male
stroke
32
9
bell
MCS14
male
trauma
55
8
own name
MCS15
male
trauma
11
8
own name
MCS16
male
trauma
221
8
own name
MCS17
male
trauma
150
9
own name
MCS18
male
trauma
40
9
own name
MCS19
female
stroke
14
9
own name
MCS20
male
stroke
61
10
own name
MCS21
male
trauma
22
10
own name
MCS22
male
trauma
19
13
own name
MCS23
male
trauma
7
13
own name
MCS24
female
trauma
54
14
own name
MCS25
male
trauma
291
14
own name
MCS26
male
stroke
115
16
own name
MCS27
male
anoxia
50
9
both
MCS28
female
trauma
7
10
both
MCS29
male
trauma
13
10
both
MCS30
male
trauma
121
10
both
MCS31
male
trauma
33
11
both
MCS32
male
stroke
13
11
both
MCS33
male
trauma
12
12
both
MCS34
male
stroke
9
13
both
MCS35
female
stroke
22
15
both
MCS36
male
anoxia
135
16
both
MCS37
male
stroke
6
16
both
MCS38
male
anoxia
57
17
both
MCS39 male trauma 3 18 both

*Time since injury in weeks, ** Total score of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (minimum 0, maximum 23).

Table 3.

Clinical data of the VS/UWS patients

Patient Gender Aetiology Time since injury* CRS-R score** Auditory localisation
VS/UWS1
male
stroke
9
2
none
VS/UWS2
male
anoxic
6
3
none
VS/UWS3
male
trauma
34
3
none
VS/UWS4
male
trauma
17
3
none
VS/UWS5
male
stroke
66
4
none
VS/UWS6
male
anoxic
82
4
none
VS/UWS7
male
trauma
39
4
none
VS/UWS8
male
trauma
13
4
none
VS/UWS9
female
anoxia
4
4
none
VS/UWS10
male
stroke
13
5
none
VS/UWS11
male
anoxia
89
5
none
VS/UWS12
male
stroke
5
5
none
VS/UWS13
female
trauma
8
5
none
VS/UWS14
male
trauma
189
5
none
VS/UWS15
female
stroke
5
5
none
VS/UWS16
male
stroke
3
5
none
VS/UWS17
female
trauma
68
6
none
VS/UWS18
male
trauma
76
6
none
VS/UWS19
male
trauma
36
6
none
VS/UWS20
male
trauma
13
6
none
VS/UWS21
male
trauma
22
6
none
VS/UWS22
male
trauma
8
6
none
VS/UWS23
male
stroke
21
6
none
VS/UWS24
male
trauma
9
6
none
VS/UWS25
male
stroke
16
6
none
VS/UWS26
male
trauma
34
6
none
VS/UWS27
male
trauma
19
6
none
VS/UWS28
male
trauma
12
6
none
VS/UWS29
female
stroke
11
6
none
VS/UWS30
male
trauma
70
6
none
VS/UWS31
male
anoxia
413
6
none
VS/UWS32
male
trauma
28
6
none
VS/UWS33
male
trauma
34
7
none
VS/UWS34
male
trauma
24
7
none
VS/UWS35
female
trauma
11
7
none
VS/UWS36
male
stroke
9
7
none
VS/UWS37
female
stroke
13
7
none
VS/UWS38
male
trauma
10
7
none
VS/UWS39
female
anoxic
16
4
bell
VS/UWS40
male
anoxic
557
8
bell
VS/UWS41
male
stroke
39
5
own name
VS/UWS42
female
trauma
18
6
own name
VS/UWS43
male
anoxia
23
7
own name
VS/UWS44
female
anoxia
15
7
own name
VS/UWS45
female
trauma
20
6
both
VS/UWS46
male
trauma
14
7
both
VS/UWS47 male trauma 38 8 both

*Time since injury in weeks, **Total score of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (minimum 0, maximum 23).

Discussion

Our data show that the assessment of localisation to sound depends on what stimulus is employed. MCS patients tend to best orient to their own name as compared to a meaningless loud sound (i.e., ringing bell). Indeed, one’s own name is a piece of information that we use to process in the auditory modality from infancy: 4–5 month-old infants are able to recognize the sound pattern of their own names [9]. In end-stage demented patients, it has also been shown that perception of the own name deteriorates well after perception of time, place and recognition [10]. Similarly, after general anaesthesia, the patient’s reactivity to the own name occurs first, before reactivity to pain and noise [11]. In MCS patients, clinical experience learns that behavioural responses to auto-referential stimuli such as the own face are amidst the first signs heralding further recovery of consciousness [12]. Event-related potential studies have also shown that hearing one’s own name, as compared to meaningless noise, leads to an increased mismatch negativity response in patients with disorders of consciousness [6]. In addition, functional MRI studies assessing brain activation to the own name have reported activation of “self”-related brain regions (i.e., anterior cingulate and mesiofrontal cortices) depending of the level of consciousness in patients recovering from coma [7,13].

28% of the studied MCS patients (11/39) failed to show auditory localisation. Neurological assessment showed that 2 of these 11 patients (18%) had absent auditory startle, while 9 (82%) showed auditory-independent signs of consciousness. In line with previous studies, auditory impairment probably explains this finding [3].

Auditory localisation seems to be related to the patient’s overall behavioural responsivity: the more the patients are conscious, the more they tend to respond to both auditory stimuli. Moreover, our results showed that most of the patients who responded to the bell also responded to their own name (condition “both” in Table 1). Three patients however showed localisation to the bell but not to their own name. Even if they retained basic auditory processing, these three patients might not have been able to process language, and hence recognize their own name. Another explanation could be the presentation of the patient's own name as last stimulus, and hence fatigue might explain orientation to a bell in the absence of orientation to the own name.

One should note that the duration and the degree of the movement towards auditory stimulation were not taken into account to assess auditory localisation (as described in the CRS-R). This should nevertheless be investigated in future studies to allow differentiating between a brief movement and a sustained fixation following auditory stimulation. Indeed, the latter may potentially be considered as a sign of consciousness, as it is the case for visual and tactile localisation (e.g., visual pursuit and localisation to noxious stimulation items in the CRS-R). Such responses may also be worth exploring further using neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI and EEG in order to compare the behavioral responses and the underlying cerebral networks involved when hearing the person's name being called.

Conclusions

Our findings emphasize the clinical importance of using the patient's own name when performing bedside testing of localisation to sound, adding to previous studies the importance of using auto-referential stimuli in patients with disorders of consciousness (i.e., the use of a mirror in the assessment of visual tracking [12]).

Abbreviations

VS/UWS: Vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome; MCS: Minimally conscious state; CRS-R: Coma recovery scale-revised; fMRI: Functional magnetic resonance imaging; EEG: Electroencephalography.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Pre-publication history

The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:

http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/13/27/prepub

Contributor Information

Lijuan Cheng, Email: aaaa_24241598@qq.com.

Olivia Gosseries, Email: ogosseries@ulg.ac.be.

Limei Ying, Email: 627705843@qq.com.

Xiaohua Hu, Email: hu_yi_sheng@126.com.

Dan Yu, Email: yudanwj@yahoo.com.cn.

Hongxing Gao, Email: ghxxxl@163.com.

Minhui He, Email: heminhui2003@yahoo.com.cn.

Caroline Schnakers, Email: c.schnakers@ulg.ac.be.

Steven Laureys, Email: steven.laureys@ulg.ac.be.

Haibo Di, Email: dihaibo@yahoo.com.cn.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China, the Science and Technology Department of Zhejiang Province, the Hangzhou Normal University and HNUEYT, the Belgian Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS), Fonds Léon Frédericq, and the University of Liège. The authors thank Cunlai Xu, Jian Gao, Kehong Liu, Kun Li,Yu Zhang, Hongyan Song and Yan Dong for their assistance in patients’ assessment, and Didier Ledoux for his statistical advices. OG and CS are postdoctoral researchers and SL is research director at FNRS.

References

  1. Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, Sannita WG, Sazbon L, Schmutzhard E, von Wild KR, Zeman A, Dolce G. European Task Force on Disorders of Consciousness. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010;8:68. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND. The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58(3):349–53. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S. Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res. 2005;150:397–413. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50028-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Mack A, Pappas Z, Silverman M, Gay R. What we see: inattention and the capture of attention by meaning. Conscious Cogn. 2002;11(4):488–506. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00028-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Wood N, Cowan N. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: How frequent are attention shifts to one's own name in an irrelevant auditory channel. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1995;21:255–60. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.21.1.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Qin P, Di H, Yan X, Yu S, Yu D, Laureys S, Weng X. Mismatch negativity to the patient’s own name in chronic disorders of consciousness. Neurosci Lett. 2008;448:24–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Qin P, Di H, Liu Y, Yu S, Gong Q, Duncan N, Weng X, Laureys S, Northoff G. Anterior cingulate activity and the self in disorders of consciousness. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(12):1993–2002. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85(12):2020–9. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Mandel DR, Jusczyck PW, Pisoni DB. Infants' recognition of the sound pattern of their own names. Psychol Sci. 1995;6:314–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00517.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fishback DB. Mental status questionnaire for organic brain syndrome, with a new visual counting test. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1977;25(4):167–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1977.tb00286.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kurtz D, Trapp C, Kieny MT, Wassmer JM, Mugnaioni MD, Pack A, Hoff E. Study of recovery and the post-anaesthetic period. Rev Electroencephalogr Neurophysiol Clin. 1977;7(1):62–9. doi: 10.1016/S0370-4475(77)80036-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Bredart S, Laureys S. Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79(2):223. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.121624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Di H, Yu SM, Weng XC, Laureys S, Yu D, Li JQ, Qin PM, Zhu YH, Zhang SZ, Chen YZ. Cerebral response to patient's own name in the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neurology. 2007;68(12):895–9. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000258544.79024.d0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BMC Neurology are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES