Abstract
The basolateral amygdala (BLA) has a fundamental role in affective processing. In vivo studies have revealed rhythmic population activity of a similar type to that seen in the hippocampus and cortical areas during learning tasks. The amygdala contains densely interconnected networks of inhibitory interneurons similar to those responsible for fast network activity generation in the hippocampus and other cortical structures. Here we report that neuronal networks of the BLA in isolation generate persistent, gamma frequency (30–80 Hz) oscillations upon kainate receptor activation with kainic acid. We show that, like other cortical structures, BLA oscillations are completely dependent upon γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic inhibition. GABAA receptor blockade abolished all oscillations, and the activity was also sensitive to the barbiturate, pentobarbital. Blockade of α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors or N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors had no significant effect on gamma activity. However, the GluR5-containing kainate receptor-specific antagonist (S)-1-(2-amino-2-carboxyethyl)-3-(2-carboxybenzyl) pyrimidine-2,4-dione (UBP302) abolished oscillations–evidence that glutamatergic receptor involvement is predominantly kainate receptor mediated. The mixed AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist 6-nitro-7-sulphamoylbenzo[f]quinoxalone-2,3-dione disodium (NBQX) abolished all oscillatory activity in 8/14 of slices tested. In the remaining slices, gamma frequency activity was abolished to reveal a low-amplitude, NMDA receptor-dependent, beta frequency (10–20 Hz) oscillation. Gamma oscillations are abolished by gap junction blockade. While these data show the BLA capable of generating gamma rhythms in common with other cortical areas studied to date, the network mechanisms appear to be different, suggesting a unique network structure underlies amygdala rhythmogenesis. Understanding how BLA networks produce synchronous activity is paramount to understanding how the BLA executes influence on important cognitive processes such as emotional learning.
Keywords: BLA, gamma oscillations, gap junction, glutamate
Introduction
Gamma oscillations (30–80 Hz) are observed in the brain in response to sensory stimuli, and have a role in memory formation, processing and retrieval (Buzsaki, 1989; Fell et al., 2001; Kaiser et al., 2003). In structures relevant for these observations (hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and neocortex), gamma rhythms are generated by heterogeneous networks of excitatory principal cells and local circuit interneurons (Whittington et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2003, 2004b). Local network gamma frequency is controlled by the kinetics of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)A receptor-mediated inhibition (Whittington et al., 1995), which appears to organise local principal cell firing into ensembles recurring with periodicity corresponding to the gamma rhythm (Harris et al., 2003).
The basolateral amygdala (BLA) has long been implicated functionally in emotional behaviour and formation of emotional memories (Kluver & Bucy, 1937; Weiskrantz, 1956; Cahill, 2000; McGaugh, 2000). It has reciprocal connections to areas that generate gamma rhythms during declarative memory encoding, storage and retrieval: rhinal cortices, parahippocampal cortex and hippocampus. Sensory and memory function-related inputs to the BLA are mostly glutamatergic, originating in cortical and thalamic structures (Sah et al., 2003). The BLA consists of three nuclei; lateral (LA), basolateral (BA) and basomedial (Sah et al., 2003). Oscillatory activity in the BLA is thought to have an important role in the consolidation of emotional memories (Pelletier & Pare, 2004). Anatomical studies show BLA networks are similar to those in other oscillating regions, although the amygdala lacks defined laminar structure (McDonald & Mascagni, 2004; Muller et al., 2005; Pantazopoulos et al., 2006). Cells in the rodent LA have intrinsic membrane potential oscillations at theta frequency (Pape et al., 2005). A repertoire of oscillatory frequencies has been recorded in the amygdala in vivo, for example slow-wave and delta (Pare & Gaudreau, 1996; Collins et al., 2001), theta (Pare & Gaudreau, 1996; Collins et al., 2001; Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Pape et al., 2005) and gamma (Pagano & Gault, 1964; Collins et al., 2001; Hakami et al., 2009; Levesque et al., 2009; Popescu et al., 2009). These frequencies correspond to those recorded in other oscillating areas, i.e. BLA target areas. Gamma oscillations were observed in the BLA, superimposed on theta rhythms in anaesthetised cats (Collins et al., 2001) and during learning in synchrony with rhinal cortices (Bauer et al., 2007). BLA-mediated facilitation of rhinal interactions could contribute to emotional facilitation of memory formation by the BLA (Paz et al., 2006; Bauer et al., 2007). Recent studies have reported synchronous gamma activity between the BLA and striatum during motor learning tasks (Popescu et al., 2009) and amygdalo-hippocampal formation during seizures (Levesque et al., 2009).
Despite the surge in recent literature linking BLA rhythms to function in vivo, to our knowledge only one in vitro investigation has been reported. Theta frequency oscillations in response to kainate receptor activation were shown to be gap junction dependent in the rodent LA (Sinfield & Collins, 2006). These authors also showed that gamma oscillations induced by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation were not blocked by gap junction blockade. The oscillations observed by these authors were also much smaller in amplitude than those we report here, possibly explained by the differences in slice preparation methods.
Here we carry out a pharmacological study to show that kainate receptor activation across the BLA in isolation leads to generation of persistent, fast, oscillatory activity similar, but not identical, to that in other oscillating areas.
Materials and methods
Slice preparation
All animal procedures were carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. Adult male Wistar rats (150–200 g; Charles River, UK) were anaesthetised with inhaled isofluorane, and then injected with a lethal dose of ketamine (> 100 mg/kg) and xylazine (> 10 mg/kg). Once all reflexes were absent, animals were intracardially perfused with 60 mL modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) made up of (in mm): sucrose, 252; KCl, 3; NaH2PO4, 1.25; NaHCO3, 24; MgSO4, 1.25; CaCl2, 1.6; glucose, 10. Following brain removal, 450-μm coronal brain slices containing the BLA were cut using a vibratome (Leica Microsystems), and trimmed to leave BLA and the surrounding cortex. No slices with any hippocampus were used to remove the possibility of volume conduction of hippocampal network activity. Slices were held in a holding chamber at room temperature for approximately 1 h (in aCSF used to maintain slices in the interface and holding chambers replacing the sucrose content with 126 mm NaCl, and the MgSO4 and CaCl2 content of 1.25 mm and 1.6 mm, respectively). The slices were transferred to an interface-chamber for electrophysiological recording, where conditions were maintained at approximately 34 °C at the interface between oxygenated aCSF and humidified 95% O2 and 5% CO2. No recordings were made until slices had been left to equilibrate for at least 1 h.
Drugs
All drugs were applied by bath perfusion at concentrations stated in the text. The drugs used were kainic acid (KA; Sigma, Gillingham, UK), (RS)-2-amino-3-(3-hydroxy-5-tert-butylisoxazol-4-yl) (ATPA; Tocris, Bristol, UK), D-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate (D-APV; Tocris, Bristol, UK); (±)-4-(4-aminophenyl)-1,2-dihydro-1-methyl-2-propylcarbamoyl-6,7-methylenedioxyphthalazine (SYM 2206; Tocris, Bristol, UK), 6-nitro-7-sulphamoylbenzo[f]quinoxalone-2,3-dione disodium (NBQX; Tocris, Bristol, UK), gabazine (Tocris, Bristol, UK); (S)-1-(2-amino-2-carboxyethyl)-3-(2-carboxybenzyl) pyrimidine-2,4-dione (UBP302; Tocris, Bristol, UK); L-glutamic acid (Sigma, Gillingham, UK), carbenoxolone (Sigma, Gillingham, UK), octanol (Fluka, Gillingham, UK), pentobarbital (Sigma, Gillingham, UK) and quinine (Sigma, Gillingham, UK).
Recording, data acquisition and analysis
Field electrodes were positioned in the BLA about 0.5 mm from the external capsule and 0.5 mm from the cortex, in the region of the BA for experiments other than those with a roaming electrode (see Supporting Information Fig. S1). KA (200–400 nm) was bath-applied in the circulating aCSF. Extracellular field recordings were made using glass microelectrodes pulled to a resistance of 2–5 MΩ filled with aCSF. Intracellular recordings were made using glass microelectrodes pulled to a resistance of 80–100 MΩ and filled with 1 m potassium acetate. For reconstructions of recorded neuron morphology, 2% biocytin was included in the recording microelectrode. After the completion of recording, the slices were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer. Standard avidin-biotin-horseradish peroxidase reaction with diaminobenzidine was used to visualise biocytin-filled neurons.
Data were recorded using an Axoprobe-1A amplifier (Axon Instruments, USA), and recorded on computer via an ITC-16 interface (Instrutech, USA). Data were collected and analysed using Axograph software (Axon Instruments, USA). Fourier transform analysis was used to detect the peak frequencies of the oscillations for 60-s epochs of field data. The peak of this was used as a measure of the peak amplitude of the oscillation, and the area underneath the curve (between 20–80 Hz) as a measure of the area power. For analysis of pharmacological effects on the oscillations, pooled data were used. The peak amplitude and area power of the oscillation for 60-s epochs of recorded oscillations were collected. Statistical analysis of filtered data was performed using Sigmastat 2.03 software (Systat Software, USA). This software determined whether data were parametric or non-parametric before statistical tests were applied. Parametric data from one slice or cell were analysed using a standard paired Student’s t-test to measure differences before and after pharmacological manipulation, and results are significantly different if P < 0.05. Parametric data were presented as mean ± SEM. For non-parametric data sets, the Mann–Whitney U-test was employed. This test compares the medians of two groups of data, and results are statistically significant if P < 0.05.
For BLA oscillations the peak amplitude and frequency were used, as the lacking laminar structure gave rise to variable levels of coherence (depicted in area power) that were likely due to cellular composition at an exact electrode position within the BLA network. For inhibitory postsynaptic potential (IPSP) decay time analysis during gamma oscillations, 50 IPSPs were measured during kainate oscillations. IPSP amplitude was determined from the onset to peak of the IPSP, and decay time was measured from the peak to the point of IPSP decay slope at 30% of peak amplitude. To measure IPSP changes in the presence of pentobarbital, 40 IPSPs in control and pentobarbital conditions were measured from three separate principal cells and data pooled for statistical analysis with a standard paired t-test.
Results
BLA networks can generate gamma oscillations in vitro
Slices containing BLA did not generate spontaneous rhythmic field potential activity, but bath-application of KA (200–400 nm) led to generation of gamma frequency oscillations. Recording position was determined in slices using the rat brain atlas (Paxinos & Watson, 1998), and the spatial origin of the rhythm in the BLA confirmed by comparing data from multiple recording sites (Supporting Information Fig. S1). BLA gamma frequency oscillations had mean power of 166 ± 29 μV2/Hz and mean frequency of 35 ± 1 Hz (n = 59; Fig. 1). In all experiments the gamma oscillations were highly stable over time (Fig. 1B), and showed a clear, narrow spectral peak indicative of a high degree of local coherence (Fig. 1C).
BLA rhythms are GABAA receptor dependent
In common with population gamma rhythms observed in other cortical structures (Whittington et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2003, 2004b), oscillatory activity in the BLA was found to be critically dependent on fast inhibitory synaptic transmission mediated by GABAA receptors. Application of GABAA receptor antagonist gabazine (1–2 μm) abolished all oscillatory activity (Fig. 2A; control vs. gabazine; peak amplitude, 26 ± 6 vs. 1 ± 0.8 μV2/Hz, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 5). On continued gabazine application, epileptiform activity was also seen (not shown). Intracellular recordings from principal cells (Fig. 2B; n = 5) in the BLA revealed trains of fast IPSPs at the same frequency as the concurrently recorded local field potential and tightly phase-locked to the local population rhythm (Fig. 2C). IPSPs had a mean amplitude of 2.00 ± 0.04 mV and monoexponential decay constant of 8.0 ± 0.2 ms. These IPSPs were also abolished by application of gabazine alongside the correlation between the field and individual cell activity (Fig. 2C).
Further demonstration of the importance of phasic GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition was obtained by modulating GABA decay constant with pentobarbital (30 μm; e.g. Whittington et al., 1995). A reduction in both the frequency and power of field gamma oscillations in the BLA was seen (Fig. 3A; control vs. pentobarbital; frequency, 34 ± 1 vs. 30 ± 1 Hz, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 13; peak amplitude, 33 ± 11 vs. 8 ± 3, Mann–Whitney U-test, P < 0.05, n = 13). Moreover, in intracellular principal cell recordings IPSP amplitudes were also reduced by pentobarbital and longer IPSP decay times (control vs. pentobarbital; amplitude, 7 ± 0.5 vs. 4 ± 0.2 mV, Student’s t-test, P ≤ 0.05, n = 4; decay time, 14 ± 0.4 vs. 17 ± 0.5 ms, Student’s t-test, P ≤ 0.05, n = 3), along with rhythmicity, evident from side peaks in the cross-correlation between field and individual cell IPSPs (Fig. 3B).
The role of glutamatergic transmission for BLA gamma oscillations
Phasic AMPA receptor-mediated excitation of interneurons is a fundamental feature of persistent gamma rhythms in the hippocampus (Whittington et al., 2000), entorhinal cortex (Cunningham et al., 2003) and neocortex (Cunningham et al., 2004b). However, the AMPA receptor-specific antagonist SYM 2206 (50 μm) had no significant effect on oscillation frequency or power in the BLA (Fig. 4A and B; control vs. SYM 2206, frequency, 36 ± 1 vs. 37 ± 1 Hz, peak amplitude, 285 ± 54 vs. 248 ± 63 μV2/Hz, Student’s t-test, P > 0.05, n = 8).
In the majority of BLA slices tested (8/14), the broad spectrum AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist NBQX abolished both field gamma rhythm generation and gamma frequency trains of IPSPs (Fig. 5A and C). GluR5-containing kainate receptor antagonist UBP302 also abolished BLA oscillations (Fig. 5B; control vs. UBP302; 24 ± 10 vs. 3 ± 1 μV2/Hz, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 4). However, in 6/14 slices exposed to NBQX, a small-amplitude, beta frequency oscillation remained in the BLA. Modal peak frequency in the power spectrum changed from a control value of 35 ± 2 Hz, down to 17 ± 2 Hz (Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 6). When manifest, the beta oscillation was abolished by further application of the N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist D-APV (50 μm;Fig. 6A; control vs. NBQX vs. NBQX/D-APV; frequency, 35 ± 2 vs. 17 ± 2 vs. 24 ± 5 Hz, Mann–Whitney U-test, P < 0.05 for control vs. NBQX, Mann–Whitney U-test, P > 0.05 for NBQX vs. NBQX/D-APV, peak amplitude, 129 ± 27 vs. 68 ± 21 vs. 6 ± 3 μV2/Hz, Mann–Whitney U-test, P < 0.05 n = 6/14 of total slices treated with NBQX). Figure 6C shows that IPSP frequency slows correspondingly with emergence of the beta rhythm, and the IPSPs seen were both smaller (control vs. NBQX; 7.5 ± 0.4 vs. 4.5 ± 0.2 mV, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05) and had slower decay constants (control vs. NBQX; 7.1 ± 0.3 vs. 12.8 ± 0.5 ms, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05). These IPSPs were abolished alongside the residual beta rhythm on application of D-APV. Correlation between field and IPSPs alters as the oscillation frequency changes and disappears when the oscillation is abolished (Fig. 6C).
NMDA receptor activity is not required for BLA gamma oscillations
In the hippocampus, NMDA receptor blockade has no significant effect on persistent gamma rhythms (Fisahn et al., 1998). However, it decimates gamma power in the entorhinal cortex (Cunningham et al., 2003). The effects of NMDA receptor blockade in the BLA were, again, different to that seen in the entorhinal cortex but the same as that seen in the hippocampus. D-APV (50 μm) caused no significant decrease in the frequency or peak amplitude of the local rhythm, but following NMDA receptor blockade, NBQX (20 μm) application abolished all oscillatory activity, as shown in Fig. 7A (control vs. D-APV vs. D-APV/NBQX; frequency, 35 ± 2 vs. 34 ± 2 vs. 27 ± 6 Hz, peak amplitude, 145 ± 60 vs. 195 ± 73 vs. 7 ± 2 μV2/Hz, P > 0.05 for control vs. NBQX, P < 0.05 for NBQX vs. NBQX/D-APV, in both cases Mann–Whitney U-test, P > 0.05, n = 5).
BLA oscillations require active gap junctions
In common with previous studies in the entorhinal cortex (Cunningham et al., 2004a) and the hippocampus (LeBeau et al., 2003), electrical communication via gap junctions appeared vital for persistent kainate-driven oscillations in the BLA. Because of problems associated with gap junction blocker specificity, a range of gap junction blockers were applied to BLA oscillations. Bath-application of 1 mm octanol almost completely blocked the KA-induced field potential rhythm (Fig. 8A; control vs. octanol peak amplitude; 148 ± 47 vs. 9 ± 7 μV2/Hz, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 6). Similarly, the gamma frequency trains of IPSPs in principal cells, and corresponding IPSP–field potential correlates were also abolished with octanol (Fig. 8B). Furthermore, no detrimental effects of octanol were apparent in intracellular recordings (Fig. 8A), and no significant changes to cell membrane properties were observed. The resting membrane potential, input resistance and action potential amplitude of BLA principal neurons were not significantly altered by the application of octanol (control vs. octanol; resting membrane potential: 58 ± 3 vs. 58 ± 2 mV, input resistance: 27 ± 6 vs. 28 ± 6 MΩ, action potential amplitude: 49 ± 1 vs. 50 ± 1, Student’s t-test, P ≥ 0.05, n = 4). Current injection (0.1 nA) in both the control and octanol condition was consistently able to produce similar patterns of firing in BLA principal neurons. Bath-application of other gap junction blockers, quinine (500 μm) and carbenoxolone (200 μm), also abolished gamma oscillations in the BLA (control vs. quinine; 64 ± 7 vs. 14 ± 6 μV2/Hz, Mann–Whitney U-test, P < 0.05, n = 5; control vs. carbenoxolone; 91 ± 30 vs. 18 ± 8 μV2/Hz, Student’s t-test, P < 0.05, n = 8).
Discussion
The present data demonstrate that the BLA generates gamma frequency population rhythms when activated by KA. Whilst previous reports have observed spontaneous gamma frequency oscillations in the hippocampus (Pietersen et al., 2009), such activity was not observed in the BLA in this current study. The gamma rhythm generated was near-identical to that seen for gamma rhythms in area CA3 (30–40 Hz), but slower than that reported for area CA1 in the hippocampus (40–100 Hz; Middleton et al., 2008a, b). BLA gamma rhythms were absolutely dependent on GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition, as seen for all other persistent gamma rhythm models in vitro. Blockade of GABAA receptors abolished the rhythms, and slowing of GABAA time constants with barbiturates also slowed the population rhythm. However, in contrast to these rhythms in the hippocampus, entorhinal and neocortical structures, BLA gamma rhythms did not require phasic AMPA receptor-mediated excitation at all, but did depend on GluR5-containing kainate receptors. In addition, a low-amplitude NMDA receptor-dependent beta frequency oscillation was also seen when gamma rhythms were blocked.
The precise nature of the frequency of the BLA rhythm may be of critical importance in shaping its involvement in the amygdala–hippocampal–entorhinal axis, as has been shown to occur in learning paradigms (Bauer et al., 2007). Coupling and decoupling along this axis has been implicated in specific types of memory (Kensinger & Schacter, 2006). It is interesting to note that in vitro studies have shown a large discrepancy in gamma frequencies generated internally by hippocampal area CA3 (about 35 Hz) and area CA1 and entorhinal cortex (about 45–55 Hz). The peak, persistent gamma frequency seen in the BLA appeared to match CA3 hippocampal gamma frequencies, but not those generated in area CA1 or entorhinal cortex. The residual beta rhythm seen in the BLA may also be critical for controlling dynamic interactions between amygdala and hippocampus. Beta rhythms are seen during serotoninergic neuromodulation in the hippocampus (Bibbig et al., 2007) and, in general, are thought to provide a temporal framework more suitable for generating synchrony over larger spatial scales than gamma rhythms alone (Kopell et al., 2000).
The profile of AMPA/KA receptor involvement in the BLA rhythms may, in part, be due to the differences in receptor subunit composition in the BLA compared with the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, but this was not explored here. AMPA receptor blockade with SYM 2206 had no significant effect on KA-driven BLA oscillations, whereas AMPA receptors played a large role in hippocampal (Whittington et al., 2000) and entorhinal oscillations (Cunningham et al., 2004a). Moreover, the specific KA receptor antagonist UBP302 abolished oscillations, suggesting GluR5-containing KA receptors provided the predominant fast, excitatory drive to BLA gamma oscillations. Evidence in the literature supports a mechanistic role for GluR5-containing kainate receptors in BLA functions, and potentially including oscillatory activity generation. The BLA contains a high density of GluR5-containing KA receptors (Wisden & Seeburg, 1993; Li et al., 2001), shown to be important for some forms of amygdala-dependent learning (Ko et al., 2005). Their activation was shown to decrease GABA release, reducing overall IPSP amplitude and generating an increased excitability in the local field potential (Braga et al., 2003). It has previously been shown that GluR5-containing KA receptors modulate GABAergic inhibition in the BLA (Wu et al., 2007). Removal of GluR5-containing KA receptor activity in GluR5 knockout mice or with infusion of a GluR5 antagonist increases anxiety-like behaviour, and this occurs as GABA release is reduced when inhibitory circuits have no GluR5 activation to stimulate GABA release. Overall this results in amygdala activation via loss of inhibition (Wu et al., 2007), potentially a loss of inhibitory tone in the form of oscillations. These receptors also have novel roles in synaptic potentiation in the BLA, not reported in other brain regions, which involves numerous synapses and occurs after low-frequency stimulation (Li et al., 2001). It is also NMDA receptor independent, as opposed to Hebbian synaptic mechanisms that are input and NMDA receptor dependent (Li et al., 2001). It is possible that GluR5-containing kainate receptors influence behaviours through their effects on BLA oscillations. However, it must be noted that although AMPA receptor blockade does not affect the oscillations, application of kainate receptor-specific agonist ATPA only generated small levels of network activity. This implies that AMPA-mediated drive may contribute to rhythm generation rather than maintenance.
While the present data show a specific role for NMDA receptors in the BLA beta – but not gamma – rhythm, there is also evidence to suggest that different interneuron subpopulations may be involved in this type of activity. Although not directly related to this study, and in mice, Marowsky et al. (2005) identified the subpopulations of interneurons and showed that the firing properties of local interneurons, which are scattered throughout the BLA, had a peak firing frequency within the gamma range. In contrast, lateral and medial paracapsular cells, which are located between the lateral BLA border and external capsule and between the medial BLA border and central nucleus, respectively, had peak firing frequencies in the beta frequency range. Different BLA rhythms could be features of independently acting networks within the BLA. In accordance with this theory, the difference in amplitude and decay kinetics for IPSPs accompanying gamma and beta rhythms seen in the present data strongly suggest different origins for the phasic inhibition underlying each rhythm; however, no interneuron recordings were made in this study and this is an aim for the continuation of this work. Evidence of a differential topography of GABAergic inhibition onto principal neurons is known from work in the neocortex (Tamás et al., 2004), and a similar divergence of inhibition may underlie the generation of gamma and beta rhythms in the BLA.
Gamma rhythms in the BLA shared the sensitivity to gap junction blockers seen in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex (Cunningham et al., 2003; LeBeau et al., 2003). Neurons in the BLA are highly interconnected via gap junctions, and anatomical studies have shown that interneurons, in particular, are interconnected by both dendritic and axo-axonic gap junctions at terminals (Muller et al., 2005). Dendritic gap junctions are present in hippocampal interneuron networks, and their removal disrupts gamma oscillations (Hormuzdi et al., 2001), and this could also explain the gap junction requirement for BLA oscillations. So, although the BLA lacks a laminar composition that is thought to contribute to the axonal plexus-mediating drive to oscillations (Traub et al., 2003), there is a structural organisation that acts like the axonal plexus in enhancing electrical communication within networks (Traub et al., 2008). Oscillatory activity in the LA was previously shown to be gap junction dependent (Sinfield & Collins, 2006).
The present data showed that BLA networks in isolation can generate gamma frequency oscillations in vitro. BLA network oscillations in vivo are synchronised with other regions during common tasks (e.g. Popescu et al., 2009). The similarity in frequency bands generated in BLA (gamma and beta seen here, and theta and gamma reported in different conditions by Sinfield & Collins, 2006) strongly suggests that this synchrony may be afforded by dynamic interactions between locally generated rhythms in BLA and these coupled regions. The network mechanisms responsible for these rhythms in BLA were similar, but not identical, to those in other cortical areas studied. The complex, non-laminar structure of the amygdala makes it less amenable for in vitro investigation than laminar cortical areas. However, this in vitro model of amygdala oscillations may provide a platform for further investigation into the mechanisms of oscillation generation in the BLA, and thus its role in modulating and coordinating higher brain function.
Acknowledgments
F.E.R. was supported by a Newcastle University studentship. The authors thank Fiona LeBeau and Claudia Racca.
Glossary
- aCSF
artificial cerebrospinal fluid
- AMPA
α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid
- ATPA
(RS)-2-amino-3-(3-hydroxy-5-tert-butylisoxazol-4-yl)
- BA
basolateral nuclei
- BLA
basolateral amygdala
- D-APV
D-(−)-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate
- GABA
γ-aminobutyric acid
- IPSP
inhibitory postsynaptic potential
- KA
kainic acid
- LA
lateral nuclei
- NBQX
6-nitro-7-sulphamoylbenzo[f]quinoxalone-2,3-dione disodium
- NMDA
N-methyl-d-aspartate
- SYM 2206
(±)-4-(4-aminophenyl)-1,2-dihydro-1-methyl-2-propylcarbamoyl-6,7-methylenedioxyphthalazine
- UBP302
(S)-1-(2-amino-2-carboxyethyl)-3-(2-carboxybenzyl) pyrimidine-2,4-dione
Supporting Information
Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article:
Fig. S1. Profile of gamma frequency oscillationin BLA in vitro. (A) Slice diagram shows the slicecomposition and the numbered recording position. As alreadydiscussed, lack of laminar structure in BLA prevents identificationof the exact recording site, and so this method was used to showthat oscillatory activity within the slice was localised to the BLAregion. (B) Corresponding power spectra for each position denotedin (A).
Please note: As a service to our authors and readers, this journal provides supporting information supplied by the authors. Such materials are peer-reviewed and may be re-organized for online delivery, but are not copy-edited or typeset by Wiley-Blackwell. Technical support issues arising from supporting information (other than missing files) should be addressed to the authors.
References
- Bauer EP, Paz R, Pare D. Gamma oscillations coordinate amygdalo-rhinal interactions during learning. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:9369–9379. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2153-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bibbig A, Middleton S, Racca C, Gillies MJ, Garner H, Lebeau FE, Davies CH, Whittington MA. Beta rhythms (15-20 Hz) generated by nonreciprocal communication in hippocampus. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:2812–2823. doi: 10.1152/jn.01105.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braga MF, Aroniadou-Anderjaska V, Xie J, Li H. Bidirectional modulation of GABA release by presynaptic glutamate receptor 5 kainate receptors in the basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:442–452. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-02-00442.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience. 1989;31:551–570. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cahill L. Neurobiological mechanisms of emotionally influenced, long-term memory. Prog. Brain Res. 2000;126:29–37. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(00)26004-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins DR, Pelletier JG, Pare D. Slow and fast (gamma) neuronal oscillations in the perirhinal cortex and lateral amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 2001;85:1661–1672. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.4.1661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MO, Davies CH, Buhl EH, Kopell N, Whittington MA. Gamma oscillations induced by kainate receptor activation in the entorhinal cortex in vitro. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:9761–9769. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-30-09761.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MO, Halliday DM, Davies CH, Traub RD, Buhl EH, Whittington MA. Coexistence of gamma and high-frequency oscillations in rat medial entorhinal cortex in vitro. J Physiol (London) 2004a;559:347–353. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.068973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MO, Whittington MA, Bibbig A, Roopun A, LeBeau FE, Vogt A, Monyer H, Buhl EH, Traub RD. A role for fast rhythmic bursting neurons in cortical gamma oscillations in vitro. Proc Natl Acad Sci (USA) 2004b;101:7152–7157. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402060101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fell J, Klaver P, Lehnertz K, Grunwald T, Schaller C, Elger CE, Fernandez G. Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:1259–1264. doi: 10.1038/nn759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisahn A, Pike FG, Buhl EH, Paulsen O. Cholinergic induction of network oscillations at 40 Hz in the hippocampus in vitro. Nature. 1998;394:186–189. doi: 10.1038/28179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hakami T, Jones NC, Tolmacheva EA, Gaudias J, Chaumont J, Salzberg M, O’Brien TJ, Pinault D. NMDA receptor hypofunction leads to generalized and persistent aberrant gamma oscillations independent of hyperlocomotion and the state of consciousness. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e6755. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Dragoi G, Buzsaki G. Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature. 2003;424:552–556. doi: 10.1038/nature01834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hormuzdi SG, Pais I, LeBeau FE, Towers SK, Rozov A, Buhl EH, Whittington MA, Monyer H. Impaired electrical signaling disrupts gamma frequency oscillations in connexin 36-deficient mice. Neuron. 2001;31:487–495. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00387-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser J, Ripper B, Birbaumer N, Lutzenberger W. Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory. NeuroImage. 2003;20:816–827. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00350-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:2564–2570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kluver H, Bucy P. “Psychic Blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. Am. J. Physiol. 1937;119:352–353. [Google Scholar]
- Ko S, Zhao MG, Toyoda H, Qiu CS, Zhuo M. Altered behavioral responses to noxious stimuli and fear in glutamate receptor 5 (GluR5)- or GluR6-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:977–984. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4059-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci (USA) 2000;97:1867–1872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeBeau FE, Traub RD, Monyer H, Whittington MA, Buhl EH. The role of electrical signaling via gap junctions in the generation of fast network oscillations. Brain Res. Bull. 2003;62:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levesque M, Langlois JM, Lema P, Courtemanche R, Bilodeau GA, Carmant L. Synchronized gamma oscillations (30-50 Hz) in the amygdalo-hippocampal network in relation with seizure propagation and severity. Neurobiol. Dis. 2009;35:209–218. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li H, Chen A, Xing G, Wei ML, Rogawski MA. Kainate receptor-mediated heterosynaptic facilitation in the amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:612–620. doi: 10.1038/88432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marowsky A, Yanagawa Y, Obata K, Vogt KE. A specialized subclass of interneurons mediates dopaminergic facilitation of amygdala function. Neuron. 2005;48:1025–1037. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald AJ, Mascagni F. Parvalbumin-containing interneurons in the basolateral amygdala express high levels of the alpha1 subunit of the GABAA receptor. J Comp Neurol. 2004;473:137–146. doi: 10.1002/cne.20101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL. Memory - a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287:248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton S, Jalics J, Kispersky T, LeBeau FE, Roopun AK, Kopell NJ, Whittington MA, Cunningham MO. NMDA receptor-dependent switching between different gamma-rhythm generating microcircuits in entorhinal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci (USA) 2008a;105:18572–18577. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809302105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton SJ, Racca C, Cunningham MO, Traub RD, Monyer H, Knopfel T, Schofield IS, Jenkins A, Whittington MA. High-frequency network oscillations in cerebellar cortex. Neuron. 2008b;58:763–774. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller JF, Mascagni F, McDonald AJ. Coupled networks of parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneurons in the rat basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:7366–7376. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0899-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagano RR, Gault FP. Amygdala Activity: a Central Measure of Arousal. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1964;17:255–260. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(64)90126-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantazopoulos H, Lange N, Hassinger L, Berretta S. Subpopulations of neurons expressing parvalbumin in the human amygdala. J Comp Neurol. 2006;496:706–722. doi: 10.1002/cne.20961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pape HC, Narayanan RT, Smid J, Stork O, Seidenbecher T. Theta activity in neurons and networks of the amygdala related to long-term fear memory. Hippocampus. 2005;15:874–880. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pare D, Gaudreau H. Projection cells and interneurons of the lateral and basolateral amygdala: distinct firing patterns and differential relation to theta and delta rhythms in conscious cats. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:3334–3350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03334.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Academic Press Inc; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Paz R, Pelletier JG, Bauer EP, Pare D. Emotional enhancement of memory via amygdala-driven facilitation of rhinal interactions. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:1321–1329. doi: 10.1038/nn1771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelletier JG, Pare D. Role of amygdala oscillations in the consolidation of emotional memories. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;55:559–562. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pietersen AN, Patel N, Jefferys JG, Vreugdenhil M. Comparison between spontaneous and kainate-induced gamma oscillations in the mouse hippocampus in vitro. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:2145–2156. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06771.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popescu AT, Popa D, Pare D. Coherent gamma oscillations couple the amygdala and striatum during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:801–807. doi: 10.1038/nn.2305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sah P, Faber ES, Lopez De Armentia M, Power J. The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiological Rev. 2003;83:803–834. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00002.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidenbecher T, Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape HC. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science. 2003;301:846–850. doi: 10.1126/science.1085818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinfield JL, Collins DR. Induction of synchronous oscillatory activity in the rat lateral amygdala in vitro is dependent on gap junction activity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;24:3091–3095. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamás G, Szabadics J, Lörincz A, Somogyi P. Input and frequency-specific entrainment of postsynaptic firing by IPSPs of perisomatic or dendritic origin. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;20:2681–2690. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03719.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub RD, Cunningham MO, Gloveli T, LeBeau FE, Bibbig A, Buhl EH, Whittington MA. GABA-enhanced collective behavior in neuronal axons underlies persistent gamma-frequency oscillations. Proc Natl Acad Sci (USA) 2003;100:11047–11052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934854100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub RD, Middleton SJ, Knöpfel T, Whittington MA. Model of very fast (>75 Hz) network oscillations generated by electrical coupling between the proximal axons of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:1603–1616. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06477.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiskrantz L. Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1956;49:381–391. doi: 10.1037/h0088009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittington MA, Traub RD, Jefferys JG. Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation. Nature. 1995;373:612–615. doi: 10.1038/373612a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittington MA, Traub RD, Kopell N, Ermentrout B, Buhl EH. Inhibition-based rhythms: experimental and mathematical observations on network dynamics. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2000;38:315–336. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00173-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisden W, Seeburg PH. A complex mosaic of high-affinity kainate receptors in rat brain. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:3582–3598. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03582.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu LJ, Ko SW, Toyoda H, Zhao MG, Xu H, Vadakkan KI, Ren M, Knifed E, Shum F, Quan J, Zhang XH, Zhuo M. Increased anxiety-like behavior and enhanced synaptic efficacy in the amygdala of GluR5 knockout mice. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.