Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
editorial
. 2013 Jun 1;36(6):815–817. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2696

Two Independent Sources of Short Term Memory Problems during Sleep Deprivation

Adrienne M Tucker 1,
PMCID: PMC3649821  PMID: 23729921

Accidents related to sleep loss are estimated to cost billions of dollars annually,1 yet the effects of sleep deprivation on aspects of cognition have yet to be unraveled. For example, short term memory (STM) plays a vital role in the performance of a wide range of tasks such as decision making and problem solving.2 During sleep loss, STM performance is slower and less accurate with large individual differences in the magnitude of impairment,38 yet the underlying mechanism(s) for these performance changes are not clear.

In this issue of SLEEP, Wee and colleagues9 take a model-based approach to understanding cognitive performance by fitting quantitative mathematical parameters which are theorized to reflect latent cognitive processes. Theoretically, this approach allows one to disentangle the multitude cognitive processes contributing to a single behavioral output variable such as reaction time (RT) or accuracy.10 Another way to isolate cognitive processes is via parametric manipulations, which place increasingly greater difficulty on a given process of interest (such as a requirement to remember 1, 3, or 6 letters).11 These approaches allow more specific conclusions to be drawn and can help pinpoint which of the following processes underlie the STM deficits seen during sleep deprivation: memory encoding, memory maintenance, memory retrieval and/or a more general deficit in basic perceptual and attentional processing. Below I summarize what is known about the effects of sleep deprivation on these specific processes from the study by Wee and colleagues,9 as well as previous approaches involving cognitive modeling, parametric task manipulation, and neuroimaging.

Memory Maintenance

Wee et al. tested 19 healthy young participants twice in a counterbalanced order, once after sleep and once after a night of sleep deprivation. Three colored squares were presented and at a delay of either 1 or 10 s subjects were cued to retrieve the square presented at the identified location by selecting the matching color from a wheel including 180 choices. Bayes' probabilistic model was used to separate the response error distribution into three parts: one representing precision of target items, one representing distractor items, and one representing random guesses.

The model parameter reflecting random guesses increased as a function of sleep deprivation and delay, which suggests that participants were more likely to drop items from STM during sleep loss (i.e., that participants had a harder time maintaining items in STM). Intriguingly, two studies using a parametric approach have also reported selective deficits in working memory maintenance in sleep deprivation, as well as one other model-based analysis described further below.1214 In this issue of SLEEP, Wee et al. also observed that sleep deprived subjects were more likely to erroneously report a non-target item.9 Sleep deprivation did not affect the model parameter reflecting the precision of remembered items, however, which suggests that encoding quality is not impacted for those items that are successfully maintained.

Memory Encoding

Rakitin et al. investigated the effects of 48-h sleep deprivation on a letter recognition task where the difficulty of encoding the probe was manipulated by randomly scrambling 0%, 25%, or 50% of the image's pixels.15 Contrary to prediction, there was no interaction between sleep deprivation and degradation. In line with the result reported by Wee et al.,9 this suggests that stimulus encoding is not primarily responsible for the working memory deficits seen.

Memory Retrieval

In other studies, the difficulty of memory retrieval was manipulated by using memory set sizes of one, three, or six letters. After two nights of sleep loss there was no interaction between sleep deprivation and memory set size on either RT or accuracy, suggesting that working memory retrieval was not impaired.16 Another study used a similar task, also after two nights of sleep loss. In this task the memory probes were manipulated such that 50% that were not in the current set were in the set previous; this recency places an additional proactive interference demand on retrieval. Again, there were no interactions between sleep deprivation and set size nor between sleep deprivation and recency, providing even stronger evidence that memory retrieval is not greatly impacted by sleep loss.17

Basic Perceptual and Attention Processing

Converging evidence suggests that decrements in basic perceptual and attention processing contribute to STM impairment during sleep deprivation. First, sleep-deprived subjects show failures to respond on STM tasks similar to the attentional lapses seen in the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT).1823 Second, more complex STM tasks which are more attentionally engaging—and thus put lesser demands on endogenous attentional control—are better preserved during sleep loss.24,25 Third, sleep deprived participants have a harder time maintaining attention in the face of distracting items presented during working memory tasks.26,27 Finally, some brain regions (e.g., occipital and parietal cortex) identified in neuroimaging studies implicate deficits in basic perceptual and attentional processing.7,2832 Notably, after 53-57 h of sleep deprivation, rTMS to facilitate left upper middle occipital cortex in BA19 (a region involved in perceptual processing) was shown to significantly decrease RT on a delayed letter recognition task.33 That is, facilitating a brain region involved in perceptual processing was able to at least partially restore performance, suggesting that perceptual processing deficits underlie some of the STM impairment seen during sleep deprivation. The finding is replicated and shown to persist for up to 18 hours in an article by Luber et al. in this issue of SLEEP.34

Two Sources of STM Impairment

Intriguingly, two independent sets of neural correlates of working memory impairment during sleep deprivation have been dissociated.35 Further, basic attentional versus working memory maintenance deficits have been dissociated using computational modeling.14 The effects of sleep deprivation on STM were investigated using a letter string recognition task across lags of 0-4 trials. A computational model based on Atkinson was used to estimate three parameters representing attention, working memory span, and encoding. In line with the report by Wee et al.,9 sleep deprivation significantly reduced attention and working memory span parameters, but it did not reduce the encoding parameter. Specifically, sleep deprivation reduced working memory span by 38%—a large effect (η2 = 0.21). This was reflected in a significantly greater drop in accuracy with lag (or delay), reflecting trouble maintaining items in working memory span. The effect on attention was slightly more than half that of working memory span (η2 = 0.13) and the change in these two parameters was not correlated with each other. Indeed, individual differences in the profile of cognitive impairment were of such a degree that some individuals were selectively vulnerable to attention decrements, while others were selectively vulnerable to working memory span decrements.

Summary

Behavioral, cognitive modeling, and neuroimaging results converge to suggest that deficits in short term memory during sleep deprivation arise from decrements in basic attentional and perceptual processing as well as decrements in memory maintenance, while memory encoding and retrieval processes seem to be relatively spared. Thus, the study by Wee et al.9 adds further evidence to the conclusion that there are at least two independent ways that short-term memory deficits can occur during sleep deprivation: one related to basic attention and perceptual processing; and the other related to memory maintenance. Individuals can experience deficits in one or both processes when sleep deprived. More studies that use quantitative modeling as reported by Wee et al. can help provide a greater understanding of the underlying mechanisms responsible for the effects of sleep loss on STM.

CITATION

Tucker AM. Two independent sources of short term memory problems during sleep deprivation. SLEEP 2013;36(6):815-817.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Dinges DF. An overview of sleepiness and accidents. J Sleep Res. 1995;4:4–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working memory. Psychol Learn Motiv. 1974;8:47–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Frey DJ, Badia P, Wright KP. Inter-and intra-individual variability in performance near the circadian nadir during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2004;13:305–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00429.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Van Dongen HPA, Baynard MD, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27:423–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Caldwell JA, Mu Q, Smith JK, et al. Are individual differences in fatigue vulnerability related to baseline differences in cortical activation? Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:694–707. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mu Q, Mishory A, Johnson KA, et al. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2005;28:433–46. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.4.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lim J, Choo WC, Chee MW. Reproducibility of changes in behaviour and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep. 2007;30:61–70. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.1.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Balkin TJ. Trait-like vulnerability to total and partial sleep loss. Sleep. 2012;35:1163–72. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wee N, Asplund CL, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation accelerates delay-related loss of visual short-term memories without affecting precision. Sleep. 2013;36:849–56. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ, Eichele T, Brown S, Serences JT. Reciprocal relations between cognitive neuroscience and formal cognitive models: opposites attract? Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:272–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sternberg S. Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. Am Sci. 1969;57:421–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Elkin AJ, Murray DJ. The effects of sleep loss on short-term recognition memory. Can J Psychol. 1974;28:192–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Raidy DJ, Scharff LFV. Effects of sleep deprivation on auditory and visual memory tasks. Percept Motor Skill. 2005;101:451–67. doi: 10.2466/pms.101.2.451-467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Turner TH, Drummond S, Salamat JS, Brown GG. Effects of 42 hr of total sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:787–95. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rakitin BC, Tucker AM, Basner RC, Stern Y. The Effects of stimulus degradation after 48 hours of total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2012;35:113–21. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tucker AM, Stern Y, Basner RC, Rakitin BC. The prefrontal model revisited: double dissociations between young sleep deprived and elderly subjects on cognitive components of performance. Sleep. 2011;34:1039–50. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G, Hinson JM, Van Dongen HP. Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 2010;33:47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34:581–91. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dorrian J, Rogers N, Dinges DF. Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In: Kushida CA, editor. Sleep deprivation: clinical issues, pharmacology and sleep loss effects. New York: Marcel Dekker; 2005. pp. 39–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chee MW, Choo WC. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4560–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0007-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Chuah LY, Chee MW. Cholinergic augmentation modulates visual task performance in sleep-deprived young adults. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11369–77. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4045-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Chuah LY, Chong DL, Chen AK, et al. Donepezil improves episodic memory in young individuals vulnerable to the effects of sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2009;32:999–1010. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.8.999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tucker AM, Rakitin BC, Basner RC, Gazes Y, Steffener J, Stern Y. fMRI activation during failures to respond key to understanding performance changes with sleep deprivation. Behav Brain Res. 2011;218:73–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Drummond SP, Brown GG, Gillin JC, Stricker JL, Wong EC, Buxton RB. Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature. 2000;403:655–7. doi: 10.1038/35001068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Drummond S, Meloy MJ, Yanagi MA, Orff HJ, Brown GG. Compensatory recruitment after sleep deprivation and the relationship with performance. Psychiat Res Neuroim. 2005;140:211–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chuah LY, Dolcos F, Chen AK, Zheng H, Parimal S, Chee MW. Sleep deprivation and interference by emotional distracters. Sleep. 2010;33:1305–13. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.10.1305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Drummond SP, Anderson DE, Straus LD, Vogel EK, Perez VB. The effects of two types of sleep deprivation on visual working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. PloS One. 2012;7:e35653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bell-McGinty S, Habeck C, Hilton HJ, et al. Identification and differential vulnerability of a neural network in sleep deprivation. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:496–502. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Habeck C, Rakitin BC, Moeller J, Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Brown T, Stern Y. An event-related fMRI study of the neurobehavioral impact of sleep deprivation on performance of a delayed-match-to-sample task. Cogn Brain Res. 2004;18:306–21. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mu Q, Nahas Z, Johnson KA, et al. Decreased cortical response to verbal working memory following sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2005;28:55–67. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Chee MW, Chuah LY, Venkatraman V, Chan WY, Philip P, Dinges DF. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: Correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. Neuroimage. 2006;31:419–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Chee MW, Chuah LY. Functional neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of capacity decline in visual short-term memory after sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104:9487–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610712104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Luber B, Stanford AD, Bulow P, et al. Remediation of sleep-deprivation–induced working memory impairment with fMRI-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2077–85. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Luber B, Steffener J, Tucker A, et al. Extended remediation of sleep deprived-induced working memory deficits using fMRI-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation. Sleep. 2013;36:857–71. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Choo WC, Lee WW, Venkatraman V, Sheu FS, Chee MW. Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. Neuroimage. 2005;25:579–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES