Abstract
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H MRS) is a valuable tool in Alzheimer’s disease research, investigating the functional integrity of the brain. The present longitudinal study set out to characterize the neurochemical profile of the hippocampus, measured by single voxel 1H MRS at 7 Tesla, in the brains of AβPPSswe-PS1dE9 and wild-type mice at 8 and 12 months of age. Furthermore, we wanted to determine whether alterations in hippocampal metabolite levels coincided with behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features, to gain a better understanding of the underlying neurodegenerative processes. Moreover, correlation analyses were performed in the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 animals with the hippocampal amyloid-β deposition, TBS-T soluble Aβ levels and high-molecular weight Aβ aggregate levels to gain a better understanding of the possible involvement of Aβ in neurochemical and behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features in AβPP-PS1 transgenic mice. Our results show that at 8 months of age AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mice display behavioral and cognitive changes compared to age-matched wild-type mice, as determined in the open field and the (reverse) Morris water maze. However, there were no variations in hippocampal metabolite levels at this age. AβPP-PS1 mice at 12 months of age display more severe behavioral and cognitive impairment, which coincided with alterations in hippocampal metabolite levels that suggest reduced neuronal integrity. Furthermore, correlation analyses suggest a possible role of Aβ in inflammatory processes, synaptic dysfunction and impaired neurogenesis.
Introduction
The cause of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is still largely unknown despite many years of extensive research. Since AD is characterized by the presence of neurofibrillary tangles and amyloid-β (Aβ) containing aggregates, it has been suggested that the Aβ peptide is a major contributor to the neurodegenerative processes in AD [1], [2]. The Aβ peptide is derived through the proteolytic cleavage of the amyloid-β precursor protein (AβPP) by the β- and γ-secretases, BACE and Presenilin (PS), and it accumulates in the brain as neuritic plaques or as vascular wall deposits that may cause cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA) [3], [4]. In recent years, research focus has shifted from the insoluble Aβ plaques to the soluble oligomeric forms of Aβ as potential culprits of AD. Several lines of evidence have indicated that soluble oligomers of Aβ may be responsible for synaptic dysfunction and cognitive impairment in AD patients and transgenic animal models [5], [6].
Besides Aβ, several other potential causal mechanisms have been proposed since the discovery of AD. Large epidemiological studies have revealed that many risk factors for AD are vascular-related, causing chronic cerebral hypoperfusion and cerebrovascular pathology [7]–[11], suggesting that vascular disorders may play an important role in the onset of AD. Another process that has been implicated in the onset and development of AD is chronic neuroinflammation [12]–[14]. Activated astrocytes and microglia are found in abundance near Aβ plaques and neurons at risk in AD brains, producing several proinflammatory signal molecules, including cytokines, growth factors, complement molecules, and adhesion molecules [15], [16]. Furthermore, several studies have shown that activated microglia can suppress hippocampal neurogenesis [17]–[19], thereby contributing to cognitive dysfunction in aging and AD.
Rather than one sole mechanism, it is much more likely that AD is a multifactorial disease, caused by a combination of these factors, compromising the functional integrity of the brain. One method of determining the functional integrity of the brain, or specific brain regions, is to examine their metabolism by using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H MRS). 1H MRS allows the non-invasive in vivo analysis of certain neurometabolites that indicate biochemical changes in the brain, which are thought to be related to the pathological processes at the molecular or cellular level [20], [21]. Altered metabolic profiles, detected by 1H MRS, have been reported in patients with AD [22], [23]. The most consistent findings in AD patients are a reduction of the metabolite N-acetylaspartate (NAA) and an elevation of the metabolite myo-Inositol (mI) in several brain regions including the hippocampus [20], [24], [25]. NAA is considered to be a marker of neuronal viability, and a reduction of NAA is commonly interpreted as a result of neuronal dysfunction or neuronal loss [26]. An elevation of mI has been associated with inflammatory processes, since mI is a putative marker for microglia and astrogliosis [27]. Furthermore, disturbances of several other metabolites have been found in AD patients, although the reports are inconsistent. Some studies identified elevated choline-containing compounds (tCho) and creatine (Cre) in AD patients [28]–[30], whereas others did not [24], [31].
Similar to AD patients, the most consistent finding in transgenic animal models is a reduction of NAA, although this was found to occur at different ages in different transgenic species apparently depending on the interplay of mouse strain, transgene and disease progression [32]. Furthermore, conflicting results have been reported for several other metabolite levels, including mI, taurine (Tau) and glutamate (Glu), even within the same transgenic animal model [33]. Since 1H MRS has great potential for the early diagnosis of AD, monitoring disease progression and evaluating the efficacy of potential therapeutic agents, it is important to characterize the alterations in neurometabolites in several transgenic animals models of AD.
The present longitudinal study set out to characterize the neurochemical profile of the hippocampus, measured by 1H MRS, in the brains of AβPPswe-PS1dE9 and wild-type mice at 8 and 12 months of age. Furthermore, we wanted to determine whether alterations in hippocampal metabolite levels coincided with behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features, to gain a better understanding of the underlying neurodegenerative processes. Moreover, the extracellular amyloid-β plaque load, TBS-T soluble Aβ levels and high-molecular weight Aβ aggregate levels were determined in the brains of the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice used in the present study [34]. We performed correlation analyses with these Aβ measures, to gain a better understanding of the possible involvement of Aβ in the neurochemical and behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features in the AβPP-PS1 transgenic mice. Providing well characterized AD animal models and better understanding of the underlying pathological processes in AD is required for the development and evaluation of potential therapeutic targets.
Animals, Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement, Animals and Housing Conditions
The experiments were performed according to Dutch federal regulations for animal protection and were approved by Veterinary Authority of the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre (Permit Number: RU-DEC2008-126). All efforts were made to minimize suffering of the animals.
The AβPPswe-PS1dE9 founders were obtained from Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA (D. Borchelt and J. Jankowsky, Dept. of Pathology) and a colony was established at the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, the Netherlands. In short, mice were created by co-injection of chimeric mouse/human AβPPswe (mouse AβPP695 harboring a human Aβ domain and mutations K595N and M596L linked to Swedish familial AD pedigrees) and human PS1dE9 (deletion of exon 9) vectors controlled by independent mouse prion protein promoter elements. The two transfected genes co-integrate and co-segregate as a single locus [35], [36]. This line (line 85) was originally maintained on a hybrid background by backcrossing to C3HeJ × C57BL6/J F1 mice (so-called pseudo F2 stage). For the present work, the breeder mice were backcrossed to C57BL6/J for 9 generations to obtain mice for the current study. Throughout the experiment animals were housed in groups of 2–3 mice per cage in a controlled environment, homogenously illuminated by normal fluorescent room light at 60 lux, with room temperature at 21°C, and an artificial 12∶12 h light:dark cycle (lights on at 7 a.m.). Food and water were available ad libitum.
Male transgenic AβPP-PS1 mice and their wild-type littermates underwent behavioral testing and MRI measurements at 8 months of age, and again at 12 months of age. In total 25 mice were used: at 8 months of age, 15 wild-type and 10 AβPP-PS1 mice, and at 12 months of age, 9 wild-type and 7 AβPP-PS1 mice. Due to some technical problems during the experiments, not all mice could be used for the statistical analyses for each measure. For example, some mice were excluded from further analyses of the 1H MRS data, since the spectra obtained did not meet the inclusion criteria. The body weights of the mice were determined one day before the start of the behavioral tests, and again on the day of the MRI measurements.
Behavioral Analyses
Behavioral testing was performed in the following order: First open field, followed by Morris water maze (MWM), and finally the reversal MWM (rMWM). All testing sessions were performed during the light phase (between 9 a.m. and 5 p.m.) and were recorded for computer-assisted analysis using Noldus Ethovision 3.1 software (Noldus Information Technology B.V., Wageningen, the Netherlands). All behavioral testing was performed in the same room, homogenously illuminated by normal fluorescent room light at 60 lux.
Open field
To analyze explorative and anxiety-related behavior, mice were placed individually in the center of a square open field (50×50×50 cm) with white Plexiglas walls, and were observed for 30 minutes. The duration (seconds) of walking, wall leaning, rearing, sitting and grooming were scored and later analyzed in three blocks of 10 minutes. These open field parameters were defined as described previously [37], [38]. In addition, total walking distance, mean velocity, and the time spent in the corners respectively the center of the open field were obtained from the recorded sessions. The center of the open field was defined as a square measuring 20×20 cm, and the corners of the open field were defined as the sum of all four 10×10 cm squared corners.
Morris water maze (MWM)
To investigate spatial learning abilities, mice were tested in the Morris water maze (MWM). In short, mice were placed in a pool (104 cm diameter) filled with water (21–22°C; made opaque by the addition of milk powder) at different starting positions and trained to find a submerged platform by using distant visual cues in the room. These spatial cues were present on the four walls of the test room at a distance of 0.5 meter. The 8 cm diameter round platform was submerged 1 cm below the water surface and placed in the middle of the northeast (NE) quadrant at a distance of 26 cm from the wall. During all trials the researcher was present and always located at the same location in the room (close to the SW quadrant).
Acquisition (spatial learning): Mice were trained to find the location of the submerged escape platform in 4 acquisition trials (maximal swimming time 120 s; 30 s on the platform; inter-trial interval 60 min) per day during 4 consecutive days. The latency time (s) to find the hidden platform was scored. Starting positions during the 4 trails/day were: S, N, E, W. After the 2 min swim the mice were placed back in their home cage, and a paper towel was available inside the cage for additional drying.
Probe (spatial memory): All mice performed a single probe trial 60 min after the last trial on day 4, in which the platform was removed from the swimming pool. Mice were allowed to swim for 60 s and the time spent swimming and searching in the NE quadrant (where the platform had been located), total swimming distance, mean velocity and total number of platform crossings (at the former platform location) were recorded.
Reverse morris water maze (rMWM)
Four days after the standard MWM probe trial, a simplified reversal MWM [38] was performed in which the platform location was changed to the southwest (SW) quadrant. In this procedure, earlier platform location need to be encoded in the long-term memory. Memory retrieval needs to be selective for the most recently learned location, introducing an episodic like component in the spatial memory task [39]. Acquisition and probe sessions were performed similar to the standard MWM sessions, except that starting positions were E, W, S, and N, the target quadrant was SW, and training lasted only 2 days (4 trials/day).
Magnetic Resonances Spectroscopy (MRS)
MR measurements were performed on a 7T/300 mm horizontal-bore MR spectrometer interfaced to a ClinScan console (Bruker Biospin, Ettlingen, Germany). An integrated circular polarized transmit 1H volume coil (200 mm/154 mm outer/inner diameter) combined with a circular polarized receive 1H surface coil was used for signal reception. During the experiments, mice were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane (Abott, Cham, Switzerland) in a mixture of N2O and oxygen (1∶2) through a nose cone. Mice were placed in a stereotactic holder to prevent unwanted movement during the scanning. Body temperature was maintained at a physiological level with heated airflow and was monitored with a rectal optical temperature probe. Respiration of the animal was monitored using a pneumatic cushion respiratory monitoring system (Small Animal Instruments Inc, NY, USA). Multislice turbo spin echo images in the coronal, transversal and longitudinal orientation were acquired to visualize the anatomy and the morphology of the mouse brain structures. Imaging parameters were: FOV = 25×25 mm, matrix size = 256×256, slice thickness = 0.5 mm, TE = 46 ms, and TR = 3500 ms.
Metabolite concentrations in the hippocampus were determined using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H MRS) with a single voxel technique. The spectroscopic volume of interest (VOI) of 1.0×1.0×1.6 mm was positioned in the hippocampus (Figure 1), according to the mouse brain atlas of Franklin and Paxinos [40]. Water-suppressed 1H-MRS spectra were acquired with the stimulated echo acquisition mode (STEAM) sequence with experiment parameters: TR = 1500 ms, TE = 13 ms, and 1024 signal averages. Water suppression was performed with variable pulse power and optimized relaxation delays (VAPOR). For each 1H MRS spectrum, a water reference spectrum was acquired without water suppression with experiment parameters: TR = 1500 ms, TE = 13 ms, and 32 signal averages. Total acquisition time for 1H MRS was 27 min per animal.
Quantification of the metabolite concentration was performed using a the Linear Combination (LC) model software package (LCModel™, S. Provencher, Oakville, Canada). The quantification algorithm of LCModel™ applies linear combinations of model spectra to calculate the best fit of the experimental spectrum. The model spectra (dataset of prior knowledge) are calibrated to match the magnetic field strength, sequence type and sequence parameters used for data acquisition. The final analysis is performed in the frequency domain with raw data (free induction decay (FID)) as the input. The unsuppressed water spectrum was used to estimate the absolute concentration of the metabolites, of which simulated model spectra, generated by NMRSIM™ (Bruker Biospin, Ettlingen, Germany) were taken into the analysis.
The criteria to select reliable metabolite concentrations were based on the Cramér-Rao lower bounds (CRLB), which are estimates of the S.D. of the fit for each metabolite [41] and are also determined by LCModel™. Only fit results with a CRLB ≤20% were included in the analysis. Concentrations with CRLB>20% were classified as not accurately detectable. Based on these criteria, 2 wild-type and 2 AβPP-PS1 mice at 8 months of age, and 2 wild-type and 3 AβPP-PS1 mice at 12 months of age were excluded from further analyses, since they displayed fit results with CRLB>20%. Only metabolite fits that had a CRLB ≤20% in more than 80% of the spectra were included, and individual concentrations with corresponding CRLB>20% were not taken into account.
Seven metabolites fulfilled the criteria: choline+glycerophosphocholine+phosphocholine (tCho), creatine+phosphocreatine (tCre), glutamate (Glu), glutamine+glutamate (Glx), myo-Inositol+glycine (mI+Gly), N-acetylaspartate (NAA) and taurine (Tau). Although the exact function of these metabolites are not fully known, NAA is considered to be a marker of neuronal viability, tCre is involved in energy metabolism, mI is a putative marker for microglia and astrogliosis, and tCho is required for the synthesis of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, and of phosphatidylcholine, a major constituent of membranes, and is therefore associated with membrane turnover [27].
Immunohistochemistry
Directly following the MR measurements at 12 months of age, mice were sacrificed by cervical dislocation, and subsequently brains were removed from the skull after decapitation. Brains were weighed and cut mid sagittal for immunohistochemistry and biochemistry. One hemisphere was snap frozen in liquid nitrogen and then stored at −80°C, before further biochemical processing. The other hemisphere was immersion fixated in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hours, and subsequently stored in 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline (PBS, pH = 7.3) with 1% sodium azide at 4°C. Before cutting, the brain tissue was cryoprotected by immersion in 30% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB, pH = 7.3). Six series of 40 µm coronal sections were cut through the brain using a sliding microtome (Microm HM 440 E, Walldorf, Germany). For every staining, one complete series with 240 µm distance between the sections was used. Immunohistochemistry was performed using standard free-floating labeling procedures [42], and was carried out on a shaker table at room temperature.
Presynaptic boutons were visualized with anti-synaptophysin antibody (1∶20,000; monoclonal rabbit anti-synaptophysin clone EP1098Y, Abcam Inc., Cambridge, UK) using one subseries of brain sections per animal. Synaptophysin is localized in small synaptic vesicles of the presynaptic terminal and functions in the regulation of exocytosis [43]. Donkey anti-rabbit biotin 1∶1500 (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA) was used as secondary antibody.
Immature neurons were visualized with anti-doublecortin antibody (1∶3000; polyclonal goat anti-doublecortin (C18): sc-8066, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Inc., Santa Cruz, CA, USA) using one subseries of brain sections per animal. Doublecortin is a microtubule-associated protein that is exclusively found in somata and processes of migrating and differentiating neurons [44], [45]. Donkey-goat biotin 1∶1500 (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA) was used as secondary antibody.
Quantification
Quantification of presynaptic boutons and doublecortin-positive immature neurons was performed using a Zeiss Axioskop microscope, equipped with hardware and software from Microbrightfield (Williston, VT, USA). Appropriate sections were digitized and photomicrographed using a computer-assisted analysis system, Stereo Investigator (Microbrightfield). Brain regions were based on the mouse brain atlas of Franklin and Paxinos [40]. All measurements were performed double blind by two independent raters, and measurements were averaged to obtain a single value per animal for every region of interest.
Quantification of synaptophysin-immunoreactive presynaptic boutons
To determine the amount of synaptophysin-immunoreactive presynaptic boutons (SIPBs) in the hippocampus and cortical regions, appropriate sections were digitized and photomicrographed using an 100× oil immersion objective. SIPBs were analyzed in the hippocampal regions stratum radiatum (SR) of the cornu ammonis (CA)1 area, stratum lucidum (SL) of the CA3 area, inner molecular layer (IML) and outer molecular layer (OML) of the dentate gyrus (DG), and in the cortical regions prelimbic area (PLA) and anterior cingulate gyrus (ACg). These regions were chosen because of their large amyloid load in AD patients and transgenic mouse models for AD and their importance in learning and memory [46], [47]. The ACg was quantified at level +1.10 up to +0.86 anterior to bregma using one appropriate section per animal, the PLA was quantified at +1.98 up to +1.78 anterior to bregma using one appropriate section per animal, and hippocampal regions were quantified at −2.18 up to −2.46 posterior to bregma using one appropriate section per animal. For every region of interest, two square boxed were placed within the borders of the intended area using an 2.5 or 5× objective and images were taken using an 100× oil immersion objective (Figure 2). Images were further processed using ImageJ software (U.S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA) for the quantification of the amount of SIPBs. All settings were kept identical for all analyses and background levels were equalized using a threshold. Shading correction was performed before measurement to correct for irregularities in illumination in the microscopic field. A differential contrast enhancement filter was applied to selectively enhance weak differences in contrast. To eliminate noise signal and to differentiate between possible artifacts and specific SIPBs, particles were classified based on size. Particles ranging between 0.1–4.5 µm2 were considered to be normal sized SIPBs [48], [49], and were included for statistical analyses. Particles smaller than 0.1 µm2 and larger than 4.5 µm2 were excluded for analyses. The amount of SIPBs/µm2 was defined as the number of particles divided by the total area analyzed.
Quantification of doublecortin-positive cells
For the assessment of immature neurons in the hippocampus as a measure for neurogenesis (Figure 3), three alternating sections per animal (at −2.18. −2.46 and −2.70 posterior to bregma) were digitized and contours were drawn along the borders of the hippocampus using an 5× objective using Stereo Investigator software. Doublecortin-positive (Dcx+) cells were counted with an 20× objective, and the values of the three alternating sections were averaged to obtain a single value per animal.
Amyloid-β Measures
The extracellular Aβ load, soluble Aβ levels and insoluble high-molecular weight Aβ aggregate levels were determined in the brains of the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, as has been described elsewhere [34].
In short, Aβ deposits were visualized using WO-2 antibody (1∶20,000, mouse anti-human Aβ4–10, a kind gift of K. Beyreuther, University of Heidelberg, Germany) using one subseries of brain sections per animal. Donkey anti-mouse biotin (1∶1500, Jackson ImmunoResearch was used as secondary antibody. Extracellular Aβ plaque load was quantified in the hippocampus, prelimbic area (PLA) and anterior cingulate gyrus (ACg) with a computer-assisted analysis system (Stereo Investigator, Microbrightfield) using Cavalieri’s probe (Figure 4). The Aβ plaque load was defined as the percentage of area covered by Aβ.
For biochemical Aβ analyses, frozen hemibrains were homogenized in Tris buffered saline with 1% Triton X-100 (TBS-T) plus protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche Applied Science, Mannheim, Germany) and centrifuged at 16,000 g for 30 min at 4°C.The supernatant, enriched with oligomeric Aβ, was collected and stored at −80°C. The pellet, containing mainly highly aggregated Aβ, was resuspended in guanidine chloride buffer and extracted for 4 hours. The TBS-T and guanidine HCl extracts were analyzed for human Aβ40 and Aβ42 (KHB3442 and KH3482, Invitrogen, Karlsruhe, Germany) according to the manufacturers protocol. Results were normalized to the protein concentration of the sample (Bio-Rad Protein Assay, Bio-Rad Laboratories, Munich, Germany).
Statistical Analysis
Data are expressed as mean±SEM and were analyzed with SPSS for windows 16.0 software (SPSS Inc. Chicago, IL, USA). The repeated measures ANOVA was used for the open field parameters (with the repeated measure: time) and the acquisition phase of the MWM and rMWM (with the repeated measure: trial block), followed by a Bonferroni post hoc to analyze possible interactions between time/trial block and genotype. If interactions between time/trial block and genotype (between-group-factors) were present, the data were split for the concerning factor and thereafter analyzed again with the repeated measures ANOVA. Multivariate ANOVA’s were conducted with between group factor: genotype, to analyze possible differences between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice in the probe trials of the MWM and rMWM, the body weight, brain weight, metabolite concentrations, and the amount of SIPBs and immature neurons. Aging effects in the AβPP-PS1 mice are represented as relative values compared to the wild-type mice (set as 100%) of the corresponding age-group, and were analyzed with between group factor: age. Correlation analyses with the Aβ measures were performed using bivariate Pearson’s correlation method. For clarity reasons, F-values are not displayed. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05.
Results
Body and Brain Weight
All mice were weighed one day before starting the behavioral test battery and again on the day of the MR measurements. Since body weights within the groups did not change significantly between those two time points, the mean weight was used for further statistical analyses. Both at 8 months and 12 months of age, AβPP-PS1 mice had a tendency towards a higher body weight than wild-type mice, but it did not reach statistical significance. At 8 months of age, overall mean body weight of the AβPP-PS1 mice was 29.79±0.65 g compared to 28.24±0.42 g of the wild-type mice (p = 0.051). At 12 months of age, overall mean body weight of the AβPP-PS1 mice was 33.03±1.23 g compared to 30.13±0.76 g of the wild-type mice (p = 0.060).
Both relative and absolute brain weights were not affected by genotype (p = 0.812 and p = 0.165 respectively). Overall mean brain weight of the AβPP-PS1animals was 0.49±0.01 g, which was 1.46±0.08% of their total body weight. Overall mean brain weight of the wild-type mice was 0.45±0.02 g, which was 1.49±0.07% of their total body weight.
Behavioral Analyses
Open field
During the 30 min observation at 8 months of age, both in wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice the time spent walking (Figure 5A; p<0.001) and wall leaning (Figure 5I; p = 0.038) decreased, as well as the distance traveled (Figure 5G; p<0.001) and mean velocity (p<0.001). Accordingly, the time spent sitting (Figure 5C; p<0.001) increased during the 30 min observation period. The time spent rearing (Figure 5E; p = 0.632) and grooming (Figure 5K; p = 0.263), as well as the time spent in the center (Figure 5 M; p = 0.673) and in the corners (Figure 5O; p = 0.106) of the open field remained constant over time.
8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice were more active in the open field than wild-type mice:
AβPP-PS1 mice traveled a longer distance (Figure 5G; p = 0.048) and had a higher mean walking speed than wild-type mice (p = 0.043; wild-type 5.6±0.3 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 6.9±0.5 cm/s), although the time spent walking and sitting did not differ from the wild-type animals (Figure 5A; p = 0.293 and Figure 5C; p = 0.964 respectively). AβPP-PS1 mice also spent less time rearing than wild-type mice (Figure 5E; p = 0.043). No differences were observed between the genotypes in the time spent wall leaning (Figure 5I; p = 0.642) and grooming (Figure 5K; p = 0.259), and in the time spent in the center (Figure 5 M; p = 0.274) and corners of the open field (Figure 5O; p = 0.189).
Like at 8 months of age, the time spent walking (Figure 5B; p = 0.001), the distance traveled (Figure 5H; p = 0.001), and mean velocity (p = 0.001) decreased over time in both wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice at 12 months of age. In contrast, the time spent grooming increased at this age during the 30 min open field (Figure 5L; p = 0.009), whereas the time spent sitting (Figure 5D; p = 0.104) and wall leaning (Figure 5J; p = 0.443) remained constant throughout the observation period. Again, the time spent rearing (Figure 5F; p = 0.866), as well as the time spent in the center (Figure 5N; p = 0.389) and the corners (Figure 5P; p = 0.465) of the open field did not change over time.
Compared to wild-type mice, 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice spent more time wall leaning (Figure 5J; p = 0.041), and slightly less time walking (Figure 5B; p = 0.058), although the distance traveled (Figure 5H; p = 0.859) and mean velocity of the AβPP-PS1 mice did not differ from the wild-type animals (p = 0.886; wild-type 4.7±0.3 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 4.7±0.5 cm/s). Furthermore, AβPP-PS1 mice spent more time in the corners (Figure 5P; p = 0.001), and less time in the center of the open field (Figure 5N; p = 0.021) as compared to wild-type mice. At this age, no differences were observed between the genotypes in the time spent sitting (Figure 5D; p = 0.349), rearing (Figure 5F; p = 0.359) and grooming (Figure 5L; p = 0.564).
Aging effects in the AβPP-PS1 mice were analyzed as relative values compared to the wild-type mice (set as 100%) of the corresponding age. With age, the time that AβPP-PS1 mice spent walking decreased, from 105.7%±3.8 at 8 months of age to 79.5%±7.1 at 12 months of age (p = 0.016), indicating decreased activity as disease progresses or decreased restlessness due to habituation to the open field. However, the traveled distance and mean velocity did not change with age (p = 0.105) as compared to age-matched wild-type animals (data not shown). Over time, AβPP-PS1 mice also spent more time in the corners of the open field (p = 0.018). Time spent in the corners of the open field increased from 115.7%±7.1 at 8 months of age to 169.3%±15.1 at 12 months of age, indicating increased anxiety-related behavior as disease progresses. The time spent sitting (p = 0.786), rearing (p = 0.481), grooming (p = 0.086), wall leaning (p = 0.087) and the time spent in the center of the open field (p = 0.116) did not change between 8 and 12 months of age (data not shown).
Altogether, these data show increased activity but decreased active exploration (rearing) in 8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice compared to wild-type mice. This increased activity and decreased rearing disappear at 12 months of age, due to habituation to the open field and/or the older age of the animals. Instead, 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice show increased anxiety-related behavior compared to wild-type mice, as indicated by the increased time spent in the corners and decreased time spent in the center of the open field, and increased time spent wall leaning, which is a type of exploration but with an anxiety-related behavioral component [38], [50].
Morris water maze (MWM)
Both AβPP-PS1and wild-type mice showed a decrease in escape latency during training at 8 months of age (Figure 6A; p<0.001). However, wild-type mice learned to find the location of the hidden platform faster than the AβPP-PS1 mice, indicated by a significant time*genotype interaction (p = 0.009). Overall escape latencies tended to differ between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 (p = 0.060) at 8 months of age, although it did not reach statistical significance. Especially on day 2 of the acquisition phase, AβPP-PS1 mice showed higher escape latencies than wild-type mice, suggesting that spatial learning might be mildly affected at this age.
During the probe trial at 8 months of age, no differences were found between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice in the time spent in the platform quadrant (NE) (Figure 6C; p = 0.266). However, only wild-type mice deviated from 25% chance performance level, suggesting that only wild-type mice showed good memorization of the platform quadrant. Furthermore, AβPP-PS1 mice crossed the exact platform location less often than wild-type mice (Figure 6D; p = 0.019), reflecting impaired spatial memory for the exact platform location. No significant differences were found between the genotypes in the total distance moved (Figure 6B; p = 0.056) and mean swim velocity (p = 0.078; wild-type 17.5±1.2 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 20.4±0.5 cm/s) during the probe trial, although the AβPP-PS1 mice tended to be slightly more active than the wild-type animals.
Both wild-type and AβPP-PS1 animals learned to find the platform during acquisition at 12 months of age (Figure 6E), as indicated by a decrease in latency over trials (p<0.001). Escape latencies did not differ between the wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice (p = 0.525), indicating that spatial learning was no longer affected by genotype at this age, most likely due to the repetition of this task.
AβPP-PS1 mice spent less time in the NE target quadrant compared to their wild-type littermates at 12 months of age (Figure 6G; p = 0.013), although both groups performed above 25% chance level, indicating memorization of the platform quadrant. No differences were found between the genotypes in the frequency of platform crossings (Figure 6H; p = 0.131), the total distance moved (Figure 6F; p = 0.221) and mean swim velocity (p = 0.232; wild-type 17.2±0.8 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 18.7±0.8 cm/s) during the probe trial.
Aging effects in the AβPP-PS1 mice were analyzed as relative values compared to the wild-type mice (set as 100%) of the corresponding age. Escape latencies during training in the MWM decreased with age (p = 0.001), such that the average escape latency of all 4 days was 128.2%±6.5 at 8 months of age relative to age-matched wild-type animals and 88.2%±7.2 at 12 months of age, indicating that 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice found the location of the hidden platform faster than 8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, most likely due to habituation to and repetition of the MWM task at 12 months of age. The time spent in target NE quadrant (p = 0.380), the number of platform crossings (p = 0.167), the distance moved (p = 0.121), and mean swim velocity (p = 0.173) did not change between 8 and 12 months of age (data not shown).
Reverse Morris water maze (rMWM)
Both wild-type and AβPP-PS1 animals learned to find the new SW platform location during acquisition at 8 months of age (Figure 7A), as indicated by a decrease in latency over trials (p = 0.046). Overall escape latencies did not differ between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 (p = 0.693).
No differences were found between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 in the time spent in the SW quadrant during the probe trial at 8 months of age (Figure 7C; p = 0.271). However, only wild-type mice deviated from 25% chance performance level, suggesting that only wild-type mice showed good memorization of the new platform quadrant. No differences were found between the genotypes in the number of platform crossings (Figure 7D; p = 0.187), total distance moved (Figure 7B; p = 0.129) and mean swim velocity (p = 0.111; wild-type 17.6±1.0 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 19.8±0.7 cm/s) during the probe trial.
At 12 months of age, the wild-type animals showed a significant decrease in escape latency over time in the acquisition phase (Figure 7E; p = 0.001), indicating spatial learning. In contrast, escape latencies did not change over time in the AβPP-PS1 mice at 12 months of age (p = 0.649), due to unexpected low latencies at the initial trials. In spite of that, there was no difference in average escape latencies between AβPP-PS1 and wild-type animals (p = 0.134).
During the probe trial at 12 months of age, AβPP-PS1 mice tended to spent less time in the target SW quadrant than wild-type mice (Figure 7G; p = 0.066), although it did not reach statistical significance. Furthermore, only wild-type mice deviated from 25% chance performance level, suggesting that only wild-type mice showed good memorization of the new platform quadrant. AβPP-PS1 mice also crossed the former platform location significantly less often than wild-type animals (Figure 7H; p = 0.029), reflecting impaired spatial memory as well. No significant differences were found between the genotypes in the total distance moved (Figure 7F; p = 0.066) and mean swim velocity (p = 0.066; wild-type 17.1±1.0 cm/s, AβPP-PS1 19.9±0.9 cm/s), although the AβPP-PS1 mice tended to be slightly more active than the wild-type animals.
Aging effects in the AβPP-PS1 mice were analyzed as relative values compared to the wild-type mice (set as 100%) of the corresponding age. Escape latencies during training in the rMWM did not change with age and repetition of the rMWM test (p = 0.123).
During the probe trial, the swim distance increased with age (p = 0.049) from 111.4%±3.7 at 8 months to 116.2%±5.2 at 12 months, indicating hyperactivity as disease progresses. The time spent in the SW target quadrant (p = 0.165) and number of platform crossings (p = 0.456) did not change between 8 and 12 months of age (data not shown).
Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy (MRS)
No differences were observed in the neurochemical profile of the hippocampus of 8-month-old wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice (Figure 8C). Wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice had similar concentrations of choline-containing compounds (tCho; p = 0.514), creatine and phosphocreatine (tCre; p = 0.712), glutamate (Glu; p = 0.785), glutamine and glutamate (Glx; p = 0.181), myo-Inositol and glycine (mI+Gly; p = 0.272), N-acetylaspartate (NAA; p = 0.456), and taurine (Tau; p = 0.245).
At 12 months of age, AβPP-PS1 mice had significantly lower concentrations of NAA (p = 0.012) than wild-type mice, indicating decreased neuronal health and/or increased neurodegeneration (Figure 8D). No differences were observed between wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice in the levels of tCho (p = 0.123), tCre (p = 0.986), Glu (p = 0.162), Glx (p = 0.807), mI+Gly (p = 0.082) and Tau (p = 0.804).
Aging effects in the AβPP-PS1 mice were analyzed as relative values compared to the wild-type mice (set as 100%) of the corresponding age. With age, the concentration of NAA decreased from 94.7%±6.0 at 8 months of age to 65.1%±8.4 at 12 months of age (p = 0.017), indicating decreased hippocampal neuronal viability as disease progresses.
Immunohistochemistry
At 12 months of age, there were no significant differences in the amount of synaptophysin-immunoreactive presynaptic boutons (SIPBs) between AβPP-PS1 and wild-type mice (Figure 9) in any of the regions analyzed (p>0.10). Furthermore, no significant differences were found in the amount of doublecortin-positive (Dcx+) immature neurons between 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 and wild-type mice (p = 0.082; wild-type 9.2±1.5 Dcx+ cells, n = 6; AβPP-PS1 5.5±0.3 Dcx+ cells, n = 4).
Amyloid-β Measures
Correlation analyses with the extracellular amyloid-β deposition, TBS-T soluble Aβ levels and high-molecular weight Aβ aggregate levels found in the brains of 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice [34] were performed using bivariate Pearson’s correlation method.
No significant interactions were found between any of the Aβ measures and the open field data (p>0.05), and the MWM and rMWM acquisition and probe data (p>0.05).
A significant negative correlation was found between the Aβ plaque load in the hippocampus and the tCho levels measured with 1H MRS (p = 0.005; R = −0.995). Furthermore, increased mI+Gly levels significantly correlated with increased levels of the high-molecular weight Aβ40 (p = 0.007; R = 0.993) and Aβ42 (p = 0.003; R = 0.997) aggregates.
A significant negative correlation was found between the TBS-T soluble Aβ42 levels and the amount of SIPBs in the PLA region (p = 0.040; R = −0.895). In contrast, positive correlations were found between the amount of SIPBs in the SR region of the hippocampus and the levels of high-molecular weight Aβ40 (p = 0.009; R = 0.962) and Aβ42 (p = 0.027; R = 0.920) aggregates. However, these correlations between the amount of SIPBs and Aβ measures did no longer reach significance when Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons was applied (p>0.008).
Finally, we found a significant negative correlation between the amount of immature neurons and the level of high-molecular weight Aβ40 (p = 0.041; R = −0.959) aggregates, suggesting impaired neurogenesis with increasing levels of Aβ40 aggregates.
Discussion
The present longitudinal study set out to characterize the neurochemical profile of the hippocampus, measured by 1H MRS, in the brains of AβPPSswe-PS1dE9 mice at 8 and 12 months of age as compared to age-matched wild-type littermates. Furthermore, we wanted to determine whether alterations in hippocampal metabolite levels coincided with behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features, to gain a better understanding of the underlying neurodegenerative processes. Moreover, we determined the extracellular amyloid-β load, TBS-T soluble Aβ levels and high-molecular weight Aβ aggregate levels of the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice in our laboratory [34]. We performed correlation analyses using bivariate Pearson’s correlation method to gain a better understanding of the possible involvement of Aβ in neurochemical and behavioral changes, cognitive decline and neuropathological features in AβPP-PS1 transgenic mice.
In agreement with previous results from our lab [38], 8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice already display behavioral changes in the open field, such as increased locomotor activity and decreased exploration. The increased activity is a specific characteristic of many AβPP transgenic mice [51]–[54] and may be explained as a result of elevated anxiety levels [53], [55] or impaired habituation learning [51], [52]. In our view increased anxiety is most likely, since the AβPP-PS1 mice also showed less rearing behavior (exploring the environment) compared to age-matched wild-type mice. Curiosity motivates mice to explore a novel environment, but this exploratory drive is in conflict with fear of the unknown. Moreover, 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice also showed increased anxiety-related behavior, as indicated by the increased time spent in the corners of the open field, increased time spent wall leaning, and decreased time spent in the center of the open field (i.e. anxious mice prefer the borders of the open field). Increased anxiety and restlessness, as noticed as hyperactivity in several AβPP transgenic mouse models, also occur in AD patients [56], [57]. However, activity levels decreased between 8 and 12 months of age both in AβPP-PS1 and wild-type mice, suggesting increased familiarity with the open field even after 4 months.
In line with previous results from our lab, AβPP-PS1 mice also showed impaired performance in the MWM and rMWM both at 8 and 12 months of age as compared to wild-type littermates [38], which might suggest visuo-spatial learning and memory deficits. Noteworthy are the relatively low escape latencies of both AβPP-PS1 and wild-type mice during the initial trials of the rMWM training at 8 months. Although 8-month-old wild-type mice displayed good short-term spatial memory for the NE platform location during the probe of the MWM, it appears that this information might not have been sufficiently consolidated in the long-term memory since they displayed more “random” search during the initial rMWM trials (resulting in lower escape latencies) than expected if animals had good long-term memory for the former platform location. One possible explanation could be that the MWM task as used in the current study was slightly too difficult to optimally master within 4 days of training. For mice, a MWM pool with a diameter of 120 cm and a platform of 10−12 cm in diameter (or even larger) is commonly used, resulting in a search area to target size of 144∶1 to 100∶1 (or even lower). In the current study, we used a pool with a diameter of 104 cm and a platform of 8 cm in diameter, resulting in a search area to target size of 169∶1, thereby increasing the MWM task difficulty [58], [59]. At 12 months of age, when the mice were exposed to the MWM for the second time, wild-type mice not only showed a higher preference for the NE target quadrant during the probe than at 8 months of age, they also persevered in searching the original platform location during the initial trials in the rMWM, suggesting that habituation to, and repetition of the MWM task resulted in enhanced short-term, and formation of long-term memory for the platform location in wild-type mice.
Standard measures of performance in the MWM as used in the current study, such as escape latency during acquisition training, time spent in platform quadrant and the number of platform crossings during a probe trial, may depend on other factors than visuo-spatial learning ability and memory capacity alone. Longer escape latencies during spatial navigation may be caused by slower swim speed, although we do not expect this to be a confounding factor, since wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice did not display any significant differences in swim speed during the probe trials at any age. Furthermore, several groups have reported no differences in swim speed during MWM training between C57BL6/J wild-type and AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mice at any age tested [60]–[63]. Longer escape latencies during spatial navigation may also be caused by the use of certain, less efficient, search strategies, such as a constant random search of the entire surface area of the pool, which would indicate a complete lack of spatial learning abilities, or by persistent performance of a less efficient than spatial (direct) search strategy, such as circling the pool at a certain distance from the wall to find the platform [64]–[67]. In this strategy, mice have not used the spatial cues to learn the location of the platform, although such a strategy would result in a successful location of the escape platform during training. To our knowledge, the search strategies used by AβPP-PS1 mice in the MWM have not be determined yet and should be addressed in future studies to further characterize the cognitive deficits in the AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mouse model. However, our results might imply that the AβPP-PS1 mice made use of a random or persistent non-spatial search strategy to locate the platform during acquisition training, since AβPP-PS1 mice showed slightly longer escape latencies to find the hidden platform in the MWM at 8 months of age, searched the target quadrant during the probe trials at chance level (∼25% or less) and they crossed the former platform location less often than wild-type mice. In contrast, wild-type mice performed well above chance level during the probe trials, indicating memorization of the platform location, which might imply that they made use of a more spatially precise search strategy. Furthermore, the high latency of the wild-type mice during the first 2 acquisition trials of the reversal task at 12 months of age might indicate that they learned and remembered the original NE platform location quite well, and persevered in searching the original platform location during those initial trials in the reversal MWM. The 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice in contrast did not improve their performance over time in the reversal task, but seemed to perform better in the first 2 acquisition trials compared to their wild-type littermates. Again this might imply that the AβPP-PS1 mice made use of a more random non-spatial search strategy and thereby coincidentally reaching the platform faster than their wild-type littermates.
This is in line with a study by O’Leary and Brown, in which the search strategies used by 16-month-old AβPP-PS1and wild-type mice during visuo-spatial navigation in the Barnes Maze were analyzed [68]. 16-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice predominantly made use of a random search strategy to locate the escape hole in the Barnes Maze, whereas wild-type mice predominantly made use of a spatial (direct and accurate) search strategy. Similar results have been found for the search strategies used by the TgCRND8 transgenic AβPP mouse model in the MWM test [64], [65]. Moreover, it was shown that C57BL6 mice treated with Temozolomide (TMZ) to suppress adult hippocampal neurogenesis, displayed a delayed (or even absent) use of directed and place specific search patterns in the (reversal) MWM test compared to untreated mice, suggesting that hippocampal neurogenesis is necessary for adding flexibility to some hippocampus-dependent qualitative parameters of learning [67]. Although we did not observe a significant decrease in the amount of immature neurons in the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice in our current study, reduced hippocampal neurogenesis has been found previously in AβPP-PS1 mice [69]–[71] and in AD patients [72], [73], and might underlie some aspects of the cognitive deficits in AD.
Synaptic loss is a pathological hallmark of AD, and it is the best correlate of cognitive impairment [2], [74]–[76]. Synaptophysin is a widely used marker for quantification of presynaptic terminals, but conflicting results have been reported on synaptophysin immunoreactivity (SYN-IR) both in AD patients [77]–[80] and AD mouse models. SYN-IR in transgenic mice that develop Aβ plaques has been reported to decline, increase, or not change, apparently depending on the interplay of mouse strain, transgene, age, and disease progression [49], [81]–[86]. In the current study, no changes were found in the amount of presynaptic boutons at 12 months of age, similar to earlier reports of no changes in the amount of presynaptic boutons in 8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice [42], but in contrast to the increased amount of presynaptic boutons found in the hippocampus of 15-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, which was suggested to reflect a synaptic compensatory response to maintain connectivity and preserve cognitive functioning [42]. However, we did find positive correlations between the levels of high-molecular weight Aβ40 and Aβ42 aggregates and the amount of SIPBs in the SR region of the hippocampus in the 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, which might suggest a possible involvement of Aβ aggregates in the initiation of this synaptic compensatory response. In contrast, a negative correlation was found between the TBS-T soluble Aβ42 levels and the amount of SIPBs in the PLA region, which might suggest a possible involvement of soluble Aβ oligomers in synaptic loss. However, these correlations between the amount of SIPBs and Aβ measures did no longer reach significance when Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons was applied. Since the necessity of using the conservative Bonferroni’s adjustment for multiple correlations is still debated [87]–[89], these findings must be interpreted with caution.
Recent studies suggest that synapse loss might not be an early event in the progression of AD, as a decrease in synapses is only seen in later stages of the disease, where especially tau pathology is more widespread [90]–[92]. It has been proposed that the pathogenesis of synaptic damage in AD can be divided into two phases: in the first phase, the disease is characterized by neuronal and synaptic dysfunction (loss of plasticity), which triggers a compensatory response to maintain connectivity by, first the formation of new synapses, and later on by increasing the size of remaining synapses [75], [86], [90], [93]. In the second phase, cycles of aberrant sprouting and neuritic disorganization eventually result in synapse loss and neurodegeneration [94], [95]. It could be hypothesized that the decreased hippocampal neuronal viability found in our 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, together with increasing levels of Aβ40 and Aβ42 oligomers and aggregates, contribute to synaptic and neuronal dysfunction, which might trigger a synaptic compensatory response that in time results in the increased amount of presynaptic terminals as seen in the hippocampus of 15-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice [42].
In vivo 1H MRS has been used to characterize cerebral metabolic alterations in mild cognitive impaired (MCI) individuals and AD patients [23]–[25], [96]–[99], and in several transgenic AβPP animal models for AD [33], [100]–[107]. The most consistent finding is a decrease in NAA level, a biomarker for neuronal integrity, in the cortex and hippocampus with aging and disease progression. Clinically, reduction of NAA has been used as an indicator of the progression of neurodegenerative pathology in AD patients, and to differentiate stable MCI from progressive MCI [23], [96], [97], [108], [109]. Longitudinal changes in 1H MRS measures have also been evaluated in AD patients during therapeutic trials with cholinergic agents. Treatment with xanomeline, an M1-selective cholinergic agonist, increased the level of NAA and decreased the level of Cho compared to baseline values before treatment. Changes in Cho levels from baseline further correlated with improved or at least stable ADAS-Cog scores [110], [111]. These findings not only support the feasibility of MRS measures in AD clinical trials, but also indicate that AD-related changes detected by MRS may be reversible, and may reflect aspects of neuronal integrity or function. Furthermore, many previous 1H MRS studies have found increased levels of mI in the temporal, parietal and occipital lobes of AD patients [22], [25], [98], [99]. mI is a sugar alcohol that is thought to be a marker for osmotic stress, astrogliosis and microglial activation [27], and an increase in cerebral mI levels is therefore associated with inflammatory processes. Although we did not observe significant differences in hippocampal mI+Gly concentration between the 12-month-old wild-type and AβPP-PS1 mice in the current study, correlation analyses revealed a positive correlation between the levels of Aβ40 and Aβ42 aggregates and mI+Gly levels in the AβPP-PS1 animals, suggesting a possible involvement of Aβ in inflammatory processes.
In transgenic animals models, significant reduction of NAA levels and elevation of mI levels were found to occur at different ages in different transgenic species apparently depending on the interplay of mouse strain, transgene and disease progression. The AβPPswe (Tg2576) model and the AβPPswe-PS2N1411 (PS2APP) model display significantly decreased NAA level in the frontal cortex at 19–24 months of age, when Aβ deposits are widespread, but on the other hand no change in mI levels compared to age-matched wild-type mice [100], [102]. In the case of the AβPPswe-PS1M146L model, a significant reduction in the NAA/tCre ratio in the frontal hippocampus was observed at 16 month of age in one study [101], but already at 6.5 months in another study [104], while the most profound increase in mI level was observed after 20 months of age [101], [103], [104]. In contrast to our findings of a lower NAA concentration in the hippocampus of 12-month-old AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mice, Xu and colleagues did not observe significantly decreased hippocampal NAA/total creatine (tCre) levels until 16 months of age, which was associated with progressive degeneration of CA3 pyramidal neurons [107]. Chen and colleagues on the other hand observed a significantly lower concentration of NAA in the frontal cortex and hippocampus already at 5 months of age in the AβPP-PS1mouse model, which coincided with neuronal loss and neuronal shrinkage. Furthermore, compared to age-matched wild-type animals the concentration of mI was significantly higher in 3-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice, and pathology showed activation and proliferation of astrocytes in the frontal cortex and hippocampus [112], [113]. The discrepancy between the results found in the latter two studies and our findings at 8 and 12 months of age are most likely due to differences in methodology, e.g. the field strength of the MR system, the acquisition parameters used, the exact position of the spectroscopic volume of interest, and the amount of animals measured.
It has been demonstrated that AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mice may exhibit various neurobiological abnormalities in the hippocampus before 8 months of age. Such abnormalities include inflammatory processes involving clusters of activated microglia and astrocytes, and TNF-α expression [114], functional pre- and postsynaptic cholinergic deficits [115], impaired survival of newborn neuronal cells [116], and increasing levels of insoluble and soluble Aβ40 and Aβ42 in parenchyma and vessel walls [36], [53], [117]. These neurobiological alterations might underlie the mild behavioral changes and cognitive decline observed in our 8-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice in the open field and (r)MWM, since cholinergic neurotransmission and the integration of newborn neuronal cells into the circuitry of the hippocampus are important for learning and memory [67], [118], [119]. Although we did not observe a decrease in the amount of immature neurons in our 12-month-old AβPP-PS1mice, reduced hippocampal neurogenesis has been found previously in AβPP-PS1 mice beyond 8 months of age [69]–[71], as well as expression of IL-1β, IL-6 and MCP-1, suggesting chronic inflammatory processes [114], decreased cerebral blood volume (CBV) [34] and gray and white matter degeneration [120]. Increasing levels of Aβ together with chronic inflammation and decreased delivery of oxygen and nutrients (due to cerebral hypoperfusion) can cause neuronal dysfunction and suppress hippocampal neurogenesis [17], [19], [121], thereby contributing to the more severe cognitive and behavioral dysfunction observed in our 12-month-old AβPP-PS1 mice alongside with a reduction in NAA levels measured with 1H MRS. Furthermore, beyond 12 months of age AβPP-PS1 mice display more progressive AD-like pathology including increased brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels and lower norepinephrine, serotonin and acetylcholine levels [122], [123], hippocampal atrophy and a reduced amount of glucose-transporter type-1 [124], increased amount of presynaptic boutons [42], and a decrease in CBV [125]. However, it should be noted that AβPP-PS1 mice do not develop the widespread neurofibrillary tangle pathology or extensive neurodegeneration as seen in AD patients. Thus it is important to use caution in interpreting results found in AβPP-PS1 (and other transgenic) mice and translating them to the human AD situation.
To summarize, in this paper we characterized the neurochemical profile of the hippocampus, measured by 1H MRS at 7 Tesla, in the brains of 8- and 12-month-old AβPPswe-PS1dE9 mice as compared to age-matched wild-type animals. Our results show that at 8 months of age no alterations in hippocampal metabolite levels could be detected, while behavioral changes and cognitive decline were present in the AβPP-PS1 mouse model. At 12 months of age, a decrease in hippocampal NAA levels, reflecting reduced neuronal integrity, correlated with more severe behavioral and cognitive impairment in AβPP-PS1 mice as compared to wild-type animals. Furthermore, correlation analyses suggest a possible role of Aβ in inflammatory processes, synaptic dysfunction and impaired neurogenesis.
1H MRS could potentially provide unique information about the underlying degenerative processes, because metabolite levels are sensitive to different in vivo pathological processes at the molecular or cellular level. Furthermore, 1H MRS has great potential for the early diagnosis of AD, monitoring disease progression and evaluating the efficacy of potential therapeutic agents, both in animals models and AD patients. However, to observe small changes related to the disease progression, 1H MRS data need to be acquired from a large number of mice. Furthermore, the sensitivity and specificity of 1H MRS depend on the field strength of the MR system, the acquisition parameters and the size and position of the spectroscopic volume of interest. Therefore, we cannot exclude the presence of subtle metabolic alterations at 8 months of age, since our 1H MRS methodology may not have been sensitive enough to detect small but important functional or anatomical abnormalities in the AβPP-PS1mouse hippocampus.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Henk Arnts and Janneke Mulders for their excellent care giving of our mice. The authors would also like to acknowledge Ilse Arnoldussen, Xiaotian Fang, Anne Rijpma and Maximilian Wiesmann for their laboratory work.
Funding Statement
The research leading to these results has received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 211696. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Hardy J, Selkoe DJ (2002) The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297: 353–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Selkoe DJ (2002) Alzheimer’s disease is a synaptic failure. Science 298: 789–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Weller RO, Nicoll JA (2003) Cerebral amyloid angiopathy: pathogenesis and effects on the ageing and Alzheimer brain. Neurol Res 25: 611–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Burgermeister P, Calhoun ME, Winkler DT, Jucker M (2000) Mechanisms of cerebrovascular amyloid deposition. Lessons from mouse models. Ann N Y Acad Sci 903: 307–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Walsh DM, Selkoe DJ (2007) A beta oligomers - a decade of discovery. J Neurochem 101: 1172–1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Larson ME, Lesne SE (2012) Soluble Abeta oligomer production and toxicity. J Neurochem 120 Suppl 1125–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Breteler MM (2000) Vascular risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: an epidemiologic perspective. Neurobiol Aging 21: 153–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Dosunmu R, Wu J, Basha MR, Zawia NH (2007) Environmental and dietary risk factors in Alzheimer’s disease. Expert Rev Neurother 7: 887–900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Meyer JS, Terayama Y, Konno S, Akiyama H, Margishvili GM, et al. (1998) Risk factors for cerebral degenerative changes and dementia. Eur Neurol 39 Suppl 17–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Breteler MM (2000) Vascular involvement in cognitive decline and dementia. Epidemiologic evidence from the Rotterdam Study and the Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann N Y Acad Sci 903: 457–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Skoog I, Gustafson D (2002) Hypertension and related factors in the etiology of Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 977: 29–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Rojo LE, Fernandez JA, Maccioni AA, Jimenez JM, Maccioni RB (2008) Neuroinflammation: implications for the pathogenesis and molecular diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Med Res 39: 1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Franceschi C (2007) Inflammaging as a major characteristic of old people: can it be prevented or cured? Nutr Rev 65: S173–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Giunta B, Fernandez F, Nikolic WV, Obregon D, Rrapo E, et al. (2008) Inflammaging as a prodrome to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuroinflammation 5: 51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Akiyama H, Barger S, Barnum S, Bradt B, Bauer J, et al. (2000) Inflammation and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 21: 383–421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Zilka N, Ferencik M, Hulin I (2006) Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease: protector or promoter? Bratisl Lek Listy 107: 374–383. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ekdahl CT, Claasen JH, Bonde S, Kokaia Z, Lindvall O (2003) Inflammation is detrimental for neurogenesis in adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100: 13632–13637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Monje ML, Palmer T (2003) Radiation injury and neurogenesis. Curr Opin Neurol 16: 129–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Voloboueva LA, Giffard RG (2011) Inflammation, mitochondria, and the inhibition of adult neurogenesis. J Neurosci Res 89: 1989–1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Ross AJ, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Valenzuela MJ, Brodaty H (2005) Cognitive correlates of 1H MRS measures in the healthy elderly brain. Brain Res Bull 66: 9–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Kantarci K, Knopman DS, Dickson DW, Parisi JE, Whitwell JL, et al. (2008) Alzheimer disease: postmortem neuropathologic correlates of antemortem 1H MR spectroscopy metabolite measurements. Radiology 248: 210–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Jones RS, Waldman AD (2004) 1H-MRS evaluation of metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Neurol Res 26: 488–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Kantarci K (2007) 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy in dementia. Br J Radiol 80 Spec No 2: S146–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Schuff N, Amend D, Ezekiel F, Steinman SK, Tanabe J, et al. (1997) Changes of hippocampal N-acetyl aspartate and volume in Alzheimer’s disease. A proton MR spectroscopic imaging and MRI study. Neurology 49: 1513–1521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Griffith HR, den Hollander JA, Okonkwo OC, O’Brien T, Watts RL, et al. (2008) Brain metabolism differs in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease dementia. Alzheimers Dement 4: 421–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Moffett JR, Ross B, Arun P, Madhavarao CN, Namboodiri AM (2007) N-Acetylaspartate in the CNS: from neurodiagnostics to neurobiology. Prog Neurobiol 81: 89–131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Govindaraju V, Young K, Maudsley AA (2000) Proton NMR chemical shifts and coupling constants for brain metabolites. NMR Biomed 13: 129–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Pfefferbaum A, Adalsteinsson E, Spielman D, Sullivan EV, Lim KO (1999) In vivo spectroscopic quantification of the N-acetyl moiety, creatine, and choline from large volumes of brain gray and white matter: effects of normal aging. Magn Reson Med 41: 276–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kantarci K, Petersen RC, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, Tang-Wai DF, et al. (2004) 1H MR spectroscopy in common dementias. Neurology 63: 1393–1398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Huang W, Alexander GE, Chang L, Shetty HU, Krasuski JS, et al. (2001) Brain metabolite concentration and dementia severity in Alzheimer’s disease: a (1)H MRS study. Neurology 57: 626–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Rose SE, de Zubicaray GI, Wang D, Galloway GJ, Chalk JB, et al. (1999) A 1H MRS study of probable Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging: implications for longitudinal monitoring of dementia progression. Magn Reson Imaging 17: 291–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Choi IY, Lee SP, Guilfoyle DN, Helpern JA (2003) In vivo NMR studies of neurodegenerative diseases in transgenic and rodent models. Neurochem Res 28: 987–1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Choi JK, Dedeoglu A, Jenkins BG (2007) Application of MRS to mouse models of neurodegenerative illness. NMR Biomed 20: 216–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zerbi V, Jansen D, Dederen PJ, Veltien A, Hamans B, et al. (2012) Microvascular cerebral blood volume changes in aging APP(swe)/PS1 (dE9) AD mouse model: a voxel-wise approach. Brain Struct Funct. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0448-8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35. Jankowsky JL, Slunt HH, Ratovitski T, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG, et al. (2001) Co-expression of multiple transgenes in mouse CNS: a comparison of strategies. Biomol Eng 17: 157–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Jankowsky JL, Fadale DJ, Anderson J, Xu GM, Gonzales V, et al. (2004) Mutant presenilins specifically elevate the levels of the 42 residue beta-amyloid peptide in vivo: evidence for augmentation of a 42-specific gamma secretase. Hum Mol Genet 13: 159–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Streijger F, Oerlemans F, Ellenbroek BA, Jost CR, Wieringa B, et al. (2005) Structural and behavioural consequences of double deficiency for creatine kinases BCK and UbCKmit. Behav Brain Res 157: 219–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Hooijmans CR, Van der Zee CE, Dederen PJ, Brouwer KM, Reijmer YD, et al. (2009) DHA and cholesterol containing diets influence Alzheimer-like pathology, cognition and cerebral vasculature in APPswe/PS1dE9 mice. Neurobiol Dis 33: 482–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. de Bruin JP, Sanchez-Santed F, Heinsbroek RP, Donker A, Postmes P (1994) A behavioural analysis of rats with damage to the medial prefrontal cortex using the Morris water maze: evidence for behavioural flexibility, but not for impaired spatial navigation. Brain Res 652: 323–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G (1997) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press. xxii, [ca. 187 p.) p.
- 41. Cavassila S, Deval S, Huegen C, van Ormondt D, Graveron-Demilly D (2001) Cramer-Rao bounds: an evaluation tool for quantitation. NMR Biomed 14: 278–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Jansen D, Janssen CI, Vanmierlo T, Dederen PJ, van Rooij D, et al. (2012) Cholesterol and synaptic compensatory mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease mice brain during aging. J Alzheimers Dis 31: 813–826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Jahn R, Sudhof TC (1993) Synaptic vesicle traffic: rush hour in the nerve terminal. J Neurochem 61: 12–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Gleeson JG, Lin PT, Flanagan LA, Walsh CA (1999) Doublecortin is a microtubule-associated protein and is expressed widely by migrating neurons. Neuron 23: 257–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Francis F, Koulakoff A, Boucher D, Chafey P, Schaar B, et al. (1999) Doublecortin is a developmentally regulated, microtubule-associated protein expressed in migrating and differentiating neurons. Neuron 23: 247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Irizarry MC, Soriano F, McNamara M, Page KJ, Schenk D, et al. (1997) Abeta deposition is associated with neuropil changes, but not with overt neuronal loss in the human amyloid precursor protein V717F (PDAPP) transgenic mouse. J Neurosci 17: 7053–7059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Shukla C, Bridges LR (1999) Regional distribution of tau, beta-amyloid and beta-amyloid precursor protein in the Alzheimer’s brain: a quantitative immunolabelling study. Neuroreport 10: 3785–3789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Mulder M, Koopmans G, Wassink G, Al Mansouri G, Simard ML, et al. (2007) LDL receptor deficiency results in decreased cell proliferation and presynaptic bouton density in the murine hippocampus. Neurosci Res 59: 251–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Rutten BP, Van der Kolk NM, Schafer S, van Zandvoort MA, Bayer TA, et al. (2005) Age-related loss of synaptophysin immunoreactive presynaptic boutons within the hippocampus of APP751SL, PS1M146L, and APP751SL/PS1M146L transgenic mice. Am J Pathol 167: 161–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Simon P, Dupuis R, Costentin J (1994) Thigmotaxis as an index of anxiety in mice. Influence of dopaminergic transmissions. Behav Brain Res 61: 59–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Lalonde R, Kim HD, Fukuchi K (2004) Exploratory activity, anxiety, and motor coordination in bigenic APPswe+PS1/DeltaE9 mice. Neurosci Lett 369: 156–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Lalonde R, Kim HD, Maxwell JA, Fukuchi K (2005) Exploratory activity and spatial learning in 12-month-old APP(695)SWE/co+PS1/DeltaE9 mice with amyloid plaques. Neurosci Lett 390: 87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Puolivali J, Wang J, Heikkinen T, Heikkila M, Tapiola T, et al. (2002) Hippocampal A beta 42 levels correlate with spatial memory deficit in APP and PS1 double transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis 9: 339–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Lalonde R, Fukuchi K, Strazielle C (2012) APP transgenic mice for modelling behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 1357–1375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Pugh PL, Richardson JC, Bate ST, Upton N, Sunter D (2007) Non-cognitive behaviours in an APP/PS1 transgenic model of Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 178: 18–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Grossberg GT (2003) Diagnosis and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Psychiatry 64 Suppl 93–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Patterson MB, Schnell AH, Martin RJ, Mendez MF, Smyth KA, et al. (1990) Assessment of behavioral and affective symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 3: 21–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Vorhees CV, Williams MT (2006) Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc 1: 848–858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Van Dam D, Lenders G, De Deyn PP (2006) Effect of Morris water maze diameter on visual-spatial learning in different mouse strains. Neurobiol Learn Mem 85: 164–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Yoshiike Y, Kimura T, Yamashita S, Furudate H, Mizoroki T, et al. (2008) GABA(A) receptor-mediated acceleration of aging-associated memory decline in APP/PS1 mice and its pharmacological treatment by picrotoxin. PLoS One 3: e3029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Stover KR, Brown RE (2012) Age-related changes in visual acuity, learning and memory in the APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 231: 75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Yang SG, Wang SW, Zhao M, Zhang R, Zhou WW, et al. (2012) A peptide binding to the beta-site of APP improves spatial memory and attenuates Abeta burden in Alzheimer’s disease transgenic mice. PLoS One 7: e48540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Su D, Zhao Y, Xu H, Wang B, Chen X, et al. (2012) Isoflurane exposure during mid-adulthood attenuates age-related spatial memory impairment in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. PLoS One 7: e50172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Lovasic L, Bauschke H, Janus C (2005) Working memory impairment in a transgenic amyloid precursor protein TgCRND8 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Genes Brain Behav 4: 197–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Janus C (2004) Search strategies used by APP transgenic mice during navigation in the Morris water maze. Learn Mem 11: 337–346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Brody DL, Holtzman DM (2006) Morris water maze search strategy analysis in PDAPP mice before and after experimental traumatic brain injury. Exp Neurol 197: 330–340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Garthe A, Behr J, Kempermann G (2009) Adult-generated hippocampal neurons allow the flexible use of spatially precise learning strategies. PLoS One 4: e5464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. O’Leary TP, Brown RE (2009) Visuo-spatial learning and memory deficits on the Barnes maze in the 16-month-old APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Brain Res 201: 120–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Taniuchi N, Niidome T, Goto Y, Akaike A, Kihara T, et al. (2007) Decreased proliferation of hippocampal progenitor cells in APPswe/PS1dE9 transgenic mice. Neuroreport 18: 1801–1805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Niidome T, Taniuchi N, Akaike A, Kihara T, Sugimoto H (2008) Differential regulation of neurogenesis in two neurogenic regions of APPswe/PS1dE9 transgenic mice. Neuroreport 19: 1361–1364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Hamilton A, Holscher C (2012) The effect of ageing on neurogenesis and oxidative stress in the APP(swe)/PS1(deltaE9) mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res 1449: 83–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Enciu AM, Nicolescu MI, Manole CG, Muresanu DF, Popescu LM, et al. (2011) Neuroregeneration in neurodegenerative disorders. BMC Neurol 11: 75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Lazarov O, Marr RA (2010) Neurogenesis and Alzheimer’s disease: at the crossroads. Exp Neurol 223: 267–281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. DeKosky ST, Scheff SW, Styren SD (1996) Structural correlates of cognition in dementia: quantification and assessment of synapse change. Neurodegeneration 5: 417–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Scheff SW, Price DA (2003) Synaptic pathology in Alzheimer’s disease: a review of ultrastructural studies. Neurobiol Aging 24: 1029–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Terry RD, Masliah E, Salmon DP, Butters N, DeTeresa R, et al. (1991) Physical basis of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease: synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 30: 572–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Hamos JE, DeGennaro LJ, Drachman DA (1989) Synaptic loss in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Neurology 39: 355–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Scheff SW, Sparks DL, Price DA (1996) Quantitative assessment of synaptic density in the outer molecular layer of the hippocampal dentate gyrus in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia 7: 226–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Scheff SW, Price DA (1998) Synaptic density in the inner molecular layer of the hippocampal dentate gyrus in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 57: 1146–1153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Leuba G, Savioz A, Vernay A, Carnal B, Kraftsik R, et al. (2008) Differential changes in synaptic proteins in the Alzheimer frontal cortex with marked increase in PSD-95 postsynaptic protein. J Alzheimers Dis 15: 139–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Rutten BP, Wirths O, Van de Berg WD, Lichtenthaler SF, Vehoff J, et al. (2003) No alterations of hippocampal neuronal number and synaptic bouton number in a transgenic mouse model expressing the beta-cleaved C-terminal APP fragment. Neurobiol Dis 12: 110–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Mucke L, Masliah E, Yu GQ, Mallory M, Rockenstein EM, et al. (2000) High-level neuronal expression of abeta 1–42 in wild-type human amyloid protein precursor transgenic mice: synaptotoxicity without plaque formation. J Neurosci 20: 4050–4058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Masliah E, Sisk A, Mallory M, Mucke L, Schenk D, et al. (1996) Comparison of neurodegenerative pathology in transgenic mice overexpressing V717F beta-amyloid precursor protein and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 16: 5795–5811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Dong H, Martin MV, Chambers S, Csernansky JG (2007) Spatial relationship between synapse loss and beta-amyloid deposition in Tg2576 mice. J Comp Neurol 500: 311–321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Yao PJ, Bushlin I, Furukawa K (2005) Preserved synaptic vesicle recycling in hippocampal neurons in a mouse Alzheimer’s disease model. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 330: 34–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. King DL, Arendash GW (2002) Maintained synaptophysin immunoreactivity in Tg2576 transgenic mice during aging: correlations with cognitive impairment. Brain Res 926: 58–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Perneger TV (1998) What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. Bmj 316: 1236–1238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Siegel AF (1990) Research Issues: Multiple Tests: Some Practical Considerations. TESOL Quarterly 24: 773–775. [Google Scholar]
- 89. Brown JD, Crookes G (1990) Research Issues: The Use of Multiple t Tests in Language Research. TESOL Quarterly 24: 770–773. [Google Scholar]
- 90. Boncristiano S, Calhoun ME, Howard V, Bondolfi L, Kaeser SA, et al. (2005) Neocortical synaptic bouton number is maintained despite robust amyloid deposition in APP23 transgenic mice. Neurobiol Aging 26: 607–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Mukaetova-Ladinska EB, Garcia-Siera F, Hurt J, Gertz HJ, Xuereb JH, et al. (2000) Staging of cytoskeletal and beta-amyloid changes in human isocortex reveals biphasic synaptic protein response during progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Pathol 157: 623–636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Masliah E, Mallory M, Hansen L, DeTeresa R, Alford M, et al. (1994) Synaptic and neuritic alterations during the progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 174: 67–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Bronfman FC, Moechars D, Van Leuven F (2000) Acetylcholinesterase-positive fiber deafferentation and cell shrinkage in the septohippocampal pathway of aged amyloid precursor protein london mutant transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis 7: 152–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Masliah E, Crews L, Hansen L (2006) Synaptic remodeling during aging and in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 9: 91–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Arendt T (2001) Alzheimer’s disease as a disorder of mechanisms underlying structural brain self-organization. Neuroscience 102: 723–765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Kantarci K, Weigand SD, Przybelski SA, Shiung MM, Whitwell JL, et al. (2009) Risk of dementia in MCI: combined effect of cerebrovascular disease, volumetric MRI, and 1H MRS. Neurology 72: 1519–1525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Zhang B, Li M, Sun ZZ, Zhu B, Yuan L, et al. (2009) Evaluation of functional MRI markers in mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Neurosci 16: 635–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Rupsingh R, Borrie M, Smith M, Wells JL, Bartha R (2011) Reduced hippocampal glutamate in Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging 32: 802–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Watanabe T, Shiino A, Akiguchi I (2012) Hippocampal metabolites and memory performances in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Learn Mem 97: 289–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Dedeoglu A, Choi JK, Cormier K, Kowall NW, Jenkins BG (2004) Magnetic resonance spectroscopic analysis of Alzheimer’s disease mouse brain that express mutant human APP shows altered neurochemical profile. Brain Res 1012: 60–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Marjanska M, Curran GL, Wengenack TM, Henry PG, Bliss RL, et al. (2005) Monitoring disease progression in transgenic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 11906–11910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. von Kienlin M, Kunnecke B, Metzger F, Steiner G, Richards JG, et al. (2005) Altered metabolic profile in the frontal cortex of PS2APP transgenic mice, monitored throughout their life span. Neurobiol Dis 18: 32–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Jack CR Jr, Marjanska M, Wengenack TM, Reyes DA, Curran GL, et al. (2007) Magnetic resonance imaging of Alzheimer’s pathology in the brains of living transgenic mice: a new tool in Alzheimer’s disease research. Neuroscientist 13: 38–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Oberg J, Spenger C, Wang FH, Andersson A, Westman E, et al. (2008) Age related changes in brain metabolites observed by 1H MRS in APP/PS1 mice. Neurobiol Aging 29: 1423–1433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Mlynarik V, Cacquevel M, Sun-Reimer L, Janssens S, Cudalbu C, et al. (2012) Proton and phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy of a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 31 Suppl 3S87–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Woo DC, Lee SH, Lee DW, Kim SY, Kim GY, et al. (2010) Regional metabolic alteration of Alzheimer’s disease in mouse brain expressing mutant human APP-PS1 by 1H HR-MAS. Behav Brain Res 211: 125–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Xu W, Zhan Y, Huang W, Wang X, Zhang S, et al. (2010) Reduction of hippocampal N-acetyl aspartate level in aged APP(Swe)/PS1(dE9) transgenic mice is associated with degeneration of CA3 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci Res 88: 3155–3160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Jessen F, Gur O, Block W, Ende G, Frolich L, et al. (2009) A multicenter (1)H-MRS study of the medial temporal lobe in AD and MCI. Neurology 72: 1735–1740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Kantarci K, Weigand SD, Petersen RC, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, et al. (2007) Longitudinal 1H MRS changes in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 28: 1330–1339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Satlin A, Bodick N, Offen WW, Renshaw PF (1997) Brain proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) in Alzheimer’s disease: changes after treatment with xanomeline, an M1 selective cholinergic agonist. Am J Psychiatry 154: 1459–1461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Frederick B, Satlin A, Wald LL, Hennen J, Bodick N, et al. (2002) Brain proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in Alzheimer disease: changes after treatment with xanomeline. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 81–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Chen SQ, Cai Q, Shen YY, Wang PJ, Teng GJ, et al. (2012) Age-related changes in brain metabolites and cognitive function in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. Behav Brain Res 235: 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Chen SQ, Wang PJ, Ten GJ, Zhan W, Li MH, et al. (2009) Role of myo-inositol by magnetic resonance spectroscopy in early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in APP/PS1 transgenic mice. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 28: 558–566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Ruan L, Kang Z, Pei G, Le Y (2009) Amyloid deposition and inflammation in APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 6: 531–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Machova E, Rudajev V, Smyckova H, Koivisto H, Tanila H, et al. (2010) Functional cholinergic damage develops with amyloid accumulation in young adult APPswe/PS1dE9 transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis 38: 27–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Verret L, Jankowsky JL, Xu GM, Borchelt DR, Rampon C (2007) Alzheimer’s-type amyloidosis in transgenic mice impairs survival of newborn neurons derived from adult hippocampal neurogenesis. J Neurosci 27: 6771–6780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Garcia-Alloza M, Robbins EM, Zhang-Nunes SX, Purcell SM, Betensky RA, et al. (2006) Characterization of amyloid deposition in the APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Dis 24: 516–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Hasselmo ME (2006) The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16: 710–715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Deng W, Aimone JB, Gage FH (2010) New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 339–350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Zerbi V, Kleinnijenhuis M, Fang X, Jansen D, Veltien A, et al. (2012) Gray and white matter degeneration revealed by diffusion in an Alzheimer mouse model. Neurobiol Aging 34: 1440–1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Palop JJ, Mucke L (2010) Amyloid-beta-induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat Neurosci 13: 812–818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Szapacs ME, Numis AL, Andrews AM (2004) Late onset loss of hippocampal 5-HT and NE is accompanied by increases in BDNF protein expression in mice co-expressing mutant APP and PS1. Neurobiol Dis 16: 572–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Savonenko A, Xu GM, Melnikova T, Morton JL, Gonzales V, et al. (2005) Episodic-like memory deficits in the APPswe/PS1dE9 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease: relationships to beta-amyloid deposition and neurotransmitter abnormalities. Neurobiol Dis 18: 602–617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Hooijmans CR, Graven C, Dederen PJ, Tanila H, van Groen T, et al. (2007) Amyloid beta deposition is related to decreased glucose transporter-1 levels and hippocampal atrophy in brains of aged APP/PS1 mice. Brain Res 1181: 93–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Hooijmans CR, Rutters F, Dederen PJ, Gambarota G, Veltien A, et al. (2007) Changes in cerebral blood volume and amyloid pathology in aged Alzheimer APP/PS1 mice on a docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) diet or cholesterol enriched Typical Western Diet (TWD). Neurobiol Dis 28: 16–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]